Sie sind auf Seite 1von 24

Faoor: IIcoin_ hc Lcn_h oI imc a 1ur_

lclihcrac: Ca:c Charaocri:io: and 1ur_


Compo:iion
THOMAS L. BRUNELL, CHETAN DAVE, NICHOLAS C. MORGAN
1

University of Texas at Dallas; University of Texas at Dallas; Collin College
We examine the time it takes to reach a verdict (deliberation time) using a unique dataset on the
deliberation times of actual juries in criminal and civil cases. Duration model results indicate that case
complexity, the unanimity of verdicts and the process of voir dire affect deliberation times, whereas jury
size, prior juror experience and the gender composition of juries are not significant correlates. The
results shed empirical light on an important correlate of trial accuracy using real-world data, in contrast
to previous research that employed mock jury data.
1. INTRODUCTION
The American jury system is firmly rooted in the Bill of Rights in the U.S.
Constitution. The sixth amendment states that those accused of a crime by the
state are to have a public and speedy trial heard by a local jury.
2
When viewed
through an economic lens, the objective behind such a jury-based judicial system
is to resolve a dispute by a group of peers engaging in collective decision-making.
If juries actually achieve this objective, there are two dependent variables of
interest. The first is the actual verdict agreed upon by a jury, that is, the outcome of
the collective decision-making process. The second variable is the process by
which a jury reaches a verdict, the process of collective decision-making itself. One
specific aspect of the deliberation process is the time that it takes for a jury to
reach a decision this is the focus of our analysis in this paper. Specifically, we

1
We would like to thank Patrick Brandt, Valerie Hans, Magnus Lofstrom, and the anonymous
reviewers for this journal for helpful comments.
2
Specifically, the sixth amendment stipulates that In all criminal prosecutions, the accused
shall enjoy the right to a speedy and public trial, by an impartial jury of the State and district
wherein the crime shall have been committed.

are interested in the length of time taken by real-world juries to render a verdict;
and the determinants of the time to verdict as a function of the characteristics
of the case at hand and the socio-demographic composition of the jury. In
contrast to previous research that has focused on mock juries, we examine a rich
dataset on 32 jury panels with over 6,000 jurors who served on over 1,000 trials.
The dataset contains a number of case-specific and juror-specific variables in
addition to the time it takes a jury to render a verdict.
Why are we interested in this aspect of the process of collective decision-
making in legal environments?
3
Our answer is that shining the empirical
spotlight on deliberation times can affect judicial economy considerations,
provide further insight into the more social sciences aspects of the law, and
help further our understanding of the jury system beyond that which we find
with mock juries. The judicial branch of government has an interest in
efficiently marshaling the scarce resources allocated to it by the legislative and
executive branches. Once a case has been accepted to be heard by the court,
the judicial system has an interest in disposing of the cases in a timely manner
since each judge typically oversees a limited number of trials at any given time.
Additionally, the court is concerned about the system costs imposed on trial
participants, in particular the jurors. For instance, the foregone earnings of
jurors may be considerable, e.g. low-income jurors who may not get
compensated from their employer for their service may suffer a cost higher
than high income jurors who do get compensated.
Finally and crucially, an important motivation for studying deliberation times
is the quality of verdicts themselves (trial accuracy). While deliberation time
may be costly, it also has benefits in terms of improving the quality of the
verdicts that a jury renders. A jury that is biased against (or in favor of) the
defendant is likely to make less effort to process information produced at trial
and jump more quickly to judgment. Hence, deliberation time may be a useful
signal of jury bias. Viewed in this light, several of the results of our analyses
have interesting welfare implications. For example, our result that deliberation
time is longer if the number of jurors dismissed during jury selection is greater
suggests jury selection might be effective at reducing jury bias. Indeed, during
the jury selection stage, lawyers have the opportunity to select juries to
minimize the biases against their clients.
4
In summary, we are interested in

3
See Mialon and Rubin (2008), Mialon and Mialon (2008), and Mialon (2005) for examples of
the analysis of judicial environments from an economic/econometric perspective.
4
For a theoretical examination of the link between jury selection and jury bias, see Neilson and
Winter (2000).
556 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

informing such judicial economy by analyzing the determinants of how long it
takes a jury to reach a verdict.
5

From an empirical legal point of view, much has been written on the subject
of individual juror perception of evidence as presented during trial, juror
comprehension of the evidence, and the individual processing of information.
6

The process of deliberation has also undergone scrutiny with several models of
decision-making presented.
7
Much of this research has been conducted using
mock juries. Since mock juries typically have built-in time constraints, they
cannot be used as a measurement of actual jury time to a decision point. Devine
et al. (2007) discuss another important aspect of deliberations, the notion of
deliberation quality and its concomitant effect on verdicts. Our analysis
complements these previous empirical legal findings. Further, Devine et al. (2007)
provide compelling arguments and evidence in favor of analyzing actual versus
mock juries. In the present analysis, data from actual juries are examined,
mitigating the causal inference issues present in analyzing mock juries. Our main
results suggest that 6-person juries are no quicker than 12-person juries; as cases
become more complex and/or more severe, juries deliberate longer; non-
unanimous decisions take longer to reach than unanimous ones and panels that
saw many potential jurors excused during voir dire end up deliberating longer
than panels with fewer challenges.
The remainder of this paper is structured as follows. Section 2 provides a
literature review that motivates in detail the various process questions that can
be asked of the data. In Section 3 the data are discussed followed by a
discussion of the empirical hypotheses. In Section 4 the empirical results are
presented and discussed, followed in Section 5 with concluding comments.
2. MOTIVATING LITERATURE
Given the difficulty of gathering data on or observing real juries, legal scholars
often rely on mock juries to better understand jury behavior and the process by
which they reach (or fail to reach) a collective decision. Clearly, using actual
juries provides the most appropriate source of research information because
jury simulations cannot fully enact important variables or environmental
features found in actual trials decided by real juries (Diamond and Rose, 2005).
More specifically with respect to the variable of interest in this paper,

5
Critics of the jury system contend that trial by jury is a costly relic, consuming vast human
resources and government expenditures, see Kassin and Wrightsman (1988) and Hans and
Vidmar (2001).
6
See for example, Ellsworth (1989) and Strodtbeck et al. (1957).
7
See Winship (2000).
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 557
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

measuring deliberation time in experimental settings has proven to be
problematic, typically due to time limitations. Ellsworth (1989) conducted 18
mock juries using the same information stimulus and none of the 18 were able
to come to agreement within the one-hour limit.
Saks and Marti (1997) reviewed eleven studies that report length of deliberations;
however, only two reported significant test statistics. They report a mean
difference in deliberation time to be 20 minutes for 12-person juries compared to
6-person juries. For the three studies examining actual juries, a mean difference
of 44 minutes is noted. They conclude that the small time savings that come
from reducing the size of juries would provide slight justification for any losses in
representation and quality of deliberation. Beiser and Varrin (1975) found that
larger juries, in addition to awarding larger damages on average relative to smaller
juries, were also more likely to deliberate for longer stretches of time. Not all
studies found that smaller juries deliberate for shorter periods of time Kessler
(1973) showed that smaller juries had greater participation rates among jurors and
tended to deliberate for longer periods of time. Nemeth (1977) found that the
unanimity requirement, not surprisingly, leads to more hung juries and longer
periods of deliberation. Saks (1977) found that neither jury size nor decision rule
affected the length of time that a jury spends on deliberations.
Katzev and Wishart (1985) examined the impact of two important factors on
length of deliberations. First, they found that when a judge summarizes the
evidence for the jury, this significantly reduces the time to reach a conclusion.
Moreover, when the judge included remarks about the fallibility of eyewitness
testimony in addition to the evidence summary, juries took significantly less
time still to deliberate. So, to the extent that jury instructions can add clarity to
how juries ought to weigh some evidence, deliberation time can be lessened.
Maass et al. (1985) demonstrate that expert testimony in a trial tends to lead to
longer deliberations than without; this is especially true when the expert did
not provide a causal explanation.
Kerr (1982) finds that systematic polling of the jury tended to decrease the
length of time that the jury took to come to a conclusion, although Davis et al.
(1993) find that when juries are mandated to take a poll, they tend to hang more
often and take longer to deliberate.
Unanimous jury verdicts were accepted as a basic function of the jury system
for over 600 years (Abramson, 1994). In 1972, the Supreme Court decided in
Apodaca v. Oregon
8
and Johnson v. Louisiana
9
that unanimous verdicts were no
longer required in non-capital state cases. As a result, states have been freed to

8
Apodaca v. Oregon, 406 U.S. 404 (1972).
9
Johnson v. Louisiana, 406 U.S. 356 (1972).
558 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

permit non-unanimous verdicts. In criminal felony trials, the jury verdict must
be unanimous at the federal level and in all states except for Louisiana and
Oregon, while in civil trials the federal requirement is the same but only 18
states require unanimous outcomes.
10

Given this brief review of the legal literature, and prior to discussing the data,
we note that the best research design would have both a 6- and 12-member jury
decide the same case simultaneously. We have to settle for the second best of
comparing juries of different sizes deciding somewhat similar cases. As we
discuss below though, the severity of the crime is related to the size of the jury,
with the most severe crimes (i.e., murder) all being heard by 12-person juries.
3. THE DATA
A recurring challenge in jury deliberation analysis is the difficulty in obtaining
data. Most of the data on juries comes from experimental situations which by
their very nature constrain the length of time deliberations may take place.
Other studies of actual juries tend to be limited in scope and scale. However,
there is a source of data that has not yet been systematically examined the
Multnomah County [Oregon] Jury Project (Grofman, 1979). The records were
obtained from the Fourth Circuit Court, Multnomah County (Portland)
Oregon during the period 1973-1976. The database contains 32 jury panels
with 199 variables covering 6,657 jurors and 1,159 trials.
11

The data include both individual-level juror and aggregate jury case
information. The information collected in the trial summaries include
identifiers for members of the particular jury, their votes, the final verdict, the
type of case, and the amount of time taken by the jury to arrive at a decision.
Each monthly juror panel recorded the jurors age, occupation and years in
residence in Oregon. Each juror also completed a self-administered
demographic data sheet which collected the jurors educational background,
spouse and children summary and any past jury or trial experience.
While jury duty today in Oregon is restricted to a single day (excepting of course
if one is picked to serve on a panel and the trial or deliberations last longer), in the
1970s jurors were called for an entire month. As Grofman and Feld (1976) note:
During that month of service, members of the jury pool may sit on as
many as ten different cases. Some 190-220 jurors serve each month,
with a case load of 40-60 trials in Circuit Court (primarily twelve-

10
See Diamond et al. (2006).
11
These data are available from the ICPSR (Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social
Research, Ann Arbor, MI).
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 559
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

member juries). No special effort is made in Multnomah County to
prevent jurors from serving together more than once during their month
of service and repeated dyads, triads, and even tetrads and occasional
higher order groupings do occur. In drawing up jury panels in
Multnomah County, court officials do, however, attempt to equalize
actual jury duty by giving priority on panel placement to those members
of the jury who were removed on challenges or who were participants in
cases dismissed or resolved out of court after the jury has been selected
but before it has had a chance to meet.
The selection of Multnomah County was fortuitous beyond the fact that
jurors did serve on multiple panels over the course of the month, because there
were other interesting features of jury service and state law in Portland,
Oregon, at the time that allow us to test other hypotheses. For instance, the
county utilized both 6- and 12-person juries and Oregon law allows for non-
unanimous jury decisions (with the exception of capital murder cases). For this
paper the most important feature of the dataset is that, for virtually every trial,
the time that the jury began deliberating was noted, as well as the time that the
jury finished deliberating. Rarely has any extensive data been collected in actual
court proceedings which examines the difference in how long juries of
different composition take to deliberate.
There are 203 6-person jury trials and 931 12-person trials. Half of the 6-person
cases are criminal trials and half are civil trials. Most of the criminal trials heard
by these smaller juries are for lesser crimes like drunk driving. Sixty percent of
12-person trials in the dataset are civil trials and forty percent are criminal. These
criminal trials do, however, include the more serious crimes like murder and
rape. Slightly more than half the jurors are female and the average age is 47.
Among criminal trials, the juries in this sample found nearly three-quarters of the
defendants guilty, while on the civil side just over half of the cases ended with a
finding of liability. The wide range of types of cases leads us to classify cases
rather broadly (criminal v. civil, crimes against persons v. property crimes, etc.).
The average jury in our data deliberates for 114 minutes prior to reaching a
decision. The quickest jury managed to come to a conclusion after just 11
minutes and the longest deliberation in our data was 470 minutes.
The primary data of interest for this analysis is the time taken by the i
th
jury
for deliberation, denoted as i in this paper. The time element was coded with
a start hour/minute and stop hour/minute. As the panel method of jury
procurement was used in Oregon at the time of the research, individual jurors
could sit on multiple juries. Therefore, analysis of an individuals juror time of
deliberation over a series of trials may be studied.
560 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

3.1. COVARIATES OF JURY DELIBERATION TIMES
The jury decision-making process is important in dispute resolution and a
proxy for the process of collective decision-making. Furthermore, deliberation
times may well be indicative of trial accuracy. We analyze this process with
specific covariates in mind. As mentioned above, the results from existing
research with respect to how jury size affects deliberation is mixed. Since,
ceteris paribus, getting more people to agree, ought to be more difficult than
getting fewer people to agree, one may simply expect that a 6-person jury will
reach a decision more quickly than a 12-person jury. However, even with
respect to this hypothesis, there is a counteracting force. Specifically, free-
riding is also likely to be a greater problem in a group of 12 than in a group of
6 people. In larger juries, information processing and decision-making tasks are
more likely to be delegated to a few leaders, which suggests that deliberation
time should be shorter in 12-person juries.
12
We therefore include jury size as a
determinant of deliberation times. It is informative to know the determinants
of what leads to a jury being a 6-person or 12-person affair. In Table 1 below
we tabulate the types of cases tried by each jury size. The table indicates that 6-
person juries tend to be employed in less severe crimes, although we were
unable to find an explicit legal source for this distinction.

Table 1. Case Types by Jury Size
6-Person Jury 12-Person Jury
Arson 1 3
Assault 13 56
Breach of contract 15 30
Burglary 10 53
Drugs 13 64
Drunk driving 23 23
Employment 0 1
Ex-con 1 9
Fraud 6 19
Kidnapping 0 6
Malpractice 1 19
Murder 0 20
Paternity 5 5
Personal injury 26 333
Property damage 16 47
Prostitution / sodomy 4 14
Public disturbance 2 12

12
For a theoretical examination of the link between jury size and trial accuracy, see
Mukhopadhaya (2003).
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 561
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

Rape 0 16
Robbery 0 53
Theft 14 28
Traffic 35 35
Wrongful death 0 4

Next, as alluded to above, some trials are relatively straightforward and others
will be more complex, and since complexity will require more of the jury we
expect juries to take longer to reach a verdict when considering complex cases.
We therefore include a measure of complexity by simply using the number of
criminal counts considered in a criminal trial and whether a jury is considering a
monetary award in a civil trial. Since the jury must reach a verdict for each
criminal count brought by the state, the length of time that it takes for a jury to
do this ought to be, ceteris paribus, longer with multiple counts relative to those
trials with a single count.
In criminal cases we expect there to be a relationship between the severity of
the crime and how long juries take to reach a decision. Since more severe
crimes involve longer sentences in jail and the trials are usually more complex
and lengthy, one may expect that the more severe the charge against the
defendant, the longer the jury will deliberate. For the purposes of this paper,
we employ a simple dichotomy where crimes against persons (i.e. murder, rape,
and assault) are more severe; crimes against property are less severe. In fact,
trial accuracy considerations would require more effort to be put forth
resulting in longer deliberation times.
Next, Oregon allows for juries to come to a conclusion with a less than
unanimous jury 10/2 or 11/1 decisions suffice under Oregon state law. At
first blush, one might hypothesize that unanimous decisions ought to take
longer, since a 10/2 split is easier to reach than a 12/0 decision. But a
unanimous decision may be one in which the jury starts out unanimous, so
these panels may be expected to finish relatively quickly. A split jury, on the
other hand, will probably never be unanimous and they will have to work to
get to 10/2 to reach a decision. While we have no extant literature to refer to
with respect to this hypothesis, it makes good sense under the decision rules
used in Oregon at the time to expect that unanimous decisions will generally
come from cases in which the decision was easier to reach.
Citizens with prior experience on a jury may bring certain deliberation skills to
the table that novice panelists would not know about. Therefore, panels with
members who have prior jury experience may tend to reach decisions more
quickly than those comprised of less experienced jurors. Moreover, past
research demonstrates that jury deliberations are typically dominated by just a
562 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

few jurors who end up having more influence on the jurys verdict (James, 1959;
Strodtbeck et al., 1957). We therefore include a measure of experience as a
potential correlate of deliberation times.
Since the burden of proof is on the prosecution in a criminal trial and on the
plaintiff in a civil dispute, juries that find in favor of the state (in criminal trials)
or the plaintiff (in civil trials) may take longer on average. As a result, we
suspect that cases that end up with a criminal conviction or a finding in favor
of the plaintiff ought to take longer, ceteris paribus, than those that end in an
acquittal or a civil finding for the respondent.
At the start of a jury trial, potential jurors are brought in and questioned by
attorneys from both sides and the presiding judge. Often times some of the
panelists are excused from that particular trial for a variety of reasons. Sometimes
the person is excused for cause and other times one side or the other uses a
preemptory challenge to remove a person from the jury. The strategic removal of
potential jurors from the jury is, in fact, big business for certain trials. Since both
sides typically remove panelists who have strong opinions or show leadership
abilities, if enough citizens are excused the panel is likely to be made up of people
that are less inclined to be strongly in favor of one side or the other. Moreover,
the jury may lack leadership, so, as the number of potential jurors who have been
excused from a panel during voir dire increases, the jury may take longer to reach a
verdict. On the other hand, this procedure may also result in the construction of
a more homogenous panel, we examine this possibility by including a variable
that counts how many potential jurors have been excused.
4. EMPIRICAL METHODS AND RESULTS
As in the previous section, letting i denote the time it takes in minutes for the
i
th
jury to reach a decision, the statistical analyses focus on (i) the shape and
characteristics of the attendant hazard function, (ii) estimation of the effect of
covariates outlined in the previous section on the hazard function and (iii) the
inclusion of unobserved heterogeneity in the model. The hazard function is the
main object of analysis in duration data such as that employed in this paper.
The idea is to characterize the probability, given that a jury has not rendered a
verdict at a certain point in time, that it will render a verdict in the next short
interval of time. The hazard function captures the likelihood of this event as it
represents the rate at which a jury may render a verdict, which can of course be
conditioned on a number of covariates.
However, prior to implementing the methods, it is important to determine
whether the data on our dependent variable has any outliers. An objective
method with which outlier determination can be made is via that of Hadi (1994),
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 563
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

use of this method indicated the same set of outliers as indicated by simply
evaluating the histograms of the times to verdict across juries.
The next step in modeling the dependent variable was computation of the
Kaplan-Meir hazard functions by the groups identified by dichotomous dummy
variables. These hazard functions are determined non-parametrically in that no
underlying distribution for the hazard is specified. This allows the researcher to
examine raw properties of the duration data by groups that are of interest (e.g.
criminal versus civil trials). Indeed these non-parametrically fitted hazard
functions indicate that the dependent variable was best treated as one exhibiting
positive duration dependence. That is, the longer the jury deliberates the higher
the probability that it will reach a verdict. Given that this assumption was
validated across a number of groups, the next step was to determine the effect of
the covariates on the hazard function. For this purpose, a parametric assumption
on the hazard function was made. First, given validation of the positive duration
assumption via the Kaplan-Meir hazard functions, the underlying distribution of
the times to verdict was assumed to be Weibull; this distribution assumes
positive duration dependence. Second, for the Weibull parametric model, it was
assumed that unobserved heterogeneity (or frailty) was distributed as a Gamma
distribution.
13
Finally, given that a number of judges presided over the various
trials, the estimation procedure was conditioned on clustering by judges, leading
to standard errors for estimated parameters to be robust.
Given the estimation results, in order to ensure that an appropriate parametric
model for the time to verdicts was assumed, Cox-Snell residuals were computed
and plotted. Plots close to the 45-degree line are considered to be indicative of
good model fit; these plots are also discussed below. With this description of the
statistical methodology in hand, we turn to a description of the estimation results.
4.1. EMPIRICAL RESULTS
As a first cut at the data, we can simply examine the smoothed Kaplan-Meir
hazard estimates for our variables of interest. These graphs depict how likely a
jury is to come to a decision over a period of time. The x-axis is always the length
of time it took a jury to come to a decision (in minutes). The y-axis is the hazard
rate, the likelihood that the jury will reach a decision at that particular point in
time. So the higher the line is on the y-axis the more likely the jury is to come to
a conclusion. Thus, when comparing two lines on these graphs, the series with
the higher value is the one that, on average, ends more quickly.

13
Alternate specifications for both modeling positive duration dependence and heterogeneity
were employed as a robustness check, the results did not change the data inference from those
reported below.
564 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

First, we want to investigate whether civil and criminal cases differ
significantly in the length of time it takes for a jury to reach a decision. We do
not have any theoretical expectations regarding potential differences in
deliberation time, but since some of our independent variables differ between
these two kinds of cases, there are already reasons to model them separately.
Looking at Figure 1 it is clear that early on, during the first 100 minutes, there
is no significant difference between the two types of trials, although civil cases
ended slightly less quickly than criminal ones. After 100 minutes the two lines
cross, and then at around 300 minutes a large gap between the two develops.
This indicates that after a jury has deliberated for around 5 hours the
conditional probability of a civil trial jury finishing at that point is much greater
than the conditional probability of a criminal trial jury to reach a verdict. Due
to these differences and the fact that we do have different variables for
criminal and civil trials, we model these two kinds of trials separately.
Next, we take up the issue of jury size and how this impacts deliberation time.
We hypothesized that the smaller the jury the more rapidly they would come to
a decision. Getting 5 of 6 people to agree on something is, ceteris paribus, easier
to do than getting 10 of 12 to agree. The data support this hypothesis. The
average length of time in a 6-person jury case is only 81.5 minutes (standard
error is 4.46), while a 12-person jury takes, on average, just over two hours to
reach a conclusion (121.5 minutes, with a standard error of 3.01). A t-test
confirms that this difference is statistically significant at all conventional levels.
Moreover, looking at Figure 2, it is plain to see that 6-person juries are a) much
more likely to end quickly, and b) do not even have any observations past 400
minutes of deliberation time.
Next we take up the issue of case complexity. We theorize that as the
complexity of a case increases, the jury will tend to take longer to make a
decision since there is more to consider in the jury room. For our purposes, we
measure the complexity of a case in a criminal trial as the number of counts
brought against the defendant. For civil trials we use whether or not the jury
decided to award damages. Figure 3 demonstrates that indeed when a jury has
to consider multiple charges rather than just a single charge, the jury tends to
take longer to deliberate. The single count juries are far more at risk of ending
relative to multiple count juries.

Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 565
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

Figure 1: Hazard Rates for the Length of Jury Deliberation
in Criminal and Civil Trials

.
0
0
5
.
0
1
.
0
1
5
.
0
2
.
0
2
5
0 100 200 300 400 500
Time to Decision
Civil Criminal
Smoothed Hazard Estimates



Figure 2: Hazard Rates for the Length of Jury Deliberation
for 6- and 12-Person Juries

0
.
0
1
.
0
2
.
0
3
0 100 200 300 400 500
Time to Decision
Six Person Jury Twelve Person Jury
Smoothed Hazard Estimates

566 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

Figure 3: Hazard Rates for the Length of Jury Deliberation
for 1 and Multiple Count Cases
0
.
0
0
5
.
0
1
.
0
1
5
.
0
2
0 100 200 300 400 500
Time to Decision
One Count Multiple Counts
Smoothed Hazard Estimates


Figure 4: Hazard Rates for the Length of Jury Deliberation
for Small versus Big Damages Cases

0
.
0
1
.
0
2
.
0
3
0 100 200 300 400
Time to Decision
Small Damages Big Damages
Smoothed Hazard Estimates
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 567
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

In Figure 4, we similarly see that juries for civil trials that award damages are
less at risk of ending relative to juries that end up not awarding damages. The
lines do cross at around the 300-minute mark, this means that if a jury which
ends up awarding no damages deliberates for more than 5 hours, they tend to
take more time to deliberate than juries that do end up awarding damages.
In criminal trials, juries that are considering charges having to do with crimes
against a person (murder, rape, assault) will end up deliberating longer on
average than a jury considering other, less severe, criminal charges (robbery,
fraud, illicit card games, etc.). A comparison of means test shows that crimes
against persons do result in long jury deliberation times relative to crimes
against property. The average time for a jury considering more severe charges
is 136.3 minutes (7.7 standard error) and the average length of time for less
severe charges is 109.9 minutes (2.74 standard error). This difference is
significant at the .001 level. Figure 5 also shows that crimes against persons
generate significantly lower hazard rates than crimes against property,
indicating they are less at risk of ending more quickly.


Figure 5: Hazard Rates for the Length of Jury Deliberation
in Crimes against Persons and Property

0
.
0
0
5
.
0
1
.
0
1
5
.
0
2
0 100 200 300 400 500
Time to Decision
Other Person
Smoothed Hazard Estimates


568 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

Figure 6: Hazard Rates for the Length of Jury Deliberation
for Unanimous and Non-Unanimous Decisions

0
.
0
1
.
0
2
.
0
3
0 100 200 300 400 500
Time to Decision
Non-unanimous Unanimous
Smoothed Hazard Estimates

Next we turn to the issue of unanimous versus non-unanimous jury verdicts. As
discussed above, Oregon has a 10 of 12 rule where, with the exception of a
capital crime, a defendant may be convicted or exonerated with just 10 of the 12
jurors in agreement. Since there is no reason for juries to achieve unanimity, this
ought to result in relatively quick deliberations for unanimous juries and longer
ones for non-unanimous juries. The logic here is that if there is a vote among the
jurors and it is split with less than 10 on one side or the other, the jury will
continue to deliberate and, most juries will eventually end up with a decision and
with the most likely split being 10-2. Whereas a jury in Oregon that is split 10-2 or
11-1 does not need to continue to work to achieve unanimity. The average
unanimous decision takes 90.6 minutes (standard error is 3.6) and the average
non-unanimous jury takes 132.5 minutes (standard error is 3.6). The difference
between these two means is significant at all standard levels of significance. Figure
6 shows that there is a more interesting story to tell, inasmuch as early on in the
deliberative process unanimous juries are more likely to reach a verdict quickly,
but at just past 200 minutes the two lines cross. So after a jury has been
deliberating for some time, the hazard ratio for a jury that is non-unanimous is
higher than that for a unanimous one. This difference could be an artifact of the
unanimity requirement of capital crimes cases in Oregon. The multivariate
models, presented below, will help us understand this dynamic better.
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 569
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

Table 2. Factors Affecting Jury Deliberation Time in Criminal Trials
Variable Hazard Ratio |p|-value
Size of Jury 0.9438 0.691
(0 = 6, 1 = 12)
Unanimous Decision 2.2590 0.000
(0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Guilty Verdict 1.2240 0.025
(0 = Innocent, 1 = Guilty)
Crime Against Person 0.6648 0.046
(0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Number of Counts 0.6445 0.000
(Number of counts considered by jury)
Prior Jury Service 2.8440 0.089
(Proportion of jurors with jury experience)
Number Excused 0.9050 0.000
(Number of jurors dismissed during voir dire)
Proportion Male 0.95790 0.750
(Proportion of male jurors)
Std. Err.
P 1.65 0.199
0.2958 0.182
Obs. 473
Clusters (Judges) 19
logL 567.45

Having covered our variables of interest in a bivariate and non-parametric
way, we now turn to multivariate models to better understand how each of the
variables affects length of jury deliberation while controlling for other
important factors. Table 2 presents the results of our parametric hazard models
for criminal trials.
14
Interpretation of hazard ratios, like those in the table, are
straightforward when a coefficient is less than 1 this factor is correlated with
longer deliberations, while coefficients greater than 1 are related to shorter
deliberations. The coefficient for jury size (6 versus 12 members) is less than 1,
but not different from zero statistically. The next variable indicates that for
criminal trials at least unanimous juries come back more quickly than do non-
unanimous juries. Guilty verdicts take less time than verdicts that declare the
defendant not guilty. This is interesting and could be indicative of cases that are
so clearly presented and evidenced so as to leave little doubt in the minds of

14
We also included a measure of the level of education of jurors. Not all jurors completed this
part of the questionnaire and so the sample size dropped significantly. However, education was
not significant at conventional levels in any of the estimated models and we therefore present
results from regressions that do not include this covariate.
570 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

jurors about the innocence or guilt of the defendant, thereby speeding up the
decision-making process.
15
Those cases that involve crimes against persons
systematically take longer to reach verdicts relative to other, less severe, crimes.
As in the graph described earlier, as a jury considers multiple counts against a
defendant, the group takes longer to reach a decision. The use of preemptory
challenges signals, we theorize, a more contentious trial. So we expect the
coefficient to be less than 1 and it is in our model. So, as the number of
excused jurors increases, so does the length of time the final jury deliberates.
The causal mechanism for this relationship is not direct. We believe that the
high number of challenges is merely an indication of a tough and divisive trial
ahead, with well-prepared attorneys on both sides. These kinds of trials are
likely to lead to more time in the jury room.
Next we examine the effect of prior jury service on the ability of a jury to come
to a relatively quick conclusion. The hazard ratio for the prior jury service
variable is 2.84, indicating that as the number of jurors with prior jury service
experience increases, the jury reaches a decision more quickly. The p-value is .089
in a two-tailed test, just outside the bounds of traditional levels of significance.
Finally, we test the effects of gender dominance on jury deliberation time. We
measured the variable in terms of the proportion of the jury membership that
was male, and the variable is not significant and the coefficient is just slightly less
than 1. We can conclude that gender composition of the jury does not have a
significant impact on the length of jury deliberations.
As mentioned above, civil and criminal trials are modeled separately, so now we
turn to the inferential model of deliberation time for civil trials. The results of
these analyses are presented in Table 3. In this model, the hazard rate for the size
of the jury is less than 1, but not statistically significant. Unanimous decisions
take significantly less time to reach than non-unanimous decisions, as we
hypothesized. The previous two results more or less matched the results in the
criminal model. The guilty variable however is different in civil trials insofar as
the verdict finds the defendant either liable or not liable and a person's freedom
is not at stake, but this is more a resolution of a dispute between two parties. The
guilty variable hazard was greater than 1 in criminal trials, which was unexpected.
In civil trials however, juries that find against the defendant do take longer than
those that find for the defendant. Prior jury service had the effect of speeding up
deliberation for criminal trials, but that is not the case in civil trials. There is no
discernible difference in time to a decision with experienced jurors on the panel.

15
Ideally we would like to have a variable that is indicative of case quality; however, the dataset
does not allow the construction of such a variable. We expect therefore that allowing for
unobserved heterogeneity across cases controls for this effect.
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 571
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

Juries with more male jurors are indistinguishable from juries with fewer male
jurors. Finally, the case complexity variable, damages awarded for civil trials,
works as expected. The higher the damages awarded, the longer the jury took, on
average to come to a conclusion. In terms of model fit for both models, we note
the following. First our assumption of the underlying Weibull distribution
describing the duration data is appropriate given that for both models the
estimated measure of curvature of the distribution (p) is positive and significant.
Second, for both models, the estimate of is significant and different from zero,
indicating the presence of unobserved heterogeneity.
16
Third, Figures 7 and 8
below show the Cox-Snell residuals from the two multivariate models for
criminal and civil cases respectively.
When the residual plot is near the 45-degree line, this indicates a good fit for the
model. Finally, the chi-squared statistics for both models indicate that the variables
included are jointly significant at 5%. This is suggestive of a good model fit.

Table 3. Factors Affecting Jury Deliberation Time in Civil Trials
Variable Hazard Ratio |p|-value
Size of Jury 0.7222 0.117
(0 = 6, 1 = 12)
Unanimous Decision 1.6760 0.001
(0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Defendant Found Liable 0.5667 0.000
(0 = Not liable, 1 = Liable)
Damages 0.9957 0.000
(Continuous variable in thousands of dollars)
Prior Jury Service 0.6340 0.358
(Proportion of jurors with jury experience)
Number Excused 0.9178 0.002
(Number of jurors dismissed during voir dire)
Proportion Male 0.9382 0.596
(Proportion of male jurors)
Std. Err.
P 1.93 0.107
0.3657 0.077
Obs. 657
Clusters (Judges) 22
logL 706.88


16
This parameter characterizes unobserved heterogeneity in the parametric forms of the hazard
functions.
572 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

Figure 7: Cox-Snell Residuals for Criminal Trials


Figure 8: Cox-Snell Residuals for Civil Trials


0
2
4
6
8
0 2 4 6 8
Cox-Snell residual
H Cox-Snell residual
Cox-Snell Residuals-Criminal

0
2
4
6
8
0 2 4 6
Cox-Snell residual
H2 Cox-Snell residual
Cox-Snell Residuals-Civil
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 573
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

4.2. ROBUSTNESS
Given the results reported regarding jury size in Tables 2 and 3, it is instructive to
discuss estimation results by breaking down our models using both the
civil/criminal dichotomy and a 6-person jury/12-person jury dichotomy. Table 4
below presents the estimation results for criminal trials for both 6- and 12-person
juries. Relative to Table 2, it is clear that regardless of jury size unanimous verdicts
were reached faster (as were guilty verdicts), but only for the larger-sized juries,
since the estimated hazard ratio for guilty verdicts in 6-person juries has a large
standard error. Overall, the pattern of results is similar to those reported in Table 2,
albeit some of the statistical significance of the estimates is weaker in the 6-person
jury data. This may well be due to the possibility, as indicated in Table 1 above,
that 6-person juries were employed in less severe crimes, but it may also be the
case that the smaller sample size for 6-person juries is a contributing factor.


Table 4. Criminal Trials by Jury Size
6-person Juries 12-person Juries
Variable
Hazard
Ratio
|p|-value
Hazard
Ratio
|p|-value
Unanimous Decision 2.66 0.063 2.176 0.000
(0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Guilty Verdict 0.674 0.18 1.364 0.000
(0 = Innocent, 1 = Guilty)
Crime Against Person 1.325 0.282 0.656 .011
(0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Number of Counts 0.546 0.059 0.681 0.000
(Number of counts considered by jury)
Prior Jury Service 2.129 0.236 3.033 .037
(Proportion of jurors with jury experience)
Number Excused 0.906 0.168 0.907 0.000
(# of jurors dismissed during voir dire)
Proportion Male 1.57 0.197 0.863 0.112
(Proportion of male jurors)
Std. Err. Std. Err.
P 2.03 0.75 1.583 0.179
0.7188 0.181 0.185 0.149
Obs. 102 371
Clusters (Judges) 14 18
logL -120.15 -441.81


574 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

Table 5. Civil Trials by Jury Size
6-person Juries 12-person Juries
Variable
Hazard
Ratio
|p|-value
Hazard
Ratio
|p|-value
Unanimous Decision 2.21 0.076 1.652 0.000
(0 = No, 1 = Yes)
Defendant Found Liable 0.635 0.10 0.569 0.000
(0 = Not liable, 1 = Liable)
Damages 0.976 0.001 0.996 0.000
(Continuous variable in thousands of dollars)
Prior Jury Service 0.223 .037 0.816 0.356
(Proportion of jurors with jury experience)
Number Excused 0.925 .186 0.918 0.002
(Number of jurors dismissed during voir dire)
Proportion Male 1.034 0.43 0.915 0.25
(Proportion of male jurors)
Std. Err. Std. Err.
P 2.386 0.325 1.86 0.144
0.752 0.274 0.293 0.121
Obs. 101 371
Clusters (Judges) 15 22
logL -104.07 -599.91


Next, Table 5 below presents the results for civil trials broken out by jury size.
Again, the pattern of results is similar to those reported in Table 3 for the
entire sample. Like the breakdown for criminal trials, the significance of the
estimates when only using 6-person jury data is a bit weaker; however, those
variates that were of weak significance before continue to be indistinguishable
from zero (e.g., Proportion Male). In summary, Tables 4 and 5 suggest that the
insignificance of jury size on determining the speed with which case verdicts
are made continues, since regardless of jury size, in either civil or criminal trials,
our main results tend to hold.
5. CONCLUSION
After being presented with the evidence in court, the jury retires to the jury
room to begin deliberations. What happens inside this room is both important
and interesting, not only to the participants, but to many scholars as well. What
factors affect how a jury goes about its business? Does the size of a jury
matter? How about the composition? In this paper, we have taken a first look
at one aspect of this collective decision-making process the factors that affect
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 575
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

the length of time a jury takes to come to a decision. The results we present
can therefore be viewed as stylized facts that do not arise from an analysis of
mock jury data but data on actual juries.
While there appears to be a big difference in time between 6- and 12-person
juries when simply comparing how long it takes each to come to a verdict, in the
multivariate models the difference between the two was not significant. This
might be because none of the 6-person juries deliberated longer than 379
minutes, while 12-member juries maxed out in our dataset at nearly 500 minutes.
So, after controlling for the other factors, e.g. severity of the crime or case
complexity, these differences disappear. Unanimous decisions are much quicker
than non-unanimous decisions, although we attribute these differences to the
structural rules that govern jury decision-making in Oregon. Since Oregon allows
less-than-unanimous decisions, this means that unanimous juries are almost
certainly panels that start out unanimous from the outset of deliberations. A
strong indication that a jury may take longer than average is the number of
potential jurors who are excused during the jury selection process. We take this to
mean that the use of many preemptory challenges (or for-cause challenges) may
be a leading indicator of a contentious trial. It could also be the case that the
potential leaders in the jury pool are removed with these challenges and the jury is
left with few or no people that might lead them to a quick and (hopefully)
accurate decision. The one stark difference between criminal and civil juries is that
when a jury comes back with a conviction in a criminal trial, it is typically much
quicker than for juries that acquit. On the civil side, finding the defendant liable
takes longer than exonerating him or her. The analyses reported here have
brought further insight into the dynamics of jury deliberation. We should note
that there is a link between the length of time that juries deliberate and the
accuracy of the verdicts. Devine et al. (2001) and Hastie et al. (1983) find that the
longer a jury deliberates the more likely they are to reach an accurate decision. So
we are not arguing that the government ought to be purposefully looking for
methods to speed up jury deliberations, since this approach could affect the
accuracy of verdicts. Future research could include more case-specific data like the
number and types of witness, a measure of the quality of the evidence, and the
race and ethnicity of the defendant and the jurors themselves. Future research
would also, given our stylized facts, develop appropriate structural models of
hazards that is, develop tightly linked optimization hypotheses to explain a jurys
time to decision, accounting for the findings presented in this paper.

576 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

References
Abramson, J. 1994. We, the Jury: The Jury System and the Ideal of Democracy. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Beiser, E.N., and R. Varrin. 1975. Six-member Juries in the Federal Courts, 58
Judicature 524-533.
Davis, J.H., M.F. Stasson, C.D. Parks, L. Hulbert, T. Kameda, S.K. Zimmerman, and K.
Ono. 1993. Quantitative Decisions by Groups and Individuals: Voting
Procedures and Monetary Awards by Mock Civil Juries, 29 Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology 326-346.
Devine, D., L. Clayton, B. Dunford, R. Seying, and J. Price. 2001. Jury Decision Making:
45 Years of Empirical Research on Deliberating Groups, 7 Psychology, Public
Policy, and Law 622-727.
_______, J. Buddenbaum, S. Houp, D.P. Stolle, and N. Studebaker. 2007. Deliberation
Quality: A Preliminary Examination in Criminal Juries, 4 Journal of Empirical
Legal Studies 273-303.
Diamond, S.S., and M.R. Rose. 2005. Real Juries, 1 Annual Review of Law and Social
Science 255-284.
_______, _______, and B. Murphy. 2006. Revisiting the Unanimity Requirement: The
Behavior of the Non-unanimous Civil Jury, 100 Northwestern University Law
Review 201-230.
Ellsworth, P.C. 1989. Are Twelve Heads Better Than One? 52 Law and Contemporary
Problems 207-224.
Grofman, B. 1979. MULTNOMAH COUNTY [OREGON] JURY PROJECT, 1973-1976 [computer
file]. Irvine, CA: Public Policy Research Organization [producer]. Ann Arbor, MI:
Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research [distributor], 1984.
_______ and S. Feld. 1976. A Note on Clique Avoidance in Repeated Jury Selection
From Among a Fixed Pool of Jurors. Comparisons of Manpower Savings in
Six and Twelve-member Juries 26 Public Choice 145-150.
Hadi, A.S. 1994. A Modification of a Method for the Detection of Outliers in
Multivariate Samples, 56 Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B 293-396.
Hastie, R., S. Penrod, and N. Pennington. 1983. Inside the Jury. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Hans, V., and N. Vidmar. 2001. Judging the Jury. New York: Basic Books.
James, R. 1959. Status and Competence of Jurors, 64 American Journal of Sociology 563-570.
Kassin, S.M., and L.S. Wrightsman. 1988. The American Jury on Trial: Psychological
Perspectives. New York: Hemisphere Publishing.
Katzev, R.D., and S.S. Wishart. 1985. The Impact of Judicial Commentary Concerning
Eyewitness Identifications on Jury Decision Making, 76 The Journal of Criminal
Law and Criminology 733-745.
Kerr, N.L. 1982. Social Transition Schemes: Model, Method, and Applications, in H.
Brandstatter, J.H. Davis, and G. Stocker-Kreichgauer, eds. Group Decision
Making, pp. 5980. London: Academic Press.
Factors Affecting the Length of Time a Jury Deliberates / 577
http://www.bepress.com/rle/vol5/iss1/art23
DOI: 10.2202/1555-5879.1334

Kessler, J. 1973. An Empirical Study of Six- and Twelve-member Jury Decision-
making Processes, 6 University of Michigan Journal of Law Reform 712-734.
Maass, A., J.C. Brigham, and S.G. West 1985. Testifying on Eyewitness Reliability: Expert
Advice is Not Always Persuasive, 15 Journal of Applied Social Psychology 207-229.
Mialon, H.M. 2005. An Economic Theory of the Fifth Amendment, 36 RAND
Journal of Economics 833-848.
_______ and P. Rubin. 2008. The Economics of the Bill of Rights, 10 American Law
and Economics Review 1-60.
_______ and S. Mialon. 2008. The Effects of the Fourth Amendment: An Economic
Analysis, 24 Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization 22-44.
Mukhopadhaya, K. 2003. Jury Size and the Free Rider Problem, 19 Journal of Law,
Economics, and Organization 24-44.
Neilson, W., and H. Winter. 2000. Bias and the Economics of Jury Selection, 20
International Review of Law and Economics 223-250.
Nemeth, C. 1977. Interactions Between Jurors as a Function of Majority vs.
Unanimity Decision Rules, 7 Journal of Applied Social Psychology 38-56.
Saks, M. 1977. Jury Verdicts: The Role of Group Size and Decision Rule. Washington, DC:
Lexington Books.
_______ and M.W. Marti. 1997. A Meta-Analysis of the Effects of Jury Size, 25 Law
and Human Behavior 451-467.
Strodtbeck, F.L., R.M. James, and C. Hawkins. 1957. Social Status in Jury Deliberations,
22 American Sociological Review 713-719.
Winship, G. 2000. Jury Deliberation: An Observation Study, 33 Group Analysis 547-555.


578 / REVIEW OF LAW AND ECONOMICS 5:1, 2009
Review of Law & Economics, 2009 by bepress

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen