Sie sind auf Seite 1von 15

Developmental Psychology Copyright 1987 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1987, Vol. 23, No. 6, 747-761 OOI2-l649/87/$OO75

Development of Early Expressive and Communicative Action:


Reinterpreting the Evidence From a Dynamic Systems Perspective
Alan Fogel Esther Thelen
Department of Child Development and Family Studies Indiana University
Purdue University

A systems approach is introduced and applied to the development of expressive and communicative
action of infants in thefirstyear of life. In this approach, expressive and communicative actions are
organized, as part of cooperative systems with other elements of the infants' physiology, cognition,
behavior, and social environment. A systems approach presumes that order arises dynamically as a
result of the interaction between the cooperating elements that are changing asynchronously, rather
than as the result of centrally coordinated developmental change that is synchronous across domains.
The systems approach further assumes that the control parameter responsible for eliciting develop-
mental change may be different depending on age, context, and task. It offers a means to understand
previously unexplained developmental phenomena: the appearance of mature forms of expression
before mature function has been achieved, the asynchronous rates of development of communica-
tive-action components, discontinuous developmental shifts arising from continuous processes, and
the process by which adults influence communicative development.

Theoretical Perspective forms? The traditional "nativist" perspectives assume that de-
velopmental changes are embodied in the genes, or represented
This article outlines an approach to the development of ex- in neural programs. Experience can modify maturational rate
pressive and communicative behavior in infancy that is rooted but it cannot introduce qualitatively new forms. This model is
in developmental biology, movement science, and systems the- difficult to falsify because it can always be defended by an ap-
ory. In this approach, expressive and communicative actions
peal to some unobservable black-box cause. Contemporary the-
are organized as a complex, cooperative system with other ele-
ories of development of cognition, language, and social and
ments of the infant's physiology, behavior, and social environ-
emotional realms fall under the umbrella of what Wolff calls
ment. Development of behavior does not occur uniformly
across these domains as the singular result of an increasingly the epigenetic-constructivist-interactionist point of view. Rather
elaborated set of structures or prescriptions, but by the emer- than rely on the nativist assumptions of biologically driven de-
gent order resulting from asynchronous changes between the velopmental change, this view states that the organism develops
component actions. We assume developmental processes are by acting on the environment, "restructuring itself as it changes
multicausal, nonlinear, and complex. Although this complexity the environment" (Wolff, 1987, p. 234). Theories of this type
and nonlinearity confounds experimental analysis and makes are those of Werner (1948), Vygotsky (1978), and Piaget (1963).
simple interactive models untenable, in real systems, it is the In Piaget's theory, development tends toward increasingly sta-
source of both stability and change. ble and equilibrated epistemic states via the invariant processes
How does our perspective differ from traditional theories of of assimilation and accommodation. In Werner's version of the
development? Reese and Overton (1970), Sameroff(1984), and theory, stages have an implicit hierarchical relation proceeding
Wolff (1987) analyzed developmental theories by asking the through an invariant and irreversible sequence overtime.These
question: What accounts for the induction of novel behavioral epigenetic perspectives, on close analysis, fail to explain how
equilibration can induce novel forms that are not already some-
how contained in the organism's structure or in the invariant
functions(Haroutunian, l983;Oyama, 1985; Wolff, 1987). As
This article was prepared with the help of National Institutes of
Health Grant 1R01 HD21036-O1 Al, and a Hatch Act grant from the Wolff succinctly states, "this perspective has not entirely elimi-
Purdue University Agricultural Experiment station to Alan Fogel, and nated its nativist core or its ultimate appeal to a priori executive
National Science Foundation Grant BNS85-09793 and National Insti- agencies that anticipate the direction and end-stages of mental
tutes of Health Grant K04 HD721 to Esther Thelen. development" (1987, p. 247).
We thank J. A. S. Kelso for his patient tutorials on dynamic systems In contrast to theories in which development is seen as pro-
theory and gratefully acknowledge the comments of the following per- gressing from global to more articulated states, our views are
sons on earlier versions of the article: Lauren Adamson, Amanda Bar-
aligned with general systems theory applied to the morphologi-
che, Kurt Fischer, Hui-Chin Hsu, Jeanne Karns, Eva Nwokah, Andy
Stremmel, Sueko Toda, and Ted Wachs. cal development of the embryo (Sameroff, 1984), with dynamic
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alan systems theory applied to the newbom's adaptation to the post-
Fogel, Department of Child Development and Family Studies, Purdue natal environment (Wolff, 1987), and with domain theory ap-
University, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907. plied to the development of knowledge and skill (Fischer & Can-
747
748 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

field, 1986; Turiel & Davidson, 1986). According to domain erate specific predictions in particular domains. Nonetheless,
theory, for example, knowledge acquisition over age is asyn- with the continued elaboration of synergetic principles to bio-
chronous across domains, and, at any given age, performance logical domains (e.g., Kelso & Schoner, in press), we believe this
is an emergent product of the heterogeneity of cross-domain approach holds promise both for more unifying principles and
knowledge and the nature of the environmental supports and for more predictive specificity in developmental analysis.
tasks. This approach was constructed in order to explain some
of the developmental "irregularities" of performance that did
not fit neatly into stage theories in which knowledge in all do-
Dynamic Action Theory
mains is presumed to advance apace. Similar puzzles of com- The behaving human, like other organisms, is a complex, co-
municative ontogeny have inspired the formulation presented operative system of high dimensionality on many scales: ana-
in this article. tomical units of muscles, joints, bones, and organ systems;
physiological processes that maintain and regulate life; and
Some Puzzles ofEarly Expressive Development neurological units of perception, integration, and motor con-
trol. If one were to count the elements involved in even a simple
By 1 year of age, the human infant has a wide repertoire of act—a smile or a pointing gesture—a nearly infinite number of
facial, manual, postural, locoraotor, vocal, and, sometimes, ver- degrees offreedom would be involved. Yet from these nearly
bal gestures with which to communicate affect and intent. Some infinite degrees of freedom, behavior emerges as a fundamen-
of these gestures have been available since birth, but change in tally low-dimensional output—as a recognizable pattern of
form and function during the first year. Newborn infants, for movement that is stable both within an individual and between
example, show differential facial expressions appropriate to members of the species (Bernstein, 1967; Kelso & Scholz,
taste or smell stimuli (Steiner, 1979), but newborn smiles occur 1986).
only in sleep. A smiling response to an appropriate social stimu- How can an internally complex system, composed of thou-
lus emerges only after 1 or 2 months. Crying communicates dis- sands of neural elements and hundreds of muscle groups, be
tress throughout the first year, yet undergoes dramatic develop- compressed to produce such low-dimensional output? One so-
mental change in acoustic structure and intentionality lution is to have plans or programs encoded in a central control-
(Zeskind* 1985). Manual actions, such as pointing, have been ling agency, which in turn marshalls the lower elements partici-
identified in 3-month-oids during social interaction, which is pating in the action. This solution is not satisfactory, however,
many months before those actions are assimilated into their because it only shifts the "degrees of freedom" problem to a
mature and conventionally functional form (Fogel & Hannan, higher level, requiring the controller to represent and store the
1985; Trevarthen, 1977). component elements and perform the necessary computations
Communicative development, like development in other do- and transformations. These control problems become monu-
mains, is puzzling because it is heterochronic; that is, elements mental in the face of the nearly infinite variability demanded
of behavior appear in an uneven sequence and are asynchro- by everyday tasks and because movements themselves are dy-
nously timed. As a consequence, these observations cannot be namic, with continually changing forces over time and space
explained by those organismic stage theories in which develop- (Bernstein, 1967).
ment of all the elements is presumed to occur in synchrony, A more satisfactory solution is to compress the internal de-
controlled by an increasingly articulated centrally organized grees of freedom of high-dimensional systems by constraining
structure. We offer instead a dynamic model where elements the elements to relate among themselves in a relatively fixed
can be assembled for functional ends in a more fluid, task-spe- and autonomous manner. In the motor system, a coordinative
cific manner determined equally by the rnaturational status and structure is a functional grouping of muscles and joints, whose
experiences of the child and by the current context of the action. mutual coordinations are regulated synergisticaily, as a whole
We assign causal equivalence to all of the elements of the sys- system (Kelso, Holt, Kugler, & Turvey, 1980; Kugler, Kelso, &
tem, including the context and the effector apparatus, suggest- Turvey, 1980; Turvey, 1977). Thus, the elements of a smile are
ing that behavior may be understood only as the systems prod- not controlled as individual muscles, each receiving a centrally
uct of these elements. generated command, but as a unitary, functional, interdepen-
We begin with a general theory of motor action that estab- dent system. In a smile, for example, the retraction of one lip
lishes how a movement can be both structurally stable and func- corner is not independent of the retraction of the other; in
tionally flexible and that relies on systems principles of emer- smiles of greater intensity the eyes and brows are also synergisti-
gent properties. The use of motor theory does not imply a pri- caily involved. This preferential linkage of certain muscle com-
macy for effectors over other developing components, but stands binations and sequences produces actions of a stable and recog-
as a paradigm for analyzing complex systems. We then show nizable topography: Smiles are seen as smiles despite great vari-
how these principles can be extended to ontogenetic systems as ability in intensity and context. We will show how infants may
well. Finally, we propose a role for the physical and social con- be endowed with such preferential linkages for actions at a very
text in the emergence of new communicative forms. In the sec- early age, which then form the substrate for further develop-
ond part of this article, we apply this dynamic system approach mental change.
to three types of early expressive actions: crying, smiling, and How can groups of neuromuscular elements be both stable
pointing. and autonomous? How can the trajectories of movements be
In its current formulation, our approach offers more a means generated without symbolic representations stored in the cen-
for framing questions about development than a method to gen- tral nervous system? There are many examples of pattern gen-
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 749

eration without implicit instructions in the physical world, in- dination is not determined by a "switch" somewhere in the ner-
cluding crystal growth and other chemical phenomena, turbu- vous system that calls forth a new program, but is emergent
lent flow patterns, cloud formation, and even the oscillating from dynamic constraints in the system's anatomical structure
movements of a simple spring. (For recent reviews of complex and its unique and nonlinear dissipation of energy. Order
systems theory in physics, biology, and chemistry, see Haken, emerges as a dynamic rather than as a prescriptive phenomena.
1983; Prigogine, 1980; Prigogine & Stengers, 1984; Rosen,
1978.) In each case, structural or temporal regularity results
from the assembly of the system under certain energy con- Process ofStability and Change: A Dynamic Systems
straints, that is, an order not simply predicted by the nature of Approach
the single elements alone, such as the molecules in the aggrega-
tion. In addition, it is characteristic of such systems to respond The motor theory just reviewed is based on a set of principles
to changes in certain parameters (e.g., the energy supplied to that we believe have general applicability to a variety of aspects
the system) in a discontinuous manner. If the pressure on water of human functioning, not only those in the motor realm. Spe-
flowing in a pipe is increased linearly, the resulting flow pat- cifically, we propose that a set of components, some from the
terns—random, turbulent, or laminar—appear as nonlinear individual and some from the context, can be organized in a
phase shifts, that is, without stable intermediate states. We pro- fluid and task-specific manner so that the resulting behavior is
pose that similar emergent phase shifts play a central role in the systems product of all these components. The phase shifts
explaining behavioral change in both the real-time, motor that arise in real time, as components change in relation to each
realm and in development. other and the context, can be used as a general model for how
complex systems change in ontogenetic time. Generalizing
One important systems property of coordinative structures from a motor theory to the development of communicative ac-
is that they simplify the problems of storage and execution of tion does not, in other words, imply that communication is an
action plans by generating new information that is not present exclusively motor process.
in the initial conditions of the structure of the muscles, bones,
and neurons. In the postural system, for example, as a person Stability and change are both fundamental characteristics of
is shifted off balance, gravity and local changes in the velocity biological systems in general and coordinative structures in par-
and acceleration of the trunk evoke compensatory responses in ticular. In spite of high dimensionality of the components and
the limbs and trunk that synergistically produce a stable pos- a variety of initial conditions and environmental perturbations,
ture. It is unnecessary to invoke a centrally stored postural "set coordinative structures can maintain their characteristic topog-
point" that corrects for fluctuations in a typical homeostatic raphy and function to reach a goal. The same components that
fashion. Rather, the dynamic interaction of the moving seg- create stability may also be involved in shifting the system dis-
ments is enough to produce balance as an emergent systems continuously to some other stable configuration. How can this
property of the unique combination of displacements in that happen?
particular situation. Thus, stability of behavioral outcomes is
an important characteristic of coordinative structures. As an- Stability
other example, a speaker can utter a target phoneme despite
natural perturbations of speech (such as with food in the With a finite population of possible component structures
mouth) or experimentally induced perturbations introduced and processes within the individual, the constraints of the task
during the utterance (Kelso & Tuller, 1984). Presumably, the produce a dynamic cooperativity of the components. Depend-
combination of tongue, jaw, and lip articulators used in the ut- ing on the task and on the current state and ontogenetic history
terance are not "hard-wired" to produce a ba sound, but are of the individual, some components will contribute dispropor-
flexibly assembled to self-adjust to the ongoing demands of nat- tionately to the behavioral outcome. These dominant compo-
ural speech and, even, to preserve the speech goal in the face nents will appear to "drive" the system, or act as synchronizers
of large perturbations. Coordinative structures, therefore, are for other ongoing components. The nature of the cooperativity
sources of order and stability, not mere recipients of com mands. is not rigidly fixed beforehand, but is strictly a function of the
A second important systems property of coordinative struc- status of the organism in a particular task context. Thus, behav-
tures are phase shifts. This means that functionally assembled ioral outcome is never "hard-wired," but is characterized by
coordinative structures retain a topographic stability within a an inherent indetermi nancy that occurs when low-dimensional
range of values of certain parameters, but when those critical behavior is assembled from the high-dimensional array of com-
values are exceeded, new modes of coordination may emerge. ponents (Weiss, 1969; Wolff, 1987).
The gait patterns of a quadruped are a useful example. When a However, within a range of contexts and organismic states,
horse increases the speed of its locomotion in a linear fashion, systems theories predict that behavior will retain a dynamic sta-
the resulting gait patterns are distinctly nonlinear. That is, the bility, that is, an overall task orientation and resistance to minor
horse will walk first, then trot, then gallop. Each pattern is perturbations (in the terminology of dynamics, these are re-
qualitatively distinct, with no stable intermediate stages. Sim- ferred to as attractor states [Abraham & Shaw, 1982]). Thus,
ilarly, if adults are asked to flex and extend their hands at the the systems will tend to prefer certain response modes (from
wrist in an asymmetrical mode (one handflexeswhile the other all possible compressions of the degrees of freedom), and these
extends) and to speed up the movements, they abruptly shift preferred responses may be stable during a particular time scale
into a symmetrical mode at a predictable point in the speed of observation. For example, one way of interpreting Wadding-
scalar (Kelso & Scholz, 1986). In both cases, the interlimb coor- ton's notion of canalization is that as organisms develop, their
750 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

response modes or attractor states become increasingly stable functional system to another. However, the environment can
and well-defined. also move the system into new modes, leading to a behavioral
performance that a child alone, or in another environment,
Change could not produce. There are many examples in cognitive de-
velopment in which contextual manipulations, adult assistance,
In both real and developmental time, new states emerge when or practice reveals abilities in children thought to reside in later-
components are pushed past critical values or the relations developing stages (see, for example, Turiel & Davidson, 1986).
among the components change, leading to discontinuous phase We present examples below in which parents especially may act
shifts in the low-dimensional output. Specifically, we envision as control parameters and temporarily enhance performance.
the sources of developmental change as arising in two ways: In the motor system examples of quadruped gait and human
1. Scalar changes in one or more existing components. manual movements mentioned earlier, the scalar increase in en-
Linear and continuous changes in single- or multiple-compo- ergy delivered to the limbs (increasing speed) was the control
nent structures and processes may act as the catalysts to shift parameter that shifted the movements from one stable mode of
the entire system into new modes of organization. The compo- coordination to another, qualitatively distinct mode. The identi-
nent that acts as the primary agent of change is known in dy- fication of the control parameter in these real-time motor exam-
namic systems terminology as the control parameter, or the ele- ples is straightforward, but, in developing systems, the nature of
ment driving the system through a phase shift (Kelso & Scholz, the control parameter may be less than obvious. When human
1986). Note that all the component elements are essential to the infants shift into new developmental stages, it is often assumed
system outcome. The control parameter is no more important that improvements in the most salient component have driven
than any other system component, because the low-dimen- the new stage. On closer inspection, however, some of these sa-
sional output is uniquely determined by all the components act- lient components appeared in the infant's repertoire considera-
ing together. bly earlier than the performance of the target behavior. Some-
This dynamic view of development is similar to Soil's (1979) times, these components are present at early ages, but at low
concept of rate-limiting processes. Soil discovered that in the frequencies. In other cases, these components are used fre-
morphogenesis of the slime mold dictyostelium, several pro- quently, but as part of entirely different functional systems. In
cesses developing in parallel were essential for the appearance either case, the identification of an important component before
of each developmental stage. However, the appearance of the the appearance of the fully functional behavior suggests that the
distinctive stage was a function of the slowest process, which control parameter driving the system into the new stage must
acted as the timer, or control parameter, to shift the organism be sought in other organismic structures or processes, or in the
into a new morphology. That is, to produce stage xf the products nature of the context or task.
of processes a, b, ct and d are all essential. If a, b, and c are It is difficult to identify the control parameters in developing
accumulated to sufficient concentrations, then the process lead- systems, because unlike real-time systems, in which the compo-
ing to the minimum concentration of d will regulate the pro- nent structures and processes are relatively stationary, the com-
cess. Soil identified at least six such rate-limiting processes, each ponents in developing systems themselves change over time in
specific for a particular stage. He determined that the processes a nonlinear and asynchronous manner. Although in broad
worked in parallel because an environmental variable, temper- sweep, developmental events appear sequential and synchro-
ature, affected the appearance of the stages differentially, sug- nous, the particular elements may be comparatively accelerated
gesting that different processes acted as control parameters foT or retarded in relation to one another, and, within each compo-
different stages. We propose below that control parameters also nent, there may be spurts and times of relative quiescence. In
change during human development. short, development can be envisioned as a heterochronic series
Because development is holistic, a small change in the control of different developmental processes, each with separate but in-
parameter can reverberate and result in major consequences for teracting timetables (Rader, 1985; Satoh, 1985). At any given
the entire system. Major reorganizations of the organism char- point in time, however, the behavior of the organism as a whole
acterized as stage shifts, therefore, may not be regulated by cen- cannot be predicted by the developmental status of only one
trally organized instructions. Developmental change may be a component, because it is the systems interactions among the
spontaneous, systemic reorganization of subsystems that occurs components that is crucial. This leads us to suggest a second
only when all of the necessary components become available source of development change.
and when the task recruits and coalesces those components. 2. Change from one control parameter to another. We hy-
Ontogenetic changes may be controlled not by an epigenetic pothesized that the underlying process in developmental shifts
ground plan, but by changes in particular control parameters was the scalar change of one or several control parameters.
(Fogel, 1985;Oyama, 1985;Thelen, 1985b; Wolff, 1987). However, as components themselves grow and differentiate and
According to our approach, the control parameter moving as the physical and social contexts of the infant and child change
the system into new forms of behavior may, at any particular as a result of its development, new components may assume the
time, reside within the organism or within the task context. role of control parameters. Indeed, the very striking changes
There is no formal difference between endogenous and exoge- both in the infant, and in its ecological niche during the first
nous sources of change because organismic and contextual vari- year would predict that different structures and processes would
ables are equally important in the dynamic assembly of behav- drive the system at different ages. The control parameters for
ior. The control parameter may arise from within an individual some developmental transitions may be changes in underlying
as a result of maturation or the shift of a component from one physical or physiological variables, including nonneural com-
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 751

ponents such as somatic growth or hormonal status. Other shifts single-causal explanations would suggest (Thelen, 1984, 1986;
may be directly related to observable events in the central ner- Thelen & Cooke, 1987).
vous system; the maturation of the hippocampus, for example, We examine specifically two developmental transitions: the
has been implicated in the appearance of certain types of spatial "disappearance" of newborn stepping, and the subsequent on-
memory (Nadel & Zola-Morgan, 1984). Whatever the neuro- set of independent locomotion. In the first month or so of life,
logical substrate, there is considerable evidence that major reor- infants perform "stepping" movements when held upright and
ganizations in behavior occur at definable periods during in- when they are sufficiently awake and aroused to move. General
fancy (e.g., McCall, Eichorn, & Hogarty, 1977) and that these behavioral activation appears to be the control parameter mov-
cognitive changes may have profound and system-wide rever- ing the system through real-time phase shifts of no-stepping
berations, acting as control parameters in a number of domains. (asleep or drowsy), stepping (awake, alert, fussy) to no-stepping
However, the identification of control parameters in both real (crying hard) (Thelen, Fisher, Ridley-Johnson, & Griffin,
and ontogenetic time must remain an empirical exercise, be- 1982). At about 2 months, however, awake infants no longer
cause developmentalists have too easily sought to explain phase usually step when held upright. Thelen & Fisher (1982) pro-
shifts by resorting to neurology or black-box cognitive reorgani- posed that the acceleration of one component process—the de-
zations. Although hippocampal maturation may be a necessary position of subcutaneous body fat, which is especially rapid in
component of spatial memory, the control parameter for partic- the first postnatal months—outpaces the growth of another
ular spatial skills may indeed be the onset of independent loco- component process, the acquisition of muscle mass and
motion (Bertenthal, Campos, & Barrett, 1984). Pipp, Fischer, strength, so that infants are unable to lift their increasingly
and Jennings (1987), showed that the acquisition of knowledge heavy legs. Reducing the biomechanical demands on the legs by
about self and mother had both age- and domain-specific con- submerging infants waist-high in water or placing them supine
trol parameters, \bung infants consistently recognized mother unmasked the underlying coordinative pattern (Thelen, Fisher,
before self; these authors suggested that the onset of representa- & Ridley-Johnson, 1984). Thus, within the context of the up-
tion in the second year allowed infants to go beyond their senso- right posture, the nature of the control parameter changed from
rimotor experience, which limited their ability to see their own general behavioral activation to the deposition of subcutaneous
bodies. In addition, when infants were asked to act on them- fat. Presumably, when some critical fat/muscle mass or strength
selves or act on the mother in various tasks of agency, older in- ratio was reached, the system switched into a new output
fants resisted the role of acting as a baby, possibly because it was phase—no stepping. The ontogenetic timing of this phase shift
affectively unpleasant for them to imagine this scenario. Thus, is a result of the asynchrony between components, not of a sin-
both constraints of the body architecture and affective compo- gle central commanding agency. Note again that the fat/muscle
nents of the task as well as knowledge structures acted as control ratio acted as a control parameter only in a particular context;
parameters. when infants were supine, the system was assembled dynami-
Although our dynamic systems approach cannot identify a cally and specifically to that context, and kicking in the supine
priori the particular control parameter for a given domain and position was again a function of behavioral activation (Thelen,
age, it is useful because it directs the researcher to a level of 1985a). It was not the availability of the components alone, but
specificity at which the constraints on developmental change their task-specific relations that determined the topography of
may be revealed. Before showing how a dynamic systems ap- the behavioral outcome.
proach helps one understand communication development, we The onset of independent locomotion can also be viewed
will illustrate the utility of the perspective with two examples: from this systems perspective. When infants take theirfirstinde-
one in the motor domain, and the second to show how parents pendent steps, the new stage is the result of many component
can act as control parameters in the performance of new skills. abilities. Some of these have been in place long before the phase
These examples are chosen because they can be supported by a shift. For example, Thelen and her colleagues have demon-
body of empirical findings. strated that the capability to generate alternating steps may be
available many months before independent walking. If infants
are supported over a motorized treadmill, they perform well-
Dynamic Systems Approaches in Two Domains coordinated stepping movements that are responsive to the
speed of the treadmill (Thelen, 1986). In a longitudinal study
1. Stepping and walking in infants. The traditional view of of nine infants, one infant demonstrated this ability at 1 month,
the ontogeny of human upright locomotion is that the develop- and all of the infants performed some steps by 4 months of age
ing motor cortex gains control over lower, subcortical or reflex- (Ulrich, Thelen, & Niles, 1987). Although involuntary, these
ive elements. Thus, McGraw (1943) attributed the decline in steps are not reflexive, because infants can exquisitely adjust
the well-known newborn stepping response to the maturation these steps to the demands of a task; specifically, infants main-
of "inhibitory centers" in the cortex, and the onset of walking tained bilateral coordination when each leg was on a treadmill
to the subsequent maturation of cortical centers of voluntary belt driven at a different speed (Thelen, Ulrich, & Niles, in
control. Other prescriptive accounts of locomotor onset empha- press).
size the role of cognitive structures (Zelazo, 1983) or a succes- Likewise, infants appear to have both the motivation to loco-
sion of hierarchical pattern-generating circuits (Forssberg, mote and the ability to initiate voluntary leg movements long
1985). Although the role of the voluntary components in the before the onset of walking, because they will move by creeping,
initiation of walking is indisputable, the actual developmental crawling, or rolling to reach desired goals. They are responsive
course of this skill is more dynamic and complex than these to and make postural corrections for visual-flow information
752 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

and vestibular perturbations in advance of actual walking, al- trate a dynamic systems approach to development. The partic-
though these abilities certainly improve after the onset of up- ular actions and objects that the adult chooses to support the
right locomotion. Thus, none of these components are the con- child's effort will depend on the child's existing level of compe-
trol parameters for independent walking. tence, on the context, and on the nature of the task. Because
Thelen has proposed that for this skill the control parameter these actions enhance the child's competence in the zone of
is the ability to support the weight on one leg, an action that is proximal development, we suggest that these actions are control
likely a combination of muscle strength and balance ability. The parameters arising from extrinsic sources. The mere presence
treadmill experiments suggested that the crucial factor in elicit- of the adult action allows the infant's existing abilities to be-
ing stepping was the stretch of the stance leg backward as the come dynamically reorganized in order to permit the emer-
weight was shifted forward, which allowed the spring-like re- gence of a skill that the child did not previously manifest. It is
lease of the forward swing of the leg. In normal locomotion, not the child's existing set of competencies alone, nor only the
walkers must be able to maintain a strong pillar of support in adult's sensitive framing of those skills, but the task-specific dy-
this stance leg, but new walkers are notoriously unstable in the namic interaction of all these elements that creates the emer-
stance leg. The treadmill, a contextual manipulation, mechani- gent skill.
cally stretched the stance leg backward and acted as a control For example, newboras have relatively rudimentary self-
parameter for the assembly of the coordinated stepping skill. calming skills (Kessen & Mandler, 1962). Adults may provide
During development, the infant must develop strength and bal- extrinsic control parameters for the emergence of a state of calm
ance to produce the requisite support ability, which allows the through holding, rocking, and the provision of nipples on which
coordinative pattern to emerge. to suck. The particular device or action leading to calming de-
Thus, in this classic U-shaped developmental phenomenon pends on what works for the infant and on what is available to
(Strauss, 1982), the disappearance and reappearance of step- the adult. In any case, a relatively discontinuous phase shift
ping was the result of system-wide reorganizations determined is occasioned by a small change in some extrinsic control pa-
by different control parameters: in the early months a critical rameter.
fat/muscle ratio, and later in the first year, a critical combina- Likewise, adult actions, such as turn taking, initiating, and
tion of strength and balance. At each time, however, the behav- complementarity of response, appear to play a crucial and
ior was not hard wired, but assembled in a context-specific changing role in the development of communication and dis-
manner, because contextual manipulations could substitute for course skills. Adults have relatively little effect on discourse with
organismic control parameters in effecting the phase shifts. a newborn whose behavior consists primarily of endogenous cy-
2. Apprenticeship interactions. In the dynamic systems cles of action and pause. Nonetheless, even in the newborn,
perspective there is noformal difference between control param- adults create a semblance of dialogue by fitting their behavior
eters that arise endogenously and those that come from exoge- (jiggling) into pauses in the infant's bursts of sucking. By the age
nous sources. The environment can contribute control parame- of 2 weeks, however, the adult actions have changed the infant's
ters—just as the treadmill mechanically stretches back the behavior; making the children look more mature as they make
stance leg of the prewalking infant—that spontaneously com- small shifts in the time of onset of sucking in response to the
bine with those available to the child to create behavioral per- mother's jiggling (Kaye, 1977).
formance that the child alone, or in another environmental con- Adults continue to provide the scaffolds by which infants
text, could not produce. build more mature social interaction. At 3 months, for exam-
During social interaction with infants, adults can temporar- ple, infants' alternations of action and pause in expressive be-
ily enhance a child's performance beyond the level at which the havior appear to cycle with endogenous activation levels.
child is capable of functioning alone. Adults supply supportive Adults, however, initiate, frame, and maintain the dialogue.
"frames" by which the infant can organize existing, but incom- They do this by a steady gaze as the baby cycles spontaneously
plete, behavior patterns into functional systems. This phenome- between looking at and away from the adult. Once the infant
non has been described in a number of ways: as framing (Fogel, gazes at the adult, dialogue is created by exchanges of facial
1977), apprenticeship (Kaye, 1982), scaffolding (Bruner, 1983), expression and vocalization (Fogel, 1977; Kaye &Fogel, 1980).
and the zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 1978). The The actions serving as control parameters for infant-parent
particular adult behavior that the child uses to enhance perfor- discourse shift again by 6 months of age, as a function of the
mance depends both on the type and level of the child's own child's development level and the altered nature of the task, as
abilities and on the nature of the task. The adult brings cultur- purely social displays are replaced by interactions with objects.
ally appropriate tools, symbols, and actions. By analyzing the By this age, infants are becoming more active social partners,
child's particular limitations, the adult creates a zone of proxi- who are themselves able to initiate and maintain bouts of mu-
mal development in which the child can use his own knowledge tual gazing and smiling (Cohn & Tronick, 1987; Kaye & Fogel,
and skill to make a contribution to a complex joint enterprise. 1980). The adult's role concomitantly changes from providing
Holding an object steady and within arm's reach while a 4- extrinsic support for social discourse to supporting object-me-
month-old attempts a grasp, elaborating on toddler's one-word diated social interactions. Adults frame social object play by
utterances and asking leading questions to elicit further re- moving objects in and out of view and of reach, by taking the
sponses, and teaching a child to play a musical instrument via initiative in games of hide and seek, and by give and take (Ad-
a series of simple yet challenging exercises are all examples of amson & Bakeman, 1985; Bruner, 1983).
interaction in the zone of proximal development. Adults continue to stay developmentally ahead of the infant
We propose that these types of adult-child interactions illus- by providing appropriately framed control parameters at each
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 753
new period in the development of social skill. In a study of ob- stochastically, rather than rigidly). This is enough to guarantee
ject-centered interactions during the second year of life, Heck- equifinal ontogenetic histories within a species built by idiosyn-
hausen (1984) found that adults attempted to balance the cratic experiences within individuals.
child's limitations in order to facilitate the task. Adult's help,
however, was not superfluous nor was it merely supportive: The
Communication as a Developing System
adult attempted to promote the infant's development through
assistance that was challenging rather than simply for the pur- Complex skills like communication are multidetermined, in-
pose of ensuring success in the task. In fact, by the end of the corporating elements of cognition, sensation, perception, affect,
second year, children began to refuse the adult's help when they and motor control. A communicative action cannot be per-
perceived it as superfluous or unchallenging. formed unless all of the components skills are in place and avail-
By age 2, the extrinsic control parameters supplied by the able to the individual, because the action is the systems product
adult shift from object manipulation and overt expressive be- of the essential cooperating elements. Because new skills seem
havior to jointly shared symbols and affective states. Slade to appear as a piece during development—uttering the first
(1987) found that both level and duration of symbolic play epi- word or pointing at a distant object—it may seem as though all
sodes for 2-year-olds increased when an adult was present. With of the components developed in synchrony, orchestrated by a
an adult, the children were more likely to display symbolic ac- language or skill acquisition "program."
tions (naming), self-directed pretend play (combing one's hair), The ability of a stable patterned output to "float" from one
object-directed pretend play (feeding a doll), object combina- functional system to another (Thelen, 1981) suggests that devel-
tions, and the planning out of pretend sequences. Apparently opmental outcome is to some degree dependent on what is avail-
adults provide concrete support for symbolic acts by entering able at the periphery, that is, what the neuromuscular system
into play routines and directly responding to the child's use of and the constraints of dynamics can generate. Thus, behaviors
symbols in communicative contexts. As in the other examples that appear "too early " or "too late" should not be dismissed
cited in this section, the adult provides an extrinsic control pa- as epiphenomenal. Rather, the ontogenetic histories of coordi-
rameter that allows the infant to behave at a level of skill more native structures can be used as core data in theories of develop-
advanced than that which might be observed when the infant is mental change (Fogel, 1985).
alone or with peers. The emergent behavior—dialogue, object Communicative actions, like other behavior, emerge from the
exploration, or symbolic action—is not hard wired, but rather ecological demands of infancy, from phylogeuetic constraints
assembled in the context of the adult-child interaction. on ontogeny, which delimit plasticity, and from the self-organiz-
ing nature of action systems. Because human infants are motor-
A Dynamic Systems Approach to Development: ically immature, expressive channels using small muscle groups
Summary (e.g., crying and facial expressions) are among thefirstto func-
tion. These issues will be considered in the next section.
The dynamic systems approach to developmental change has The subsequent course of expressive development must then
three important characteristics. It is; be viewed as the interaction of these coordinative structures in
1. Independent of time scale. In this perspective, the same an everchanging matrix of cooperating structures and pro-
principles govern processes of system stability and change in cesses. As we will show, with increased postural support, with
real and ontogenetic time scales, (The perspective may be the growth of voluntary control, and with changing ecological
equally valid over phylogenetic time; see for example, Gould, demands, the nature and uses of these coordinations will
1977.) The same systems properties that account for the discon- change.
tinuous shift between walking and running in real time, for ex-
ample, may be used to explain the acquisition of a new skill in
developmental time. The approach therefore applies equally to Phylogenetic Perspectives on Communicative Ontogeny
behavior and development and points to how developmental Developing organisms are faced with conflicting demands.
change can be understood as the result of real-time processes. On the one hand, they must undergo growth and differentiation
2. Independent of source of change. There is no formal toward becoming reproductively active adults. On the other
difference between endogenous and exogenous changes in com- hand, they must have physiological and behavioral adaptations
ponents and their relationships. Emergent states can be created that ensure their survival at every point during development
either by means of environmental support or as a result of (Oppenheim, 1981). For example, human newborns can only
changes of components within the individual. The approach, survive within a limited range of body temperature, yet their
therefore, does not artificially separate infant and environment physiological capability to thermoregulate on their own func-
and can account specifically for the role of the adult in the con- tions poorly at birth. A covering of fur or feathers would help,
struction of new skills. but this is clearly an adaptive solution beyond the range of phy-
3. Independent ofepigenetic instructions. In a complex sys- logeny.
tem with high dimensionality, discontinuous phase shifts of sys- Thus, compromises must be reached within these phyloge-
tem organization at the low-dimensional level are brought netic constraints and thefiniteenergy resources available to the
about by relatively small changes in just a few control parame- developing organism. A number of possibilities can be imag-
ters. It is not necessary, therefore, to genetically program the ined. First, some necessary systems might be developmentally
timing of emergence of developmental stages. All that is needed accelerated while others are retarded. For example, in the case
is the specification of components linked preferentially (that is, of thermoregulation, it appears that the deposition of subcuta-
754 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

neous body fat takes precedence in thefirstfew months over the ties that evolved separately in the primate line, presumably in
increase in muscle tissue: Thermoregulation, in this case, is a the service of other functions" (p. 132). The evolution of lan-
more important developmental adaptation at this age than guage was not, therefore, a result of a dedicated genetic struc-
strength. ture, but of many component abilities coalescing around a
A second strategy for both survival and development is to ap- task—the need to communicate symbols across acoustic-artic-
propriate the specific behaviors suited to meet the particular ulatory channels.
ontogenetic demands. For human infants, one way to compen-
sate for poor thermoregulation is to have the parent provide Reinterpreting Communicative and Expressive
necessary warmth and cooling. For this to happen, however, be-
havioral adaptations must ensure the close contact of the child Development From a Dynamic Systems Perspective
and the parent and the ability of the parent to provide artificial Communication is defined broadly as information from a
temperature regulation for the infant (or both). Because infants sender that causes a change in a receiver. This simple definition
lack the motoric ability to establish and maintain close contact, of communication has been used in a wide range offields,from
they have evolved expressive actions that signal to parents their semiotics (Sebeok, 1965) to animal behavior (Smith, 1977; Wil-
physiological discomfort, just as parents have evolved sensitiv- son, 1975) to psychology (Buck, 1982). It has the advantage of
ity to those signals. recognizing both spontaneous and intentional information
By viewing expressive behavior, then, as a result of both con- transfer in biological systems as diverse as the human infant and
temporaneously adaptive demands and phylogenetic con- the honeybee.
straints, we can begin to understand the heterochronic develop- Cognitive developmentalists have conceptualized the exis-
ment of component structures and processes. The relatively tence of communicative milestones that cut across specific mo-
large size of the human cerebrum in relation to female pelvic dalities of expression, such as the transition from spontaneous
dimensions means that human infants must be born at a com- to intentional communication (corresponding to cognitive ad-
paratively earlier state in fetal development than other primates vances in the understanding of causality and space occurring
(Gould, 1977). As a result, this "fetal" infant has a relatively near the end of thefirstyear of life) and from preverbal to verbal
weak body lacking the muscle mass and strength necessary to forms of communication (corresponding to the cognitive ad-
support its body against gravity and to maintain the stances and vance of the acquisition of symbols at the end of the second
postures for instrumental action (Reed, 1982;Thelen, 1984). year of life). The intention to communicate is judged from the
Despite the general immaturity of the motor system, infants* infant's persistent efforts to signal the other person, the sequenc-
ensure their viability by the accelerated development of sensory ing of the gesture with looking at the other person, and looking
systems and selective motor systems that can perform commu- at the object about which the child intends to communicate
nicative actions requiring little muscle strength. Some of these (Harding & Golinkoff, 1979). In the second year, as symbolic
coordinations, like crying and smiling, may have been selected abilities grow, nonverbal gestures become integrated with words
by phytogeny for both current and future communicative ac- and sentences into linguistic communication.
tions; others, like kicking and mouthing, may be coordinations The dynamic systems approach does not ignore cognitive
available for other purposes—locomotion or feeding—and only changes contributing to communicative development. The sys-
used opportunistically by younger infants as expressions. These tems approach directs our attention to cognitive and also to
coordinative structures can then be reused as part of other func- other potential contributors to communication, contributors
tional systems later in life (Fogel, 1985;Thelen, 1985b). such as facial and manual motor actions that appear in the rep-
Because the infant uses a variety of behavioral forms for on- ertoire of infants long before they are used intentionally by the
togenetic adaptations (i.e., to meet a specific need during a lim- infant to communicate. By tracing the ontogenetic history of
ited period of ontogeny), a close examination of the ontogenetic individual actionsfromfirstappearance to later communicative
histories of individual coordinative structures would "consist usage, we can show that they often develop within functional
of multiple arrests, deletions, regressions, and the elimination systems that are not, at the outset, related to communication
of earlier behavioral forms, and the emergence of qualitatively or expression. Coordinative structures that are precursors to
new forms whose antecedents are by no means self-evident" communicative skill can be found early in life in systems serv-
(Wolff, 1987, p. 249). In his classic monograph on evolution, ing respiratory, arousal regulatory, locomotor, and exploratory
Gould (1977) makes the compelling argument that organisms functions.
build new forms from already existing parts. Precisely because Developmental changes in emotion have been explained
organisms exist as cooperative systems, they are stable assem- from a number of different theoretical perspectives. Cognitive
blies that resist drastic structural or physiological changes. It is developmental accounts tie ontogenetic changes in crying and
easier for phylogeny and ontogeny to recruit, modify, and reas- distress expression, for example, with cognitive-structural shifts
semble elements already integrated into the system than to (Campos & Stenberg, 1978; Piaget, 1963; Sroufe, 1979). Spe-
build parts anew (Gould & Lewontin, 1979). It is this opportu- cifically, the transition from the emotion of distress (merely a
nistic use of elements already in place that explains many of the visceral discomfort response) present at birth to disappoint-
discontinuities and shifting functions of early communicative ment beginning in the second or third month of life is due pri-
action (Fogel, 1985). marily to the emergence of primary circular reactions and sim-
Bates (1979) has applied Gould's concepts to language evolu- ple anticipation. The shift from mere distress to anger, occur-
tion. Bates proposed that language capacity did not arise de ring later in the first year is presumed to be related to the
novo phylogenetically, but through a "reorchestration of capaci- infant's growing conception of causality and intentionality.
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 755

Similarly, fear replaces wariness at the end of the first year as a a pin prick to the sole of the foot had mean values of 3.83,3.99,
result of the acquisition of object permanance. . 18, and .16 s, respectively. Even though respiration is a continu-
An alternative perspective is that emotional development is ous behavior and the duration of respiration and rest periods
driven developmentally by noncognitive, affective aspects of hu- can vary continuously, there are discretely different types of cry,
man action (Campos & Stenberg, 1978;Izard&Haynes, 1986; suggesting the existence of discontinuous phase shifts in real-
Steiner, 1979). In this perspective, emotional expressions can be time processes- In many cases, Wolff found that the transition
elicited spontaneously in the absence of cognition. For example, to each of these cry types could be controlled by specific envi-
facial expressions of emotion can be elicited in animals who ronmental stimulation at specific levels of behavioral activation.
have had their cerebral hemispheres removed, leaving the brain Around the second month, a qualitatively different cry
stem intact (Steiner, 1979). Anger expressions in response to emerges, the so-called irregular or fussy cries. The cry becomes
pain stimuli can be induced at 2 months, before cognitive ap- more elaborated during thefirstyear with the addition of novel
praisal believed to be associated with anger has developed articulatory features, such as variations in vocal intensity, bila-
(Izard, Hembree, Dougherty, & Spizzirri, 1983). In this view, bial friction noises, and "squealing." These changes in the abil-
affect plays a primary role and cognition a secondary role in ity to control the auditory features of the cry become coordi-
ontogeny. Infants do not first appraise a situation cognitively nated with infant goal-directed action as cries become per-
and then decide what to feel. Rather, emotion is a spontaneous ceived by caregivers as demands for particular objects or
response that serves to guide both individual and interpersonal actions. Corresponding to changes in the infant's ability to ref-
behavior (Bretherton, Fritz, Zahn-Waxier, & Ridgeway, 1986; erence distant objects with a reach or a point, at about the age
Izard, 1977). of 9 months, the cry becomes "less persistent, more punctuated
In the traditional cognitive and affective theories of emo- by pauses during which the infant checks on the uptake by the
tional development, change is driven by structural neurological mother or by other adults" (Bruner, 1983, p. 92). Soon, accord-
shifts. Whether that change occurs in the cerebrum or the brain ing to Bruner, the cry becomes more ritualized and perfunctory
stem matters little here. Structural views presume the existence as it becomes incorporated into a demand/request function in
of a single developmental clock that imposes order on legions coordination with referential gestures of the arms and hands.
of subcomponents: The components advance synchronously, The cry continues to become more complex after the first
subject perhaps to minor horizontal decalage shifts within do- year as well. Compared with younger infants, older infant's cries
mains. In the dynamic systems view, components develop at are slower to build and decline, and there are more variations in
different times and with different rates. The particular subcom- the intermediate states. Infants observed in Ainsworth's strange
ponent that becomes the control parameter changes over time. situation at 12 Vz and 19 Vt months (Thompson & Lamb, 1984)
At one age, a cognitive shift may control developmental reorga- initiated crying with intermittent brief distress vocalizations,
nization of emotion. At another age, this reorganization may be proceeded to longer but arrhythmic cries (fussing), then re-
controlled by a motor advance, a shift in state control, a change turned to a regular, rhythmic pattern with higher intensity (sob-
of social interactive skill, or the learning of a display rule regu- bing). The toddlers increased the intensity of the cries by varia-
lating the timing of emotional displays with regard to social tions of pitch in addition to rhythm and duration (screaming).
context (Fogel, 1985; Fox & Davidson, 1984; Thelen, 1985b; Infants added audibility of respiration in producing the most
Thelen & Fogel, in press; Wolff, 1987; Zivin, 1986). intense cries (panic and hyperventilated cries).
Although there are no prospective studies of expressive and What are the potential control parameters that shift the in-
communicative development based on a dynamic systems ap- fant into these new forms of vocal expression? As Wolff showed,
proach per se, the following review attempts to reinterpret exist- the basic structure of the cry is intimately tied to respiratory
ing data from a systems perspective. In the next section, we re- cycles (and, indeed, crying is likely to have been phylogeneti-
view the research on three different coordinative structures that cally derived from respiratory movements). Changes in the res-
appear at or near birth—crying, smiling, and pointing—and piratory apparatus may serve as control parameters for the de-
show that they are organized developmentally into a variety of velopmental shift in crying seen at 2 months. The irregular/
different functional systems and subject to a series of different fussy cry can only be produced after the development of the
control parameters as each of the functional systems emerges. explosive respiratory phenomenon known as forced expulsion
of air, a kind of cough-cry combination. Increasing lung
Crying strength and efficiency also alters the pitch and duration of cry
phases (Prescott, 1975).
Crying exhibits a complex ontogenetic history during the first Subsequent changes in the cry pattern may also be regulated
years of life and stands as an excellent illustration of the conflu- by anatomical and physiological control parameters. For exam-
ent and shifting influences of organismic and environmental ple, the large forces generated during feeding and oral explora-
factors (especially social environmental factors) on expressive tion during the early months may induce major changes in the
development. oral musculature (Bosma, 1975). These motor structures may,
In the newborn period, Wolff(1967) showed that at least four in turn, be used opportunistically by both the crying and speech
different types of cry could be created and controlled by varia- system. Similarly, there is increasingly elaborated fine motor
tions in duration of the components of the basic respiration cy- control over the speech articulators during the second half of
cle: expiration, rest, inspiration, rest. The "rhythmical" cry had the first year. It is not surprising, therefore, to note that the in-
average durations of .62, .09, .04, and .20 s for each of the four crease of complexity of cry sounds coincides with the period of
components respectively, whereas the "pain" cry in response to speech-like babbling, and that these new features—including
756 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

variations in vocal intensity, bilabial friction noises, and history. Neonatal smiles have a rather simple morphology. The
"squealing"—appear also in noncry vocalizations (Stark, face remains relaxed while only the mouth stretches sidewards
1978). In short, although the context and functional conse- and upwards bilaterally, suggesting a preferential linkage of the
quences of cry and noncry vocalizations are different, develop- retractor muscles on either side of the mouth. Facial grimaces
mental changes in components common to both may shift both that are morphologically identical to smiling have been ob-
systems into new emergent forms. served in preterm infants as young as 26 weeks gestational age
In addition to respiration and the oral motor-control system, (Wolff, 1987). The newborn smile is a spontaneous behavior
the emergence of other skills may also control the timing of on- that occurs primarily in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep
togenetic changes in crying. Head turning (Turkewitz, 1977), states. REM smiles tend to occur in bursts of several smiles in a
hand-to-mouth and kicking (Wolff, 1987), and sucking (Kessen row, followed by pauses with no smiles (Emde & Koenig, 1969).
& Mandler, 1962) accompany and inhibit (or both) cries in new- Smiling thus is a stable coordinative structure that appears in
borns. Gaze aversion in 3-month-olds (Stern, 1974), controlled the repertoire before it has any function vis-a-vis emotional ex-
hand-to-mouth behavior in 5-month-olds (Fogel, 1985), and in- pressiveness, and perhaps even before the coordinative struc-
tentional avoidance behavior in 12-month-olds (Ainsworth, ture is associated systematically with some internal motiva-
Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) may all play important roles in tional state.
the ontogeny of crying. By the second week, smiles begin to appear during waking
This multicausal perspective makes a wide variety of pre- states. The basic morphology of the mouth movement is the
sumed ontogenetic control parameters amenable to experimen- same, but the lip corners retract farther and the mouth may
tal manipulation. If we believe that the control of developmental open slightly, the cheek muscles contract, the skin around the
change lies primarily in neurological structures, we cannot be eyes wrinkles, while the eyes themselves remain somewhat
expected to study human development experimentally. If, on glassy. Although REM smiles occur in bursts, waking infants
the other hand, we assume that neurological factors are no more under 2 months will smile only once or twice in a 5-min period
or less important than peripheral and contextual factors in of face-to-face social interaction. A dramatic change occurs at
causing developmental change, then we can search for develop- about three months as smiling begins to increase in frequency
mental phenomena that are ethically amenable to experimental and becomes nonrandomly distributed in real time. Smiling oc-
analysis. Suppose, for example, that we wish to study hand-to- curs in "clusters" containing a series of smiles separated by rel-
mouth behavior as a hypothetical control parameter in the on-
atively brief pauses (Fogel, 1982b). In a longitudinal study of 52
togeny of crying. We would first want to document the natural
subjects, the mean rate of smile clusters increased from .04 per
history of both crying and hand-to-mouth in relation to each
other over a particular developmental period, possibly by rela- min at 6 weeks, to .72 per min at 13 weeks, and 1.10 per min
tively frequent observations of a small number of infants (Fogel, at 26 weeks, a statistically significant change (Fogel, 1982b;
1981, 1982a; 1985; Wolff, 1966; 1987). Kaye& Fogel, 1980).
Demos (1982) found wide individual differences in smiling
The changing temporal associations between the behaviors behavior between 7 and 24 months. Infants developed idiosyn-
will serve as hypotheses for morerigorousexperimental manip- cratic "blends" of smiling with other types of facial expressions
ulations. An example of such a manipulation is to gently inhibit such as surprise, excitement, and anger. In addition, the infants
infants' spontaneous hand-to-mouth action during crying, fuss-
Demos observed had a considerable repertoire of nonaffective
ing, and noncrying periods. Alternatively, one could facilitate
expressive movements accompanying the smile. These included
hand-to-mouth contact by reducing the degrees of freedom of
uncontrolled arm movements, comparing the effects of such fa- nose wrinkles, jaw drops, blinks, blows, and brow raises that
cilitation during both cry and noncry states. The specific exper- served to communicate affects from pleasure to mischief. To-
imental strategy must be guided by a thorough knowledge of ward the end of the second year, these children could produce
the spontaneous behavior of the infant at different ages and in messages related to mugging,flirtation,coyness, and teasing by
varying conditions. No single dynamic systems perspective can an appropriate combination of the invariant smile morphology
provide the specific experimental strategy nor predict a systems with some other expressive movement.
outcome in the absence of a detailed natural history. However, These studies show that during the first year smiling changes
the concept that extrinsic components can substitute for intrin- in morphology, temporal organization, and in its sequential
sic ones in the emergence of new behavioral organizations sug- linkage with other expressive actions. Equally dramatic devel-
gests a general experimental strategy useful in the study of ex- opmental shifts characterize the function and eliciting contexts
pressive and communicative action. of smiles over the first year. Newborn smiles are clearly associ-
We do not wish to argue that cognitive and affective systems ated with behavioral state, especially REM sleep and drowsi-
are unimportant for developmental changes in crying, but ness. Wolff (1987) found that drowsy smiles were most likely to
rather that such explanations ignored the organizing power occur within 2 s of eye closure in infants whose lids were already
of other components that enter into the cry. The size of the drooping. This suggests that smiling in newborns occurs when
lungs, the strength of the oral muscles, and the steadiness of the external sensory input is abruptly removed.
hand are essential factors in the ontogeny of the cry expressive During thefirstfew weeks of life, specific perceptual features
system. of the environment replace endogenous state shifts or general
levels of sensory input as elicitors of smiles. In particular, in-
Smiling fants smile upon hearing familiar voices and sounds, less regu-
Like crying, smiling can be observed as a stable pattern of larly upon sensing tactile stimulation, and not at all upon re-
movement present at birth and having a complex ontogenetic ceiving visual stimulation. However, this again changes, so that
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 757
by 6 weeks visual stimuli become the predominant elicitors of One of the major investigators of smiling during this period has
smiles, and the infant smiles primarily in alert states. There is been Wolff (1966, 1987), who systematically varied the modal-
an increasing specificity in the visual stimuli required to elicit ity of stimulation, the form of presentation, and the type of
a smile, from two eye spots at 6 weeks to a familiar face at 12 stimulus across a variety of sleep and waking states of infants
weeks (Spitz, 1965). at different ages between birth and 3 months. For older infants,
During the period from 3 to 6 months, infant's smiles are one could vary the visual access of the social partner, the avail-
more likely to co-occur with their looking at mother than with ability and relation of the social partner to objects, and the re-
periods of looking at objects. By 6 months, smiling may serve sponses of the partner to the infants' smiles. This type of re-
the infants' widening range of intentions to acquire and explore search, in which a variety of potential parameters are covaried
objects, as smiles are used systematically to regulate social in- across an epoch of developmental transition, can serve as one
teractions and to bring adults into their object exploratory rou- model of inquiry generated by a dynamic systems perspective.
tines (Adamson&Bakeman, 1985; Fogel, 1982b; Kaye&Fogel,
1980). By the end of the first year smiling serves a variety of
communicative functions, including the intent to flirt or to do Pointing
mischief (Demos, 1982).
How, then, can we begin to characterize this complex ontoge- Pointing as a referential gesture emerges near the end of the
netic course of an expressive behavior that changes in morphol- first year of life. One commonly held view is that pointing
ogy, in timing, in eliciting contexts, and in function? It is clear differentiates from the movements of reaching and grasping,
that no one control parameter orchestrates these shifts, but that partly in order to economize the movement as a gesture needed
a number of variables, some more "central" and others more for signaling and partly as a conventionalization of the adult
"peripheral" may be responsible for the emergence of new expression learned through imitation and shaping (Bates, Ca-
forms. maioni, & Volterra, 1975; Bruner, 1983; Leung & Rheingold,
The transition in the newborn period from state-dominated 1981;Masur, 1983; Murphy, 1978). This assumes that cognitive
to visually elicited smiles, for example, may result from steady developments at this age—the ability to carry out, plan, and
increases in motor efference as smiling "spreads" awayfromthe signal intentions—induce the child to learn new behavior; this
mouth to include larger regions of the face, from the gradually is a purely cognitive interpretation. This view, however, is based
increasing control over attention and over sleep/wake cycles, on observations of infants beginning at the age of 8 months. In
and from the growing dominance of visual over tactual stimula- this section, we shall review research showing that index-finger
tion. At 3 months the control parameters for the emergence of extension is a coordinative structure used reliably by infants as
repeated sequences of smiling to familiar persons and objects young as 2 months of age. A dynamic systems perspective on
may be cognitive-developmental shifts related to stimulus rec- the development of referential gestures, such as pointing, should
ognition as well as noncognitive factors related to the affective account not only for the emergence of the function of inten-
regulatory shifts occurring during this same period (Fogel, tional communication at the end of the first year, but also the
1982a). organization and reorganization of the action of the hand and
Although cognitive and neurological changes no doubt con- its changing functions over thefirstyear of life.
tribute to the developmental shift in smiling as well as the There are no systematic studies of the repertoire of manual
changes in attention and motor control occurring between 2 action in newborn s comparable to the studies on facial action.
and 6 months (McCall, Eichorn, & Hogarty , 1977), centrally However, index-finger extensions have been reported in infants
orchestrated stage shifts cannot be the sole explanation for the as young as 2 months old (Trevarthen, 1977). These index-finger
ontogenetic course of smiling. Piaget (1963), for example, as- extensions are coordinative structures appearing in nonrandom
sumed that the emergence of exogenous smiling at 2 months contexts and with ontogenetic significance. Index-finger exten-
was the result of the cognitive development of recognitory as- sions have been coded reliably in real time when points of less
similation, a view taken up later by Sroufe (1979). This per- than 2-s duration were eliminated (Hannan. 1982). In a sample
spective does not take into account the development of the smile of 28 three-month-old infants, 64% of the subjects had at least
response from its origins in neonatal sleep states, the increasing one index-finger extension, and some as many as six in a 2-min
dominance of visual over tactual elicitors, nor the changes in session of face-to-face interaction with the mother (Fogel &
smile morphology and temporal organization. The cognitive- Hannan, 1985). For convenience, we use the word pointing to
structural explanation only accounts for the increasing speci- refer to index-finger extensions in infants, although by doing so
ficity of eliciting stimuli during this period. we do not mean to imply that the expression is an intentional
The value of the dynamic systems perspective is to open the act of the infant.
issue of developmental induction in human infants to rigorous Several studies have shown that pointing remains in the rep-
empirical analysis by abandoning appeals to central organizers, ertoire of the infant between the 2nd and 10th month. In a lon-
genetic ground plans, and other black-box agencies of change. gitudinal case study of two infants observed weekly over the first
In lieu of experimental studies that systematically vary the con- year, the frequency of pointing did not vary systematically with
ditions under which each of the components listed above may age. Reaching and grasping, on the other hand, had a marked
influence the form, timing and function of smiling over the first increase in both babies at the age of 4 months (Fogel, 1981;
2 months, we can only speculate about the causes of develop- Platzman, 1983), showing clearly that pointing does not differ-
mental change. Experimental strategies similar to those dis- entiate from reaching at the end of the first year. In addition, a
cussed for the study of crying may also be applied to smiling. longitudinal study of 15 infants followed monthly over the first
758 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

year found instances of pointing at every observation (Hannan, 1982; 1987; Platzman, 1983;Trevarthen, 1977), which we have
1987), interpreted from the perspective of a dynamic systems view.
Like smiling and crying, pointing also changes in function Without further experimental study, however, these conclusions
over the first year. In 2- and 3-month-old infants, the pointing remain tentative. Presumed control parameters at a given age
action occurs just before or just after a vocalization or mouth should be varied in relation to tasks in which pointing is typi-
movement such as sucking, mouthing, or chewing. When babies cally elicited and under various levels of behavioral activation.
point, they are neither smiling nor crying, but in a neutral and For example, during the period between 8 and 12 months, when
attentive affective state and not concurrently vocalizing (Fogel pointing seems to shift from an exploratory to an indicating
& Hannan, 1985; Hannan, 1982; Platzman, 1983). These early function, the infant could be shown objects at a distance as well
points are not coordinated with gaze direction or arm exten- as objects within reach. Motivational state could be varied by
sion. At 3 months, pointing rarely occurs when the arm is ex- using both familiar and unfamiliar objects. In addition, the in-
tended. More often, one or both arms are in a moderate state fant's arms could be left free, gently restrained in the infant's
offlexion.Furthermore, early pointing does not seem to be di- sides, or propped so as to discourage or enhance the possibility
rected to the focus of attention. of pointing. One would expect higher rates of pointing as an
At 6 months, pointing continues to occur in social situations emergent phenomenon only under specific conditions.
and nondirectional pointing may also appear on the hand of the
nonextended arm when an object gains the infant's attention
Discussion
(Thelen & Fogel, in press). Between 6 and 9 months, pointing
occurs when the infant is sitting, usually with aflexedarm, and In this article we have presented evidence that communica-
is used, along with other manipulative actions of the hands, to tive action develops in thefirstyear of life in the context of pro-
touch objects that the infant is exploring (Thelen & Fogel, in cesses and functions that are not, for the infant at least, commu-
press; Wakaba, 1981). nicative. We have discussed communicative ontogeny from a
By 11 months, pointing occurs directionally to an object dynamic systems perspective that focused on heterochronic de-
from a sitting or standing posture and with an extended arm velopment and on functional reorganization of coordinative
(Hannan, 1987; Thelen & Fogel, in press). Pointing at the end structures. We have also conceptualized aspects of the social
of the first year is an intentional action, used primarily as an and physical environment as control parameters that unmask
extension of the orienting response, which can be inferred be- emergent patterns of action unavailable to the infant in other
cause the infant does not look systematically between the object contexts.
of interest and a nearby adult (Kinsbourne, 1986; Lempert & The data presented in support of the dynamic systems ap-
Kinsbourne, 1985). proach are not meant to be conclusive, but suggestive. Most of
What explains these developmental changes in the eliciting the current research on communication and expression during
context and functions of pointing over the first year? The shift the first year is from longitudinal natural-history observations,
around 6 months from spontaneous and nondirectional index- many of which are done on a small number of subjects. This is
finger extension on a nonextended arm to the use of index-finger a necessary first step in documenting the variety of potential
extension as a tool for exploring the object world may be ex- control parameters that may be operating in complex develop-
plained by the development of arm control and articulation ing systems, but more needs to be done. Specifically, we have
over thefingersin manipulative tasks. Pointing, available as a outlined a number of concrete experimental strategies that fol-
coordinative structure since at least 2 months of age and already low from a dynamic systems approach.
associated with the direction of the infant's attention, may be First, the investigator must identify a set of candidate control
used opportunistically with other newly developed skills of parameters that are ethically manipulable: in many cases these
grasping, palming, rotating, and the picking up and letting go will be motor and context factors (cf. Blass, 1987; Heckhausen,
of objects. 1984;Slade, 1987;Thelenetal., 1984).Cognitive,affective,and
It has been assumed in previous research that the shift from neurological control parameters may also be studied by relying
spontaneous pointing to the emergence of referential pointing on natural variations across ages and infants (cf. Pipp et al.,
at the end of the first year is a manifestation of the three-way 1987; Wolff, 1987).
cognitive integration between self, adult, and object and the in- Second, experimental situations must be invented in which
fant's awareness of their roles in communicative exchanges the behavioral system under study is predicted to emerge spon-
(Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975; Masur, 1983). Although taneously once requisite levels of the control parameter are pro-
this cognitive factor may act as a control parameter for this de- vided. Systematic variation in the control parameter should
velopmental shift in the function of pointing, other control pa- lead to observable change in the behavioral organization of the
rameters may also play a role, such as gross motor control over system under study. For example, in Blass' (1987) research on
posture and upper limb movement contributing to the direction newborns, sucrose delivered to the lips elicited coordinated
of pointing displayed with an extended arm. These factors are hand-to-mouth behavior only when the infant's level of behav-
not the ones likely to explain the shift in the function of pointing ioral activation (the control parameter) brought the infant into
at 6 months, reminding us that the dynamic systems approach a prior state of distress. In Wolff's study (1987), newborn smil-
predicts changes over time in the parameters controlling devel- ing was spontaneously emergent during drowsiness only if the
opmental change. infant's eyes were drooping before they closed. Other instances
Data on the ontogeny of pointing come primarily from longi- of eye closure (blinking, squinting, etc.) did not lead to smiling.
tudinal natural history studies (Fogel, 1981; 1985; Hannan, The control parameter in this case again is behavioral activation
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 759

leading to sleep and an abrupt cut off of external stimulation. Bates, E., Camaioni, L., Sc Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of per-
In Thelen's research, stepping could be elicited after it had "dis- formatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21, 105-226.
appeared" by placing infants supine or in water, revealing that Bernstein, N. (1967). Co-ordination and regulation ofmovements. New
the fat-to-muscle ratio of the legs is the control parameter re- \brk: Pergamon Press.
sponsible for the suppression of stepping in early infancy. Bertenthal, B. I., Campos, J. J., & Barrett, K. C. (1984). Self-produced
locomotion: An organizer of emotional, cognitive, and social develop-
Finally, these demonstrations of emergent phenomena in re-
ment in infancy. In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.), Continuities
lation to experimentally controlled parameters must be tested and discontinuities in development (pp. 175-210). New York: Ple-
against reasonable alternatives. If one believes that the experi- num.
mental evidence is confounded by possible covarying factors, it Blass, E. (1987, April). A psychobiologjcal perspective on newborn be-
is necessary to independently vary those factors. The centrally havior. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in
generated neurological control parameter as an alternative Child Development, Baltimore, MD.
hypothesis may be studied by conducting tests with other man- Bosma, J. F. (1975). Anatomic and physiologic development in the
ipulable parameters across a narrow range of ages presumed to speech apparatus. In D. B. Tower (Ed.), The nervous system: Vol. 3.
be associated with neurological shifts, a strategy used by Pipp, Human communication and its disorders. New 'Vbrk: Raven Press.
Fischer, and Jennings (1987) in their study of the role of affect, Bremerton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxier, C, & Ridgeway, D. (1986). Learn-
cognition, and body architecture in the development of the ing to talk about emotions: A functionalist perspective. Child Devel-
child's concept of self. Examples of research models that might opment, 57, 529-548.
be applied to the study of communication and expression Bruner. J. (1983). Child's talk: Learning to use language. New York:
are presented above in the sections on crying, smiling, and Norton.
pointing. Buck, R. (1982). Spontaneous and symbolic nonverbal behavior and the
ontogeny of communication. In R. S. Feldman (Ed.), Development
From a dynamic systems perspective, as opposed to a unified
of nonverbal behavior in children (pp. 29-62). New York: Springer-
cognitive or epigenetic ground plan for development, there is Verlag.
little reason—aside from the surface analogy between early vo- Campos, J. J., 8L Stenberg, C. R. (1978). Perception, appraisal, and emo-
cal and manual gestural behavior and later linguistic behavior— tion: The onset of social referencing. In M. E. Lamb & L. R. Sherrod
to choose any particular early action system over any other in (Eds.), Infant social cognition (pp. 273-314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
the search for the developmental origins of conventionalized Cohn, J. F, ATronick, E. Z. (1987). Mother-infant face-to-face interac-
communication. Indeed, we have shown that a number of "non- tion: The sequence of dyadic states at 3, 6, and 9 months. Develop-
communicative" systems in early infancy may serve as control mental Psychology, 23.68-77.
parameters in the development of communication skills, sys- Demos, V. (1982). Facial expressions of infants and toddlers: A descrip-
tems such as respiration, locomotion,finemotor control, visual tive analysis. In T. Field & A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interac-
orienting, postural adjustments, perception. tion, (pp. 127-160. NJ: Erlbaum.
Ontogeny is organized so that the infant can be engaged in Emde, R. N., & Koenig, K. L. (I969). Neonatal smiling, frowning, and
necessary adaptive tasks of the moment, allowing the relations rapid eye movement states. Journal of the American Academy of
between components to emerge spontaneously over the course Child Psychiatry. 8, 637-656.
Fischer, K. W., & Canfield, R. L. (1986). The ambiguity of stage and
of development. The arm and postural actions that sustain
structure in behavior Person and environment in the development of
pointing are developed in noncommunicative, large motor-ac-
psychological structures. In I. Levin (Ed.), Stage and structure: Re-
tion contexts. The baby's goals in controlling posture and loco- opening the debate (pp. 246-267). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
motion are not related to communication or attention per se. Fogel, A. (1977). Temporal organization in mother-infant, face-to-face
The speech motor apparatus used by both crying and noncry interaction. In H. R. Schaffer(Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interac-
vocalizing is probably developed as those same muscles are used tion (pp. 119-152). London: Academic Press.
for the relatively "large motor" oral tasks of chewing, swallow- Fogel, A. (1981). The ontogeny of gestural communication: Thefirstsix
ing, and oral/tactile exploration. If there is a phylogenetic months. In R. Stark (Ed.), Language behavior in infancy and early
"ground plan" for the emergence of communicative action, it is childhood (pp. 17-44). New York: Elsevier.
only that fundamental survival tasks, such as eating and mov- Fogel, A. (1982a). Affect dynamics in early infancy: Affective tolerance.
ing, come first, that muscle cells can grow only at a finite rate, In T. Field and A. Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction (pp.
and that opportunistic coordinations are possible when compo- 15-56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
nents are designed to be preferentially linked in the context of Fogel, A. (1982b). Early adult-infant face-to-face interaction: Expect-
the near universal task demands of human infancy. able sequences of behavior. Journal ofPediatric Psychology, 7,1-22.
Fogel, A. (1985). Coordinative structures in the development of expres-
sive behaviors in early infancy. In G. Zivin (Ed.), The development of
References expressive behavior: Biology-environment interaction (pp. 249-267).
Abraham, R. H., & Shaw, C. D. (1982). Dynamics: The geometry of New \brk: Academic Press.
behavior. Santa Cruz, CA: Aerial Press. Fogel, A., & Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of 2- to 3-month-
Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (1985). Affect and attention: Infants old human infants during social interaction. Child Development, 56,
observed with mothers and peers. Child Development, 56,582-893. 1271-1279.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns Forssberg, H. (1985). Ontogeny of human locomotor control: 1. Infant
of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hills- stepping, supported locomotion, and transition to independent loco-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. motion. Experimental Brain Research, 57,480-493.
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence ofsymbols: Cognition and communica- Fox, N. A., & Davidson, R. J. (1984). Hemispheric substrates of affect:
tion in infancy. New "fork: Academic Press. A developmental model. In N. A. Fox & R. J. Davidson (Eds.) The
760 ALAN FOGEL AND ESTHER THELEN

psychobiology of affective development (pp. 353-381). Hillsdale, NJ: Leung, E., & Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a
Erlbaum. social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17,215-220.
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny andphytogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Masur, E. F. (1983). Gestural development, dual directional signalling,
University Press. and the transition to words. Journal ofPsycholinguistic Research, 12,
Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The squandrels of San Marco 93-109.
and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist pro- McCall, R. B., Eichorn, D. H., & Hogarty, P. S. (1977). Transitions in
gramme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 205, 581— early development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
598. Development, 42 (3, Serial No. 171).
Haken, H. (1983). Synergetics: An introduction (3rd ed.). New \brk: McGraw, M. B. (1943). The neuromuscular development of the human
Springer-Verlag. infant. New \brk: Columbia University Press.
Han nan, T. E. (1982). \bung infant's hand and finger movements: An Murphy, C. M. (1978). Pointing in the context of a shared activity. Child
analysis of category reliability. In T. Field and A. Fogel (Eds.), Emo- Development, 49, 371-380.
tion and early interaction (pp. 253-266). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Nadel, L., & Zola-Morgan, S. (1984). Infantile amnesia: A neurobiolog-
Hannan, T. £. (1987). A cross-sequential assessment of the occurrences ical perspective. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Advances in the study of
of pointing in three- to twelve-month-old infants. Infant Behavior communication and affect: Vol. 9. Infant memory (pp. 144-172). New
and Development. 10, 11-22. \brk: Plenum.
Harding, C. G., & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive
vocalizations in prelinguistic infants. Child Development, 50, 33-40. processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior A
Haroutian, S. (1983). Equilibrium in the balance: A study ofpsychologi- neuroembryological perspective. In K. J. Connolly & H. F. R. Prechtl
cal explanation. New York: Springer-Verlag. (Eds.), Maturation and Development: Biological and Psychological
Heckhausen, J. (1984). Mother-infant dyads in joint object-centered ac- Perspectives (pp. 73-109). Philadelphia: Lippincott.
tion: Developmental change in the child's second year. Unpublished Oyama, S. (1985). The ontogeny ofinformation: Developmental systems
doctoral dissertation, University of Strathclyde, Glasgow, Scotland. and evolution. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum. Piaget, J. (1963). The origins ofintelligence in children. New York: Nor-
Izard, C. E., & Haynes, O. M. (1986). A commentary on emotion ex- ton.
pression in early development: An alternative to Zivin's framework. Pipp, S., Fischer, K. W., & Jennings, S. (1987). Acquisition of self-and
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 32, 313-319. mother knowledge in infancy Developmental Psychology, 23, 86-96.
Izard, C. E., Hembree, E. A., Dougherty, L. M., & Spizzu-ri, C J. Platzman, K. (1983). The ontogeny of pointing in thefirstten months of
(1983). Changes in facial expression of 2- to 19-month-old infants life. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.
following acute pain. Developmental Psychology, 19, 418-426. Prescott, R. (1975). Infant cry sound: Developmental features. Journal
Kaye, K. (1977). Toward the origin of dialogue. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), ofthe Acoustic Society ofAmerica, 57. 1186-1991.
Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp. 89-118). London: Aca- Prigogine, I. (1980). From being to becoming. San Francisco: Freeman.
demic Press. Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1984). Order out ofchaos. New \brk: Ban-
Kaye, K. (1982). The mental and social life of babies. Chicago: Univer- tam.
sity of Chicago Press. Rader, N. (1985). Change and variation: On the importance of hetero-
Kaye, K., &. Fogel, A. (1980). The temporal structure of face-to-face chrony for development. In G. Butterworth, J. Rutkowska, & M.
communication between mothers and infants. Developmental Psy- Scaife (Eds.), Evolution and developmental psychology (pp. 229-29).
chology, 16, 454-464. New \brk: Saint Martin's Press.
Kelso, J. A. S., Holt, K. G., Kugler, P. N., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On Reed, E. S. (1982). An outline of a theory of action systems. Journal of
the concept of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: 2. Motor Behavior, 14.98-134.
Empirical lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Reese, H. W., & Overton, W. F. (1970). Models of development and
Tutorials in motor behavior (pp. 49-70). New \brk: North-Holland. theories of development. In L. R. Goulet & P. B. Bakes (Eds.), Life-
Kelso, J. A. S., & Scholz, J. P. (1986). Cooperative phenomena in biolog- span developmental psychology: Research and theory (pp. 115-145).
ical motion. In H. Haken (Ed.), Synergetics of complex systems in New York: Academic Press.
physics, chemistry, and biology. New \ork: Springer-Verlag. Rosen, R. (1978). Fundamentals of measurement and representation of
Kelso, J. A. S., & Schonei; G. (in press). Toward a physical (synergetic) natural systems. New York: North-Holland.
theory of biological coordination. In R. Graham (Ed.). Lasers and Sameroff, A. J. (1984). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution.
synergetics. New York: Springer. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.) & W. Kessen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of
Kelso, J. A. S., & Tuller, B. (1984). A dynamical basis for action systems. Child Psychology: Vol. 1. History, theory, and methods. (4th ed., pp.
In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Handbook ofNeuroscience (pp. 321-356). 237-294). New York: WUey.
New York: Plenum. Satoh, N. (1985). Recent advances in our understanding of the temporal
Kessen, W., & Mandler, G. (1962). Anxiety, pain, and the inhibition of control of early embryonic development in amphibians. Journal of
distress. Psychological Revien 69, 396-404. Embryology and Experimental Morphology. 89 (Supplement), 257-
Kinsbourne, M. (1986). Brain organization underlying orientation and 270.
gestures: Normal and pathological cases. In J. L. Nespoulous, P. Per- Sebeok, T. A. (1965). Animal communication. Science, 147. 1006-
ron, & A. R. Lecours (Eds.), The biological foundations ofgestures: 1014.
Motor and semiotic aspects (pp. 65-76). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Slade, A. (1987). A longitudinal study of maternal involvement and
Kugler, P. N., Kelso, J. A. S., & Turvey, M. T. (1980). On the concept symbolic play during the toddler period. Child Development, 58,
of coordinative structures as dissipative structures: 1. Theoretical 367-375.
lines of convergence. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials Smith, W. J. (1977). The behavior ofcommunicating. Cambridge, MA:
in motor behavior (pp. 3-47). New York: North-Holland. Harvard University Press.
Lempert, H., & Kinsbourne, M. (1985). Possible origin of speech in Soil, D. R. (1979). Timers in developing systems. Science, 203, 841-
selective orienting. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 62-73. 849.
COMMUNICATIVE ACTION 761
Spitz, R. (1965). Thefirstyear ofiife. New York: International Uaiversi- Thompson, R. A., & Lamb, M. E. (1984). Assessing qualitative dimen-
ties Press. sions of emotional responsiveness in infants: Separation reactions in
Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Socioernotional development. In J. Osofsky (Ed.), the strange situation. Infant Behavior and Development, 7,423-445.
Handbook ofinfant development (pp. 462-518). New York: Wiley. Trevarthen, C. (1977). Descriptive analysis of infant communicative be-
Stark, R. E. (1978). Features of infant vocalization. The emergence of havior. H. R. Schajfer(Ed.), Studies in mother-infant interaction (pp.
cooing. Journal ofChild Language, 3, 379-390. 227-270). London: Academic Press.
Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and Turiel, E., & Davidson. P. (1986). Heterogeneity, inconsistency, and
smell stimulation. In H. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in asynchrony in the development of cognitive structures. In I. Levin
child development and behavior (Vol. 13, pp. 257-293). New York: (Ed.), Stage and structure: Re-opening the debate. Norwood, NJ:
Academic Press. Ablex.
Stem, D. N. (1974). Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction Turkewitz, G. (1977). The development of lateral differentiation in the
involving gaze, facial, and vocal behavior. In H. R. Schafier (Ed.), human infant. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 299,
Studies in mother-infant interaction. London: Academic Press. 309-318.
Strauss, S. (1982). Ancestral and descendant behaviors: The case of U- Turvey, M. T. (1977). Preliminaries to a theory of action with a refer-
shaped behavioral growth. In T. G. Bever(Ed.)T Regressions in mental ence to vision. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.). Perceiving, acting,
development; Basic phenomena and theories (pp. 191-220). Hills- and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 211-267). Hills-
dale, NJ: Erlbaum. dale, NJ: Erlbaum,
Thelen, E. {1981). Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological per- Ulrich, B. D,, Theien, E., & Niies, D. (1987, April). Interlimb dynam-
spective. Developmentai Psychology, 17,237-257. ics: Ontogenetic and adaptive synergies for walking. Paper presented
Thelen, E. (1984). Learning to walk: Ecological demands and phylogen- at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
tic contraints. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in ment, Baltimore, MD.
infancy research {Vol. 3, pp. 213-260) Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Vygotsky, L. S. < 1978). Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
Thelen, E. (1985a). Developmental origins of motor coordination: Leg versity Press.
movements in human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 18, 1- Wakaba, Y. K. (1981). Development of pointing in the first two years.
22. RIEEC Research Bulletin, RRB18, 1-35.
Thelen, E. (1985b). Expression as action: A motor perspective on the Weiss, P. A. (1969). The living system: Determinism stratified. In A.
transition from spontaneous to instrumental behaviors. In G. Zivin Koestler& J. R. SmythiesfEds.), Beyondreductionism.-Newperspec-
(Ed,), The development of expressive behavior: Biology-environment tives in the life sciences. Boston: Beacon Press.
interactions (pp. 221 -248). New "*brk: Academic Press. Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development
Thelen, E. (1986). Treadmill-elicited stepping in seven-month-old in- New York: International Universities Press.
fants. Child Development, 57, 1497-1506. Wilson, E. (1975). Sociobiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Thelen, E., & Cooke, D. Wt (1987). The relation between newborn step- Press.
ping and later locomotion: A new interpretation. Developmental Wolff, P. H. (1966). The causes, controls and organization of behavior
Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 380-393. in the neonate. Psychological Issues 5 [Monograph No. 17].
Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M, (1982). Newborn stepping: An explanation Wolff, P. H. (1967). The role of biological rhythms in early psyche oal
for the "disappearing reflex." Developmental Psychology, 18, 760- development. Bulletin of theMenninger Clinic, 31, 197-218.
775. Wolff, P. H. (1987). The development of behavioral states and the ex-
Thelen, E., Fisher, D, W., & Ridley-Johnson, R. (1984). The relation- pression of emotions in early infancy. Chicago: University of Chicago
ship between physical growth and a newbornreflex.Infant Behavior Press.
and Development, 7,479-493. Zelazo, P, R. (1983). The development of walking: Newfindingsand old
Thelen, E., Fisher, D. M., Ridley-Johnson, R., & Griffin, N. (1982). assumptions. Journal ofMotor Behavior, 15,99-137.
The effects of body build and arousal on newborn infant stepping. Zeskind, P. S. (1985). A developmental perspective on infant crying. In
Developmental Psychobiology, 15,447-493. B. Lester & C. Z. Boukydis (Eds.), Infant crying(pp. 159-185). New
Thelen, E., & Fogel, A. (in press). Towards an action-based theory of York: Plenum.
infant development In J, Lockman & N. Hazen (Eds.), Action in a Zivin, G. (1986). Processes of expressive behavior development. Merrill-
social context: Perspective on early development. New \brk: Plenum. Palmer Quarterly, 32, 103-140.
Thelen, EM Ulrich, B. D., & Niles, D. (1987). Bilateral coordination in
human infants: Stepping on a split-belt treadmill. Journal ofExperi- Received December 31,1986
mental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 405- Revision received June 4, 1987
4iO. AcceptedJunell,i987 •

1988 APA Convention "Call for Programs"


The APA "Call for Programs" for the 1988 annual convention will appear in the October issue
of the APA Monitor. The 1988 convention will be in Atlanta, Georgia, from August 12 to August
16. Deadline for submission of programs and papers is December 21,1987. This early deadline
is required because the 1988 convention is earlier in August than in the past. Additional copies
of the "Call" will be available from the APA Convention Office in October.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen