Sie sind auf Seite 1von 18

Psychological Review

1971, Vol. 78, No. S, 391-408

COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 1
MICHAEL I. POSNER 2 AND STEPHEN J. BOIES
University of Oregon

The study of human attention may be divided into three components. These
are alertness, selectivity, and processing capacity. This paper outlines ex-
perimental techniques designed to separate these components and examine
their interrelations within comparable tasks. It is shown that a stimulus
may be used to increase alertness for processing all external information, to
improve selection of particular stimuli, or to do both simultaneously. Devel-
opment of alertness and selectivity are separable, but they may go on to-
gether without interference. Moreover, encoding a stimulus may proceed
without producing interference with other signals. Thus, the contact be-
tween an external stimulus and its representation in memory does not appear
to require processing capacity. Limited capacity results are obtained when
mental operations such as response selection or rehearsal must be per-
formed on the encoded information.

The subject of attention is a broad one. and maintain an optimal sensitivity to ex-
There are many definitions of it and subcate- ternal stimulation is also studied when sub-
gories included under it. In a recent book, jects (Ss) receive a warning to prepare
Moray (1970) has proposed at least six dif- themselves to take in information. The fore-
ferent meanings of the term attention in cur- period of a reaction time task may be con-
rent psychological research. It would be sidered as a miniature vigilance situation
premature to propose any single taxonomy where alertness must be developed rapidly
or exhaustive set of categories for the term. and maintained over a relatively brief inter-
However, both in looking at the various defi- val. Recent studies have investigated the
nitions that Moray proposes and in review- time course (Bertelson, 1967) and the brain
ing the literature, there do appear to be three activity (Karlin, 1970; Naatanen, 1970;
major topics under which studies of atten- Posner & Wilkinson, 1969) during this in-
tion might be grouped. terval. It appears that there is some simi-
First is the notion of alertness. Maintain- larity between the brain processes which take
ing attention in the sense of alertness a is place during the foreperiod and those in-
presumably involved in human ability to volved in vigilance tasks (Wilkinson &
perform in long, boring tasks like those Haines, 1970). These studies provide some
that psychologists design to study vigilance rationale for including alertness as a general
(Mackworth, 1970). The ability to develop component of attention.
1
A second sense of attention is the ability
Portions of this paper were presented in an in- to select information from one source or of
vited address to the Western Psychological Associ- one kind rather than another (Egeth, 1967;
ation, Los Angeles, April 1970. The research in-
cluded here was sponsored by National Science Triesman, 1969). Studies of selective atten-
Foundation Grants GB 5960 and 21020 and by the tion may require that ^s report information
Advanced Research Projects Agency of the De- from a particular sensory modality, spatial
partment of Defense monitored by the Air Force location, or content (letters rather than dig-
Office of Scientific Research under Contract No.
F44620-67-C-0099 all to the University of Oregon.
its). Broadbent (1958) has argued that at
The authors are grateful to Barbara Lewis and least some aspects of selection are performed
Larry Moore for help in running the subjects. by filtering mechanisms which block out or
2
Requests for reprints should be sent to Michael attenuate input. Other theories have stressed
I, Posner, Department of Psychology, University additional operations which are performed
of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403. on information of particular importance
3 The term alertness is used here rather than
arousal to avoid the emotional connotation of many (Deutsch & Deutsch, 1963; Norman, 1968).
uses of the latter term. The degree of separation These views bear a relationship to physio-
of the terms is not clear at this point. logical models of attention that have stressed
391
392 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES

either a tuning process which blocks input for studying the separation and interrelation-
from unselected sources (Hernandez-Peon, ship of these components of attention at the
1966) or a general alerting reaction which behavioral level.
enhances input from whatever source (Naa- Consider first a task which involves all
tanen, 1970). The former view is more in three components mixed in a fairly complex
line with filtering mechanisms, while the lat- way. The 6"s see a single letter for a brief
ter fits better with the idea of additional period of time, followed after a variable in-
operations performed on highly processed in- terval by a second letter. The task is to re-
put. One way of viewing this issue is to ask spond as rapidly as possible "same" if the
whether 5s can prepare for signals in a way two letters have the same name; otherwise,
which is not selective. If they prepare for a "different" (Posner, 1969; Posner, Boies,
visual signal, is the capacity to process audi- Eichelman, & Taylor, 1969). The first let-
tory signals decreased or enhanced over an ter has two functions. It serves as a warning
unprepared state? Thus, the separability of signal and also tells the 5" what letter he is
alertness and selection as internal processes looking for. Thus, one might expect it to
has important consequences for a theory of increase alertness and to introduce some bias
attention. of the perceptual system toward tests related
A third sense of attention relates to the to the configuration of the first letter. The
idea of a limited central processing capacity bias might involve, for example, activation of
(Broadbent, 1958). It is well known that trace systems related to the physical con-
.S"s have difficulty in handling two tasks figuration of the letter (Posner, 1969).
simultaneously, even when they wish to do What happens during the interval between
so. Signals which arrive during the reaction the two letters depends greatly on how the
time (RT) period tend to be delayed (Wei- attention (processing capacity) of 5 is di-
ford, 1968). Yet it is not clear what aspects rected. For example, if he knows that all
of signal processing are subject to the single- matches will be physically identical, he main-
channel limitation. Some have argued that tains his speed on physical matches much
the single channel extends to almost all pro- better than if some matches must be based on
cessing (Welford, 1968), while other have the name (Posner, 1969). If the first letter
seen it as applying mainly to response ex- disappears, there is a rapid increase in times
ecution (Reynolds, 1964). A limited capac- for a physical match, while name matches re-
ity mechanism suggests that any two opera- main relatively constant, suggesting that 5"s
tions requiring it will interfere with each keep the letter name in mind. However, if
other. Thus, a study of the ability of 5"s to the physical form remains present during the
time share mental operations can lead to an interval, physical matching remains constant
understanding of what aspects of processing while name matches increase in efficiency
require access to the limited capacity system. until they resemble the physical matches.
Most recent articles related to attention in- This suggests that 5"s generate a visual code
volve one of these three components (see of the opposite case (Boies, 1969; Posner,
e.g., Koster, 1969; Sanders, 1967, 1970). 1969). These results indicate that 5s are
Unfortunately, the experiments used to study very flexible in the coding they use for let-
each of the components are different and ters and similar visual material (Cohen,
there has been little attempt to develop tasks 1969; Tver sky, 1969), but their choices are
which allow each of them to be separated and constrained by the rivalry between physical
interrelated. Moray (1970) suggests the and name codes for SV limited processing
importance of doing so as follows: capacity.
It might well be, for example, that the relation be- This brief description of the letter-match-
tween selective listening and arousal is such that ing sequence suggests that it may be a useful
arousal level acts as a parameter which will alter task for separating the three components of
the over-all efficiency of selection and rejection as attention described above. The experiments
it varies. But, no systematic investigation has so reviewed in this paper are an effort to do so.
far been carried out [p. 85].
Moreover, they attempt to connect the opera-
This paper is an attempt to develop means tions of this experimental paradigm to the
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 393

three theoretical components of attention not as pronounced, particularly when feed-


outlined above. Two major questions are back is used to keep motivation high.
considered. First, Can the warning function Figure 1 indicates functions obtained using
and the selective function of the first letter be two-letter matching tasks (Posner & Wilkin-
separated? If so, how do the two functions son, 1969). Both tasks involved a warning
relate? Second, What is the relationship signal followed after a foreperiod by a pair
between these two components of attention of letters. The foreperiod was constant for
and central processing capacity? a block of trials but varied between blocks.
The 5"s had a single key which they pressed
PREPARATION when the letter pair was defined as "same"
for that task. The solid line is for a task
Many investigators have studied the period which required 51 to press a key if the letters
of time between a warning signal and a sig- were physically identical, while the dotted
nal related to the required response (Bertel- line required him to press the key if both
son, 1967). Of primary interest are studies letters were either vowels or consonants.
where 51 has little uncertainty about when the Although RT differs greatly in the two tasks,
response-related signal will occur. This is the time course of preparation is very similar
achieved by using blocks of trials with a fixed and like that described in the studies cited
warning interval. Whether the response above.
task involves reaction time (Bertelson,
1967), signal detection (Egan, Greenberg, ENCODING
& Schulman, 1961), or other types of pro- If 5 is provided with part of the informa-
cessing (Leavitt, 1969), the results are simi- tion he needs to perform a speeded task, his
lar. Performance is worse with no warning RT when he receives the remainder will be
and improves as the warning interval in- reduced (Cohen, 1969; La Berge, Van
creases to some optimal value. This value Gelder, & Yellott, 1970; Leonard, 1958).
is most frequently about .2-.5 second. An If the time between the two signals is varied,
interval longer than optimal tends to produce it is possible to discover how long it takes to
a decrease in performance, but this is usually encode the first signal in a form which is op-

550 750

S2
450 650 >

a
CO i
O--OV-C MATCH
I PI MATCH
350 550

10 100 IOOO 6000


WARNING INTERVAL (MILLISEC)

FIG. 1. Preparation functions for physical (PI) match task (solid line,
left ordinate) and vowel-consonant (V-C) match task (dotted line, right
ordinate). (Data are from constant block foreperiods of each warning
interval collapsed across four days; after Posner & Wilkinson, 1969.)
394 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES

timal for processing the second. If the first Two separate experiments were run. In the first
experiment, S responded "same" if the letters were
signal is a letter that must be matched against physically identical; in the second, "same" was de-
a second letter, the improvement should rep- fined as both vowels or both consonants. Experi-
resent the encoding of the information from ment I used only capital letters, while Experiment
the first letter needed for the match. In II used mixed uppercase and lowercase letters.
order to reduce the involvement of general The computer chose the letters at random from an
alphabet of 15 letters with the restriction that half
preparation in such functions, a warning sig- of the trials in each block be "same." In these ex-
nal was given .5 second before the first letter. periments, 5s had two keys and were instructed to
It was supposed that preparation could be press the left one if the letters were defined as
maintained during the time between presen- "same" and the right one if "different."
tation of the first letter and the presentation In Experiment I, each of eight vS"s ran in six 40-
trial blocks for each of four days. Each block con-
of the second letter. tained one of six intervals between the two letters.
The intervals were assigned randomly so that each 6"
Method had all six blocks on each day. Experiment II was
Experiments were run on a PDP-9 computer. substantially the same except that each block con-
Displays were presented on a Tektronix 503 scope tained four physical matches (e.g., AA), eight
using a P 11 phosphor which decays to .01 of name matches (e.g., Aa), 12 vowel consonant
its energy in 20 milliseconds (msec.). The warn- "same" responses, and 24 "different" responses.
ing signal was a cross presented .5 second be-
fore the first letter. The first letter was presented Results
directly below the warning signal and remained
on until S's response. After a variable interval First, it is clear from Figures 2 and 3,
(interstimulus interval [ISI]), the second letter as found previously (Posner & Mitchell,
was presented adjacent to the first. At a distance 1967), that the level of match has sys-
of 16 inches the two letters were foveal (3 degrees
of visual angle). The time between the two letters tematic effects on the reaction time. More-
was varied between blocks of trials. over, the encoding function for all three

430

400

O---O DIFFERENT
SAME
_J
s

350

300 PHYSICAL MATCH

200 400 600 800 1000


INTERVAL (MILLISEC)

FIG. 2. Encoding function for physical matches. (Constant warning


interval of SOO msec., with constant blocks of various ISIs.)
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 395

TOO

600
V-C SAME
<0
_l
I
fe
500

400

200 400 6OO 800 1000


INTERVAL (MILL!SEC)

FIG. 3. Encoding functions for vowel-consonant (V-C) match task.


(Constant warning interval of 500 msec, with constant blocks of various
I Sis. NI=name identity, PI = physical identity.)

levels is strikingly similar in both experi- matches do not show much more improve-
ments. The optimal RT is at 500 msec, in ment than physical matches.
all functions. This finding appears to be In general, encoding and preparation func-
robust over the distribution of intervals, tions appear to be rather similar. Both show
since a control study using intervals of 0, 1, an optimal point at about SOO msec. Both
SO, ISO, 250, and 500 showed almost the tend to give greater improvement for vowel-
same function as that obtained for the first consonant than for physical matches. The
five points of Figure 2. The same result is next experiment is designed to compare them
also obtained in encoding functions discussed more directly.
in the next section (see Figures 4 and 5).
There is some tendency for sharpest im- PREPARATION AND ENCODING
provement in RT to occur later for more If S is given 500 msec, to prepare, his RT
complex matches. Most of the improvement will be about optimal. If he is given infor-
for physical matches appears to come in the mation at the end of that 500 msec, about
first 150 msec. This is much less true of what he is to select, he shows a further im-
vowel-consonant matches, which show a provement which also goes on for about 500
sharp improvement between 250 and 500 msec. Are these two improvements due to
msec. The extent of improvement appears two separate processes, or are they merely
to be slightly greater for vowel-consonant two different ways of studying the same
matches than for other types, but name overall process? A paradigm was designed
396 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES

TABLE 1 varied only ISI. These produced an encoding


CONDITIONS OF WARNING AND INTERSTIMULUS function. Three conditions used zero foreperiod
INTERVALS FOR EXPERIMENTS ON ENCODING and varied ISI. In this case, the first letter served
AND PREPARATION as both a warning signal and as a source of spe-
cific information. Two conditions enter into two
functions, making a total of seven different combi-
Warning interval"
Inter- nations of WI and ISI as shown in Table 1.
interval * 1 Each condition involved a block of 40 trials; 20
0 150 500 "same" and 20 "different." Each trial was sepa-
rated by a long variable intertrial interval of
0 Preparation Preparation Preparation 8-15 seconds. This was followed by a warning
Both Encoding signal, the first letter and the second letter. The
150 Both Encoding warning signal remained on only during the WI,
500 Both Encoding while the first and second letters remained present
a
until ^s responded.
In milliseconds. Each experiment involved 10 6"s run in one block
of each of the seven conditions in a random order
to compare them directly. The method used on each of two days. The first day was considered
was to obtain preparation functions, encod- practice. After each trial, -S^ were given feed-
ing functions, and functions where both pro- back as to the correctness of their responses and
the RTs. The three experiments differ only in the
cesses might go on simultaneously. level of instruction. In the first experiment, "same"
was defined as physical identity and all letters were
Method uppercase; in the second experiment, "same" re-
Three experiments were run involving a com- sponses were defined as letters with the same name;
mon method. The apparatus and displays were while in the final experiment, "same" was defined
the same as discussed previously. Three con- by the rule both vowels or both consonants. In all
ditions varied only the warning interval (WI) and experiments, the "same" response was made with
were used to produce a preparation function. Three the left index finger and the "different" response
conditions involved an optimal WI (.5 second) and with the right index finger.
600
0/0

500

PREPARATION

^
500/0
.J
2

400
500/150
/ENCODING
500/500

300

100 200 300 400 600


INTERVAL (MILLISEC)

FIG. 4. Preparation, encoding, and "both" functions for physical matching


task. (Values of WI/ISI are shown for each point.)
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 397

760

700

650

600 PREPARATION
500/0
I

K 600

500/150 BOTH
500

ENCODING
500/500
450

100 200 300 400 600


INTERVAL (MILLISEC)
FIG. 5. Preparation, encoding, and "both" functions for name matching
task. (Values of WI/ISI are shown for each point.)

Results tween two independent variables has been in-


Three functions can be drawn from the terpreted (Sternberg, 1969) as indicating
seven conditions. Each condition involving that they affect the same stage of processing.
a combination of one WI and one ISI. A In the present case, WI and ISI may be
preparation function involves conditions 0/0, thought to interact because they both af-
150/0, and 500/0 (WI/ISI). This function fect alertness. At zero WI, alertness must
is shown in the upper curve of Figures 4, be varying during the ISI, while with an
5, and 6. optimal WI, 5" may already be alert and thus
The encoding function is obtained from ISI would mainly involve encoding of in-
conditions where S has always had a full 500- formation from the first letter.
msec. WI and either 0-, 150-, or 500-msec. Table 2 shows the amount of improvement
ISI. This is shown in the lower curve of (from the 0/0 condition for preparation and
each figure. When there is no warning sig- "both" and from the 500/0 for encoding)
nal, the first letter serves both as a warning due to preparation, encoding, and both to-
signal and to provide selective information. gether. The results clearly indicate that in
The function obtained from conditions 0/0, the case of physical and name instructions,
0/150, and 0/500 is called the "both" func- the improvement obtained in the "both" con-
tion and is shown as the middle line of each dition is almost exactly the sum of that ob-
figure. The figures show the mean of the tained separately from the preparation and
median RTs for each condition. encoding functions. In the vowel-consonant
The main effects of WI and ISI on RT case, there appears to be a more substantial
are highly significant in all experiments. In departure from additivity, but these are not
all three experiments there is an interaction significant due to high variability among 6"s
between WI and ISI. An interaction be- in this condition.
398 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES

900

850

800

760
500/0
••
PREPARATION
700

650

600
500/150

ENCODING
550 500/500

100 200 300 400 500


INTERVAL (MILLISEC)
FIG. 6. Preparation, encoding, and "both" functions for vowel-consonant
matching task. (Values of WI/ISI are shown for each point.)
The additivity of improvement due to Nielsen, 1970; Tversky, 1969). In many
preparation and encoding suggests that the experiments where there is an interval be-
two processes can proceed in parallel with- tween the two items to be matched, "same"
out interference. This is true even at 150 responses are faster than "different" re-
msec, when the functions are changing sponses (Bamber, 1969; Nickerson, 1965).
rapidly. Moreover, the additivity indicates Figures 7 and 8 show the data from the prep-
that the amount of improvement due to en- aration, encoding, and "both" functions for
coding is the same regardless of the initial the physical match. It is clear that the rela-
level of alertness. tive efficiency in processing "same" as com-
It remains to be shown that the encoding pared to "different" stimuli increases as a
function is really related to specific informa- function of interval, but only when 5 has
tion from the letter presented. A striking been given the first letter. The relative in-
finding in the physical match encoding func- crease in efficiency of the "same" response
tion is a significant interaction between in- as compared to "different" shown in the
terval and type of response (same vs. differ- RTs is also reflected in the error rates. No
ent) F = 3.9, df = 2/9, p < .05. Recently a such change is found when the interval in-
number of investigators have pointed out that volves the period between a warning signal
the response "different" does not appear to and the letter pair. Nor does it occur for
be the mirror image of the response "same" name or vowel-consonant level matches.
(Bamber, 1969; Nickerson, 1965; Smith & The finding that "same" and "different"
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 3Q9
responses are identical in simultaneous pre- TABLE 2
sentation has not been universal (Krueger, AMOUNT OF IMPROVEMENT IN REACTION TIME
1970; Posner & Mitchell, 1967). This ap- FROM PREPARATION, ENCODING, AND BOTH
pears to depend on many factors. It was
Interval0
suspected that one such factor was assign- .. , .
JVL&tcn
ment of hands. To check on this, a control ISO 500
study was run which assigned six ,9s "same"
on the left and six 5"s "same" on the right. Physical
The experiment involved ISIs of 0, SO, 150, Preparation 58 122
Encoding 53 73
and 500 msec, with a 500-msec. WI. The Both 101 195
physical match instruction was used. The Name
data for left-hand "same" 5s replicated Fig- Preparation 42 127
ure 7 perfectly. For Ss assigned "same" Encoding 58 H98
Both 111 201
on the right, however, "same" was faster Vowel-Consonant
even with simultaneous presentation. How- Preparation 71 170
ever, regardless of hand assignment, the sig- Encoding 124 150
nificant divergence over time between "same" Both 157 296
and "different" responses was found (see
Note.—Improvement is measured by subtracting RT at
also Figure 2 and Corballis, Lieberman, & specified interval from RT at 0/0 for preparation and "both"
function and 500/0 for encoding.
Bindra, 1968). a
In milliseconds.
The finding that presentation of the first
letter improves the efficiency of handling an tical form. One way of conceptualizing this
identical letter suggests that the first letter is in terms of Sokolov's neural model idea
changed 5's sensitivity to a letter of iden- (Sokolov, 1963). The first letter serves as

600

800
PREPARATION

1
1
I-
K
400

ENCODING

O O DIFFERENT
300
•—• SAME

100 200 300 400 900


INTERVAL (MILLISEC)
FIG. 7. "Same" versus "different" match RTs as a function of interval
for preparation and encoding functions of physical match task.
400 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES
600

500

2w
3
i-
tc
400

O—--O DIFFERENT
• • SAME

300

100 200 300 400 SCO


INTERVAL (MILLISEC)
FIG. 8. "Same" versus "different" match RTs as a function of
interval for "both" functions of physical match task.
a model of what 5" is looking for. When the The results of this section suggest that
second letter matches it, processing proceeds encoding and preparation are separable func-
rapidly; if it does not, further tests are made. tions with different effects on behavior.
A similar view would be that the first letter However, it appears possible to carry on
activates internal units representing the both simultaneously as efficiently as either
stored information concerning that letter one alone. In some ways this provides an
(Morton, 1969; Posner, 1969), which may answer to Moray's question concerning
then be more sensitive to identical stimula- whether selectivity is more efficient for an
tion for some period of time. The latter idea alerted organism than for an unalerted one.
would presumably allow for activation of re- According to these data, whether S has had
lated items necessary to handle improve- a previous warning signal does not change
ments at the name and vowel-consonant the efficiency with which he encodes a letter,
levels, since access to past experience is at least up to the level of a name match.
clearly needed for these matches. Both of Nor does the relative bias in physical match-
these ideas are consistent with recent ideas ing for "same" as opposed to "different"
about "same" and "different" responses to differ for alerted and unalerted conditions.
successive stimuli (Bamber, 1969; Tversky, These data strongly suggest that a warning
1969). What is new is that the divergence signal does not improve the process of en-
over time indicates that at least part of the coding information from the first letter.
advantage of "same" is due to the encoding So far, we have defined two separate func-
of the first letter rather than in the criterion tions having to do with attention and ob-
governing the match itself. served something of their relationship. It
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 401
now remains to relate each of them to the probe RTs could provide a dynamic picture
third component of attention. of the central processing demands of primary
tasks.
PROCESSING CAPACITY
The probe technique applied to the letter-
One sense of attention involves competi- matching sequence might indicate how prep-
tion between signals for some limited capac- aration and encoding are related to central
ity system (James, 1890). The idea is that processing capacity. Of particular interest
many tasks place demands on a central is whether preparation for a visual signal
limited capacity system and thus tend to in- served to improve or retard RT to signals
terfere. This basic idea has led to a number coming from another modality (Hernandez-
of experiments which have attempted to mea- Peon, 1966; Naatanen, 1970). Another
sure the attention demands of one task by its question is whether encoding itself requires
interference with a secondary task (Welch, processing capacity in the sense of interfer-
1898; Welford, 1968). One way of doing ence with a probe RT. Finally, there is the
this is to use a probe as the standard second- question of what aspects of letter matching
ary task. For example, Naatanen (1970) seem to demand the greatest involvement of
measured the evoked potential to auditory processing capacity.
clicks, which were probes occurring during
a visual RT task. Posner and Keele (1967) Method
and Ells (1969) used a simple or choice RT The same apparatus was used as described pre-
task as a probe during a movement task. viously. A trial consisted of a warning signal
Foss (1969) used a probe RT to study followed after a WI with the first letter, which
remained on until the presentation, after an I SI
processing of sentences. The advantage of of a second letter adjacent to it. After each
a probe RT task is that the experimenter trial, feedback was provided on the correctness and
can measure both the RT to primary and RT for the letter match. The values of the WI,
secondary tasks and thus be certain what ISI, and intertrial interval (ITI) are given for
effects each is having on the other. In each experiment in Table 3. On half of the trials,
SO decibels of white noise was turned on in 6"s left
studying movements, it was found that the ear at one of eight positions in the trial. The probe
probe had little effect on the primary move- occurred equally often in each of the eight positions.
ment task, but that the probe RT reflected The SB were instructed that the letter task was
the central processing demands of the pri- primary and that they were to press their right
mary task in a very sensitive way. For index finger if the letter had the same name and
their right middle finger if the names were differ-
example, prior to the movement, probe time ent. They were told to press their left index finger
was a function of a number of alternative whenever they heard the white noise burst. Feed-
movements and was unrelated to target back on the letter-matching task did not occur until
width. During the movement, probe time both responses were complete. No feedback was
was a function of target width and unrelated provided on the noise RTs.
In Experiment I, 5"s ran six 48-trial blocks per
to number of alternatives (Ells, 1969). day. Each block was identical in form. All the
These findings suggested very strongly that first letters were uppercase and the second letters
TABLE 3
INTERTRIAL, INTERSTIMULUS, AND WARNING INTERVALS AND FIRST-LETTER
EXPOSURE DURATIONS (IN SECONDS) FOR EACH EXPERIMENT

Probe positions
(milliseconds following warning signal)
Experi- ITI WI Exposure duration ISI
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

i 8-15 variable 1 .5 .5 -3000, -2000, 150, 600, 1100, 1300, 1600, 1900
ii 4-6 variable .5 1 1 -500, 150, 550, 650, 800, 1000, 1650, 1800
in 4-6 variable .5 .05 "I 1 -500, 150, 550, 650, 800, 1000, 1650, 1800
son rvariab'e
l.OOOJ
402 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES
lowercase, so all matches were at the name level. The first point of interest is that the RT
In Experiment II, half the blocks were all upper- to the probe is longer when it is in the ITI
case (physical match) and half were identical to
Experiment I (name match). than when it occurs after the warning signal.
All 6"s were run individually for two days. Data Some evidence on this point appears in Fig-
from the second day only were used for those 5s ure 9 where the two probes after the warning
whose median probe RTs were under 1 second for are slightly faster than those during the ITI.
all positions. Twelve ,5s were included in the However, this improvement does not reach
analysis of each experiment (a total of three were
excluded for not meeting the criterion). significance. In Experiment II, RTs at
Position 2 are faster than at Position 1, but
Results again this is not significant. However, at
The basic results of each experiment Positions 3 and 4, each 5* shows a definite
are the mean of the median probe RTs improvement in RTs over the times obtained
at each position and the mean of the median during the ITI (p < .01, by sign test).
letter match RTs at each probe position. Thus, a prepared -S" gives faster RTs to the
These are shown in Figures 9,10,11, and 12. secondary task than an unprepared S, even
Differences in letter match times between though the 6" is instructed to prepare for the
physical and name matches are consistent visual signal. An analysis of the distribu-
with the instructions and suggest 6"s used a tions at each probe position revealed no evi-
visual code for the former and the letter dence of anticipation errors (e.g., RTs to the
names for the latter (Posner, 1969). noise of less than 200 msec.). Every effort

600

500

i
400

300

FIRST SECOND
WARNING LETTER LETTER

-4 -2 .5 I 1.5
TIME (SEC)
FIG. 9. Probe RTs as a function of probe position for Experiment I.
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 403

BOO

400

e
<0

a
i
300

WARNING FIRST SECOND


I I

NONE -4 -2 0 .9 1.5
TIME (SEC)
FIG. 10. Letter match RTs as a function of probe position for Experiment I.

600

900

P.I.

H
et 400

300

FIRST SECOND
WARNING LETTER LETTER

-I 0 .5 I 1.5 2
TIME (SEC)
FIG. 11. Probe RTs as a function of probe position for Experiment II.
404 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES

O--

400

CO
J

1
300

FIRST LETTER
WARNING SECOND LETTER
I I

NONE -I 0 .5 1.5
TIME (SEC)
FIG. 12. Letter match RTs as a function of probe position for
physical (PI) and name (NI) matches of Experiment II.

was made to insure that the preparation of This result suggests that 5"s have trouble
.S"s was specific to the visual letter-matching processing the probe during the response
task. The auditory probe occurred on only phase of the letter-match task. If probes are
.5 of the trials and were distributed over delayed until close to the letter-match RT,
eight different positions. All feedback in- they drop back to nearly the same values as
volved the times for the letter task and none obtained during the ITI (Figure 9, Probe
referred to times on the probe task. Position 8). Probes which occur between
A second point of interest is that probe the two letters are increased if they fall close
times do not necessarily increase as a con- to the occurrence of the second letter (see
sequence of presenting the first letter. When Figure 11, Probe Position 6). This increase
the first letter was on for only .5 second does not appear to be due to the actual ap-
(Figure 9), RT to probes after the first let- pearance of the second letter. When probe
ter (Probe Positions 5 and 6) was increased distributions were computed for probes 500
sharply. However, when the first letter was msec, before the second letter in Experiment
present for 1 second (Figure 11), RT to the 11 (Figure 11, Probe Position 6), it was
probe did not begin to increase until 300-500 found that few of the RTs were long enough
msec, after the first letter was presented to occur after the second letter. Even with
(Probe Positions 5 and 6). From previous these excluded from the analysis, RTs to
results (see Figures 2 and 3), it would seem probes in this position still remained longer
likely that the encoding of the first letter was than those during the ITI and WI for all
pretty much complete by the time the probe 12 5s.
began to show interference. Do the probe RTs really reflect the pro-
Third, it is clear that probe times are cessing requirements of the letter-matching
systematically increased at certain points. task? With the long I SI it might be possi-
Probes which occur shortly after the second ble for 5"s to switch from processing the let-
letter are always long (Figure 9, Probe Posi- ter to listening for the probes. In order to
tion 7, and Figure 11, Probe Positions 7 and check on this, an experiment was run in
8). These times decrease quite consistently which the first letter was turned off ran-
from 100-400 msec, after the second letter. domly after 50, 150, 500, or 1000 msec. The
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 405

500

•—•50
X—X 500

g400
<2

.1

300

WARNING FIRST SECOND


I I
.5 1.5
TIME (SEC)
FIG. 13. Probe RTs compared for exposure durations of the
first letters of SO and 500 msec.

experiment was otherwise identical to the mid positions and should be fastest based on
name letter-matching condition of Experi- an expectancy idea. These show very differ-
ment II. Figure 13 shows the results for ent results in both Figure 9 and in the name
seven ^"s who ran in this experiment for five match condition of Figure 11. Moreover,
days. The data represent probe times on the probes at Position 8 are quite fast al-
Days 4 and 5 for trials which had only 50- though they are at the end of the distribu-
msec. exposure of the first letter and those tion. In addition, the results shown in Fig-
for which the first letter was on for 500 ure 13 illustrate that the point at which the
msec. The RT results together with gen- first letter is turned off has small but sys-
ally very low error rate indicates that 5"s are tematic effects on the probe RTs in the di-
not switching away from the letter task. If rection which would be expected if 5"s were
they were, they would miss many of the showing interference from letters which were
50-msec. exposures. Moreover, the probe removed somewhat more rapidly than those
curves from this study are quite similar to which remained present. Thus, it appears
those illustrated in the previous studies. that probe RT reflects the attention demands
Even if the first letter was turned off after of the primary task.
50 msec., 5" does not show substantial inter-
CONCLUSIONS
ference with the probe until 500 msec.
Another possibility is that ,9s are learning The introduction outlined three component
the distribution of the probes and respond processes involved in the study of attention.
most rapidly to those in the middle of the These are alertness, selectivity, and process-
distribution. This possibility seems rather ing capacity. In this paper each of these
remote—in part because of the low frequency components was identified with a particu-
of probes at any position (only on 1/16 of lar experimental operation. Alertness was
the trials), but also because of the complex studied by varying the time between a warn-
shape of the RTs as a function of probe posi- ing signal and a pair of letters which 5" was
tion. Probe Positions 4 and 5 are the two required to match. Since the warning signal
406 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES

told -S nothing about what letters he was to tions with as great efficiency as either one
receive, it provided only rather minimal se- alone.
lective information. Selectivity was studied This conclusion is similar to that reached
by providing one of the two letters so that 6" by Posner and Wilkinson (1969), in which
knew what he was to look for. Improve- preparation, as measured by a negative shift
ments in RT to the second letter provide a in the electroencephalogram (contingent neg-
function related to encoding of the first let- ative variation), was unaffected by making
ter. Both alertness and selectivity were the warning signal a letter which had to be
found to improve performance in the letter- classified. The correspondence between the
matching task. In both cases, the time re- findings of the present study and previous
quired to reach optimal performance was work suggests a relationship between alert-
about 500 msec. This value appeared to be ness, as used in this paper, and the contingent
surprisingly consistent both between the two negative variation. If this relationship is
processes and over various levels of process- correct, then alertness might be thought of
ing (physical, name, vowel). The encoding as a general and widespread increase in cor-
function appeared to be somewhat steeper tical activation. Results of studies with the
over the first 150 msec, than the preparation contingent negative variation support that
function. As the level of encoding required view and suggest that this improvement is not
by the match increased, the steepness of the confined merely to tasks in which there is an
function appeared to be somewhat reduced. overt motor response (Karlin, 1970). Thus,
The main consistent difference between the improvement does not appear to be con-
the preparation and encoding functions was fined to the motor system. How does alert-
in the rate of responding to identical versus ness improve task efficiency ? The failure of
different letter pairs. The preparation in- the initial level of alertness to vary the ef-
terval improved physical matches and differ- ficiency of encoding indicates that alertness
ent responses equally. The encoding of one does not influence the earliest stages of pro-
letter appeared to improve processing of cessing. However, it is possible that with
physical matches more than of mismatches. our method, the parallel development of
This divergence between "same" and "differ- alertness and encoding obscures the impor-
ent" responses was particularly sharp in the tance of the initial state of alertness so that
first 150 msec. For higher levels of instruc- it does not affect the encoding function.
tions, no such divergence was shown. Perhaps a more interesting possibility is that
The preparation and encoding functions alertness varies the time when the results
are additive at least for physical and name of the encoding process begin to occupy the
matches. This is true even after only a 150- limited capacity system. This would suggest
msec. interval when both are improving the early stages of processing are unaffected
rapidly. The additivity between the two by level of alertness. Studies of the rela-
functions suggest that encoding may proceed tion of early components of the evoked po-
with nearly equal efficiency regardless of the tential to alertness (Karlin, 1970; Naatanen,
initial level of alertness. The bias intro- 1970) are not conclusive on this point.
duced from the first letter appears to develop If preparation is viewed as a general pro-
just as rapidly whether £ has been prepared cess which is not selective, there must be
by an earlier warning signal or not. This other mechanisms which account for the ob-
provides one kind of answer to Moray's vious ability of 5"s to select information.
question about the relationship of selection The first letter appears to have a very spe-
and alertness. The additivity also implies cific effect on selection of a new letter which
that the two operations can be carried on resembles it. If the first letter is viewed as
simultaneously without interference. If, for activating a trace system related to the pro-
example, 5" must both extract the name of a cess of recognition (Morton, 1969; Posner,
letter for later matching and use it as a 1969), this activation may serve to make
warning signal, he may perform both func- selection of already active areas of memory
COMPONENTS OF ATTENTION 407

more efficient. Thus, in the "both" condi- sponse, but is not related to the contact be-
tion, a single letter would lead to a general tween the input and long-term memory that
alertness and a specific activation, which con- leads to the letter name. Several lines of
tribute additively to the overall reduction in evidence have tended to relate single-channel
RT. Of course, the activation of such areas effects to response selection rather than all
of memory could come either from internal forms of stimulus processing (Herman &
or external sources. Kantowitz, 1970; Keele, 1970). Moreover,
The nearly perfect time sharing between other research has indicated that conscious
preparation and encoding suggests that at awareness is itself rather late in the sequence
least one of these mental operations does not of mental processing (Fehrer & Raab, 1962;
require central processing capacity. If they Libet, 1966). If single-channel effects were
both did, they would be expected to interfere. tied to conscious processing, one would not
In order to further test whether alertness expect to see them closely correlated with en-
and encoding were free of demands on pro- coding of the first letter (Deutsch & Deutsch,
cessing capacity, the attention demands of 1962; Norman, 1968), but rather with opera-
the letter-matching tasks were measured by tions on the retrieved product of such encod-
introduction of a simple probe RT task ing. The data obtained in this study appear
to white noise. The results suggest that to be consistent with such a view.
a warning signal serves to reduce RT
both to the main task and to the probe REFERENCES
task, even when probes were introduced on BAMBER, D. Reaction times and error rates for
only half the trials. Moreover, the encoding "same" and "different" judgments of multi-
of the first letter did not seem to interfere dimensional stimuli. Perception and Psycho-
with the probe task, provided 5s had one physics, 1969, 6, 169-174.
second before the second letter was intro- BERTELSON, P. The time course of preparation.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,
duced. This was the case even when the 1967, 19, 272-279.
first letter was turned off so that 5" had to BOIES, S. Retention of visual information from a
encode quickly. This suggests that encoding single letter. Unpublished master's thesis, Uni-
of a letter does not require processing ca- versity of Oregon, 1969.
pacity as we have defined it here. Of course BROADBENT, D. E. Perception and communication.
the reasonableness of the conclusion depends London: Pergamon Press, 19S8.
COHEN, G. Pattern recognition: Differences be-
on the assumption that probe RT will be tween matching patterns and matching descrip-
sensitive to processing demands of the pri- tions to patterns. Journal of Experimental Psy-
mary task. chology, 1969, 82, 427-434.
Interference with the probe was significant CORBALLIS, M. C., LlEBERMAN, W., & BlNDRA, D.
when it was introduced 500 msec, prior to Discriminability and central intermittency in
the second letter. The interference was not same-different judgments. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 1968, 20, 51-61.
due to the physical occurrence of the second DEUTSCH, J. A., & DEUTSCH, D. Attention: Some
letter, since the effect was still present when theoretical considerations. Psychological Review,
all RTs above 500 msec, were eliminated. 1963, 70, 80-90.
What aspect of the letter matching causes EGAN, J. P., GREENBERG, G. E., & SCHULMAN, A. J.
interference with the late probes ? One pos- Interval of time uncertainty in auditory detection.
Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
sible explanation is that such mental opera- 1961, 35, 771-728.
tions as rehearsal or generation of distinctive EGETH, H. E. Selective attention. Psychological
features for testing, which follow encoding Bulletin, 1967, 67, 41-56.
ELLS, J. Attention requirements of movement con-
of the first letter, require processing capacity, trol. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni-
while encoding itself does not. versity of Oregon, 1969.
These results suggest that attention in the FEHHER, E., & RAAB, D. Reaction time to stimuli
sense of central processing capacity is related masked by meta contrast. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology, 1962, 63, 143-147.
to mental operations of which we are con- Foss, D. J. Decision processes during sentence
scious, such as rehearsing or choosing a re- comprehension: Effects of lexical item difficulty
408 MICHAEL I. POSNER AND STEPHEN J. BOIES
and position upon decision time. Journal of attention. Psychological Review, 1968, 75, 522-
Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1969, 8, 536.
457-462. POSNER, M. I. Abstraction and the process of
HERMAN, L. M., & KANTOWITZ, B. H. The psy- recognition. In G. Bower & J. T. Spence (Eds.),
chological refractory period: Only half the double Psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 3.
stimulation story. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, New York: Academic Press, 1969.
73, 74-88. POSNER, M. L, & KEELE, S. W. Attention demands
HERNANDEZ-PEON, R. Physiological mechanisms of movement. In, Proceedings of the 16th Inter-
in attention. In R. W. Russell (Ed.), Frontiers national Congress of Applied Psychology. Am-
in physiological psychology. New York: Aca- sterdam : Swets & Zeitlinger, 1967.
demic Press, 1966. POSNER, M. I., BOIES, S. J., EICHELMAN, W. H., &
JAMES, W. Principles of psychology. Vol. 1. TAYLOR, R. L. Retention of visual and name
New York: Holt, 1890. codes of single letters. Journal of Experimental
KARLIN, L. Cognition, preparation, and sensory- Psychology, 1969, 79(1, Pt. 2).
evoked potentials. Psychological Bulletin, 1970, POSNER, M. I., & MITCHELL, R. F. Chronometric
73, 122-136. analysis of classification. Psychological Review,
KEELE, S. W. The effects of input and output 1967, 74, 392-409.
modes on decision time. Journal of Experimental POSNER, M. I., & WILKINSON, R. T. On the pro-
Psychology, 1970, 85, 157-164. cess of preparation. Paper presented at the meet-
ing of the Psychonomics Society, November
KOSTER, W. G. (Ed.) Attention and performance 1969.
II. Amsterdam: North-Holland Press, 1969. REYNOLDS, D. Effects of double stimulation:
KRUEGER, L. E. Effect of bracketing lines on the Temporal inhibition of response. Psychological
speed of "same"—"different" judgment of two ad- Bulletin, 1964, 62, 333-347.
jacent letters. Journal of Experimental Psy- SANDERS, A. F. (Ed.) Attention and performance
chology, 1970, 84, 324-330. I. Amsterdam: North-Holland Press, 1967.
LA BERGE, D., VAN GELDER, P., & YELLOT, J. A SANDERS, A. F. (Ed.) Attention and performance
cueing technique in choice reaction time. Percep- III. Amsterdam: North-Holland Press, 1970.
tion and Psychophysics, 1970, 7, 57-62. SMITH, E. E., & NIELSEN, G. D. Representations
LEAVITT, F. Accuracy of report and central readi- and retrieval processes in short-term memory:
ness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, Recognition and recall of faces. Journal of Ex-
81, 542-546. perimental Psychology, 1970, 85, 397-405.
LEONARD, J. A. Partial advance information in a SOKOLOV, Y. N. Perception and the conditioned re-
choice reaction task. British Journal of Psy- flex. London: Pergamon Press, 1963.
chology, 1958, 49, 89-96. STERNBERG, S. The discovery of processing stages:
LIBET, B. Brain stimulation and the threshold of Extension of Bonders method. In W. G. Koster
conscious experience. In J. T. Eccles (Ed.), (Ed.), Attention and performance II. Amster-
Brain and conscious experience. New York: dam: North-Holland Press, 1969.
Springer-Verlag, 1966. TRIESMAN, A. Strategies and models of selective
MACKWORTH, J. A. Vigilance and habitation. attention. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 282-
London: Penguin Press, 1970. 299.
MORAY, N. Listening and attention. London: TVERSKY, B. Pictorial and verbal encoding in a
Penguin Press, 1970. short-term memory task. Perception and Psy-
MORTON, J. Interaction of information in word chophysics, 1969, 6, 225-233.
recognition. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, WELCH, J. On the measurement of mental activity
165-178. through muscular activity and the determination
of a constant of attention. American Journal of
NAATANEN, R. Evoked potential, EEG, and slow
Physiology, 1898, 1, 288-306.
potential correlates of selective attention. In
A. F. Sanders (Ed.), Attention and performance WELFORD, A. T. Fundamentals of skill. London:
Methuen, 1968.
III. Amsterdam: North-Holland Press, 1970.
WILKINSON, R. T., & HAINES, E. Evoked response
NICKERSON, R. S. Response times for same-differ- correlates of expectancy during vigilance. Acta
ent judgments. Perceptual and Motor Skills, Psychologica, 1970, 33, 402-413.
1965, 20, 15-18.
NORMAN, D, A. Toward a theory of memory and (Received November 4, 1970)

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen