Sie sind auf Seite 1von 60

Word-of-Mouth 1

Word-of-Mouth and Interpersonal Communication: An Organizing Framework and Directions for Future Research JONAH BERGER*

*Jonah Berger is the Joseph G. Campbell assistant professor of Marketing (jberger@wharton.upenn.edu) at the Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, 700 Jon M. Huntsman Hall, 3730 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104. Thanks to Rebecca Greenblatt for helping to review and organize the material that formed the basis of this review. Zoey Chen, Sarah Moore, Grant Packard, Christophe Van den Bulte all provided helpful feedback that greatly improves the manuscript.

Word-of-Mouth 2

CONTRIBUTION STATEMENT There is currently huge popular interest in word-of-mouth and social media more broadly (e.g., Facebook and Twitter). But while quantitative research has demonstrated the causal impact of word-of-mouth on behavior across a host of domains, less is known about what drives word-ofmouth and why people talk about certain things rather than others. How does the audience people are communicating with, as well as the channel they are communicating through, affect what gets shared? And when and why does word-of-mouth have a stronger impact on behavior? This paper addresses these, and related questions, as it integrates various research perspectives to shed light on the behavioral drivers of word-of-mouth. It provides an integrative framework to organize research on the causes and consequences of word-of-mouth and outlines additional questions that deserve further study

ABSTRACT People often share opinions and information with others, and such word-of-mouth has an important impact on what consumers think, eat, buy and do. But what drives word-of-mouth and why do people talk about certain things rather than others? How does the audience people are communicating with, as well as the channel they are communicating through, affect what gets shared? And when and why does word-of-mouth have a stronger impact on behavior? This paper addresses these, and related questions, as it integrates various research perspectives to shed light on the behavioral drivers of word-of-mouth. First it provides an integrative framework to organize research on the causes and consequences of word-of-mouth. Then, after reviewing extant literature, it discusses additional questions that deserve further study. Taken together, the paper provides insight into the psychological factors that shape word-of-mouth, as well as the consequences of these processes for consumer behavior.

Word-of-Mouth 3

Consumers often talk about and share opinions, news, and information with others. They chitchat about a recent vacation, complain about a bad movie, or rave about a new restaurant. They gossip about co-workers, discuss important political issues, and debate the latest sports rumors. New technologies like Facebook, Twitter, and texting have only made it faster and easier for people to share stories and information with others. There are thousands of blogs, millions of tweets, and billions of emails written every day.1 Such interpersonal communication can be described as word-of-mouth, or informal communications directed at other consumers about the ownership, usage, or characteristics of particular goods and services or their sellers," (Westbrook 1987, 261).Word-of-mouth includes product related discussion (e.g., the Nike shoes were really comfortable) as well as the sharing of product related content (e.g., Nike ads on YouTube). It includes direct recommendations (e.g., youd love this restaurant) as well as mentions (e.g., we went to this restaurant last week). It includes literal word-of-mouth, or face-to-face discussions, as well as so called word of mouse, in online mentions and reviews. Word-of-mouth has a huge impact on consumer behavior. Social talk generates over 3.3 billion brand impressions each day (Keller and Libai 2009). It shapes everything from the movies we watch and the books we read to the websites we visit and the restaurants we patronize (Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006; Chintagunta, Gopinath, and Venkataraman 2010; Godes and Mayzlin 2009; Trusov, Bucklin, and Pauwels 2009). The consulting firm McKinsey (2010) suggests that word-of-mouth is the primary factor behind 20 to 50 percent of all purchasing

Social media has received lots of attention but word-of-mouth is not a new phenomenon. Interpersonal communication has existed since the advent of language. Cavemen shared opinions about which animals were easy to hunt or where to search for food. Social media has made it easier to quickly share with large groups, but in all the hype people have forgotten how much word-of-mouth actually happens offline. Some estimates suggest that 75% of word-of-mouth is face-to-face and only 7% is online (Keller and Fay 2009). Consequently, it is important to consider the drivers and consequences of both online and offline word-of-mouth.

Word-of-Mouth 4

decisions and that word-of-mouth generates more than twice the sales of paid advertising in categories as diverse as skincare and mobile phones (p 8). But while it is clear that word-of-mouth is frequent, and important, less is known about the intervening behavioral processes. What do people talk about and why? How does who people are talking to (e.g., friends vs. acquaintances) shape what they talk about? Does the channel consumers talk over (e.g., face-to face or online) impact what gets discussed, and if so, how? Indeed, some have called word-of-mouth The worlds most effective, yet least understood marketing strategy (Misner 1999). This article addresses these, and related questions, as it integrates various research perspectives to shed light on the behavioral drivers of word-of-mouth. Though Godes et al. (2005) discussed issues in the firms management of social interactions, no papers have reviewed the word-of-mouth literature with a more behavioral perspective in mind. First, I provide a framework to organize research on the causes and consequences of word-of-mouth.2 Then, after reviewing extant literature related to this framework, I discuss additional open questions that deserve further study. Taken together, this paper provides insight into the psychological factors that shape word-of-mouth, as well as the consequences of these processes for consumer behavior.

AN ORGANIZING FRAMEWORK

In 1948, Harold Lasswell asked a question that underlies the key problems of what has become communications science: Who says what to whom in what channel with what effect? Numerous models of communication have been proposed since then (e.g., Berlo 1960; Schramm 1954), but the major components have remained similar (Figure 1).
2

Given potential issues with self-report, I focus on papers that have looked at actual behavior whenever possible.

Word-of-Mouth 5

FIGURE 1: Visual Depiction of Key Communication Factors

Message: What Gets Shared Effect

Channel Source
There is a 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. Source, or communication sender Message, or thing that is being communicated Audience, or person that is receiving the message Channel, or medium through which the message is being shared Effect, or consequence of the communication.

Audience

This framework is extremely useful in organizing word-of-mouth research. Consumers dont just communicate, they communicate about something, like politics or different car brands. Why do they talk about some things more than others? What goals drive what people talk about? Consumers also communicate to someone, like friends or neighbors. How does who people are talking to shape what they talk about? Communication also happens through a particular communication channel, like over the phone or face-to-face. How does the channel influence what people communicate? And finally, that communication has consequences, impacting what

Word-of-Mouth 6

the talker, or listener, thinks, does, or buys in the future.3 When does word-of-mouth have a larger impact on behavior and why? In this paper, I consider the word-of-mouth message, audience, channel, and effect, reviewing relevant research and touching on potential directions for future work. While research has also looked at who is more likely to talk, given space constraints, I focus on the other four components in greater detail. 4 Most work on word-of-mouth has focused on its effects, or the impact interpersonal communication has on the person receiving that communication. Consequently, we turn to this topic to start.

EFFECTS OF WORD-OF-MOUTH

Broadly speaking, word-of-mouth affects consumer behavior through two key routes (Van den Bulte and Wuyts 2009). The first is awareness. Word-of-mouth can inform people that a product or behavior exists (and make it more accessible or top-of-mind). This function is particularly important for new, unknown, or low-risk products and ideas (Godes and Mayzlin 2009).

A great deal of work has examined how social networks determine the flow of information and influence (see Van den Bulte and Wuyts 2009; Watts 2004 for reviews). Different network structures, for example, may speed or impede diffusion. Given this articles focus on opportunities for behavioral research, however, I focus on the more micro-level topic of word-of-mouth. 4 Early adopters, opinion leaders, market mavens and other people who believe that they have more knowledge or expertise tend to report sharing their opinion more often people (Engel, Kegerreis & Blackwell 1969; Feick and Price 1987; Katz and Lazersfeld 1955). Personality factors also play a role, as more extraverted and less conscientiousness people are more likely to forward emails they receive from others (Chiu, Hsieh, Kao, and Lee 2007) and neurotic people post more frequent (and more emotional) Facebook status updates (Buechel and Berger 2012, see Wilson, Gosling, and Graham 2012 for a broader discussion of factors driving Facebook use). Situational factors also shape who talks. Highly satisfied or dissatisfied customers seem to share more word-of-mouth (Anderson 1998) and people asked to rate an experience on a 5-pt scale report higher word-of-mouth intentions compared with those rating on a 100-pt scale (Chen and Godes 2012).

Word-of-Mouth 7

Word-of-mouth can also have a persuasive function. It can change opinions about whether something is right or worth doing (similar to informational influence, Deutsch and Gerard 1955), lead people to change their behavior to be liked or avoid being ostracized (similar to normative social influence, Deutsch and Gerard 1955), and generate competitive concerns related to status. Word-of-mouth can also impact the social identity consumers associate with a product or behavior, which may, in turn, affect likelihood of purchase or consumption (Berger and Heath 2007; 2008). All of these more persuasive functions may be particularly important when the uncertainty is high (i.e., risk reduction). Research on the effects of word-of-mouth can be broadly divided into two categories: quantitative research using field data and more behaviorally based experimental laboratory research.

Quantitative Research on Word-of-Mouth Effects Across a variety of domains, quantitative research finds that word-of-mouth has a causal impact on individual behavior (e.g., purchase or new product adoption) and the firm more broadly (e.g., aggregate sales or financial performance).5 Word of mouth has been shown to boost sales of books (Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006), bath and beauty products (Moe and Trusov 2011), and restaurants (Godes and Mayzlin 2009) and speed the adoption and diffusion of new pharmaceutical drugs (Iyengar et al 2010). Other work suggests that word-of-mouth may boost sales of music (Dhar and Chang 2009), movies (Chintagunta, Gopinath, and Venkataraman 2010;
5

People may tend to behave similarly to their social ties, but in the field it is often unclear whether this correlation is driven by influence (i.e., one person affecting anothers behavior), homophily (i.e., the fact that people tend to interact with similar others), or other factors (e.g., interdependent sampling, Denrell and Le Mens 2007). If someone goes to a movie their friend recommends, did they go because their friend recommended it, or would they have seen it anyway simply because they like those types of movies in the first place? This is an important issue that field work often deals with when trying to estimate the impact of word-of-mouth on behavior (see Aral, Muchnik, and Sundararajan 2009).

Word-of-Mouth 8

Dellarocas, Zhang, and Awad 2007; Duan, Gu, and Whinston 2008; Liu 2006) and video games (Zhu and Zhang 2010) and increase microfinance loans (Stephen and Galak 2012), television show viewership (Godes and Mayzlin 2004), and sign-ups to a social network website (Trusov, Bucklin, and Pauwels 2009). Some data even suggests that negative word-of-mouth may hurt stock prices (Luo 2009) and stock returns (Luo 2007). Most studies that have collected word-of-mouth content have focused on online word-ofmouth, in part because it is easier to acquire. Online word-of-mouth includes consumer reviews and blog posts, and can be decomposed into volume, valence, and variance (Dellarocas and Narayan 2006; Moe and Trusov 2011). Volume is the number of reviews or posts that a given item receives, where more postings are usually associated with increased sales. Valence is the average rating a product receives (e.g., 3.7 out of 5 stars, Dellarocas et al 2007), or the number of reviews of different types (e.g., 37 1-star reviews, Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006), and more positive reviews are generally associated with increased sales (though see Berger, Sorensen, and Rasmussen 2010). Finally, variance is either the statistical variance (Clemons, Gao, and Hitt 2006) or entropy (Godes and Mayzlin 2004) of the reviews. One interesting question for future research is when and why different online word-ofmouth metrics have a stronger impact on (or are more predictive of) behavior, sales, or other relevant outcomes. Different papers have found different metrics to be more or less important. Some papers have found significant effects on both the volume and valence of reviews (Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006; Dellarocas, et al. 2007). Other papers have found either only effects of review volume (Duan, et al. 2008; Liu 2006) or review valence (Chintagunta et al. 2010), but not the other. While some of the difference may be due to the specific modeling framework used, or dependent variable examined, the distinction between awareness and

Word-of-Mouth 9

persuasion may also be important. Word-of-mouth may be valued or used differently depending the novelty and risk involved with the thing being adopted (Godes and Mayzlin 2009; Van den Bulte and Wuyts 2009). For products that are relatively high-risk, or already quite well-known, the persuasive function of word-of-mouth should be particularly important. Thus valence should matter: Positive word-of-mouth should increase choice, while negative word-of-mouth may decrease it. For low-risk or novel products, however, word-of-mouth should also impact behavior through increasing awareness. Here, volume should matter more than valence, and even negative word-of-mouth may boost trial (Berger et al. 2010). Another rich area for behavioral research is the social dynamics of online reviews. Online review systems are organized in such a way that consumers are likely to see others reviews before they write their own. How does the volume and valence of existing reviews impact whether (1) consumers write a review and (2) the nature of the review they write? Consumers might be more (or less) likely to post their opinion if there are few reviews about a product already, or if their opinion differs from the prevailing view. Similarly, existing reviews might generate either assimilation or contrast effects. Indeed, social dynamics may be part of the reasons that the average product rating tends to decrease as more ratings arrive (Godes and Silva 2012; Li and Hitt 2008). Consequently, researchers have pointed out the importance of considering (and explicitly modeling) how existing reviews impact the arrival of new reviews (Moe and Trusov 2011; Moe and Schweidel 2012).

Behavioral Research on Word-of-Mouth Effects Behavioral research on word-of-mouth has focused on when word-of-mouth may have a

Word-of-Mouth 10

larger impact on behavior and why.6 Most of this work has looked at when and how word-of-mouth affects the word-of-mouth recipient.7 One important factor is characteristics of the word-of-mouth source. People tend to listen to more to credible sources, or those that are more trustworthy or have more expertise (Hovland and Weiss 1951; Petty and Wegner 1998; Pornpitakpan 2004). Other important factors are the strength of the tie (i.e., friends vs. acquaintances, or strong vs. weak ties) and their similarity to the word-of-mouth recipient. Per dose or instance of word-of-mouth, strong ties may be more impactful because people tend to trust them more and think they know more about their tastes and interests (Bakshy, et al. 2012; Brown and Reingen 1987). That said, people have more weak ties, or acquaintances, so the overall impact of these types of individuals may be larger (Bakshy, et al. 2012; see Brown and Reingen 1987; Granovetter 1973; Goldenberg, Libai, and Muller 2001 for related discussions).8 Similarly, word-of-mouth from similar others may have a more positive effect (Brown and Reingen 1987; Forman, Ghose, and Wiesenfeld 2008; also see Naylor, Lamberton, and Norton 2011) because people think their tastes are similar (Brock 1965). That said, word-of-mouth from dissimilar others may have benefits because these individuals have access to different information (Granovetter 1973) and may be more familiar with alternative ways of thinking (Burt 2004). Consequently, whether word-of-mouth from strong or weak ties and similar or dissimilar others is more impactful may depend on the

Though it focuses more on responses to persuasive messages in general, rather than word-of-mouth per se, the huge literature on attitude change (e.g., the elaboration likelihood model) is also relevant to considering when and why word-of-mouth may affect behavior. Though a comprehensive review of this literature is beyond the scope of this paper, see Petty, Wheeler, and Tormala 2012 for a recent review. 7 Word-of-mouth also impacts the person who shares it. If the speaker uses a lot of explaining language when sharing, for example, they will like a positive experience less and a negative experience more, impacting their likelihood of retelling that information again (Moore 2012). Sharing self-relevant content online (e.g., Facebook status updates or tweets) can also aid in emotion regulation, helping neurotic individual manage their emotions and repair wellbeing after negative emotional experiences (Buechel and Berger 2012). 8 Tie strength also impacts how easily it is to learn from word-of-mouth information and whether such information leads people to make better decisions (Zhang and Godes 2012).

Word-of-Mouth 11

particular situation. Finally, heavy users seems to have a larger impact on social contagion (Iyengar et al. 2011), but it is unclear whether this is because they talk more frequently or because they have higher status and are thus more likely to be listened to (Godes 2011). Another important factor is the nature of the word-of-mouth itself. Word-of-mouth varies in its valence: People can recommend a restaurant, say they hated it, or merely mention that they went there. Recommendations likely have the most positive impact on behavior, but even mentions should have a positive effect if they increase product awareness or accessibility (see Lynch and Srull 1982; also see Berger, et al. 2010; Nedungadi 1990; Stigler 1961). In terms of absolute impact, negative word-of-mouth may have a stronger impact than positive word-ofmouth, in some cases (Basuroy, Chatterjee, and Ravid 2003; Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006; see Chen and Lurie 2012 for a potential behavioral explanation) and may be more impactful when the word-of-mouth event happened further in the past (Smith and Schwarz 2012). Word-ofmouth also varies in its intensity or depth: People can talk briefly about an experience or they can go on at length. Longer or more in-depth word-of-mouth discussions should have a stronger impact on behavior [though this may be mitigated for online word-of-mouth, see Godes and Mayzlin (2004), as people may not end up reading an entire post or review]. Along these lines, face-to-face word-of-mouth may have a stronger impact than online or written word-of-mouth because it tends to be more engaging and vivid (Herr, Kardes, and Kim 1991).9 Whether the word-of-mouth is solicited also matters. Solicited advice seems to have a more positive impact than unsolicited advice (East et al. 2005) and unsolicited recommendations which go against an individuals opinion may even lead to reactance and strengthen the initial opinion (Fitzsimons and Lehmann 2004). Finally, the level of certainty expressed along with an opinion can also have

More generally, chatter can evolve over a huge variety of online channels (e.g., Facebook, Twitter, blogs, etc.), and research might examine which of these channels actually influence sales versus merely predict them.

Word-of-Mouth 12

an effect, with uncertainty actually being beneficial in some cases (Karmarkar and Tormala 2010). The susceptibility of the word-of-mouth recipient is also important (Watts and Dodds 2007). Just like some people may be more susceptible to catching a cold or a disease (e.g., because they have a weaker immune system), some people may be more susceptible to, or prone to be affected by, social influence (Aral and Walker 2012; Bearden, Netemeyer, and Tell 1989; Godes 2011). More susceptible individuals, for example, should be more likely to adopt new products or ideas if they hear about or see others using them. Though relatively little research has examined this issue, some data suggests that young people are more susceptible (Park and Lessig 1977; Pasupathi 1999) and people who perceive themselves as opinion leaders are less susceptible (Iyengar et al. 2011). Beyond individual differences, situational factors should also shape susceptibility. The closer people already are to taking some action, the more likely it is that a dose of word-of-mouth will push them over the edge. People who are searching online reviews, for example, are often close to being ready to make a purchase, and thus may be particularly susceptible to influence. Another potentially interesting question is how multiple instances, or doses, of word-ofmouth aggregate over time. Research distinguishes between simple and complex contagions (Centola and Macy 2007; Centola 2010). In some cases, awareness may be all that is needed, and thus one dose of word-of-mouth may be enough to get people to adopt a product or idea (i.e., simple contagion). Being forwarded a link to a newspaper article once may get people to take a look. But for most products, ideas, or innovations, multiple sources of influence increase the likelihood of adoption (i.e., complex contagion). Trying a new surgical procedure, or even

Word-of-Mouth 13

deciding to see a new movie, may require multiple sources of word-of-mouth before adoption occurs. In these situations, the persuasive impact of word-of-mouth may be more important. Research has recognized that some products and behaviors require more doses of influence, and documented that multiple doses increase adoption, but little empirical work has examined this area. What types of products tend to be complex contagions (e.g., potentially newer or riskier ones, Woodside and Delozier 1976)? How does the temporal distance between doses affect their overall impact (i.e., are two doses this week the same as one this week and one the next)? Are the behavioral processes behind these effects driven by memory, belief updating, or some other factor? Research might also consider how doses of word-of-mouth combine with advertising to impact behavior. One possibility is that advertising helps spread general awareness and word-ofmouth then persuades people to take action. A related question is the effect of advertising on word-of-mouth. Advertising not only combines with word-of-mouth to impact sales, but may boost word-of-mouth as well (Moon, Bergey and Iacobucci 2010; Onishi and Manchanda 2011; Stacey, Pauwels, Lackman 2012; Stephen and Galak 2012).10 What types of ads generate more word-of-mouth (Moldovan and Lehmann 2010) or are more likely to be shared? This is a particularly interesting question because some of the factors that make ads more likely to be shared may also make them less persuasive. One might imagine that consumers are hesitant to share advertisements that seem like direct influence attempts. At the same time, if advertisements make no attempt to be persuasive, they are less likely to benefit the brand. So it
10

Note that word-of-mouth may also effect advertising. More word-of-mouth, for example, may allow companies to advertise less because everyone already knows about the product. Word-of-mouth might also impact the content of advertising. Companies and organizations sometimes design their ads based on the everyday language people are using to describe the product. Further, if consumers are already talking about certain features of a product, advertisers may want to highlight other features, or try to stamp out false word-of-mouth by sharing the truth.

Word-of-Mouth 14

may be hard for ads to both be shared and boost brand evaluation or choice. Indeed, research suggests that virality and ad persuasiveness are negatively correlated; every million more view an ad receives is associated with 10% lower persuasiveness (Tucker 2011). One way to address this conundrum may be to generate entertaining content in which the product or brand is integral to the narrative (i.e., BlendTecs Will it Blend campaign). Such ads are interesting enough to be shared, but provide enough information about the brand to boost brand evaluation, purchase likelihood, and choice after delay (Akpinar and Berger 2012). Finally, future work might more deeply examine the relative impact of word-of-mouth and advertising on behavior. Some research suggests that word-of-mouth referrals have a larger short and longer term effects than media (Trusov, Bucklin, and Pauwels 2009) and that while the per-event (e.g., one TV or blog post mention) sales impact of earned media is larger than social media, social media has a larger impact overall because the events are more frequent (Stephen and Galak 2012). This work highlights that audience size (i.e., number of people reached) and per-person impact may be two key factors. Television ads, for example, tend to reach more people than most word-of-mouth conversations (though online ads may have similar reach). Advertising can also be used to reach many people faster than word-of-mouth usually can. Compared to a dose of advertising, one might imagine that a dose of word-of-mouth is more likely to change behavior because it is more persuasive. People likely trust word-of-mouth more than they trust traditional marketing efforts for a number of reasons. First, consumers recognize that advertisements are trying to persuade them, and may react against this influence attempt (Friestad and Wright 1994). Relatedly, word-of-mouth is usually more objective (though see Tuk, Verlegh, Smidts, and Wigboldus 2009). Ads always say the product is good, so they are not very diagnostic. Peoples friends, however, tend to tell them the truth, so word-of-mouth

Word-of-Mouth 15

is more trustworthy and more persuasive. Consequently, advertising may be most useful for spreading awareness while word-of-mouth may be most useful for persuasion. Word-of-mouth is also usually more targeted than advertising. Ads try to appeal only to the target demographic, but word-of-mouth is even more focused. Consumers tend to seek out the people in their social network who would be most interested in a given piece of information, and that may be part of the reason referred customers are more profitable overall (Schmitt, Skiera, and Van den Bulte 2011).

MESSAGE: WHY PEOPLE TALK AND WHAT THEY TALK ABOUT

A great deal of research has examined the consequences of word-of-mouth, but there has been less attention to its causes, or why people talk about and share certain things rather than others. Why do certain products get more word-of-mouth? Why does certain online content go viral? Why do certain rumors spread faster than others? Early research looked mostly at what topics received more discussion. In 1922, for example, Henry Moore walked up and down the streets of New York, eavesdropping on what others discussed. He found that men talked a lot about money and business while women (at least in the 1920s), talked a lot about clothes. Landis and Burt (1924) found that the prevalence of different topics varied with the situation: food is often talked about in restaurants while clothes are talked about near store windows. More recent research found that people often talk about personal relationships and experiences (Dunbar, Marriott, and Duncan 1997). Knowing what topics people talk about is interesting, but it says little about the drivers of discussion, or why people talk about some things more than others. Fortunately, however,

Word-of-Mouth 16

pockets of research in psychology, sociology, communications, and consumer behavior have begun to consider this issue. Existing work on the psychological drivers of word-of-mouth can be grouped into four key areas: Self-Enhancement, emotion, useful information, and accessibility. I review each of these areas in turn, noting both the underlying psychology or motivation that drives sharing (i.e., why people share), as well the types of things it leads people to pass on (i.e., what people are more likely to talk about and share).

Self-Enhancement A variety of theories suggest that one reason people share word-of-mouth is to selfenhance, or look good to themselves and others (Dichter 1966; Engel, Blackwell, and Miniard 1993; Gatignon and Robertson 1986; Hennig-Thurau, et. al. 2004; Sundaram, Mitra, and Webster 1998). People love talking about themselves. Studies of human conversation suggest that 30-40% of everyday speech involves telling others about ones personal experiences or relationships (Dunbar, et al. 1997; Landis and Burtt 1924). This percentage is even higher in social media. Over 70% of Twitter posts are about the self or ones own immediate experiences (Naaman, Boase, and Lai 2010). Neuroscientific evidence suggests that such self-disclosure is intrinsically rewarding (Tamir and Mitchell 2012). Sharing ones thoughts and feelings activates the same brain regions that respond to things like food, money, and seeing attractive members of the opposite sex. Importantly, this work finds two independent effects. While sharing information with others is rewarding on its own, there is also independent value for self-referential thought.

Word-of-Mouth 17

But people do not share all their thoughts and opinions equally. The tendency to selfenhance is one of the most dominant human motivations (Fiske 2001). And just like the car people drive, or the clothes they wear, what people say communicates things about them. If someone always talks about new restaurants, people may infer that they are a foodie. If someone always quotes Shakespeare and Thoreau, people may assume they are well-read. Word-ofmouth is a form of self-presentation. Consequently, people should be more likely to share things that make them look good rather than things that make them look bad. Things that allow them to gain attention, show connoisseurship, or indicate or achieve status. Self-enhancement should lead people to talk about interesting, novel, surprising, funny, or entertaining things. Indeed, ads that are more outrageous, provocative, or funny receive more views (Tucker 2012) and more interesting or surprising New York Times articles are more likely to make the papers Most Emailed List (Berger and Milkman 2012). Consumers report being more likely to share word-of-mouth about more original products (Moldovan, Goldenberg, and Chattopadhyay 2011) and more interesting and surprising urban legends (Heath, Bell, and Sternberg 2001). Controversy affects word-of-mouth in part because it makes discussion more interesting (Chen and Berger 2012) and interesting products get more online word-of-mouth (Berger and Iyengar 2012). Interesting products also receive more face-to-face word-of-mouth right after people first experience the product (Berger and Schwartz 2011). Interesting, surprising, novel, outrageous, provocative or funny things should all be entertaining and reflect positively on the person sharing them, making the sharer seem more interesting, cool, and in-the-know. Self-enhancement also affects whether people share positive or negative word-of-mouth. Most people would prefer not to be seen as a Debbie Downer, so sharing things that make others feel positive rather than negative should facilitate that goal. Consistent with this notion, more

Word-of-Mouth 18

positive New York Times articles are more likely to be highly shared (Berger and Milkman 2012). Self-enhancement motives also impact whether people share positive or negative experiences. Some data suggests that there are more positive than negative reviews (Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006; East, Hammond, and Wright 2007; Fowler and De Avila 2009). One reason may be that talking about positive experiences serves as evidence of ones expertise (i.e., I make good choices, Wojnicki and Godes 2011). Other work, however, suggests that negative word-of-mouth may be self-enhancing. Book review writers were seen as more intelligent, competent, and expert when they wrote negative as opposed to positive reviews (Amabile 1983). Similarly, concerns about public evaluation led people to express more negative ratings in some situations (Schlosser 2005). So when is positive versus negative word-of-mouth more self-enhancing? One moderating factor may be whether the item or experience being discussed signals something about the speaker. When someone chooses a restaurant, or a piece of online content to forward, the positivity or negativity of that experience or item reflects back on them. If it is good (bad) that makes them look good (bad). Consequently, people may spread more positive word-of-mouth to show they make good choices. If the reviewer or rater did not choose the item or experience they are rating, however, then whether that thing is good or bad signals less about them. Consequently, people may spread negative word-of-mouth to show they have discriminating taste. Consistent with this perspective, research finds that who people are talking about moderates the valence of word-of-mouth they tend to share (De Angelis, Bonezzi, Peluso, Rucker, and Costabile 2012). People generate positive word-of-mouth when talking about their own experiences but transmit negative word-of-mouth when talking about others experiences, in

Word-of-Mouth 19

part because it makes them look relatively better than others. When transmitting word-of-mouth about someone with whom they are closely attached, however, people share positive word-ofmouth because talking about close others reflects on them as well (De Angelis et al 2012). Self-enhancement also impacts what people talk in about in a host of other ways. The act of lying, for example, is partially driven by impression management concerns (DePaulo, Kashy, Kirkendol, & Wyer, 1996). People lie to protect their sense of self-worth and self-image (Sengupta, Dahl, and Gorn 2002), particularly in the face of negative social comparison information (Argo, White, and Dahl 2006). Self-enhancement may also lead people to talk about things that connote status or expertise (e.g., luxury, high priced, exclusive, or scarce items).

Emotion Experiencing emotion also boosts interpersonal communication. A good deal of research in psychology has investigated the social sharing of emotion (see Rime 2009 for a review). This work looks at emotional events, such as seeing a car crash or getting promoted, and examines when, why, and how people share these emotion inducing experiences with others. Some research, (e.g., Mesquita 1993 and Vergara 1993), for example, suggests that people share 90% of their emotional experiences with others (Rime et al., 1992; also see Walker et al. 2009). Experimental work shows that emotion boosts sharing across a variety of domains. Movies are more likely to be discussed, and news articles as more likely to be highly shared, if they are higher, rather than lower, in emotional intensity (Berger and Milkman 2012; Luminet, et al. 2000). People are more willing to forward emails with higher hedonic value (Chiu et al. 2007) and share urban legends that evoked more disgust, interest, surprise, joy, or contempt (Heath, et al. 2001). Highly satisfied and highly dissatisfied customers are more likely to share word-of-

Word-of-Mouth 20

mouth (Anderson 1998). Looking across a range of emotions, people were more willing to share more emotional social anecdotes (Peters, Kashima, and Clark 2009). There are a number of reasons why people might share emotional content or experiences. First, sharing can provide catharsis and help people deal with their emotional states (Pennebaker 1999; Pennebaker, Zech, and Rim 2001). Second, emotional stimuli often elicit ambiguous sensations (e.g., am I angry, sad, or both?), and through talking about and sharing with others, people can gain a deeper understanding of how they feel (Rime, Mesquita, Philippot, and Boca 1991). Third, to the extent that emotional material challenges peoples beliefs or way of seeing the world, they may share it with others to cope or reduce feelings of dissonance (Festinger, Riecken, and Schachter 1956). Fourth, sharing an emotional story or narrative with others increases the chance that they will feel similarly, which in turn, facilitates empathy and can deepen social connection (Peters and Kashima 2007). Some have even argued that this bonding function is the main reason language evolved in the first place (Dunbar 2004). While these benefits are valuable consequences of emotion sharing, it is unclear whether they actually cause people to share emotional content. Researchers have theorized that people share word-of-mouth to reduce anxiety (Sundaram, et al 1998), for example, or vent negative feelings (Hennig-Thurau, et. al. 2004), but it is unclear whether these consequences truly drove people to share in the first place, or are merely by-products of such sharing. Beyond emotion in general, it is also important to consider whether different emotions have different effects on transmission. This question is not only important from a practical standpoint, but also sheds light on the underlying mechanisms behind emotions influence on social transmission.

Word-of-Mouth 21

Valence. The main way emotions differ is their valence, or positivity and negativity. Certain emotions (e.g., excitement) are positive, while others (e.g., anxiety) are negative. One intuitive question is whether positive and negative emotions have a differential impact on transmission. Are people equally likely to talk about a positive or negative emotional experience? Share positive and negative word-of-mouth? Note that this question is different than simply asking whether there is more positive or negative word-of-mouth overall. Research suggests that there is more positive than negative word-of-mouth (Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006; East, Hammond, and Wright 2007), but this could simply reflect base rates of positive and negative events (i.e., more things are good than bad), rather than preferential transmission based on valence (i.e., given both experiences, people prefer to share positive word-of-mouth). Thus it is important to separate preferential transmission from mere base rates (see Berger and Milkman 2012; Godes et al. 2005). There are reasons to believe that either positive or negative things could be more likely to be shared. Bad news (e.g., earthquakes and product recalls) often spreads quickly, and the old news adage that if it bleeds, it leads, is based on the idea that negative content (e.g., tragedy or crime) is most likely to get attention and interest. Negative information has survival value because it tells people what to avoid, and across a range of domains bad things have a stronger impact than good ones (see Baumeister et al. 2001 for a review). At the same time, positive content provides information about potential rewards (i.e., this restaurant is worth trying) and sharing positive or upbeat content can boost the receivers mood. Further, as noted in the section on self enhancement, people may prefer to share positive things because they want to be associated with making others feel better rather than bringing them down. Being a Positive Polly rather than a Debbie Downer.

Word-of-Mouth 22

There is much more work to be done to examine when and why positive and negative things are shared, but a few papers have examined this area empirically. People report greater willingness to share pleasant ads than unpleasant ones (Eckler and Bolls 2011) and more positive news articles are more likely to be highly shared (Berger and Milkman 2012). Similarly, highly controversial topics induce discomfort which decreases peoples willingness to talk about them (Chen and Berger 2012). But the issue is likely more complicated than just positive gets shared and negative does not. While positive news articles are more likely to be highly shared, negative articles are more likely to make the list than articles that evoke no emotion at all (Berger and Milkman 2012). Congruence may play an important role, where people prefer to pass along news that is congruent with the valence of the domain in question (Heath 1996; also see Donovan, Mowan and Chakaborty 1999). So in negative domains, like crime, people are more likely to pass on more negative stories, while in positive domains, like vacations, people are more likely to share more positive stories. As discussed in the Self-Enhancement section, people may also be more willing to share positive things when talking about themselves and negative things when talking about others (De Angelis et al 2012). In addition, people with high needs for uniqueness may avoid sharing positive word-of-mouth (Cheema and Kaikati 2010), or talk positively but discourage adoption (Moldovan, Steinhart, and Ofen 2012), to prevent others from imitating them. Arousal. Emotions also differ in their level of physiological arousal. Physiological arousal is an excitatory state characterized by increased heart rate, blood flow, and a readiness for action (Heilman 1997). Some emotions, like anxiety or excitement are high arousal, while others, like sadness or contentment, are low arousal (Barrett and Russell 1998). Anxiety and

Word-of-Mouth 23

sadness are both negative emotional states, for example, but they differ in the level of arousal they induce. Anxiety is associated with activation while sadness is associated with deactivation. Research suggests that high arousal emotion increases social transmission. News articles that evoke more high arousal emotions, like awe, anger, or anxiety, are more likely to be highly shared, while articles that evoke more low arousal emotion, like sadness, are less likely to be highly shared (Berger and Milkman 2012). Taking one piece of content, and manipulating how much anger, amusement, or sadness it evokes has similar effects. People are more willing to share an article when it evokes more anger or amusement and less willing to share an article when it evokes more sadness. These effects are mediated by participants felt arousal. Arousal has such a strong impact on sharing that even incidental arousal, from watching a scary or funny film clip, or running in place, can spillover and boost sharing of unrelated content (Berger 2012). The idea that arousal increases sharing helps explain a diverse set of prior findings. Early work on rumor transmission suggested that rumors flourished in times of conflict, crisis, and catastrophe, due to the generalized anxiety those situations induce (Koenig 1985, see Heath et al 2001). More recent work suggests that extremely dissatisfied customers share more word-ofmouth (Richins 1983) and that ads that evoke more amusement are viewed more (Tucker 2012). Arousal may help integrate these disparate findings. Anxiety, extreme dissatisfaction, and amusement are all high arousal states that should increase social transmission. Arousal may also help explain the increased sharing of surprising, novel, or outrageousness content (Berger and Milkman 2012; Heath, et al. 2001; Moldovan, et al 2011; Tucker 2012). People may share such content to self-enhance, but they may also share it because of the arousal watching it evokes. The underlying mechanism(s) behind the effect of arousal on transmission, however, remains unclear. One possibility is that arousal generally increases action, and sharing happens

Word-of-Mouth 24

to be an action. Another possibility is misattribution, whereby arousal causes people to think content is better or more interesting, and thus more worthy of being shared. A third possibility is that arousal increases self-focus (Wegner and Giuliano 1980), which might lead people to weigh their own opinions more when deciding what to share. The effects of arousal on transmission are likely multiply determined, but the different mechanisms have different implications for when the effects should be larger. Other Emotional Moderators. Other factors also likely moderate the impact of emotion on sharing (see Rime 2009). Shame and guilt, for example, are two emotions that seem to lead to less sharing, rather than more (Finkenauer and Rime 1998), potentially because sharing such things should make people look bad. Extremely strong emotions (e.g., very high levels of fear) may also stunt sharing as they generate a state of shock that decreases the chance people take any actions.

Useful Information Another factor that shapes interpersonal communication is useful information. Psychologists, sociologists, and folklorists have long theorized that people share rumors, folktales, and urban legends not only for entertainment, but also because they contain practically valuable information or social morals (Allport and Postman 1947; Rosnow 1980; Shibutani, 1966; Rosnow and Fine, 1976; Brunvand 1981). People share rumors to help understand and simplify complicated events, (Allport and Postman 1947, p. 5) and tell legends not only because of their inherent plot interest but because they seem to convey true, worthwhile and relevant information (Brunvand, 1981, p. 11). Indeed, there is some evidence that stories that are more useful (i.e., people would change their behavior upon hearing them) are more likely to be shared (Heath, et al. 2001).

Word-of-Mouth 25

Some theories of gossip (e.g., Baumeister, Zhang, and Vohs 2004) have taken a similar approach, arguing that gossip is a key source of cultural learning. Gossip often implicitly communicates societal rules through stories, showing the negative repercussions that befell someone who violated social norms. Rather than trying to acquire information through trial and error, or direct observation of others (which may be difficult), gossip serves as a form of observational learning, allowing people to acquire relevant information quickly and easily. Others have argued that one of the main functions of conversations is social grooming or to acquire relevant information about other people (Dunbar, et al. 1997; Dunbar 1998). Some consumer behavior findings are consistent with these theoretical perspectives. Experimental work finds that people are more willing to share marketing messages that have more utilitarian value (Chiu et al. 2007) and suggests that usefulness may be particularly relevant in shaping the valence of word-of-mouth (Moldovan, et al. 2011). Empirical analyses find that more useful news articles are more likely to be highly shared (Berger and Milkman 2012). But while it is clear that people share useful information, the exact motive for sharing this type of content may be multiply determined. One reason people share useful information may be altruism, or the desire to help others (Dichter, 1966; Engel et al. 1993; Hennig-Thurau et al. 2004; Price et al. 1995). Critical interviews, for example, suggest that over 20% of word-of-mouth conversations were motivated by the desire to help others avoid problems or make satisfying purchase decisions (Sundaram, et al. 1998). Other drivers are more self-focused. Useful information has social exchange value (Homans 1958) which can generate future reciprocity (Fehr, Kirchsteiger, and Riedl 1998). People may also share useful content because it is selfenhancing, and reflects positively on them (i.e., makes them seem like they know useful things). Which of these motives is most important, and when, remains unclear.

Word-of-Mouth 26

People also seek useful information, or advice, from others (Dichter 1966; HennigThurau et al. 2004; Sundaram et al. 1998). Theoretical frameworks suggest that such information may help reduce risk, decrease search time, and allow people to support and justify their decisions (Engel, Blackwell, and Miniard 1993; Gatignon and Robertson, 1986; Hennig-Thurau and Walsh 2003). That said, relatively little empirical work has actually looked at when and why people seek advice, so this may be a fruitful area for future research

Accessibility Accessibility also impacts what people talk about and share. Products, information, and conversation topics more broadly vary in their accessibility (Higgins and King 1981; Wyer and Srull 1981). Some things are more top of mind, while others are less so. But stimuli in the environment, such as sights, sounds, or smells, can act as cues, or triggers, activating associated concepts in memory and making them more accessible (Higgins, Rholes, and Jones 1977). This activation can then spread to related concepts through an associative network (Anderson 1983; Collins and Loftus 1975). So eating peanut butter not only makes peanut butter more top-ofmind, but it also makes its frequent partner jelly more accessible. A great deal of research has shown how accessibility affects judgment and choice (Feldman and Lynch 1988; Hauser 1978; Nedungadi 1990; see Lynch and Srull 1982 for a review). Accessibility also shapes word-of-mouth. Consistent with the notion that people tend to talk about situation consistent topics (Landis and Burt 1924), people reported that 80% of wordof-mouth about a new coffee product was driven by related conversational or environmental cues (e.g., seeing an ad, but also simply drinking coffee or talking about food, Belk 1971). Similarly, many word-of-mouth referrals seem to be driven by related topics happening to be discussed

Word-of-Mouth 27

(Brown and Reingen 1987). Cues can also be internal. People who care a lot about a particular brand, or are heavily involved in a certain product category should have those constructs more chronically accessible, which should lead them to be more likely to be discussed. More recent empirical work supports this suggestion, finding that products that are cued or triggered more frequently by the surrounding environment get 15% more word-of-mouth (Berger and Schwartz 2011). Experimental evidence underscores this notion, and shows that increasing the prevalence of triggers for a product increases the amount of word-of-mouth it receives. Similarly, research shows that compared to consumers opinions, certainty information (i.e., consumers feeling of certainty or uncertainty associated with an opinion) is less likely to be shared because it is less accessible (Dubois, Rucker and Tormala 2011). The effect of public visibility on word-of-mouth may also be driven by accessibility. Some products and behaviors (e.g., shirts and greetings) are more public while others (e.g., socks and donations to charity) tend to be more private. This increased visibility, in turn, should increase the chance that others see and talk about those products. People talk about shirts more than socks, for example, because the former is more likely to be seen, made accessible, and discussed. Indeed, publicly visible products receive 8% more word-of-mouth (Berger and Schwartz 2011). Accessibility may also explain why people tend to talk about things they have in common with others (Clark 1996; Stalnaker 1978). Consider how often people talk about the weather, how teaching is going (if theyre faculty members), or how classes are going (if theyre students). These subjects are not the most interesting, but they are common ground (Grice 1989), or topics that are shared across people. As discussed later in the section on audience tuning, conversation partners should automatically activate related conversation topics and make them more

Word-of-Mouth 28

accessible. Consequently, topics that are common ground should automatically be more top-ofmind. Even controlling for actual performance, for example, more familiar baseball players get more mentions in online discussion groups (Fast, Heath, and Wu 2009). More familiar topics should be more top-of-mind and thus more likely to be discussed. That said, discussing common ground topics may also be driven by self-enhancement and the desire for social connection. Having things in common facilitates social bonding, and particularly when meeting new people, bringing up topics that everyone can relate to increases the chance that conversation goes well, making the sharer look better as a result.

Other Drivers of Word-of-Mouth Though self-enhancement, emotion, useful information, and accessibility are the four drivers of word-of-mouth that have received the most empirical support, other potential drivers that have also been suggested. Some have theorized that people share word-of-mouth to enact vengeance, or punish a company for a bad product or negative service experience (Richins 1983; Sundaram et al. 1998). This could be placed under emotion, as it seems driven in part by emotional arousal, but it also seems slightly distinct. Others have suggested that people share to bond with others (Dunbar 2004; Hennig-Thurau, et. al. 2004). The fact that neurotic individuals microblog more frequently (e.g., post more Facebook status updates, Buechel and Berger 2012) is consistent with this notion, as is the finding that sharing emotions deepens social connections (Peters and Kashima 2007).

Word-of-Mouth 29

Open Questions In addition to thinking about how certain factors affect why people talk and what they talk about, research might also examine when different word-of-mouth drivers matter more. Self enhancement, emotion, useful information, and accessibility all influence word-of-mouth, but when does each play a relatively larger or smaller role? One important moderator may be memory. Sometimes people talk about a shirt they just bought or a thing that happened to them last night. Other times they talk about a shirt they bought last month or something that happened to them last year. These examples distinguish between immediate and ongoing word-of-mouth. Immediate word-of-mouth describes mentions that happen right after people first experience a product. Ongoing word-of-mouth describes the product mentions that occur in the weeks and months that follow. Certain product characteristics may be associated with more word-of-mouth over one of these time horizons than the other. Some research suggests that more interesting products only get more immediate word-of-mouth, while products that are cued more frequently, or are more publicly visible, receive both more immediate and more ongoing discussion (Berger and Schwartz 2011). People may talk about interesting things right after they happen, but for something to continue to be talked about, people may need to be cued or triggered to think about it. Thus self-enhancement and emotion may shape whether people talk about things right after they happen, but as time elapses memory (i.e., accessibility) plays a larger role. Memory based processes like accessibility may also play a larger role in communication channels where people are more likely to talk about things that happened further in the past. Online content often seems to be shared soon after it is experienced. People watch a video, or read an article, and then share it or not. Face-to-face discussion, however, often seems to involve

Word-of-Mouth 30

things that happened more distally. Thus memory based processes (like accessibility) may play a larger role in face-to-face than online word-of-mouth. The audience may also moderate which word-of-mouth drivers are more important. We discuss audience effects in more detail below, but intuition suggests people should be more likely to care about self-enhancement when sharing with acquaintances (vs. friends) or when discussion is public (e.g., on Facebook) rather than private. Research might also more deeply examine how people say what they say. Emotional arousal might lead people to talk about an experience, for example, but there are a variety of ways people could talk about that same event. They could use different words, be more or less assertive, and express varying degrees of certainty. Some research has begun to look at language use, investigating explaining language (Moore 2012), expressions of modesty (Packard, Gershoff, and Wooten 2012), and linguistic mimicry of conversation partners (Moore and McFerran 2012), but much more remains to be done.

AUDIENCE

Another major question is who people share word-of-mouth with. People have a variety of social ties: friends, neighbors, family members, acquaintances, and colleagues. Even within a particular category, each tie has different interests, expertise, likes and dislikes. When do people share word-of-mouth with certain people rather than others? And what role does the audience play in what gets shared?

Word-of-Mouth 31

Strong vs. Weak Ties One audience factor that shapes what people talk about and share is the strength of the social tie. As noted earlier, research distinguishes between strong and weak ties; people we know well, trust, and/or speak to often, versus acquaintances with whom we do not have as strong a connection (Granovetter 1973; Brown and Reingen 1987). Tie strength should affect what people share in a number of ways. One relates to the value of information. When the moral hazard, or value of information is high, people may only share with strong ties (Frenzen and Nakamoto 1993). If there is a restricted sale that only a certain number of people can get access to, for example, people may only share that information with close friends. But when the information value is lower, or the consequences less restrictive, people seem to be more willing to share that information with their weak ties as well. Tie strength should also impact whether people are willing to share things that are revealing, potentially damaging to the self, or controversial. People tend to share unusual or emotional experiences with close others or individuals they know well (Brown and Reingen 1987; Heath, et al. 2001; Rime 2009). Because we feel a closer bond with our strong ties, we may be more willing to reach out to them, particularly when we want to talk about things that are embarrassing, damaging, or negative (e.g., a love of pop music or dealing with a tough breakup).11 Similarly, people are more willing to talk about controversial topics with friends (than strangers) because it reduces the discomfort from talking about dicey, potentially divisive issues (Chen and Berger 2012). That said, one could argue that people may feel more comfortable saying embarrassing things to strangers because we dont see them often. Further, people are

11

Though people certainly share embarrassing things over social media, which is sent primarily to their weak ties, this seems more like an effect of social presence rather than tie strength per se. Because the audience isnt actually present, people may feel more uninhibited.

Word-of-Mouth 32

more likely to lie to friends rather than strangers because they are relevant social comparison targets (Argo, et al. 2006). Some work has also examined how tie strength and referral rewards interact (Ryu and Feick 2007). This work suggests that while consumers receive an economic benefit from referral rewards, the potential cost is a dissatisfied receiver and the self-perception that ones personal recommendation can be bought. They find that people are more likely to naturally tell their strong ties about a product they liked, but providing a reward for referral increases the likelihood of telling weaker ties. Finally, tie strength also affects what people share through activating constructs related to psychological distance. Strong ties should be represented as closer to the self than weak ties. This difference in psychological distance should change construal level (Trope and Liberman 2010), whereby close others activate a concrete mindset while distant others activate an abstract mindset. Once activated, these mindsets may change what people think about and share. Product disadvantages (cons) are often more concrete than advantages (pros, Eyal et al. 2004), for example, and as a result, sharing with close others prompts people to talk more about cons than sharing with distant others (Dubois, Bonezzi and De Angelis, 2012).

Audience Size Sometimes people talk to a large audience, chatting with a group of co-workers or emailing a bunch of friends. Other times people talk to a small audience, chatting with just one co-worker or emailing with just one friend. The former can be described as broadcasting, while the latter can be described as narrowcasting.

Word-of-Mouth 33

These differences in audience size can impact what people discuss (Barasch and Berger 2012). Broadcasting is often public, and may involve communicating with weaker social ties that do not know the sharer as well. Consequently, broadcasting should encourage selfenhancement. Narrowcasting, on the other hand, makes people less egocentric because it focuses them on their audience. Consequently narrowcasting leads people to focus less on selfpresentation and more on what the audience would find useful. Broadcasting also encourages acknowledging multiple perspectives while at the same time paradoxically dumbing down content to the lowest denominator. Larger audiences often contain many different viewpoints. Thus sharers must appeal to different people with the same message, presenting multiple viewpoints at the same time (Schlosser 2005), and adjusting the message to offer a more balanced opinion (Fleming et al. 1990). Finding something everyone can relate to may also become more difficult as audience size increases, which could lead simpler, more basic, or less controversial things to be discussed.

Audience Tuning People also engage in audience tuning, tailoring what they share to the knowledge, attitudes, and interests of their audience (Clark and Marshall 1981; Clark and Murphy 1982; Higgins 1981; Higgins 1992; Krauss and Fussell 1991).12 When people are talking to foodies, they mention restaurants, but when they are talking to sports junkies, they mention football. Just like other stimuli in the environment, conversation partners can act as triggers, activating related concepts in memory (Fitzsimons and Bargh 2003) and leading them to be more likely to be discussed. The presence of someone who likes football also makes football a more relevant conversation topic (Grice 1989; Sperber and Wilson 1986; also see Berger and Heath 2005).
12

This, may in turn, affect what the communicator remembers (Higgins 1999)

Word-of-Mouth 34

The reverse should also occur, whereby content activates related individuals. Reading a newspaper article about football should increase the accessibility of social ties who might find that article interesting or useful. The increased accessibility of content-related receivers has interesting implications for whether broadly or narrowly relevant content is more likely to be shared. Some information (e.g., an article about football or American food) has a broader potential audience than other information (e.g., an article about water polo or Ethiopian food). Consequently, intuition suggests that broadly-relevant content should be more likely to be shared. Narrowly relevant content, however, may benefit from stronger associations with particular individuals. While narrowly relevant content is, by definition, not as broadly interesting, it is more likely to be tied one or two particular individuals in ones social network. Further, because it is tied to fewer individuals, those ties are likely to be stronger (i.e., the fan effect, Anderson 1974; 1983). Most people dont eat Ethiopian food, but because of that, it is more likely that a particular social tie (or two) is strongly activated when someone comes across an article about Ethiopian food. This stronger activation, in turn, may increase the likelihood that the article is shared, because people may use activation strength as a signal that content is worth sharing. Thus while broadly relevant content could be shared with more people, narrowly relevant content may be more likely to be shared because it strongly activates a few people who might find the content highly relevant. This discussion highlights the important distinction between sharing likelihood and number of shares overall. In this case, there is likely more overlap across people in what is broadly-relevant than what is narrowly relevant. Most people have some friends that like American food, but while some people may have a few friends that like Ethiopian food, and

Word-of-Mouth 35

other people have a few friends who like water polo, most people may not have any friends that like either. Consequently, compared to broad relevant topics, each person may be more likely to share whatever topic is narrowly relevant to them, but broadly relevant topics may get more overall shares because they could be shared by, and to, many more people. The underlying psychological driver of sharing is the same, but what it leads people to share may differ across different individuals.

Open Questions Beyond tie strength, audience size, and tuning, however, there has been little work examining the relationship between audience and sharing. There may be a variety of other audience differences that shape what people share.

COMMUNICATION CHANNELS

Consumers communicate through different channels. They talk face-to-face, on the phone, and in chat rooms; through blogs, on Facebook, and over text. Almost all word-of-mouth research, however, only examines data from a single channel, limiting the ability to speak to the role of channels in word-of-mouth. Might the channel consumers communicate through affect what they talk about, and if so, how?

Computer-Mediated Communication One difference between communication channels is whether they are computer-mediated. Though some work has used face-to-face word-of-mouth (e.g., Berger and Schwartz 2011; Brown and Reingen 1987; Frenzen and Nakamoto 1993; Godes and Mayzlin 2009; Granovetter

Word-of-Mouth 36

1973; Iyengar, et al. 2010; Richins 1983) most recent word-of-mouth research using field data has focused on computer-mediated commination like online reviews (Chevalier and Mayzlin 2006; Moe and Trusov 2011), social media mentions (Stephen and Galak 2012), newsgroups (Godes and Mayzlin 2004), and blogs (Dhar and Chang 2009). Various perspectives have theorized about how computer-mediated communication shapes interpersonal relations (see Walther 2011 for a review). When people communicate faceto-face, they often use tone of voice and other nonverbal cues (e.g., appearance, facial expressions, and posture) to make inferences about others. These features are absent from computer-mediated communication (unless people video chat), which can make it difficult to communicate accurately and effectively (Epley and Kruger 2005; Kruger, Epley, Parker, and Ng 2005). This had led some to argue that computer-mediated communication is more impersonal and makes it difficult to manage impressions (e.g., Culnan and Markus 1987). More recently, however, the hyperpersonal model (Walther 1996; 2007; 2011) has argued that computer-mediated communication actually facilitates self-presentation and selfenhancement. Since people are physically separated, they do not have to monitor the nonverbal signals they or their conversation partner(s) sends, allowing them to avoid sending mistaken signals and freeing up cognitive resources to focus on what they want to say. Further, while face-to face interaction tends to be synchronous (i.e., almost no delay between conversational turns), computer-mediated communication is usually asynchronous (i.e., people respond to emails or texts hours, rather than seconds later). This asynchronicity may allow people to construct and refine the messages they send. They can abort messages and begin anew, or go back and change what they wrote before they send it. Asynchronicity may then allow them to

Word-of-Mouth 37

strategically develop and edit how they present themselves, selecting and transmitting desired information. Little empirical work has investigated the consequences of these differences, but they should have important implications for what people talk about and the nature of communication more generally. One paper, for example, suggests that channel synchronicity moderates whether interest drives discussion (Berger and Iyengar 2012). While one might think that interesting products are more likely to be talked about more than boring ones, this may only be the case in more asynchronous channels where people have time to think about what to say (i.e., online communication and texting). When people talk face-to face or over the phone, interesting products are not talked any more likely to be discussed. Further, merely having people pause before they talk face-to-face leads more interesting products to be discussed. This suggests that self-enhancement has a bigger impact on what people talk about when they have time to think about what to say. Along these lines, other work suggests that requests made by email are seen as more polite than those made by voicemail, ostensibly because people have time to compose their requests (Duthler, 2006). In instances where people do not have as much time to think, however, accessibility likely drives discussion. The content of computer-mediated communication may also be more interesting because communication is relatively optional. Rather than sitting next to someone and feeling the need to fill conversational space, most social media posts do not require responses. Further, social norms of politeness make it harder to exit a boring face-to-face conversation than end an email chain. Consequently, one could imagine that computer-mediated conversations are shorter, more interesting, and more goal directed (e.g., driven by the desire to find particular information). So

Word-of-Mouth 38

people may still tweet about what they had for lunch, but on average computer-mediated conversations may be less vacuous. The cost of computed-mediated communications may also be higher. One could argue that writing is more effortful than speech, and if so, conversations should be shorter and peoples willingness to talk about unimportant issues may decrease. Factors that make typing relatively harder (e.g., smaller keyboards on smartphones than laptops) may also moderate these effects. Computer-mediated interactions may also impact communication through their permanency. Compared to speaking, any sort of writing tends to provide a more permanent record of what was said. This suggests that people might be less likely to say embarrassing things, and more likely to self-enhance, through computer-mediated communication. That said, the fact that others are not present during the formation of the communication may lead people to be less aware of their audience, which could lead more embarrassing things to be shared. Finally, text-based communication may also have different effects on the information sharer. Warm interpersonal contact can help release oxytocin and reduce stress, but instant messaging does not seem to provide the same beneficial responses (Seltzer, Ziegler, and Pollak 2010). Similarly, research suggests that compared to conversations that involve voice, text-based communication lead observers to infer that the communicator possess weaker mental capacitiesless agency, less experience, less basic human nature, and less uniquely human traits (Epley and Schroeder 2012).

Identifiability Another way conversation channels differ is the degree to which communicators are identifiable. People often post reviews, blog, or tweet anonymously with usernames that readers

Word-of-Mouth 39

may not easily be able to trace back to their actual identity. In face-to-face discussions, and even on Facebook, however, identities are disclosed, and people know who they are talking to. Identifiability should lead self-enhancement concerns to play a larger role (Goffman 1959). Similar to effects of public consumption (Ratner and Kahn 2002), identifiability should make people more conscious of what they are saying and what it communicates about them (Schlosser 2005). Along these lines, research on controversy and conversation shows that people are less willing to talk about controversial topics when their identity is disclosed (Chen and Berger 2012). Similarly, research suggests that public discussion may lead people to adjust their attitudes downward so as not to appear indiscriminant (Schlosser 2005). Anonymous posting, however, should reduce identity signaling concerns. This may be one reason people say nasty or repulsive things in online forums where their identity is not disclosed.

Audience Size Though it is not an effect of channel per se, channels also tend to differ in the size of the audience. Some channels (e.g., blog posts and Facebook status updates) tend to involve broadcasting, while others (e.g., most phone calls or email) involve narrowcasting. These differences should lead to the same effects as noted in the Audience Size portion of the Audience section.

Channel Selection Most of this discussion has centered on how channels impact communication, but people may also select different channels depending on what they want to discuss. People who are embarrassed about a particular preference or interest, for example, may use computer-mediated

Word-of-Mouth 40

channels that allow them to solicited information while remaining anonymous. We discuss this issue of channel selection versus channel influence in more detail below.

BROADER QUESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH

There are many interesting and important questions related to word-of-mouth that would benefit from further research. While the preceding sections have raised some of them, a few broader questions and points are worth considering.

(When) Is Word-of-Mouth Goal Driven? One overarching question is the degree to which word-of-mouth is goal-driven. Most research has assumed that word-of-mouth is motivated action. Researchers ask questions like what motivates people to talk about their experiences? (Dichter 1966, p. 147) or set out to reveal the underlying motives for consumers engaging in both positive and negative word-ofmouth, (Sundaram, et al. 1998, p. 527). Most work on self-enhancement takes a similar perspective, suggesting that people choose to talk about things that make them look good. In using words like motivates and choose, however, research has implicitly assumed that word-of-mouth is driven by some sort of goal. To look good. To help others. As weve discussed, these goals or motivations certainly shape some word-of-mouth. When going on a date, or interviewing for a job, people care a lot about how they are perceived, and may be motivated to talk about things that help others or make them look good.

Word-of-Mouth 41

But most interpersonal communication does not seem so motivated. What people talk about when they have lunch with their friends or grab a beer after work seems less like goaldriven choices and more like small talk about whatever happens to be going on that day. The importance of arousal and accessibility in driving word-of-mouth is consistent with this perspective. The fact that people are more likely to share things that make them angry or anxious (Berger and Milkman 2012) or after they have exercised (Berger 2011) seems to have little to do with a motivation to appear a particular way. Similarly, the fact that people are more likely to talk about accessible things has little to do with decisions or goals. In fact, while intuition and practitioners suggest that interesting products should get more word-of-mouth than boring ones, some data suggests that this only occurs for online conversations (Berger and Iyengar 2012), or for immediate word-of-mouth in offline conversations (Berger and Schwartz 2011). Thus, like many other behaviors (Bargh and Chartrand 1999), most word-of-mouth may not be as motivated as once imagined. Situational factors likely moderate when word-of-mouth is goal-driven versus shaped by accessibility. Communication over most channels is voluntary. People dont have to send a text or respond to a blog post, and so the decision to share word-of-mouth over these channels is almost always a choice. Some face-to-face communication, in contrast, is imposed by the situation. It is awkward to have dinner in silence, or not say anything when you run into a colleague on the subway, so the situation pushes people to talk. In these instances, the goal is simply to say something rather than say nothing. Further, given the expected synchronicity of the conversation (i.e., long pauses are uncomfortable), there is little time to select and craft the best possible thing to say. So rather than talking about the most self-enhancing or useful thing, people may be more likely to talk about what is top of mind. In such situations, the act of saying

Word-of-Mouth 42

something may be motivated (i.e., the desire to avoid awkward pauses in conversation), but what in particular people say may not be as motivated. This suggests that face-to-face word-of-mouth is driven more by accessibility, while word-of-mouth over the phone, text, or online, may be more goal driven.

When Is Word-of-Mouth More Content versus Context Driven? Situational factors should also shape whether word-of-mouth is content or context driven. Some word-of-mouth is driven by the content itself. People find a useful article or have a highly emotional experience and they just have to pass it on. In these situations, people seem to share if the content is above some threshold (e.g., of interest, utility, or emotion). In other situations (e.g., sitting face-to-face at dinner), conversation is exogenously imposed by the context. So the question becomes less about whether something is worth talking about, and instead given that conversation is occurring, which of the things that could be discussed are actually discussed. In situations where word-of-mouth is context driven, the channel and audience are fixed, so the only degree of freedom is what to talk about. In situations where word-of-mouth is content driven, however, the sharer can decide whether to share, what channel to share over, and who to share with. Though this distinction is intuitive, it likely has important implications for what gets discussed. Context driven word-of-mouth should depend a lot on accessibility, where the audience and other surrounding factors act as triggers to bring up certain things to discuss. In these instances, the key question may be given that something is top-of-mind, should it be talked about or held back.

Word-of-Mouth 43

Content driven word-of-mouth, however, raises many more questions. How do people decide who to share something with? Is the audience simply driven by accessibility, or who comes to mind, and fit, or how useful or valuable that potential receiver might find the content? What role does the strength of tie or frequency of interaction play? And how do people decide what channel to share the content through? Is channel selection simply driven by convenience? Some work suggests that at least in the online sphere, where people post a review may depend in part on where they think it will get the most attention (Chen and Kirmani, 2012). Further research might delve into these issues more directly.

When Is Word-of-Mouth More Source versus Audience Driven? Another general question is how much the audience matters in word-of-mouth. One could argue that most word-of-mouth is audience driven. After all, people are not just sharing word-of-mouth, they are picking a particular person or people to share that word-of-mouth with. Further, as shown in work on audience tuning (Clark and Murphy 1982; Higgins 1992) people seem to tailor what they are saying to their audience, sharing things that are at least moderately relevant or interesting to the people they are communicating with, particularly when those individuals are made salient. At the same time, however, the fact that arousal boosts sharing suggests that internal sharer states play an important role in whether transmission occurs. Similarly, people suggest that most of the reasons why they share word-of-mouth are self-focused (Stephen and Lehmann 2009). So is the audience really the driving force behind sharing or merely a filter that leads people to share slightly different things depending on who they share with? More broadly, when does the source (audience) play a relatively larger role in what people talk about?

Word-of-Mouth 44

There are likely a number of moderators. One may be audience size. As noted, because narrowcasting (i.e., talking with one person) is more directed, it is driven more by the audience and what will be useful to them (Barasch and Berger 2012). Another moderator may be arousal. As mentioned, arousal may lead people to be more self-focused, and thus lead word-of-mouth to be driven more by the sharer. The stage of the conversation may also matter. People may be more focused on themselves when the conversation starts, but focus relatively more on others as the conversation evolves and utterances are more in response to what the other person has said.

Evolution of Conversation Most work on word-of-mouth treats each utterance as an isolated event, but in reality, each utterance is embedded in a broader conversation. How do conversations evolve from one topic to the next? What psychology underlies conversational evolution, and what types of things may be more likely to be discussed at different stages of conversation? Accessibility likely plays a big role in how conversations evolve. Just as conversation partners or contextual factors can act as triggers, so can prior conversation topics, increasing the accessibility of related ideas and making them more likely to be discussed. Thus conversations may move from one cued topic to the next, along a line of related concepts. Topics may likely become more personal, revealing, and abstract as the conversation evolves. Particularly for strangers, but even for friends, talking for a period can create familiarity and connection that encourages trust and deeper revelation (particularly when conversation involves self-disclosure, Aron et al 1997). It is also difficult to start a conversation with high-level issues or controversial topics (e.g., abortion or beauty norms in society).

Word-of-Mouth 45

Consequently, conversations may start with more concrete, pedantic topics and through associated cues, move to broader more abstract higher-level discussions.

Word-of-mouth and Social Networks Though a full review of social networks research is beyond the scope of this paper, it is also important to consider how networks and word-of-mouth interact. Word-of-mouth can inform and influence, but how it flows throughout a population depends on the pattern of social ties (Brown and Reingen 1987; see Van den Bulte and Wuyts 2009; Watts 2004 for reviews). A particular video may be highly sharable, but if it originates in a disconnected corner of the network, it may be unlikely to ever become viral. Similarly, complex contagions may be more likely to catch on if a group of tightly connected individuals are seeds, rather than if the product is diffused across the network (Stephen and Berger 2012; Kalish, Mahajan, and Muller 1995).

Self-Report Biases Whenever research involves asking people about their behavior, researchers must also ask themselves whether participants responses are truly accurate. People often have little insight into why they did something, and may be unaware of the stimulus that caused their response and the fact that it impacted their behavior (Nisbett and Wilson 1977). Further, attitudes do not always translate into behavior (Ajzen and Fishbein 1977) and misreporting may occur even when people are asked about their actual behavior. Rather than reflecting reality, responses may be based on implicit theories, social desirability, memory biases, and other factors. These same issues should impact responses about word-of-mouth. Take questions about prospective behavior. Researchers often ask people how likely they would be to talk about

Word-of-Mouth 46

different brands or stories. But given that people prefer to see themselves in a positive light, it would not be surprising if people report being more likely to talk about things that make them look good rather than bad (e.g., funny stories or helpful topics), even if that is not what they would actually end up discussing. Indeed, people thought they would mention more interesting brands in face-to-face discussion, even though more direct measures of behavior found that this was not the case (Berger and Schwartz 2011). Issues also affect questions about retrospective behavior. Peoples responses about what they talked about in the past are shaped by memory. People will be more likely to report talking about things that stick out in memory, rather than things that dont. So even if people are not trying to self-enhance, they may remember talking about interesting things more than boring ones because interesting discussions are more memorable (the same is true of more emotional events). Most people talk a lot about meals for example, but retrospective surveys would not show this because those conversations are unlikely to be striking enough to persist in memory. This suggests that when self-enhancement is involved, studies that involve read behavior may be particularly useful. Have participants engage in real conversations, pick things to talk about, or even write to others expecting that they would actually converse. Even including just one real behavior study in a paper provides some assurance that the phenomena studied is not restricted to self-report. Otherwise, responses may be particularly biased towards selfenhancement and away from factors like accessibility or non-conscious process that may be hard for people to anticipate in advance.

Word-of-Mouth 47

Thinking outside the Lab The emergence of online word-of-mouth has provided a wealth of data to examine (Lazer, et al. 2009). Tweets, online reviews, and blogs are only a few of the many big data sources of real sharing behavior. Researchers have begun using text mining and natural language processing to pull insights from large corpuses of written information (Ghose, Iperirotis, and Li, forthcoming; Netzer, Feldman, Goldenberg, and Fresko 2012; Tirunillai and Tellis 2012). But even less complex tools can be useful. Simple textual analysis programs (e.g., Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count and ANEW) can shed light on a host of psychological processes (Pennebaker, Mehl, and Niederhoffer 2003; also Berger and Milkman 2012; Chen and Lurie 2012). Others have manually coded psychological constructs (Moore 2012; Schlosser 2011). Researchers have also figured out clever ways to capture and analyze offline chatter using small recording devices (Mehl, et al. 2001; Mehl and Robbins 2012) or transcripts of customer service calls (Moore, Packard, and McFerran 2012). Others have devised field experiments, creating online applications that allow randomization of different word-of-mouth messages over time (Aral and Walker 2012; Centola 2011). Laboratory experiments are vital for teasing out underlying mechanisms behind word-ofmouth. But by collecting field data, or running field experiments, researchers can examine the relative importance of different process. Further, given that individuals may not always be aware of what they talk about, or will talk about, measuring actual behavior may be particularly valuable.

Word-of-Mouth 48

Conclusion In conclusion, word-of-mouth is both frequent and important. But while researchers have shown the consequences that word-of-mouth has on behavior, we have only barely begun to understand its causes. What do people talk about, who they talk about it with, and what channel do they talk about it through? These are only a few of the overarching questions that deserve further research. Word-of-mouth is a multiply determined phenomenon. It is an interaction of the source, the audience, the channel, and the content. Hopefully this review will spur more researchers to delve into this exciting area.

Word-of-Mouth 49

REFERENCES Ajzen, Icek and Martin Fishbein (1977), Attitudebehavior Relations: A Theoretical Analysis and Review of Empirical Research, Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888918. Akpinar, Ezgi and Jonah Berger (2012), Valuable Virality, Wharton Working Paper. Allport, Gordon W., and Leo Postman (1947), The Psychology of Rumor, New York, NY: Henry Holt. Amabile, Teresa M. (1983), Brilliant but Cruel: Perceptions of Negative Evaluators, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(2), 14656. Anderson, John R. (1974), Retrieval of Propositional Information from Long-term Memory, Cognitive Psychology, 6, 451-474. Anderson, John R. (1983), The Architecture of Cognition, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Anderson, Eugene W. (1998), Customer Satisfaction and Word of Mouth, Journal of Service Research, 1(1), 5-17. Aral, Sinan and Dylan Walker (2012), Identifying Influential and Susceptible Members of Social Networks, Science, 337(6092), 337-341. Aral, Sinan, Lev Muchnik and Arun Sundararajan (2009), Distinguishing Influence-Based Contagion From Homophily-Driven Diffusion in Dynamic Networks, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(51), 2154421549. Argo, Jennifer J., Katherine White and Darren. W. Dahl (2006), Social Comparison Theory and Deception in the Interpersonal Exchange of Consumption Information, Journal of Consumer Research, 33(1), 99-108. Aron, Arthur, Edward Melinat, Elaine N. Aron, Robert Vallone and Renee Bator (1997), The Experimental Generation Of Interpersonal Closeness: A Procedure And Some Preliminary Findings, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 363-377. Bakshy, Eytan, Itamar Rosenn, Cameron Marlow, and Lada Adamic (2012), The Role of Social Networks in Information Diffusion, 21st ACM World Wide Web Conference. Lyon, France. April, 16-20. Barasch, Alix and Jonah Berger (2012), Broadcasting and Narrowcasting, Wharton Working Paper. Bargh, John A., and Tanya L. Chartrand (1999), The Unbearable Automaticity of Being, American Psychologist, 54, 462-479. Barrett, Lisa Feldman and James A. Russell (1999), The Structure of Current Affect: Controversies and Emerging Consensus, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(1), 1014. Basuroy, Suman, Subimal Chatterjee, and S. Abraham Ravid (2003), How Critical Are Critical Reviews? The Box Office Effects of Film Critics, Star Power, and Budgets, Journal of Marketing, 67, 103-117. Baumeister Roy F., Ellen Bratslavsky, Catrin Finkenauer and Kathleen D. Vohs (2001), Bad Is Stronger Than Good, Review of General Psychology, 5, 323-370. Baumeister, Roy F., Liqing Zhang and Kathleen D. Vohs (2004), Gossip as Cultural Learning, Review of General Psychology, 8, 111-121. Bearden, William O., Richard G. Netemeyer and Jesse E. Teel (1989), Measurement Of Consumer Susceptibility To Interpersonal Influence, Journal of Consumer Research, 15(4), 473481.

Word-of-Mouth 50

Belk, Russell W. (1971), "Occurrence of Word-of-Mouth Buyer Behavior as a Function of Situation and Advertising Stimuli," in Proceedings of the American Marketing Association's Educators Conference, ed. Fred C. All-vine, Chicago, IL: American Marketing Association, 419-422. Berger, Jonah (2011), Arousal Increases Social Transmission of Information, Psychological Science, 22(7), 89193. Berger, Jonah and Chip Heath (2005), Idea Habitats: How the Prevalence of Environmental Cues Influences the Success of Ideas, Cognitive Science, 29, 195-221. Berger, Jonah and Chip Heath (2007), Where Consumers Diverge from Others: IdentitySignaling and Product Domains, Journal of Consumer Research, 34(2), 121-134. Berger, Jonah and Chip Heath (2008), Who Drives Divergence? Identity-Signaling, Outgroup Dissimilarity, and the Abandonment of Cultural Tastes, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 593-607. Berger, Jonah and Raghuram Iyengar (2012), Drivers of Online and Offline Word of Mouth, Working Paper, the Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania. Berger, Jonah and Katherine Milkman (2012), What Makes Online Content Viral? Journal of Marketing, 49(2), 192-205. Berger, Jonah and Eric Schwartz (2011), What Drives Immediate and Ongoing Word-ofMouth? Journal of Marketing, 48(5), 869-880. Berger, Jonah, Alan T. Sorensen and Scott J. Rasmussen (2010), Positive Effects of Negative Publicity: When Negative Reviews Increase Sales, Marketing Science, 29(5), 815-827. Berlo, David K. (1960), The Process of Communication, New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston. Brock, Timothy C. (1965), Communicator-recipient Similarity and Decision Change, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1(6), 65054. Brown, Jacqueline Johnson and Peter H. Reingen (1987), Social Ties and Word-of-Mouth Referral Behavior, Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 35062. Brunvand, Jan H. (1981), The Vanishing Hitchhiker: American Urban Legends and Their Meanings, New York, NY: Norton. Buechel, Eva and Berger, Jonah (2012), Facebook Therapy: Why People Share Self-Relevant Content Online, University of Miami Working Paper. Bughin, Jacques, Jonathan Doogan and Ole Jrgen Vetvik (2010), A New Way to Measure Word-of-Mouth Marketing, McKinsey Quarterly, April 2010. Burt, Ronald S. (2004), Structural Holes and Good Ideas, American Journal of Sociology, 110(2), 349-399. Centola, Damon (2010), The Spread of Behavior in an Online Social Network Experiment, Science, 329(5996), 1194-1197. Centola, Damon (2011), An Experimental Study of Homophily in the Adoption of Health, Science, 334(6060), 1269-1272. Centola, Damon and Michael Macy (2007), Complex Contagions and the Weakness of Long Ties, American Journal of Sociology, 113, 702-34. Cheema, Amar and Andrew M. Kaikati (2010), The Effect of Need for Uniqueness on Word-ofMouth, Journal of Marketing Research, 47(3), 553 563. Chen, Yu-Jen and Amna Kirmani (2012), Persuasion Knowledge in Online Communication: The Consumer as Media Planner, University of Maryland Working Paper.

Word-of-Mouth 51

Chen, Zoey and Jonah Berger (2012), When, Why, and How Controversy Causes Conversation, Wharton Working Paper. Chen, Zoey, and Nicholas Lurie (2012), Temporal Contiguity and the Negativity Bias in Online Word-of-Mouth, Presented at the Behavioral Decision Research in Management Conference, Boulder, CO. Chen ,Yu-Jen and David Godes (2012), Rating With Confidence: Rating Certainty and Wordof-Mouth Behavior, University of Maryland Working paper. Chevalier, Judith A. and Dina Mayzlin (2006), The Effect of Word of Mouth on Sales: Online Book Reviews, Journal of Marketing Research, 43, 345-354. Chintagunta, Pradeep K., Shyam Gopinath and Sriram Venkataraman (2010), The Effects of Online User Reviews on Movie Box-Office Performance: Accounting for Sequential Rollout and Aggregation Across Local Markets, Chicago Booth School of Business Research Paper No. 09-09. Chiu, Yi, Jin-Chern Chiou, Weilun Fang , Yung-Jian Lin and Mingching Wu (2007), Design, Fabrication, and Control of Components in MEMS-Based Optical Pickups, IEEE Trans. Magn., 43(2), 780-785. Clark, Herbert H. (1996), Using Language, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Clark, H. H., and Marshall, C. R 1981. Definite Reference and Mutual Knowledge, Elements of Discourse Understanding, 10-63. Clark, Herbert H. and Gregory L. Murphy (1982), Audience Design In Meaning And Reference, in Language and Comprehension, eds. Jean Francois Le Ny and Walter Kintsch, New York, NY: Elsevie, 287296. Clemons, Erik K., Guodong Gao and Lorin Hitt (2006), When Online Reviews Meet Hyper Differentiation: A Study of the Craft Beer Industry, Journal of Management Information Systems, 23 (2), 149-171. Collins, Allan M. and Elizabeth F. Loftus (1975), A Spreading-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing, Psychological Review, 82(6), 407428. Culnan, Mary, and M. Lynne Markus (1987), Information Technologies, Handbook Of Organizational Communication: An Interdisciplinary Perespective, Newbury Park, CA: Sage, 420-443. De Angelis, Matteo, Andrea Bonezzi, Alessandro Peluso, Derek Rucker and Michele Costabile (2012), On Braggarts and Gossips: A Self-Enhancement Account of Word-of-Mouth Generation and Transmission, Journal of Marketing Research, forthcoming. Dellarocas Chrysanthos and Ritu Narayan (2006), A Statistical Measure of a Population's Propensity to Engage in Post-Purchase Online Word-of-Mouth, Statistical Science, 21(2), 277-285. Dellarocas, Chrysanthos, Xiaoquan Zhang and Neveen Awad (2007), Exploring the Value of Online Product Reviews in Forecasting Sales: The Case of Motion Pictures, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 21(4), 23. Denrell, Jerker and Gael Le Mens (2007), Interdependent Sampling and Social Influence, Psychological Review, 114 (2), 398-422. DePaulo, Bella M., Deborah A. Kashy, Susan E. Kirkendol and Melissa M. Wyer (1996), Lying In Everyday Life, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 979995. Deutsch, Morton, and Harold B. Gerard (1955), A Study of Normative and Informational Social Influences Upon Individual Judgment, The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 629-636.

Word-of-Mouth 52

Dhar, Vasant and Elaine Chang (2009), Does Chatter Matter? The Impact of User-Generated Content on Music Sales, Journal of Interactive Marketing, 23(4), 300-207. Dichter, Ernest (1966), How Word of Mouth Advertising Works, Harvard Business Review, 44, 147-166. Donovan, D. Todd, John C. Mowen and Goutam Chakraborty (1999), Urban Legends: The Word-of-Mouth Communication of Corporate Morality, Marketing Letters, 10(1), 2334. Duan, Wenjing, Bin Gu and Andrew B. Whinston (2008), The Dynamics of Online Word-ofMouth and Product Sales: An Empirical Investigation of the Movie Industry, Journal of Retailing, 84(2), 233-242. Dubois, David, Andrea Bonezzi and Matteo De Angelis (2012), " Intrinsic Versus Image-Related Motivations in Social Media: Why Do People Contribute Content to Twitter," Insead Working Paper. Dubois, D., Rucker, D.D., & Tormala, Z.L. (2011). From rumors to facts, and facts to rumors: The role of certainty decay in consumer communications. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 1020-1032. Dunbar, Robin (2004), The Human Story, London: Faber and Faber. Dunbar, Robin, Anna Marriott and N. D. C. Duncan (1997), Human Conversational Behavior, Human Nature, 8, 231-246. Dunbar, R. (1998). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Duthler, Kirk W. (2006), The Politeness Of Requests Made Via Email And Voicemail: Support For The Hyperpersonal Model, Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 11(6), 500521. East, Robert, Kathy Hammond, Wendy Lomax and Helen Robinson (2005), What Is The Effect of a Recommendation? The Marketing Review, 5, 145-157. East, Robert, Kathy Hammond and Malcolm Wright (2007), The Relative Incidence Of Positive and Negative Word Of Mouth: A Multi-Category Study, International Journal of Research in Marketing, 24(2), 175-184. Eckler, Petya and Paul Bolls (2011), Spreading the Virus: Emotional Tone of Viral Advertising and Its Effect on Forwarding Intentions and Attitudes, Journal of Interactive Advertising,11(2), 1. Engel James E., Roger D. Blackwell and Paul W. Miniard (1993), Consumer Behavior, 7th ed., Chicago: Dryden Press. Engel James E., Roger D. Blackwell and Paul W. Miniard (1993), Consumer Behavior, 8th ed., Fort Worth: Dryden Press. Engel, James E., Roger D. Blackwell and Robert J. Kegerreis (1969), How Information is Used to Adopt an Innovation, Journal of Marketing Research, 9(4), 3-8. Epley, Nicholas and Juliana Schroeder (2012), The Humanizing Voice, University of Chicago Working Paper. Epley, Nicholas, and Justin Kruger (2005), When What You Type Isn't What They Read: The Perseverance of Stereotypes and Expectancies Over Email, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 414-422. Eyal, Tal, Nira Liberman, YaacovTrope, and Eva Walther (2004), The Pros and Cons of Temporally Near and Distant Action, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 781-795. Fast, Nathanael J., Chip Heath and George Wu (2009), Common Ground and Cultural Prominence: How Conversation Reinforces Culture, Psychological Science, 20, 904-911.

Word-of-Mouth 53

Fehr, Ernst, Georg Kirchsteiger and Arno Riedl (1998), Gift Exchange and Reciprocity in Competitive Experimental Markets, European Economic Review, 42(1), 1-34. Feick, Lawrence F. and Linda L. Price (1987), 'The Market Maven: A Diffuser of Marketplace Information, Journal of Marketing, 51, 83-97. Feldman, Jack M. and John G. Lynch Jr. (1988), Self-Generated Validity and Other Effects of Measurement on Belief, Atti- tude, Intention, and Behavior, Journal of Applied Psychology, 73 (3), 42135. Festinger, Leon, Henry W. Riecken and Stanley Schachter (1956), When Prophecy Fails, New York, NY: Harper and Row. Finkenauer, Catrin and Bernard Rime (1998), Socially Shared Emotional Experiences vs. Emotional Experiences Kept Secret: Differential Characteristics and Consequences, Journal of Social Clinical Psychology, 17, 295-318. Fiske, Susan T. (2001), Social and Societal Pragmatism: Commentary on Augustinos, Gaskell, and Lorenzi-Cioldi, in Representations Of The Social: Bridging Research Traditions, eds. K. Deaux & G. Philogene, New York, NY: Blackwell, 249-253. Fitzsimons, Gavan J. and Donald R. Lehmann (2004), Reactance to Recommendations: When Unsolicited Advice Yields Contrary Responses, Marketing Science, 23, 82-94. Fitzsimons, Grainne M. and John A. Bargh (2003), Thinking of You: Nonconscious Pursuit of Interpersonal Goals Associated with Relationship Partners, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148-164. Fleming, John H., John M. Darley, James L. Hilton and Brian A. Kojetin (1990), Multiple Audience Problem: A Strategic Communication Perspective on Social Perception, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(4), 593609. Forman, Chris, Anindya Ghose and Batia Wiesenfeld (2008), Examining the Relationship Between Reviews and Sales: The Role of Reviewer Identity Disclosure in Electronic Markets, Information Systems Research, 19(3), 291-313. Fowler, Geoffrey and Joseph De Avila (2009), On the Internet, Everyone's a Critic But They're Not Very Critical, Wall Street Journal: Technology, http://online.wsj.com/article/SB125470172872063071.html. Frenzen, Jonathan and Kent Nakamoto (1993), Structure, Cooperation, and the Flow of Market Information, Journal of Consumer Research, 20, 360-375. Friestad, Marian and Peter Wright (1994), The Persuasion Knowledge Model: How People Cope with Persuasion Attempts, Journal of Consumer Research, 21 (June), 131. Gatignon, Hubert and Thomas S. Robertson (1986), An Exchange Theory Model of Interpersonal Communications, in Advances in Consumer Research, ed. Richard J. Lutz, 13, 629 632. Ghose, Anindya, Panagiotis Ipeirotis and Beibei Li (2012), Designing Ranking Systems for Hotels on Travel Search Engines by Mining User-Generated and Crowd-Sourced Content, Marketing Science, forthcoming. Godes, David and Dina Mayzlin (2004), Using Online Conversations to Study Word-of- Mouth Communication, Marketing Science, 23(4), 54560. Godes, David, Dina Mayzlin, Yubo Chen, Sanjiv Das, Chrysanthos Dellarocas, Bruce Pfeiffer, Barak Libai, Subrata Sen, Mengze Shi and Peeter Verlegh (2005), The Firms Management of Social Interactions, Marketing Letters, 16 (3/4), 415-428. Godes, David and Dina Mayzlin (2009), Firm-Created Word-of-Mouth Communication: Evidence from a Field Test, Marketing Science, 28(4), 721-739.

Word-of-Mouth 54

Godes, David and Jose Silva (2012), The Dynamics of Online Opinion, Working Paper. Godes, David (2011), Invited Comment on Opinion Leadership and Social Contagion in New Product Diffusion, Marketing Science, 30(2), 224-229. Goffman, Erving (1959), The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life, New York, NY: Anchor. Goldenberg, Jacob, Barak Libai and Eitan Muller (2001), Talk of the Network: A Complex Systems Look at the Underlying Process of Word-of-Mouth, Marketing Letters, 12(3), 20921. Granovetter, Mark S. (1973), The Strength of Weak Ties, American Journal of Sociology, 78(6), 1360-80. Grice, Paul (1989), Studies in the Way of Words, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Hauser, John R. (1978), Testing the Accuracy, Usefulness, and Significance of Probabilistic Models: An Information-theoretic Approach, Operation Research Society of America, 26(3), 406421. Heath, Chip (1996), Do People Prefer to Pass along Good news or Bad news? Valence and Relevance of News as a Predictor of Transmission Propensity, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68, 7994. Heath, Chip, Chris Bell, and Emily Sternberg (2001), Emotional Selection in Memes: The Case of Urban Legends, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1028-1041. Heilman, Kenneth M. (1997), The Neurobiology of Emotional Experience, Journal of Neuropsychiatry, 9, 439448. Hennig-Thurau, Thorsten, Kevin Gwinner, Gianfranco Walsh and Dwayne Gremler (2004),Electronic WordofMouth Via ConsumerOpinion Platforms: What Motivates Consumers to Articulate Themselves on the Internet? Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18(1), 38-52. Herr, Paul M., Frank R. Kardes and John Kim (1991), Effects of Word-of-Mouth and ProductAttribute Information on Persuasion: An Accessibility-Diagnosticity Perspective, Journal of Consumer Research, 17(4), 454-462. Higgins, E. Tory (1981), The Communication Game: Implications For Social Cognition And Persuasion, in Social Cognition: The Ontario Symposium, eds. E. Tory Higgins, C. P. Herman, and Mark P. Zanna, Vol. 1, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 343392. Higgins, E. Tory (1992), Achieving Shared Reality in the Communication Game: A Social Action that Creates Meaning, Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 11, 107131. Higgins, E. Tory (1999), Saying is Believing Effects: When Sharing Reality About Something Biases Knowledge and Evaluations, in Shared Cognition in Organizations: The Management of Knowledge, eds. L.L. Thompson, J.M. Levine, and D.M. Messick, Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 3349. Higgins, E. Tory and Gillian King (1981), Accessibility of Social Constructs: Information Processing Consequences of Individual and Contextual Variability, in Personality, Cognition, and Social Interaction, Vol. 14, eds. Nancy Cantor and John Kihlstrom, Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 621-635. Higgins, E. Tory, William S. Rholes and Carl R. Jones (1977), Category Accessibility and Impression Formation, Journal of Social Psychology, 13(2), 14154. Homans, George C. (1958), Social Behavior as Exchange, American Journal of Sociology, 63(6), 597-606. Hovland, Carl I. and Walter Weiss (1951), "The Influence of Source Credibility on Communication Effectiveness". Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), 635650.

Word-of-Mouth 55

Iyengar, Raghuram, Christophe Van den Bulte and Thomas W. Valente (2010), Opinion Leadership and Social Contagion in New Product Diffusion, Marketing Science, 30(2), 195-212. Kalish, Shlomo, Vijay Mahajan and Eitan Muller (1995), Waterfall and Sprinkler New-Product Strategies in Competitive Global Markets, International Journal of Research in Marketing 12, 105-119. Karmarkar, Uma R. and Zakary L. Tormala (2010), Believe Me, I Have No Idea What I'm Talking About: The Effects Of Source Certainty On Consumer Involvement And Persuasion, Journal of Consumer Research, 46, 1033-1049. Katz, Elihu and Paul Felix Lazarsfeld (1955), Personal Influence: The Part Played by People in the Flow of Mass Communication, Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Keller, Ed and Brad Fay (2009), Comparing Online and Offline Word of Mouth: Quantity, Quality, and Impact, New Brunswick, NJ: Keller Fay Group. Keller, Ed and Barak Libai (2009), A Holistic Approach to the Measurement of WOM, Presented at the ESOMAR Worldwide Media Measurement Conference, Stockholm. Koenig, Frederick (1985), Rumor In The Marketplace: The Social Psychology Of Commercial Hearsay, Dover, MA: Auburn House. Krauss, Robert M. and Susan R. Fussell (1991), Perspective-Taking In Communication: Representations Of Others Knowledge In Reference, Social Cognition, 9, 224. Kruger, Justin, Nicholas Epley, Jason Parker and Zhi-Wen Ng (2005), Egocentrism Over Email: Can We Communicate As Well As We Think? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 925-936. Landis M. H. and H. E. Burtt (1924), A Study of Conversation, Journal of Comparative Psychology, 4(1), 8189. Lazer, David, Alex Pentland, Lada Adamic, Sinan Aral, Albert-Laszlo Barabasi, Devon Brewer, Nicholas Christakis, Noshir Contractor, James Fowler, Myron Gutmann, Tony Jebara, Gary King, Michael Macy, Deb Roy and Marshall Van Alstyne (2009), Computational Social Science, Science, 323(5915), 721-722. Li, Xinxin and Lorin M. Hitt (2008), Self-Selection and Information Role of Online Product Reviews, Information Systems Research, 19(4), 456474. Liu, Yong (2006), Word of Mouth for Movies: Its Dynamics and Impact on Box Office Revenue, Journal of Marketing, 70(3), 74-89. Luo, Xueming (2007), Consumer Negative Voice and Firm-Idiosyncratic Stock Returns, Journal of Marketing, 71, 75-88. Luo, Xueming (2009), Quantifying the Long-Term Impact of Negative Word of Mouth on Cash Flow and Stock Prices, Marketing Science, 28(1), 148-165. Luminet, Olivier, Patrick Bouts, Frederique Delie, Antony S.R. Manstead and Bernard Rime (2000), Social Sharing of Emotion Following Exposure to a Negatively Valenced Situation, Cognition & Emotion, 14, 661688. Lynch, John G. and Thomas K. Srull (1982), Memory and Attentional Factors in Consumer Choice: Concepts and Research Methods, Journal of Consumer Research, 9(1), 1837. Mehl, Matthias R. and Megan L. Robbins (2012), Naturalistic Observation Sampling: The Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR), in Handbook Of Research Methods For Studying Daily Life, eds. M. R. Mehl and T. S. Conner, New York, NY: Guilford Press. Mehl, Matthias R., James W. Pennebaker, D. Michael Crow, James Dabbs and John H. Price (2001), The Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR): A Device for Sampling Naturalistic

Word-of-Mouth 56

Daily Activities and Conversations, Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 33, 517-523. Mesquita, Batja G. (1993), Cultural Variations in Emotions. A Comparative Study of Dutch, Surainamese, and Turkish People in the Netherlands, Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam. Misner, Ivan R. (1999), The Worlds Best Known Marketing Secret: Building Your Business with Word-of-Mouth Marketing, 2nd ed., Austin, TX: Bard Press. Moe, Wendy W. and David A. Schweidel (2012), Online Product Opinion: Incidence, Evaluation and Evolution, Marketing Science, forthcoming. Moe, Wendy W. and Michael Trusov (2011), Measuring the Value of Social Dynamics in Online Product Forums, Journal of Marketing Research, forthcoming. Moldovan, Sarit and Donald R. Lehmann (2010), "The Effect of Advertising on Word-of-Mouth," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 37, eds. Margaret C. Campbell, Jeff Inman, and Rik Pieters, Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research. Moldovan Sarit, Jacob Goldenberg and Amitava Chattopadhyay (2011), The Different Roles of Product Originality and Usefulness in Generating Word of Mouth, International Journal of Research in Marketing, 28, 109-119. Moldovan, Sarit, Yael Steinhart and Shlomit Ofen (2012), Share and Scare: Solving the Communication Dilemma of Early Adopters with a High Need for Uniqueness, Industrial Engineering and Management, forthcoming. Moon, S., Bergey, P. K., & Iacobucci, D. (2009). Dynamic effects among movie ratings, movie revenues, and viewer satisfaction. Journal of Marketing, 74 (1), 108-121. Moore, Henry T. (1922), Further Data Concerning Sex Differences, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 17, 210-214. Moore, Sarah G. and Brent McFerran (2012), Linguistic Mimicry in Online Word of Mouth, University of Alberta Working Paper. Moore, Sarah G. (2012), Some Things are Better Left Unsaid: How Word of Mouth Influences the Storyteller, Journal of Consumer Research, 38(6), 1140-1154. Moore, Sarah, Grant Packard and Brent McFerran (2012), Do Words Speak Louder Than Actions? Firm Language in Customer Service Interactions, University of Alberta, Working Paper. Naaman Mor, Jeffrey Boase and Chih-Hui Lai (2010), Is It Really About Me?: Message Content in Social Awareness Streams, Proceedings of the 2010 ACM Conference on Computer Supported Cooperative Work (Association for Computing Machinery), 189-192. Naylor, Rebecca Walker, Cait Poynor Lamberton and David A. Norton (2011), Seeing Ourselves in Others: Reviewer Ambiguity, Egocentric Anchoring, and Persuasion, Journal of Marketing Research, 48, 617-631. Nedungadi, Prakash (1990), Recall and Consumer Consideration Sets: Influencing Choice Without Altering Brand Evaluations, Journal of Consumer Research, 17(3), 26376. Netzer, Oded, Ronen Feldman, Jacob Goldenberg and Moshe Fresco (2012), Mine Your Own Business: Market Structure Surveillance Through Text Mining, Marketing Science, forthcoming. Nisbett, Richard, and Timothy Wilson (1977), Telling More Than We Can Know: Verbal Reports on Mental Processes, Psychological Review, 84, 231-259. Onishi, Hiroshi and Puneet Manchanda (2011), Marketing Activity, Blogging and Sales, International Journal of Research in Marketing, forthcoming.

Word-of-Mouth 57

Packard, Grant, Andy Gershoff and David Wooten (2012), Is Immodesty a Vice When Sharing Advice? Consumer Responses to Self-Enhancing Sources of Word-of-Mouth Information, University of Michigan Working Paper. Park, C.Whan, V. Parker Lessig (1977), Students And Housewives: Difference In Susceptibility To Reference Group Influence, Journal of Consumer Research, 4(2), 102110. Pasupathi, Monisha (1999), Age Differences In Response To Conformity Pressure For Emotional And Nonemotional Material, Psychology and Aging, 14(1), 170174. Pennebaker, James W. (1999), The Effects of Traumatic Disclosure on Physical and Mental Health: The Values of Writing and Talking about Upsetting Events, International Journal of Emergency Mental Health, 1(1), 9-18. Pennebaker, James W., Emanuelle Zech and Bernard Rim (2001), Disclosing and Sharing Emotion: Psychological, Social, and Health Consequences, in Handbook of Bereavement Research: Consequences, Coping, and Care, eds. Margaret S. Stroebe, Robert O. Hansson, Wolfgang Stroebe, and Henk Schut, Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 51744. Pennebaker, James W., Matthias R. Mehl and Katie Niederhoffer (2003), Psychological Aspects of Natural Language Use: Our Words, Our Selves, Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547-577. Peters, Kim and Yoshihasa Kashima (2007), From Social Talk to Social Action: Shaping the Social Triad with Emotion Sharing, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 780-797. Peters, Kim, Yoshihasa Kashima and Anna Clark (2009), Talking About Others: Emotionality and the Dissemination of Social Information, European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 207-222 Petty, Richard E. and Duane T. Wegner (1998), Attitude Change: Multiple Roles For Persuasion Variables, in The Handbook Of Social Psychology, Vol. 1, New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, 323-390. Petty, Richard E., S. Christian Wheeler and Zakary T. Tormala (2003), Persuasion And Attitude Change, in Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 5. Personality and Social Psychology, eds. I. B. Weiner, & M. J. Lerner, New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical re- view of five decades evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 243281 Price, Linda L., Eric J. Arnould and Sheila L. Deibler (1995), Consumers Motional Responses to Service Encounters: the Influence of the Service Provider, International Journal of Service Industry Management, 6(3), 34 63. Ratner, Rebecca K. and Barbara K. Kahn (2002), The Impact of Private versus Public Consumption on Variety-Seeking Behavior, Journal of Consumer Research, 29, 246-257. Richins, Marsha L. (1983), "Negative Word-of-Mouth by Dissatisfied Consumers: A Pilot Study," Journal of Marketing, 47 (Winter), 68-78. Rime, Bernard (2009), Emotion Elicits the Social Sharing of Emotion: Theory and Empirical Review, Emotion Review, 1, 6085. Rime, Bernard, Catrin Finkenauer, Olivier Luminet, Emmanuelle Zech and Pierre Philippot (1992), Social Sharing of Emotion: New Evidence and New Questions, European Review of Social Psychology, 9, 145-189 Rime, Bernard, Batja Mesquita, Pierre Philippot and Stefano Boca (1991), Beyond the Emotional Event: Six Studies on the Social Sharing of Emotion, Cognition and

Word-of-Mouth 58

Emotion, 5, 435-465. Rosnow, Ralph L. (1980), Psychology of Rumor Reconsidered, Psychological Bulletin, 87, 578-591. Rosnow, Ralph L. and Gary A. Fine (1976), Rumor and Gossip: The Social Psychology of Hearsay, New York, NY: Elsevier. Ryu, Gengseog and Lawrence Feick (2007), A Penny for Your Thoughts: Referral Reward Programs and Referral Likelihood, Journal of Marketing, 71, 84-94. Schlosser, Ann E. (2005), Posting versus Lurking: Communicating in a Multiple Audience Context, Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 260-265. Schlosser, Ann E. (2011), Can Including Pros and Cons Increase the Helpfulness and Persuasiveness of Online Reviews? The Interactive Effects of Ratings and Arguments, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(3), 226-39. Schmitt, Philipp, Bernd Skiera and Christophe Van den Bulte (2011), Referral Programs and Customer Value, Journal of Marketing, 75, 46-59. Schramm, W. (1954), How Communication Works, in The Process And Effects Of Communication, ed. W. Schramm, Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press, 3-26. Seltzer, Leslie J., Toni E. Ziegler and Seth D. Pollak (2010), Social Vocalizations Can Release Oxytocin In Humans, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences, 277, 2661-2666. Sengupta, Jaideep, Darren W. Dahl and Gerald G. Gorn (2002), Misrepresentation in the Consumer Context, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 6979. Shibutani, Tamotsu (1966), Improvised News: A Sociological Study of Rumor, Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. Smith, Robert W. and Norbert Schwarz (2012), When it Happened Tells Us What Happened: Metacognitive Inferences in Word-Of-Mouth Communications, University of Michigan Working Paper. Sperber, Dan and Deidre Wilson (1986), Relevance: Communication and Cognition, Oxford: Blackwell. Stacey, E. Crag, Koen H. Pauwels and Andrew Lackman (2012), Beyond Likes and Tweets: How Conversation Content Drives Store and Site Traffic, Working Copy. Stalnaker, Robert C. (1978), Assertion, in Syntax and Semantics 9: Pragmatics, ed. P. Cole, New York, NY: Academic Press, 315332. Stephen, Andrew and Jonah Berger (2012), Creating Contagious: How Social Networks and Item Characteristics Combine to Spur Ongoing Consumption and Drive Social Epidemics, Working Paper. Stephen, Andrew T. and Jeff Galak (2012), The Effects of Traditional and Social Earned Media on Sales: A Study of a Microlending Marketplace, Journal of Marketing Research, forthcoming. Stephen, Andrew T. and Donald R. Lehmann (2009), Is Anyone Listening? Modeling the Impact of Word-of-Mouth at the Individual Level, Working Paper. Stigler, George J. (1961), The Economics of Information, The Journal of Political Econom, 69(3), 213225. Sundaram, D.S., Kaushik Mitra and Cynthia Webster (1998), Word-of-Mouth Communications: A Motivational Analysis, Advances in Consumer Research, 25, 527-531. Tamir, Diana I. and Jason P. Mitchell (2012), Disclosing Information About the Self Is Intrinsically Rewarding, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(21),

Word-of-Mouth 59

8038-8043. Trope, Yaacov and Nira Liberman (2010), Construal-level Theory of Psychological Distance, Psychological Review, 117(2), 440-463. Trusov, Michael, Randolph E. Bucklin and Koen Pauwels (2009), Effects of Word-ofMouth Versus Traditional Marketing: Findings from an Internet Social Networking Site, Journal of Marketing, 73(5), 90 102. Tucker, Catherine (2011), Privacy Regulation and Online Advertising, Management Science, 57(1), 57-71. Tucker, Catherine (2012), Ad Virality and Ad Persuasiveness, MIT Working Paper. Tuk, Mirjam, Peeter Verlegh, Ale Smidts and Daniel Wigboldus (2009), Sales and Sincerity: The Role of Relational Framing in Word-of-Mouth Marketing. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 19 (January), 3847. Van den Bulte, Christophe and Stefan Wuyts (2009), Leveraging Customer Networks, in The Network Challenge: Strategy, Profit and Risk in an Interlinked World, eds. Jerry Yoram Wind and Paul Kleindorfer, Upper Saddle River, NJ: Wharton School Publishing, 243- 258. Vergara, A. (1993), Sex and gender identity: Differences in social knowledge on emotions and in ways of sharing them, Unpublished doctoral dissertation,Universidad del Pais Vasco, San Sebastian, Spain. Walker, W. Richard, John J. Skowronski, Jeffrey A. Gibbons, Rodney J. Vogl and Timothy D. Ritchie (2009), Why People Rehearse Their Memories: Frequency of Use and Relations to the Intensity of Emotions Associated with Autobiographical Memories, Memory, 17, 760773. Walther, Joseph B. (1996), Computer-mediated Communication: Impersonal, Interpersonal, and Hyperpersonal Interaction, Communication Research, 23, 3-43. Walther, Joseph B. (2007), Selective Self-presentation in Computer-mediated Communication: Hyperpersonal Dimensions of Technology, Language, and Cognition, Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 2538-2557. Walther, Joseph B. (2011), Theories of Computer-mediated Communication and Interpersonal Relations, in The Handbook Of Interpersonal Communication, eds. M. L. Knapp & J. A. Daly, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 443-479. Watts, Duncan J. (2004), The 'New' Science of Networks, Annual Review of Sociology, 30: 243-270. Watts, Duncan J. and Peter S. Dodds (2007), Influentials, Networks, and Public Opinion Formation, Journal of Consumer Research, 34(4), 441458. Wegner, Daniel M. and Toni Giuliano (1980), Arousal-induced Attention to Self, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 719726. Westbrook, Robert A. (1987), "Product/Consumption-Based Affective Responses andPostpurchase Processes," Journal of Marketing Research, 24 (August), 258-270. Wilson, Robert E., Samuel D. Gosling, and Lindsay T. Graham (2012), A review of Facebook research in the social sciences, Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(3), 203-220. Wojnicki, Andrea C. and David Godes (2011), Word-of-Mouth as Self-Enhancement, HBS Marketing Research Paper No. 06-01. Woodside, Arch G. and M. Wayne Delozier (1976), Effects of Word of Mouth Advertising on Consumer Risk Taking, Journal of Advertising, 5(4), 1219. Wyer, Robert S., Jr. and Thomas K. Srull (1981), Category Accessibility: Some Theoretical and Empirical Issues Concerning the Processing of Social Stimulus Information, in Social

Word-of-Mouth 60

Cognition: The Ontario Symposium, Vol. 1, eds. E. Tory Higgins, C. P. Herman, and Mark P. Zanna, Hillsdale, N J: Erlbaum, 161-197. Zhang, Yuchi and David Godes (2012), Learning How to Lean From Opinions, University of Maryland Working Paper. Zhu, Feng and Xiaoquan Zhang (2010), Impact of Online Consumer Reviews on Sales: The Moderating Role of Product and Consumer Characteristics, Journal of Marketing, 74, 133-148.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen