Sie sind auf Seite 1von 10

Exp Brain Res (2004) 155: 186–195

DOI 10.1007/s00221-003-1716-x

RESEARCH ARTICLES

Dominique van Roon . Bert Steenbergen .


Ruud G. J. Meulenbroek

Trunk recruitment during spoon use in tetraparetic cerebral palsy

Received: 20 August 2002 / Accepted: 5 September 2003 / Published online: 20 December 2003
# Springer-Verlag 2003

Abstract In the present study we investigated the extent (e.g. soup) because of the additional risk of spilling when
to which individuals suffering from spastic tetraparesis as bringing the spoon to the mouth. Compared to regular
a consequence of cerebral palsy tune their trunk involve- reaching and grasping tasks, in which accuracy demands
ment to accuracy demands in a spoon-handling task. impose themselves mainly at the end of the task (e.g.
Twenty-two participants (ten adolescents with spastic dependent on target size), transporting substances with a
tetraparesis and 12 control participants) had to transport spoon requires the continuous monitoring of required
a spoon filled with water or sugar to a small or a large accuracy as the movement unfolds. It is this feature of a
bowl that was placed within reach. Even though trunk spoon-handling task that makes it particularly interesting
displacement was larger in the tetraparetic participants and new to use such a task to study the way in which
than it was in the control participants, the effects of the people who have to cope with a permanent motor disorder
imposed accuracy constraints were remarkably similar. handle combinations of accuracy demands.
Participants in both groups increased trunk displacement In the present study, we investigated to what extent the
with increasing precision requirements. Furthermore, in specific accuracy demands of spoon use are reflected in
both groups the largest trunk involvement was found in the the recruitment of the trunk in participants suffering from a
initial and final part of the substance-transporting phase, spastic tetraparesis as a consequence of cerebral palsy
when wrist velocity was lowest. We propose several (CP). CP is a condition caused by chronic, non-progres-
explanations for these findings and conclude that the large sive brain damage in young children. This damage can be
trunk involvement in individuals with tetraparetic cerebral caused by, for instance, an oxygen shortage at or around
palsy should, in any case, not be regarded as a primary birth, intra-uterine infections, prematurity or (the removal
symptom of the disorder, but rather as an adaptive reaction of) a brain tumour at a very young age. It can cause motor
to increased task demands. deficits (e.g. spasticity, dystonia, ataxia, athetosis, hypo-
tonia; Albright 1996) and sensory deficits (e.g. impaired
Keywords Spoon use . Spastic tetraparesis . Trunk proprioception and stereognosis; Cooper et al. 1995), but
involvement . Accuracy constraints . Adaptation can also cause seizures and behavioural and cognitive
problems.
Spasticity arises in approximately 60% of the CP cases
Introduction (Sugden and Keogh 1990). Sanger et al. (2003) argued
that it is a disorder characterized by hypertonia, in which
Eating with a spoon seems to be an easy task to perform. one or both of the following signs are present: (1) The
This everyday form of tool use, however, is quite complex resistance to an externally imposed movement increases
when we consider the precise coordination of movements when the velocity of stretch increases and varies with the
of the trunk, arm, hand, and fingers that is required to direction of joint movement, and/or (2) this resistance rises
successfully complete the task. The task becomes fast above a threshold speed or joint angle. In general,
particularly complex when a liquid has to be transported voluntary muscle activation is disturbed. The impairment
may involve muscular weakness, disordered coordination
D. van Roon (*) . B. Steenbergen . R. G. J. Meulenbroek of synergistic agonist muscles, failure to inhibit antag-
Nijmegen Institute for Cognition and Information, University onistic muscles, associated movements, stereotyped move-
of Nijmegen, ment synergies, and decreased dexterity (Lance 1980;
PO Box 9104, 6500 HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands
e-mail: D.vanRoon@nici.kun.nl Barnes et al. 1994; Filloux 1996). As has been shown in
Tel.: +31-24-3612148 studies of reaching, grasping and eating (Van Thiel and
Fax: +31-24-3616066 Steenbergen 2001; Van Roon et al. 2003), a behavioural
187

characteristic of individuals with a spastic paresis as a participants (mean age 25 years 6 months, SD 3 years 0 months,
result of CP is an increased involvement of the trunk. A range 20;0–30;0 years;months) with no known history of neurolo-
gical disorders took part in the experiment. The study was approved
similar characteristic was found in hemiparetic stroke by the local ethics committee and performed in accordance with the
patients (e.g. Cirstea and Levin 2000; Levin et al. 2002). ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. At
Here, we wanted to find out whether this larger trunk the time of testing, all participants with tetraparesis were students at
involvement reflects a flexible adaptation to the disorder the Werkenrode Institute (Groesbeek, The Netherlands), where they
followed an adapted educational program. They all had sufficient
and the task constraints or, alternatively, whether it should physical and cognitive abilities to perform the task under study and
be regarded as a manifestation of the disorder per se normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Four tetraparetic participants
(Latash and Anson 1996; Steenbergen et al. 2000; Van were able to walk independently, while the other six sat unstrappped
Thiel and Steenbergen 2001; Steenbergen and Meulen- in a wheelchair from which they performed their daily activities. We
therefore inferred that all had functional sitting balance. All
broek 2003). Studying the combined effects of task participants were naive as to the purpose of the task and gave
constraints and the disorder in everyday perceptuomotor their informed consent prior to data collection. Additional
tasks provides, in our view, a unique way to try to participant information is given in Table 1.
disentangle indices of flexible and rigid behaviour.
Research in healthy individuals has shown that an
increase in accuracy constraints, both during the move- Task and procedure
ment and at the end of it, gives rise to a larger
displacement of the trunk (Steenbergen et al. 1995; Van Participants were seated at a table on an ergonomic chair that could
be adjusted such that the following criteria were met. When the
der Kamp and Steenbergen 1999; Mackey et al. 2000). forearms were on the table, the elbows were flexed at 90°.
Steenbergen et al. (1995) and Mackey et al. (2000) Additionally, the feet were flat on the ground (or placed on a foot
suggested that an increased use of the trunk, with its large rest) and the knees were also flexed at 90°. Prior to the start of the
mass and inertia, might help stabilize the arm–hand system experiment, the participants of the tetraparetic group performed the
Purdue Pegboard test (Tiffin 1968). As recommended in the
because a damping effect might occur. Alternatively, the examiner’s manual, they performed the test three times for 30 s
lower velocity of the wrist in tasks demanding higher with their preferred hand followed by three times for 30 s with their
accuracy might also underlie the larger trunk displace- non-preferred hand. The total score for each hand is presented in
ment. As demonstrated by several researchers, reaching Table 1.
After the experimenter had given a verbal go-signal, the
movements at higher velocities are achieved primarily by participant had to reach for a spoon filled with a particular
smaller-mass body segments, while during such move- substance, and pick it up. Subsequently, they had to transport the
ments at lower velocities an increased contribution of spoon to a cylindrical bowl that was placed further along the sagittal
greater-mass segments (with a larger inertia) is observed axis towards the back of the table (hereafter denoted as substance-
(Rosenbaum et al. 1991, 1995; Vaughan et al. 1996; Wang transporting phase), and empty the spoon into the bowl (see Fig. 1
for the experimental set-up). The substance-transporting phase was
and Stelmach 2001). the focus of our study, and all analyses are performed on this phase.
In the present study we examined whether individuals Participants had to transport red-coloured water in half of the trials,
with spastic tetraparesis as a result of CP either flexibly whereas in the other half they had to transport sugar. The diameter of
tune the amount of trunk involvement to the various the bowl was 5 cm or 11 cm.
To determine a participant-dependent, appropriate position of the
precision requirements or rigidly displace the trunk over a bowl in the workspace, participants were instructed to hold the
fixed distance during spoon-use, independent of the spoon in a power grip so that the stem of the spoon was parallel to
imposed accuracy demands. Our participants had to the front edge of the table (x-axis, see Fig. 1), and actively stretch
transport a spoon filled with either a fluid or a solid their arm as far as possible without moving their trunk.
substance (manipulation of accuracy during transport) to a
small or a large bowl (manipulation of accuracy at the end
of the transport).
Based on the findings of the studies mentioned above,
we expected a larger trunk involvement in the tetraparetic
group than in the control group. In addition, we expected a
larger trunk involvement if water, rather than sugar, had to
be transported and if the bowl into which the spoon had to
be emptied was small rather than large. An absence of
such accuracy effects would be expected if trunk use
constituted a primary symptom of the disorder.

Methods
Participants

Ten individuals diagnosed as having spastic tetraparesis as a


consequence of CP (mean age 17 years 2 months, SD 1 year 3 Fig. 1 Experimental set-up from the participant’s viewpoint: a
months, range 14;7–18;6 years;months) and twelve healthy control starting block, b spoon holder, c spoon, d letter balance, e bowl
188
Table 1 Participant information (CP cerebral palsy)
Subject Sex Age Active range Hand Purdue Pegboard scorec Diagnosis
ID (years; months) of motion (cm)a preferrenceb
Pref. hand Non-pref. hand

Tetraparetic participants
1 F 18;1 29 Right 25 14 CP, spastic tetraparesis, epileptic
2 M 18;6 34 Left 39 14 CP, spastic tetraparesis
3 F 17;4 43 Right 30 16 CP, spastic tetraparesis
4 M 18;6 42 Right 28 22 CP, spastic tetraparesis
5 M 16;1 42 Left 32 18 CP, spastic tetraparesis
6 F 14;7 32 Left 33 21 CP, spastic tetraparesis
7 M 17;4 47 Right 15 7 CP, spastic tetraparesis
8 F 17;9 39 Left 18 16 CP, spastic tetraparesis
9 M 16;4 50 Right 30 28 CP, spastic tetraparesis
10 F 17;4 32 Right 25 13 CP, spastic tetraparesis, epileptic
Control participants
1–12 8F/4M 25;6 35–42 Right 53 49 –
Norm scores for healthy individuals
a
Distance between the front edge of the table and the midpoint of the bowl. See Task and Procedure section for a detailed description of the
method of determination of this active range of motion
b
As indicated by the participant
c
The total number of pins placed into the holes in three 30-s periods (see Task and Procedure section)

Subsequently, the bowl was positioned so that its midpoint positioned 2 cm from the front edge of the table and approximately
corresponded to the midpoint of the spoon. We made the position 8 cm to the ipsilateral side of the midsagittal plane of the body, so
of the bowl dependent on the distance that could be covered by that the midpoint of the block corresponded with the midpoint of the
moving only the arm, because we wanted to study the involvement stem of the spoon. The other hand rested on the tabletop.
of the trunk during task performance when this involvement was not Participants were not restrained by any means, nor were any
strictly necessary to perform the task. On average, the distance speed demands imposed. In addition, no instructions were given
between the front edge of the table and the midpoint of the bowl concerning the type of grip that had to be used. However, they were
(active range of motion; see Table 1) in the tetraparetic group (mean instructed to try not to spill any of the substance. At the end of each
39 cm, SD 7.0 cm) was the same as this distance in the control group trial, the substance that was successfully transported to the bowl was
(mean 39 cm, SD 2.4 cm; t=0.085, P=0.933). Note that the distance weighed by means of a letter-balance (precision 0.5 g). Before the
that needed to be covered with the spoon in the substance- experiment started, each participant performed six practice trials in
transporting phase (hereafter denoted as transport distance), was which three spoons filled with water and three spoons filled with
less than half the active range of motion (or more precisely, this sugar had to be transported to the small bowl. After half of the
distance minus 7 cm—the distance between the front edge of the experimental trials, there was always a short break, but participants
table and the midpoint of the starting block—divided by two). The could always indicate when they needed extra rest. The total
spoon had a stem that measured 12 cm in length and an aluminium experiment took approximately 1.5 to 2 h of which 30 to 45 min
round bowl (diameter of 4.5 cm and depth of 1.2 cm). It weighed were spent on preparations and 60 to 75 min on the performance of
approximately 80 g. the 180 trials.
The manipulations resulted in four unique conditions, defined by A 3-D motion-tracking device (Optotrak 3020) was used for
Substance (water, sugar), and Bowl (small, large). For each movement recording. Four infrared light-emitting diodes (IREDs)
condition, 45 trials were performed resulting in a grand total of were placed on the wrist, elbow and both shoulders and their
180 trials per participant. The two substances were presented in two positions were sampled at a rate of 200 Hz with a spatial accuracy of
blocks of 90 trials, counterbalanced across the participants of each 0.2 mm in the x-, y-, and z-dimension. The actual testing was also
group. Within each block, the factor Bowl was randomized. videotaped.
The tetraparetic participants performed the task with their least
affected hand, as indicated by the participant and confirmed by the
Purdue-Pegboard scores. The control participants used the hand that Data analysis
they had indicated as being used for everyday unimanual tasks. At
the start of each trial, the experimenter placed the spoon on top of a
plastic cube of approximately 10 cm in height (located half-way The positional data were filtered using a zero-phase lag, second-
between the midpoint of the starting block and the midpoint of order Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 10 Hz and then
bowl), just underneath a plastic slat and on top of a little metal differentiated to calculate movement velocity and acceleration.
detector, thereby leaving the stem ‘free in the air’ (see Fig. 1). Semi-automatic segmentation routines were used to define the start
Presenting the spoon like this allowed the participants to grasp the and end of the substance-transporting phase, on which all
spoon in their preferred way. The spoon was filled with subsequent analyses were performed. The start of the movement
approximately 6 g of either sugar or water. Participants were was determined by the moment at which the wrist velocity in the y-
asked to sit upright, with their back against the back of the chair. The direction (see Fig. 1) reached 20 mm/s after the spoon had lost
palm of their preferred hand rested on the starting block to
standardize the starting position of the hand. This block measured
10.0 cm in length, 14.3 cm in width, and 6.5 cm in height. It was
189
Fig. 2 Typical examples of
task performance. Y-position
profiles of the wrist, the ipsilat-
eral and the contralateral shoul-
der (upper graphs) and the
tangential wrist velocity profile
(lower graphs) as a function of
movement duration for one trial
of a control participant (left
graphs) and for one trial of a
tetraparetic participant (right
graphs). In both depicted trials
water had to be transported to
the small bowl

contact with the metal detector.1 The end of this phase was shoulder. To determine the distribution of sagittal trunk displace-
determined by the moment at which the wrist velocity in the y- ment across the initial, middle and final part of the substance-
direction fell below 10% of its peak velocity. transporting phase, we divided this phase into three equal parts on
On the basis of the displacement data of the wrist several standard the basis of the distance covered by the wrist in the y-direction.
kinematic variables (mean tangential velocity, maximum tangential Subsequently, we determined the displacement of the contralateral
velocity, percentage of the movement time to peak velocity, shoulder in the y-direction in each of the three parts of the substance-
dysfluency of the movement, and wrist displacement [in the y- transporting phase.
direction] relative to the transport distance) were determined.
Dysfluency was determined by the number of zero-crossings per
second in the acceleration profile of the wrist. The forward wrist Statistical analysis
displacement relative to the transport distance was taken as a
measure of the way in which the bowl was approached (which was
also dependent on the type of grip used) in combination with the Means of the dependent variables across the replications of each
location at which the spoon was emptied (e.g. in front of or behind condition were analysed using repeated measures analysis of
the midpoint of the large bowl). When the bowl of the spoon was variance (ANOVA). The design consisted of one between-subjects
held out further to empty the spoon, or when the spoon was emptied factor, Group (tetraparetic versus control), and two within-subjects
earlier (closer to the front edge of the bowl), the distance that was factors, namely, Substance (water versus sugar) and Bowl (small
covered by the wrist was shorter. versus large). To analyse the distribution of the displacement of the
To examine the trunk involvement in the substance-transporting trunk across the three successive parts of the substance-transporting
phase we determined the absolute displacement in the y-direction of phase, and the mean wrist velocity in these three parts, we added an
both the ipsilateral shoulder (i.e. the shoulder of the same body-side extra within-subjects factor Part (initial, middle and final part).
as the arm that performed the task) and the contralateral shoulder When requirements for homogeneity of variances (Levene’s test)
between the start and the end of that phase. The displacement of the were not met, which was only the case for the displacement of the
contralateral shoulder in the y-direction was taken as a measure of trunk in the final part of the substance-transporting phase, we used a
sagittal trunk displacement. When the sagittal displacement of the parametric modification of the F-test (Welch’s test; see Maxwell and
ipsilateral shoulder was larger than the sagittal displacement of the Delaney 1990) to study the differences between groups. Step-down
contralateral shoulder, the trunk was rotated. Trunk rotation between analyses of statistically significant interactions were performed by
the start and the end of the substance-transporting phase was means of contrasts. To determine the relationship between the
calculated as the difference in the angle between the x-axis and the sagittal trunk displacement and the mean wrist velocity, Pearson’s
line connecting the IREDs on the ipsilateral and contralateral correlation coefficients were calculated for each participant
separately, across all 180 trials. The level for significance was set
1We checked in two control participants and two tetraparetic to P≤0.05. As a measure of effect size η2-values are reported.
participants (the slowest and the fastest one of each group) whether
different results were obtained if we determined the start by the
moment at which the wrist velocity in the y-direction rose above
10% of peak wrist velocity in the y-direction after the spoon had lost
contact with the metal detector. It appeared that the results were
qualitatively similar to the results reported below.
190

subject SD and CV for each condition (combination of factors Substance and Bowl) and
Results

General task performance

Tetraparetic
Coefficient of variation (%)

17***
17***
25***
47***
12
then averaging across the participants of each group (control and tetraparetic)
Both the control participants and the participants with
spastic tetraparesis were able to perform the task. Hardly
any of the substance was spilled. In each condition a total
of 90 g of the substance (6 g in each of the 15 trials) had to
be transported to the bowl. The control participants

Control
transported on average 89.5 g per condition (SD 1.1 g,
range 85–90 g) and the tetraparetic participants 89 g (SD

11
10

12
59
15
1.1 g, range 84.5–90 g). It must be noted that the spilling
almost always took place at the end of the task when the
spoon was emptied. In Fig. 2, two examples of the

Tetraparetic

2.3*
recorded movements and corresponding movement kine-

23*
34
11***

8
matics are displayed (one trial for each group).

Within-subject SD
Upon careful checking of the video recordings, it was
established that the employed grip types were comparable
across groups. In each group, two participants used

Control
predominantly power grips, while the other participants

1.7
19
31
6

9
used precision grips.

5.2 (1.5)***
Kinematic data of the end-effector

Table 2 shows the kinematic characteristics of the wrist


movement in the substance-transporting phase for the

48 (4)*

70 (11)
143 (32)***
212 (53)***
tetraparetic group and the control group separately.
Tetraparetic
Mean (between-subject SD)
Table 2 Kinematic characteristics of the wrist for the substance-transporting phase (across
both bowls and substances). The average within-subject standard deviations (SDs) and the
average coefficients of variation (CVs) were determined by first calculating the within-

Mean tangential wrist velocity

To check whether our manipulations of accuracy had been


2.8 (1.0)

effective, we examined their effects on the mean tangential


wrist velocity. We expected that the task was performed at
192 (19)
278 (29)
52 (4)

64 (11)

a lower speed if accuracy constraints were higher, as is


Control

normally the case in regular reaching tasks (e.g. Marteniuk


et al 1987).
The participants with spastic tetraparesis made slower
movements than the control participants in the substance-
Movement time to peak velocity (% total movement time)

transporting phase (143 mm/s and 192 mm/s, respectively;


F(1,20)=20.20, P<0.001, η2=0.502). Across both groups,
the movements were performed slower when water had to
be transported than for sugar transportation (147 mm/s
versus 193 mm/s; F(1,20)=117.89, P<0.001, η2=0.855), and
when the bowl was small rather than large (157 mm/s
Wrist displacement (% transport distance)

versus 183 mm/s; F(1,20)=55.87, P<0.001, η2=0.736).


Mean tangential wrist velocity (mm/s)
Peak tangential wrist velocity (mm/s)

Number of zero-crossings per second

A step-down analysis of the interaction between


Substance, Bowl and Group (F(1,20)=4.84, P=0.04,
*P<0.05, **P<0.01, ***P<0.001

η2=0.195) showed that the interaction between Substance


and Bowl was only significant for the control group
(F(1,11)=32.35, P<0.001, η2=0.746). In that group the
difference in mean wrist velocity between the water
condition and the sugar condition was larger when the
substance was transported to the large bowl (difference
53 mm/s; F(1,11)=74.03, P<0.001, η2=0.871) rather than
the small bowl (difference 38 mm/s; F(1,11)=66.52,
P<0.001, η2=0.858).
191

the adopted emptying strategy (see Table 3). The distance


that was covered by the wrist relative to the transport
distance of the spoon was taken as a measure of this
strategy. In two cases, the distance that was covered by the
wrist was shorter than the transport distance of the spoon:
when the bowl of the spoon was extended further to empty
the spoon (which was easier when the spoon was held in a
precision grip), and when the spoon was emptied closer to
the front edge of the bowl. It appeared that the relative
distance that was covered by the wrist was smaller when
sugar was transported than for water transportation
(F(1,20)=40.67, P<0.001, η2=0.670), and when the bowl
Fig. 3 Mean tangential wrist velocity for each group separately in was large rather than small (F(1,20)=18.69, P<0.001,
three successive parts of the substance-transporting phase. Error bars η2=0.483). However, no difference was found between
represent between-subjects variability (SDs) the control group and the tetraparetic group.

Mean tangential wrist velocity in the initial, middle,


and final part of the substance-transporting phase Trunk recruitment

To further examine the course of wrist velocity during the Total trunk displacement and trunk rotation
substance-transporting phase, we divided this phase into
three equal parts on the basis of the distance covered by It was verified that the participants with spastic tetraparesis
the wrist. It appeared that the mean tangential velocity of displaced their trunk more than the control participants
the wrist was highest in the middle part of the substance- during the substance-transporting phase (F(1,20)=6.49,
transporting phase and lower in the initial and final part P=0.019, η2=0.245; see Fig. 4), as we expected on the
(F(2,40)=153.50, P<0.001, η2=0.885), as is usually the case basis of previous research. The tetraparetic group
in regular reaching tasks (see Fig. 3). In both groups the displaced the trunk on average 11.8 mm, which equates
difference in wrist velocity between the initial part and with an average of 11.2% of the displacement of the wrist.
middle part, and between the middle and final part were The control group showed an average trunk displacement
statistically significant (statistics respectively of 4.7 mm, which equates with an average of 5.5% of the
F(1,11)=324.58, P<0.001, η2=0.967 and F(1,11)=178.68, displacement of the wrist. As a control measure, we
P<0.001, η2=0.942 for the control group, and F(1,9)=86.68, examined trunk displacement prior to the substance-
P<0.001, η2=0.906 and F(1,9)=33.47, P<0.001, η2=0.788 transporting phase (reaching towards and picking up the
for the tetraparetic group). No significant difference was spoon), which also was larger in the tetraparetic group
found between the wrist velocity in the initial part and that than in the control group (21.3 mm and 7.3 mm,
in the final part. Furthermore, it was shown that a respectively; F(1,20)=4.83, P=0.040, η2=0.194).
difference in wrist velocity between the two groups existed The within-subjects standard deviation of the trunk
in all three parts (part 1: F(1,20)=9.99, P=0.005, η2=0.333; displacement in the substance-transporting phase was
part 2: F(1,20)=17.54, P<0.001, η2=0.467; part 3: larger for the tetraparetic group than for the control
F(1,20)=23.20, P<0.001, η2=0.537). group, but this was probably a consequence of the larger
mean displacement. This was substantiated by the absence
of any difference between groups for the within-subjects
Wrist displacement as a percentage of the transport coefficient of variation for trunk displacement.
distance of the spoon An important finding was that the participants were able
to tune the amount of trunk displacement to the various
Participants were left completely free with respect to the precision requirements. Averaged across groups, the trunk
way in which the spoon was emptied in to the bowl. Here, displacement was larger when the substance had to be
we examined the effects of Group, Substance and Bowl on transported to the small bowl rather than to the large bowl

Table 3 Means and between-subject standard deviations (in midpoint of the bowl in the substance-transporting phase: effects of
parentheses) of mean tangential wrist velocity and wrist displace- factors Substance (sugar, water) and Bowl (large, small)
ment as percentage of the distance between spoonholder and
Control participants Tetraparetic participants
Sugar Water Sugar Water
Large Small Large Small Large Small Large Small

Mean tangential wrist velocity (mm/s) 230 (32) 199 (20) 177 (17) 161 (19) 182 (39) 152 (37) 133 (32) 105 (31)
Wrist displacement (% transport distance) 57 (15) 63 (10) 64 (13) 71 (10) 66 (12) 69 (10) 70 (14) 75 (9)
192

(F(1,20)=8.22, P=0.010, η2=0.291). It appeared that the


effect of Substance was only present in the tetraparetic
group (F(1,9)=20.69, P=0.001, η2=0.697).
Besides a larger trunk displacement, the participants
with spastic tetraparesis also showed a larger trunk
rotation than the control participants during the sub-
stance-transporting phase (4.5° and 3.3°, respectively;
F(1,20)=5.13, P=0.035, η2=0.204). As can be observed in
Fig. 4, the difference between the ipsilateral and the
contralateral shoulder displacement was larger in the
tetraparetic group.

Fig. 4 Sagittal displacement of the ipsilateral and the contralateral


shoulder (in millimetres) during the substance-transporting phase, The distribution of trunk displacement across the
for each group separately. Error bars represent between-subjects initial, middle and final part of the substance-
variability (SDs) transporting phase

To examine the time-course of trunk involvement during


the transporting phase, we examined the trunk displace-
ment in each of the three parts of the substance-
transporting phase (initial, middle and final part). As
shown in Fig. 6, the involvement of the trunk decreased
from the initial to the middle part of the substance-
transporting phase and then increased from the middle to
the final part for both groups. A main effect of Part
(F(2,40)=9.72, P<0.001, η2=0.327) was found, as well as an
interaction between Part and Group (F(2,40)=5.04,
P=0.011, η2=0.201). In the control group the displacement
was 2.7 mm in the initial part, 0.5 mm in the middle part
and 1.4 mm in the final part of the movement (note that
these values are not cumulative but represent distances
Fig. 5 Sagittal trunk displacement (in millimetres) during the covered in each part separately). Since especially the last
substance-transporting phase as a function of Substance (sugar,
water), Bowl (large, small) and Group (control, tetraparetic two displacements are very short, it must be kept in mind
subjects). Error bars above the mean bars represent between- that they could have been a result of skin displacement
subjects variability (SDs) and error bars within the bars represent the during task performance. Nevertheless, step-down ana-
mean within-subject variability lyses showed that in this group the difference between the
initial part and middle part, and between the middle and
final part were statistically significant (statistics respec-
tively F(1,11)=19.18, P=0.001, η2=0.635 and F(1,11)=13.89,
P=0.003, η2=0.558). In the tetraparetic group the distances
covered by the trunk were 3.8 mm, 2.3 mm and 5.6 mm, in
the initial, middle, and final part, respectively. The trunk
displacement did not differ between the initial and middle
part, while this displacement increased during the final
part (F(1,9)=9.33, P=0.014, η2=0.509). Furthermore, the
difference between the two groups only existed in the
middle and final part of the substance-transporting phase
(F(1,20)=4.49, P=0.047, η2=0.183 and Welch’s
W(1,11.2)=10.91, P<0.01, respectively), whereas the differ-
Fig. 6 Sagittal displacement of the trunk (in millimetres) for each
ence between the groups was not statistically significant in
group separately in three successive parts of the substance- the initial part.
transporting phase. Error bars represent between-subjects variability
(SDs)
Relationship between trunk displacement and mean
(10.5 mm and 5.3 mm, respectively; F(1,20)=32.93, tangential wrist velocity
P<0.001, η2=0.622) and when water had to be transported
rather than sugar (9.6 mm and 6.2 mm, respectively; To determine the relationship between the sagittal trunk
F(1,20)=25.04, P<0.001, η2=0.556; see Fig. 5). However, displacement and the mean tangential wrist velocity,
an interaction between Substance and Group was found Pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated for
193
Table 4 Correlations between Subject ID Correlation coefficient Trunk displacement (mm) Mean tangential wrist velocity (mm/s)
sagittal trunk displacement and
mean tangential wrist velocity in Mean (SD) Range Mean (SD) Range
the substance-transporting phase
(across all 180 trials) for each Control participants
control and tetraparetic partici- 1 −0.18* 1 (5) −12 to 27 164 (27) 105 to 272
pant
2 −0.53*** 5 (7) −8 to 38 211 (36) 136 to 307
3 −0.28*** 5 (4) −4 to 16 204 (38) 144 to 308
4 −0.32*** 5 (3) −3 to 13 169 (31) 123 to 284
5 −0.63*** 2 (5) −11 to 18 166 (32) 97 to 262
6 −0.19** −1 (2) −6 to 4 213 (27) 138 to 281
7 −0.63*** 4 (4) −5 to 16 196 (34) 130 to 278
8 −0.19* 0 (2) −8 to 6 180 (26) 125 to 242
9 −0.22** 4 (5) −5 to 20 184 (35) 103 to 286
10 +0.17* 0 (2) −5 to 7 198 (21) 140 to 260
11 −0.31*** 15 (10) −1 to 50 217 (61) 118 to 337
12 −0.07 16 (12) −4 to 52 203 (30) 140 to 289
Tetraparetic participants
1 −0.28*** 10 (7) −1 to 36 108 (27) 59 to 199
2 −0.26*** 2 (3) −13 to 15 152 (18) 114 to 201
3 −0.63*** 16 (9) −5 to 40 142 (42) 63 to 267
4 −0.53*** 20 (13) −3 to 66 125 (50) 43 to 228
5 −0.47*** 22 (15) −1 to 93 141 (38) 63 to 230
6 −0.40*** 17 (13) −11 to 75 162 (45) 79 to 309
7 +0.31*** −2 (7) −20 to 20 168 (55) 78 to 302
8 −0.46*** 12 (9) −1 to 59 115 (37) 51 to 197
9 −0.07 14 (15) −20 to 65 210 (42) 102 to 301
*P<0.05, **P<0.01, 10 −0.26*** 7 (5) −7 to 29 107 (31) 42 to 194
***P<0.001

each participant separately across all 180 trials. Significant corroborated previous findings in a healthy subject
negative correlations between trunk displacement and population (cf. Van der Kamp and Steenbergen 1999).
mean wrist velocity were found for 10 of 12 control Our main question was whether the larger trunk
participants (one-tailed sign-test, P=0.019) and for 8 of 10 recruitment commonly found in individuals with a paresis
tetraparetic participants (one-tailed sign-test, P=0.055), as a result of CP represents a fixed movement pattern or,
indicating that a decrease in the speed of moving alternatively, might be regarded as a flexible adaptation to
coincided with an increase in trunk displacement. On deal with the combination of disorder and task constraints.
average this correlation was approximately −0.30 in both Overall, participants with tetraparesis showed an increase
groups (see Table 4). in trunk involvement compared to control participants.
Although absolute values of trunk displacement (and
rotation) were small, when taken relative to wrist
Discussion displacement trunk displacement in the tetraparetic group
was more than double that of the control group (11.2%
In this study, we examined how the damaged nervous versus 5.5%). This finding extends previous findings on
system of individuals with tetraparesis as a result of CP trunk involvement in individuals with unilateral brain
deals with increased accuracy constraints. For this damage, either from CP (cf. Steenbergen et al. 2000; Van
purpose, we studied spoon use because this task allowed Thiel and Steenbergen 2001) or from stroke (cf. Cirstea
us to manipulate accuracy demands both during transport and Levin 2000; Levin et al. 2002), to individuals with
(by variations in the substance to be transported) and at the more diffuse damage of the brain (tetraparetic patients).
end of the task (by variations in the size of the bowl in Hence, a general reorganization may involve an increased
which the spoon needed to be emptied). The effect of the involvement of the trunk.
latter accuracy constraint on the mean wrist velocity is in Two additional remarks must be made to put this
line with reaching and grasping studies that also examined finding into perspective. First, we showed that prior to the
the effects of accuracy constraints at the end of the substance-transporting phase, the tetraparetic participants
movement (Marteniuk et al. 1990; Bootsma et al. 1994; already displayed a larger trunk involvement than the
Berthier et al. 1996; Weir et al. 1998). In addition, the control participants did. This means that a larger
effects of manipulation of accuracy during the task anticipatory trunk displacement which might have been
present in the control group can be ruled out as an
194

explanation for the larger trunk displacement in the Therefore, variations in movement speed did not exclu-
tetraparetic group in the substance-transporting phase. sively account for variations in trunk involvement. How-
Second, as explained in the Methods section, the bowl was ever, it has to be pointed out that this experiment was not
placed within the active range of motion for each designed to test this specific hypothesis, as we did not
participant and trunk recruitment was therefore not explicitly manipulate movement speed. To be able to draw
necessary for the performance of the task. more definite conclusions on this matter, a wider range of
The effects of the requirement for accuracy on trunk velocities should be investigated. Are there alternative
involvement were straightforward. Increasing the accuracy explanations for the increased trunk involvement?
constraint at the end of the task caused an increased trunk One factor that might have influenced trunk involve-
displacement for both the control and the tetraparetic ment is the emptying strategy that participants adopted.
participants. Consequently, rather than displacing the trunk The emptying strategy entails both the way in which the
a fixed, standardized distance, tetraparetic participants spoon is oriented before emptying (cf. Roby-Brami 1997)
flexibly tuned the degree of trunk involvement to the and, especially relevant for the large bowl, the location at
accuracy demands imposed, as has previously been shown which the spoon is emptied (e.g. close to the front edge of
in control participants (cf. Steenbergen et al. 1995; the bowl). The different emptying strategies were reflected
Mackey et al. 2000). Whereas the accuracy constraint by the distance that was covered by the wrist relative to the
during the movement had no effect on trunk involvement transport distance of the spoon. Most participants used
for the control participants, participants with tetraparesis precision grips, which make it easier to extend the bowl of
increased trunk involvement when transporting water. In the spoon further. The distance that was covered by the
sum, tetraparetic participants dealt with increased accuracy wrist relative to the transport distance was on average
constraints by an increased use of the trunk. This leads us 67%, indicating that the participants used the length of the
to conclude that trunk involvement in this group may be stem of the spoon to shorten the distance that had to be
regarded as a flexible adaptation to the imposed task covered by the wrist. Importantly, no difference in
demands. Note that the preferred hand of all tetraparetic emptying strategy was found between the two groups,
participants was relatively mildly impaired, although they although, in general, the relative wrist displacement was
made slower movements, needed more time decelerating, larger when water was transported and when the bowl was
and moved less fluently than the control participants. They small. Nevertheless, these findings can only partly account
were all able to perform the task without spilling. It for the larger trunk displacement in these conditions,
therefore remains to be seen whether this pattern of results because the trunk displacement was not necessary to
will hold true when more severely affected people are empty the spoon into the bowl, as was assessed prior to the
tested. start of the experiment. Combining this finding with the
To further examine the nature of trunk involvement, we observed relationship between speed and trunk displace-
also analysed its time-course. In both groups, the ment leads us to the following explanation: when larger
displacement was largest during the initial and the final distances are covered by the wrist in the more complex
part and smallest during the middle part, although in the conditions (small bowl, water), participants move their
tetraparetic group the decrease in trunk displacement from hand for a longer time at a lower velocity, leading to a
the initial to the middle part was not significant. One larger trunk displacement.
reason for this distribution of trunk displacement might be Another factor that may have influenced trunk involve-
the speed of the movement in the three parts of the task. ment is the increased cocontraction of agonist/antagonist
When mean wrist velocity was highest (in the middle part muscle pairs, which is a typical symptom accompanying
of the substance-transporting phase), the trunk displace- spasticity in individuals with CP (O’Sullivan et al. 1998).
ment was smallest. In line with this, correlations between As a result of the increased limb stiffness, the functional
mean wrist velocity and trunk displacement were mainly range of motion during task performance in the elbow and
negative in both groups (10 of 12 control participants, and shoulder joints might be reduced. Increasing the displace-
8 of 10 tetraparetic participants), indicating there was a ment of the trunk might compensate for this decreased
clear tendency for increased trunk use with lower move- functional range of motion. Indeed, it was recently shown
ment speed. A combination of these findings might, at in a healthy subject population that cocontraction at the
least partly, explain why individuals with spastic tetrapa- elbow and shoulder increases when pointing movements
resis displace their trunk more than healthy individuals, have to be made towards small targets rather than large
and also why trunk displacement increases when accuracy ones (Gribble et al. 2003). Gribble and colleagues
demands increase. If a task becomes more complex (e.g. as suggested that cocontraction might be a strategy employed
a result of motor and somatosensory problems or higher by the motor system to increase arm movement accuracy.
accuracy demands or both), the movement is performed An electromyographic study is currently underway in our
more slowly. As a consequence of that, the trunk is lab to examine whether the same strategy is used by
recruited more (see also Rosenbaum et al. 1991, 1995; individuals with spasticity.
Vaughan et al. 1996; Wang and Stelmach 2001). A note of A final factor for the increased trunk use may be sought
caution must be made here. The correlation coefficients in the need to reduce variability of the end-effector
that we found were not very large and we even found a trajectory, which is most prominent for high accuracy
positive correlation for one participant in each group. tasks (cf. Cirstea and Levin 2000). Our finding that the
195

displacement of the trunk was largest in the tetraparetic Levin MF, Michaelsen SM, Cirstea CM, Roby-Brami A (2002) Use
participants in the final part of the movement might of the trunk for reaching targets placed within and beyond the
reach in adult hemiparesis. Exp Brain Res 143:171–180
provide evidence for this since the instability of the end- Mackey DC, Bertram CP, Mason AH, Marteniuk RG, Mackenzie
effector is assumed to be largest near the end of the CL (2000) The effect of task complexity on trunk-assisted
movement. In this final part, the elbow is most extended reaching. J Sport Exerc Psychol 22:S73–S74
and, due to the muscular weakness in these individuals, Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Jeannerod M, Athenes S, Dugas C
(1987) Constraints on human arm movement trajectories. Can J
movements of the end effector may then become more Psychol 41:365–378
variable. However, with the present set-up we could not Marteniuk RG, Leavitt JL, MacKenzie CL, Athenes S (1990)
draw any definite conclusion with respect to this point and Functional relationships between grasp and transport compo-
further research is needed to examine whether fixating the nents in a prehension task. Hum Mov Sci 9:149–176
Maxwell SE, Delaney HD (1990) Designing experiments and
trunk will increase the end-effector dysfluency in this analyzing data: a model comparison perspective. Brooks/Cole,
group. Pacific Grove CA
Summing up, similar to healthy people, tetraparetic O’Sullivan MC, Miller S, Ramesh V, Conway K, Gilfillan K,
individuals flexibly tune the amount of trunk involvement McDonough S, Eyre JA (1998) Abnormal development of
to variations in accuracy demands, despite the somatosen- biceps brachii phasic stretch reflex and persistence of short
latency heteronymous reflexes from biceps to triceps brachii in
sory and motor problems that these individuals have to spastic cerebral palsy. Brain 121:2381–2395
cope with. An increased trunk displacement was found Roby-Brami A, Fuchs S, Mokhtari M, Bussel B (1997) Reaching
when accuracy constraints of the task were higher. We and grasping strategies in hemiparetic patients. Motor Control
therefore conclude that the increased trunk involvement in 1:72–91
Rosenbaum DA, Slotta JD, Vaughan J, Plamondon R (1991)
individuals with mild tetraparesis as a consequence of CP Optimal movement selection. Psychol Sci 2:86–91
should not be regarded as a primary symptom of the Rosenbaum DA, Loukopoulos LD, Meulenbroek RGJ, Vaughan J,
disorder. Engelbrecht SE (1995) Planning reaches by evaluating stored
postures. Psychol Rev 102:28–67
Sanger TD, Delgado MR, Gaebler-Spira D, Hallett M, Mink JW
Acknowledgements We thank all volunteers who participated in (2003) Classification and definition of disorders causing
this study, Marcel Mutsaarts for his assistance in carrying out the hypertonia in childhood. Pediatrics 111:e89–e97
experiment, and Chris Bouwhuisen for his technical support. This Steenbergen B, Meulenbroek RGJ (2003) Signs of long-term
research was supported by a grant awarded by The Netherlands adaptation to permanent brain damage as revealed by prehen-
Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) to the second author sion studies of children with spastic hemiparesis. In: Latash M,
for the research project ‘Adaptation in movement disorder’. Levin MF (eds) Progress in motor control III. Human Kinetics
Publishers, Champaign IL, pp 207–234
Steenbergen B, Marteniuk RG, Kalbfleisch LE (1995) Achieving
coordination in prehension: joint freezing and postural
References contributions. J Mot Behav 27:333–348
Steenbergen B, Van Thiel E, Hulstijn W, Meulenbroek, RGJ (2000)
Albright AL (1996) Spasticity and movement disorders in cerebral The coordination of reaching and grasping in spastic hemipar-
palsy. J Child Neurol 11:S1–S4 esis. Hum Mov Sci 19:75–105
Barnes M, McLellan L, Sutton R (1994) Spasticity. In: Greenwood Sugden DA, Keogh JF (1990) Problems in movement skill
RJ, Barnes MP, McMillan TM, Ward CD (eds) Neurological development. University of South Carolina Press, Columbia
rehabilitation. Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, pp 161–172 Tiffin J (1968) Purdue Pegboard examiner manual. Science
Berthier NE, Clifton RK, Gullapalli V, McCall DD, Robin DJ Research Associates, Chicago
(1996) Visual information and object size in the control of Van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B (1999) The kinematics of eating
reaching. J Mot Behav 28:187–197 with a spoon: bringing the food to the mouth, or the mouth to
Bootsma RJ, Marteniuk RG, MacKenzie CL, Zaal FTJM (1994) The the food? Exp Brain Res 129:68–76
speed-accuracy trade-off in manual prehension: effects of Van Roon DB, Van der Kamp J, Steenbergen B (2003) Constraints
movement amplitude, object size and object width on kinematic in children’s learning to use spoons. In: Savelsbergh GJP,
characteristics. Exp Brain Res 98:535–541 Davids K, Van der Kamp J, Bennet S (eds) Development of
Cirstea MC, Levin MF (2000) Compensatory strategies for reaching movement coordination in children: applications in the field of
in stroke. Brain 123:940–953 ergonomics, health sciences and sport. Taylor and Francis,
Cooper J, Majnemer A, Rosenblatt B, Birnbaum R (1995) The London, pp 75–93
determination of sensory deficits in children with hemiplegic Van Thiel E, Steenbergen B (2001) Shoulder and hand displace-
cerebral palsy. J Child Neurol 10:300–309 ments during hitting, reaching and grasping movements in
Filloux FM (1996) Neuropathophysiology of movement disorders in hemiparetic cerebral palsy. Motor Control 2:166–182
cerebral palsy. J Child Neurol 11:S5–S12 Vaughan J, Rosenbaum DA, Diedrich FJ, Moore CM (1996)
Gribble PL, Mullin LI, Cothros N, Mattar A (2003) Role of Cooperative selection of movements: the optimal selection
cocontraction in arm movement accuracy. J Neurophysiol model. Psychol Res 58:254–273
89:2396–2405 Wang J, Stelmach GE (2001) Spatial and temporal control of trunk-
Lance JW (1980) Symposium synopsis. In: Feldman RG, Young assisted prehensile actions. Exp Brain Res 136:231–240
RR, Koella WP (eds) Spasticity: disordered motor control, Weir PL, MacDonald JR, Mallat BJ, Leavitt JL, Roy EA (1998)
Yearbook Medical, Chicago, pp 485–494 Age-related differences in prehension: the influence of task
Latash ML, Anson JG (1996) What are “normal movements” in goals. J Mot Behav 30:79–89
atypical populations? Behav Brain Sci 19:55–106

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen