Sie sind auf Seite 1von 19

This article was downloaded by: [University Library Utrecht] On: 18 November 2013, At: 08:46 Publisher: Routledge

Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rady20

The role of music in adolescent development: much more than the same old song
Dave Miranda
a a

School of Psychology, University of Ottawa , Ottawa , Ontario , Canada Published online: 21 Feb 2012.

To cite this article: Dave Miranda (2013) The role of music in adolescent development: much more than the same old song, International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 18:1, 5-22, DOI: 10.1080/02673843.2011.650182 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2011.650182

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/termsand-conditions

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 2013 Vol. 18, No. 1, 522, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02673843.2011.650182

The role of music in adolescent development: much more than the same old song
Dave Miranda*
School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada (Received 1 December 2011; nal version received 13 December 2011)

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

There is an increasingly robust literature of recent research ndings that support the developmental importance of music in adolescence. However, this intriguing literature is not familiar to many developmental psychologists, possibly due to a lack of communication among researchers and because of publication trends in developmental journals. This review aims at informing on current knowledge of how music listening can play a role in the psychosocial development of adolescents. To this end, three arguments are discussed in light of recent empirical research: music inuences important aspects of adolescent development; music can represent a protective and a risk factor; and music can serve as an adjunct component in prevention and intervention. Therefore, it is proposed how music is a developmental resource in adolescence. It is argued that research on the developmental role of music can create a window to the everyday psychological, social, and cultural needs of contemporary adolescents. Keywords: music; development; adolescence; psychosocial; literature review

Introduction Music can have many social psychological impacts and meanings for people at different periods of their development (Hargreaves, 1986; McPherson, 2006; North & Hargreaves, 2008). It can be a mothers lullaby, an artists exploration and expression, a performers dream and profession, a listeners passion and leisure, a social settings ambience and a signier of ritual. Music is a resource of considerable intellectual, artistic, cultural, technological, and economical breadth and depth. Billions of dollars are invested in and generated by music across many domains (e.g. arts, entertainment, education, science), while serious legal consequences and societal issues concern the unauthorised sharing of music (North & Hargreaves, 2008). Young people, especially, devote huge amounts of time and money to music listening (Roberts, Henriksen, & Foehr, 2009), while the technological industry cleverly attunes music applications to resonate at the core of their multi-tasking computers. Listening to music is thus particularly important and ubiquitous in the contemporary lives of media-socialising and multi-tasking adolescents (Brown & Bobkowski, 2011; Roberts et al., 2009). This article will thereby argue that music has become a genuine developmental resource young people would say a killer app that deserves much more attention from developmental psychology in adolescence.

*Email: dave.miranda@uottawa.ca
q 2013 Taylor & Francis

D. Miranda

But can music have any signicant inuence on individuals? At the outset, developmental psychologists may wonder whether music has any signicant inuence on some of the biological, psychological, and social factors that compose human nature. The answer seems to be yes; and prototypical examples can be provided.

Biological effects From an evolutionary perspective, music could be an evolved psychological mechanism in as much as it seems to have been potentially adaptive for increasing our tness for survival in terms of better mate selection, social cohesion, synchronised group effort, perceptual development, motor skill development, conict reduction, safe time passing, transgenerational communication of culture, and self-regulation (Huron, 2003; McDermott & Hauser, 2005). From a contemporary perspective, music is mediated by and impacts some of our biological structures and processes (Peretz & Zatorre, 2003). Findings from brain lesion and neuroimaging studies indicate that musical processing is embedded within a complex network of cortical and subcortical pathways (Peretz & Zatorre, 2005). Music activates neurotransmitters involved in pleasure (dopamine; Menon & Levitin, 2005), modulates hormones involved in stress (cortisol; Khalfa, Dalla Bella, Roy, Peretz, & Lupien, 2003), and social bonding (oxytocin; Nilsson, 2009). Research also reveals analgesic effects for music (Mitchell & MacDonald, 2006).

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

Psychological effects The psychological effects of music are obviously discussed throughout this article. But emotions are the quintessential example. In fact, the Oxford English Dictionary denes music as That one of the ne arts which is concerned with the combination of sounds with a view to beauty of form and the expression of emotion . . . (1989, p. 126). Hence, it seems that common sense has explicitly taken for granted that music is mainly an emotional experience. The psychology of music and emotions helps to better understand emotions as synchronised responses (cognitive appraisal, subjective feeling, physiological response, expression, action tendency and regulation) to changes in the environment (Juslin, m, Va stfja ll, Barradas, & Silva, 2008). This area of research also contributes to Liljestro our understanding of how musical emotions interact with key psychological phenomena (e.g. cognitions, aesthetics, motivation, performance, creativity, personality, social behaviours, health, and cross-cultural similarities and differences; Juslin & Sloboda, 2010). Experimental methods developed by the psychology of music offer sound methodological and ethical strategies to induce and manipulate strong emotions in stfja ll, 2008). Correlational studies are using experience laboratory settings (Juslin & Va sampling methods to shed light on how music can elicit complex patterns of positive and negative emotions during the unfolding of our daily lives (Juslin et al., 2008). Worthy of note, this eld of studies is advancing our knowledge about the mechanisms explaining how music induces emotions (e.g. brain stem reexes, evaluative conditioning, emotional stfja ll, contagion, visual imagery, episodic memory, and musical expectancy; Juslin & Va 2008). Furthermore, a promising research direction is that music can serve the adaptive ` -i-Freixanet, Furnham, & purpose of emotion regulation (Chamorro-Premuzic, Goma Muro, 2009).

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

Social effects Music has social effects which are so noticeable that Hargreaves and North have claimed music has many different functions in human life, nearly all of which are essentially social (1997, p. 1). In infancy, lullabies promote a fundamental social bond, namely maternal attachment (Milligan, Atkinson, Trehub, Benoit, & Poulton, 2003). The potency with which music stimulates and modulates interpersonal relationships in social events (e.g. concerts, sporting events, parties, dates, dances, ceremonies, rallies, dinners) has led authors to refer to it as a social lubricant (for example, Lewis, 2002, p. 364). When individuals become acquainted, they often use music preferences to manage social impressions, evaluate each others similarity, and subtly acquire a social perception about the personality and values of the person they meet (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2006). For instance, music tastes are commonly used as sensible social matching criteria on the Internet (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2006). Music tastes can thereby be utilised as a badge about ones personality and social status (North & Hargreaves, 2008). People also develop social schemas and even stereotypes about fans of different music genres (Rentfrow, McDonald, & Oldmeadow, 2009), and this can inuence intergroup dynamics by shaping in-groups and out-groups that are based on different music tastes (Bakagiannis & Tarrant, 2006). Nevertheless, the implications of this vast and rich psychology of music literature are not generally accessed in developmental psychology. At least two factors can explain why developmental psychologists can underestimate the importance of music in adolescence: lack of scientic communication; and trends of publications on music in developmental journals. Lack of scientic communication Throughout history, the importance of music was noticed by many erudite thinkers (e.g. Aristotle, Confucius, Einstein, Nietzsche, Plato, Sartre). Charles Darwin (1871) noted the emotional potency of music, its usefulness for courtship among animals, and he hypothesised that music had probably been adaptive for sexual selection during human evolution. William James had also hypothesised that music was adaptive in terms of selfregulation when he suggested that . . . the habit of excessive indulgence in music . . . has probably a relaxing effect upon the character (1890/1950, pp. 125 126). In the rst part of the twentieth century, music developed into a signicant area of research in psychology. Scholarly books were already published, for instance The Social Psychology of Music by Paul R. Farnsworth in 1954. Professional organisations also convened; for instance, the National Association for Music Therapy (nowadays the American Music Therapy Association) had been established in 1950. Surprisingly, during this period, Cattell and Saunders maintained that . . . one is surprised to nd in the history of psychology and psychotherapy so little experimental, or even speculative, reference to the use of music in psychiatry (1954, p. 3). However, 17 years before, Mursell (1937) had already published an entire book entitled The Psychology of Music citing 605 references and from which he claimed that During the past forty or fty years a great many investigations relating to the psychological aspects and problems of music have been carried on in many centers throughout the world (1937, p. 9). It is paradoxical that mainstream psychologists so easily underscored the lack of research, whereas music psychologists were enthused by the wealth of studies. At the time, Mursell (1937) explained that the problem was not the lack of studies on music, but that they remained unfamiliar to mainstream psychologists. Nowadays, this lack of communication among researchers seems to be lingering and may partially

D. Miranda

explain why developmental psychologists can often underestimate or simply take for granted the role of music listening in adolescence. For instance, the most authoritative handbooks and textbooks on adolescence will typically allude to the obvious fascination of adolescents for music, yet without devoting much scientic scrutiny to it (for example, Lerner & Steinberg, 2009a, 2009b; Steinberg, 2011). This is very surprising given the notable publishing initiatives that have recently promoted a sound developmental psychology of music in childhood, adolescence, and youth (for example, Hargreaves, 1986; McPherson, 2006).

Trends of publications on music in developmental journals

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

The turning of the twenty-rst century has witnessed the blossoming of sophisticated and diversied studies on music, especially in cognitive/affective neuroscience (Peretz & Zatorre, 2003), child/educational psychology (McPherson, 2006), and social psychology (North & Hargreaves, 2008). Prominent researchers are even publishing bestselling books that popularise cutting-edge scientic work in the cognitive neuroscience of music (for example, Levitin, 2008; Sacks, 2007). The PsycINFO database, an authoritative archive of most references to scientic research in psychology, was consulted in order to describe the trends of publications on music in peer-reviewed journals over the last 150 years. In the PsycINFO database (1806 to third week of May 2011), the keyword music generated 12,934 references from peerreviewed journals. From the distribution of these references across decades, Figure 1 clearly shows that the last decade (2001 2010) has seen the sharpest increase in scientic publications on the psychology of music. Nonetheless, music seems to be relatively slower at becoming a major theme of research in developmental psychology. Figure 2 presents the mere 78 references about music that PsycINFO (1806 to third week of May 2011) identied in specialised journals on adolescence (Adolescence, International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, Journal of Adolescence, Journal of Adolescent Heath, Journal of Adolescent Research, Journal of Early Adolescence, Journal of Research on Adolescence, and Journal of

Figure 1. Trends of publications on music in psychology. Source: Data (number of references) from the PsycINFO database, 2011.

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

Figure 2. Trends of publications on music in developmental journals. Source: Data (number of publications) from the PsycINFO database, 2011.

Youth and Adolescence). From these, an early article was that of Santiago (1969), which discussed how the Beatles songs were pertinent to understand and communicate with adolescents during therapy. Interestingly, Figure 2 shows that in the last decade (2001 2010) there was still a moderate increase of studies on music among journals specialised in adolescence. Figure 2 also presents 99 references about music, which PsycINFO identied in some of the major journals of developmental psychology (British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Child Development, Developmental Psychology, Developmental Review, Developmental Science, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, European Journal of Developmental Psychology, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Infancy, Infant and Child Development, Infant Behavior and Development, and International Journal of Behavioral Development). In the 1930s, Child Development by itself had already published seven articles involving music. For instance, Fitchen (1931) presented a case study on the music development of a child. Overall, as can be seen from Figure 2, the last decade (2001 2010) has again seen a moderate increase in music studies among journals on developmental psychology. Therefore, in absolute terms, there is a steady yet disappointedly modest increase of studies on music in journals specialised in adolescence and development. It may be that music psychologists prefer to publish in their own specialist journals instead of developmental journals. Still, it may also be that developmental editors and reviewers have rarely been provided with sufcient evidence in favour of publishing articles on the developmental signicance of music in adolescence. Some researchers may argue that these dozens of references to music in adolescence are quite enough considering the peripheral nature of music for serious developmental psychologists. Such a point of view would be a false note, of course. To the contrary, it is unfortunate that so little developmental research has been conducted on music in youth, especially when considering the millions of songs that are being sold to adolescents, as well as the billions of hours that they devote listening to music. In fact, music is still considered a novel and intriguing theme of research in developmental psychology, whereas it is business as usual for a youth music industry that has long developed an impressive repertoire of artistic, technological, and economical expertise. In other words, a more prolic developmental

10

D. Miranda

psychology of music would allow mainstream developmental psychology to catch up with the increasingly musical lives of contemporary adolescents. A developmental psychology of music in adolescence A developmental psychology of music in adolescence investigates the inuence that musical behaviours, emotions, cognitions, and motives can have on normative and positive development, as well as psychopathology (Miranda & Gaudreau, 2011). In the last 20 years, studies have gradually documented the signicance of music in adolescence (North & Hargreaves, 2008; Zillmann & Gan, 1997). Unfortunately, this increasingly robust psychology of music literature is not generally accessed in developmental psychology. The overarching aim of this literature review is to build bridges between the psychology of music and developmental psychology in adolescence. It thereby provides a concise and representative summary of the extant literature on music in adolescence. This should inform developmental psychologists on how music listening is a resource that plays a role in adolescent development. In light of this, along with references to recent empirical ndings, three arguments will be developed in support of the pertinence of music as a theme of research in adolescent psychology. First, music can inuence key aspects of adolescent development. This central argument presents theoretical implications and draws research directions. Second, music can represent a protective and a risk factor, which is pertinent for researchers studying developmental psychopathology. Third, music can serve as an adjunct component in prevention and intervention, which is of particular interest to clinical psychologists and those working in prevention science for adolescents. Music is important for the development of many adolescents Adolescence is a period of transition and plasticity from childhood to adulthood, in which transactions between a self-determined (and predisposed) individual and his/her changing (and stable) social environment are intertwined within and across ecological systems as they cascade over time (Lerner & Steinberg, 2009a, 2009b; Masten & Cicchetti, 2010). This period of biopsychosocial reorganisation brings novel stressors, complex issues, and developmental challenges (Arnett, 1999) and most adolescents are able to adapt to this and thrive developmentally (Steinberg & Morris, 2001). Music is their soundtrack during this intense developmental period. On average, adolescents listen to music for up to three hours daily and accumulate more than 10,000 hours of active music listening throughout adolescence (Roberts et al., 2009; Tarrant, North, & Hargreaves, 2000; Zillmann & Gan, 1997). The huge amount of time they dedicate to music makes them young expert listeners. Moreover, their time spent listening to music keeps increasing thanks to ever more media-socialising and multi-tasking portable computers. This highlights the timeliness of studying music among adolescents of the Internet generation. This review is thus grounded in the assumption that adolescents self-initiated and ever more intense exposure to music occurs during a life period of plasticity in which they experience (and need to resolve) numerous developmental tasks, transitions, and issues (for example, Larson, 1995; Miranda & Claes, 2009; Schwartz & Fouts, 2003; Zillmann & Gan, 1997). This reviews central theoretical implication is thereby that such developmental timing the transaction between music and adolescence opens a critical window in which music can inuence at least seven major areas of development:

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

11

aesthetics; identity; socialisation; emotion regulation and coping; personality and motivation; gender roles; and positive youth development. Music and aesthetics Aesthetic development can pertain to how people develop their perception of beauty in great a many stimuli of life, including artistic work like music. Developmental psychology has actually understudied aesthetic and artistic development in youth (Lin & Thomas, 2002) and the psychology of aesthetics has generally focused on visual rather than musical k, Brattico, Tervaniemi, & Huotilainen, 2011). experiences (but see Nieminen, Isto Arguably, adolescents are primarily exposed to huge amounts of auditory artistic stimuli (i.e. songs) from which they may develop an increasingly mature understanding for aesthetics and the arts. They listen to music because it sounds good for its aesthetic pleasure (North, Hargreaves, & ONeill, 2000). Although some may opine that studying aesthetics for music is not a priority in adolescent psychology, ironically it is serious business in other high-prole professional areas (e.g. music industry, marketing). In adolescence, exploring music tastes can develop a sense of competency through in youth culture. In discerning the cool from the boring and the fashionable from the passe fact, adolescence seems to be a critical period for developing musical tastes as they may develop into familiar cognitive prototypes through acculturation (Hargreaves, North, & Tarrant, 2006). In fact, a 21-month longitudinal study conrmed that aesthetic appreciation for music is developed in early adolescence and that it stabilises during late adolescence (Mulder, ter Bogt, Raaijmakers, Gabhainn, & Sikkema, 2010). Hence, developmental psychology should study whether aesthetic development for music may stimulate adolescents autonomous originality and relatedness to real or perceived social norms in terms of what is considered to be artistically and creatively beautiful in a given culture and at a given time in history. Music and identity te , 2009). Adolescence is a critical period for the gradual development of identity (Co Social media can provide opportunities for adolescents to explore potential selves and develop identity (Roberts et al., 2009). Kistler, Rodgers, Power, Austin, and Hill (2010) showed that music is a source of social cognitive norms that impacts the development of adolescents self-concept. Through structural equation modelling, Kistler and collaborators (2010) found that adolescents evaluate their physical attractiveness and self-worth by comparing themselves with music media characters. Also, adolescents understand that music can be used as a resource to develop a social image (North et al., 2000). They use music as a badge that shapes their peer groups and peer crowds, which are often known as musical subcultures (Miranda & Claes, 2009; North & Hargreaves, 2008). These musical subcultures develop a youth culture identity and provide informational and normative social inuences (Zillman & Gan, 1997). Popular musicians also serve as rolemodels or even idols (Raviv, Bar-Tal, Raviv, & Ben-Horin, 1996) that can inuence adolescents through social learning (e.g. modelling). From a more sociological perspective, musical subcultures may provide resources for protest, resistance, and resilience when adolescents, as a social class, feel disrespected in their rights by adult authorities. For young ethnic minorities, music can be a resource for developing cultural identity. In fact, ones preference for music, songs, and dances from his/her heritage culture has been used as an indicator of ethnic identity (Phinney, 1990). And Saether

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

12

D. Miranda

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

(2008) found that some adolescent immigrants can sometimes use music to shape their identity and promote cultural learning. In their review of the literature, Hargreaves and collaborators (2006) noted that adolescents can use music preferences to enhance their social identity by creating exclusive group norms by which they distinguish their in-group from an out-group. A study by Bakagiannis and Tarrant (2006) also suggested that music preferences can even create a more inclusive social identity that can supersede adolescents more exclusive social backgrounds. In sum, developmental psychology should take notice that music is not only a social lubricant in adolescence (as it can be in adulthood). Music is a resource from which adolescents decide to explore possible selves, rehearse social roles, manage intergroup dynamics, and envisage future orientations (e.g. artistic careers) by observing their peers and favourite musicians. Music and socialisation Peer socialisation, friendships, as well as parents are obviously very important during adolescence (Brown & Larson, 2009; Laursen & Collins, 2009). In terms of social transition from parents to peers during adolescence, many authors theorised that music preferences promote socialisation with friends, while causing some degree of disengagement from parents (for example, Zillman & Gan, 1997), which is in line with normative development (Brown & Larson, 2009). Selfhout, Branje, ter Bogt, and Meeus (2009) have conducted a one-year longitudinal study which showed that similarity in music preferences does promote the formation of adolescent friendships. However, Miranda and Gaudreau (2011) also found that while adolescents share similar music preferences with friends, they also share music preferences with their parents. It may be that adolescents of the early twenty-rst century will increasingly share music preferences with both friends and parents without much controversy. Adolescents understand how to listen to music to please their friends (North et al., 2000), which may foster their reciprocal liking. Music tastes thus involve impression s, 1989). Even functional magnetic resonance imaging management among peers (Finna suggests that adolescents conformity in music preferences is partially mediated by neural mechanisms (Berns, Capra, Moore, & Noussair, 2010). But recent sociometric evidence reveals only a moderate degree of similarity between adolescents personal music tastes and those of their friends (Miranda & Claes, 2009). Hence, most adolescents seem to maintain personal music preferences that are not necessarily shared by friends. It is also intriguing that adolescents can report engaging in fantasising while listening to music, in which they imagine social scripts and rehearse social skills while using music as soundtrack (Miranda, Gaudreau, Morizot, & Fallu, 2012). Understandably, the capacity of music to evoke the presence of others may also explain that adolescents can report listening to music to alleviate loneliness (North et al., 2000). Thus, by studying music in adolescence, developmental psychology would learn substantially more about how adolescents can socialise with peers, friends, and parents whether real in interpersonal settings or even imagined while listening to music. Music, emotion regulation, and coping Emotion regulation and coping are primordial for adolescents to successfully adapt to developmental issues, as well as to remain resilient amid everyday stress and stressful life

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

13

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

events (Compas, 2009; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner, 2011). Adolescents feel that music is a resource that can full some of their emotional needs, notably for the purpose of , 2007). Their emotion emotion regulation (North et al., 2000; Saarikallio & Erkkila regulation through music may occur when they listen to music to distract themselves from distressing emotions, to nd solace and validation, or even to experience venting (Schwartz & Fouts, 2003). However, music is not a panacea in itself as it can relate to either adjusted (self-reection) or maladjusted (rumination) forms of emotion regulation (Greenwood & Long, 2009). Young people also listen to music to relieve tension and distract themselves from worries (Gantz, Gartenberg, Pearson, & Shiller, 1978). In other words, they use music to cope with stress. This has often been theorised but rarely operationalised. Recently, Miranda and Claes (2009) proposed a three-factor model of coping by music that measures emotional management, problem-solving, and avoidance through listening to music. It revealed that adolescents depression was linked to more emotional management and avoidance, but to less problem-solving through music listening. Thus, coping by music listening is complex as it involves both benecial and deleterious patterns of coping strategies (Miranda & Claes, 2009). Hence, developmental psychology should devote more attention to music as a developmental resource for emotion regulation and coping in adolescence. Music is abundant and ubiquitous, and these characteristics render it particularly practical for adolescents everyday management of emotions and stress. Personality and motivation Research has now mapped links between personality and musical behaviours (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003). Notably, a two-year longitudinal study indicated that traits from the vefactor model of personality (e.g. Extraversion and Openness) can predict music tastes in adolescence (Delsing, ter Bogt, Engels, & Meeus, 2008). This supports the notion that traits are dispositions that inuence adolescents musical behaviours. In turn, the sixmonth longitudinal study of Miranda, Gaudreau, and Morizot (2010) also determined that coping by music can predict the short-term development of personality traits (Neuroticism) in adolescence. These ndings move towards a transactional implication as music and personality may share reciprocal inuences. Research on motivation is essential as it addresses our reasons for listening to music. Surprisingly, this question was understudied. But recently, Chamorro-Premuzic and colleagues developed a three-factor model of motives for listening to music: emotional use, rational/cognitive use, and background use (for example, Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009). North and collaborators (2000) also found a three-factor model of reasons for listening to music among adolescents: creating social image, satisfying emotional needs, and enjoyment. Recently, ter Bogt, Mulder, Raaijmakers, and Gabhainn (2011) identied four uses of music among young people: mood enhancement, coping with problems, dening personal identity, and marking social identity. From these, they conducted a latent class analysis that identied three proles of listeners: high-involved (19.7%), mediuminvolved (74.2%), and low-involved (6.1%). These results are in keeping with cluster analyses from Miranda and Gaudreau (2011), which showed that 19.3% of adolescents were emotionally-negative listeners, 31.6% were emotionally-positive listeners, but that most (49.1%) were emotionally-limited listeners. Taken together, these personcentred ndings suggest a provocative nuance: many (but not most) adolescents are passionate about (or react passionately to) music. Nonetheless, the main functional

14

D. Miranda

implication of this line of research yet again is that emotion regulation is the primary motive for listening to music, especially in youth (Lonsdale & North, 2011). Developmental psychology would probably be able to nd unprecedented reciprocal inuences between motivation and personality if it considered adolescents reasons for listening to music as an original mediator of some of these (motivation $ personality) transactions. Music and gender differences Gender differences inuence adolescent development and the determinants of these differences involve biological, psychological, and social factors (Galambos, Berenbaum, & McHale, 2009). There seems to be gender differences in musical behaviours during adolescence. However, the evidence is more mixed than often thought. Adolescent girls can spend more time listening to music (for example, Roberts et al., 2009); however, this gender difference is not always replicable (for example, Schwartz & Fouts, 2003). It is also not clear if girls give more importance to songs lyrics because studies rarely account or control for lyrics. In terms of music preferences among adolescents, Mulder and collaborators (2010) remarked that while some gender differences remain (e.g. pop music is more liked by girls), others may gradually disappear (e.g. rock music is now equally appreciated by girls and boys). Interestingly, they observed that the longitudinal stability of music preferences does not reveal gender differences. As for adolescents personality, their Openness predicts greater music eclecticism (diversied music tastes) among both girls and boys (Miranda & Claes, 2008). Gender differences may be more pronounced with regards to motives for listening to music. Adolescent girls give more importance to fullling their emotional needs through music, whereas adolescent boys give more importance to managing their social identity with music (North et al., 2000). Also, coping by music listening seems to be used more frequently by girls than boys (Miranda & Claes, 2009). This may explain why the links between music and symptoms of mood disorder are also more signicant among girls as they could make more use of music listening as an attempt to cope with their symptoms (Miranda & Claes, 2008). Still, in terms of adolescents emotional reactions to music, Roberts, Dimsdale, East, and Friedman (1998) reported no gender differences. Moreover, Miranda and Gaudreau (2011) used a person-centred approach (cluster analysis) that yielded signicant links between emotional reactions to music and emotional well-being, and these links remained similar among girls and boys. Musical activities can also impact adolescents gender-role development, for instance through gender stereotyping (ONeill, 1997). In their review, Brown and Bobkowski (2011) point out that media provide models, scripts, expectations, and stereotypes about romantic and sexual relationships in youth. Media could be an educational resource for developing progressive gender-role attitudes, but unfortunately, it can also perpetuate conventional gender roles in youth (Brown & Bobkowski, 2011). For instance, using a multivariate structural analysis among adolescents, ter Bogt, Engels, Bogers, and Kloosterman (2010) found that different music preferences could be associated either with more or less gender stereotypes. In their literature review, North and Hargreaves (2008) report studies showing that those songs lyrics that convey gender stereotypes may encourage gender stereotyping and even sexism during youth. In sum, developmental psychology should address the impact that songs can have on the development of gender roles and stereotypes in adolescence. Moreover, gender differences in a given culture and at a given time may also partially explain why

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

15

different adolescents can develop different motives (emotional and social reasons) for listening to music. Music and positive youth development Positive psychology is a scientic movement that promotes research on positive aspects of growth in lieu of psychopathology (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000). Research on music and positive adolescent development seems to be in its beginning. Still, ndings suggest that adolescents emotional well-being is bolstered when they can experience stronger positive emotions (versus negative emotions) from music, as well as when they can develop more musical relatedness (in terms of music tastes) with friends and family (Miranda & Gaudreau, 2011). Furthermore, music lessons in youth may have small but robust benets for intellectual abilities and academic achievement (Schellenberg, 2006). And music education can promote peak experiences (ow) and creativity among students (MacDonald, Byrne, & Carlton, 2006). In closing, developmental psychology would probably benet from capitalising on the huge amounts of music that adolescents already listen to anyway and every day. More research on the role of music in positive adolescent development is not only about how adolescents could use music to optimise their development, but mostly about how adolescents have been maintaining resilience and thriving by creatively using music as an everyday resource. Music can represent a protective and a risk factor for psychosocial development It seems that every generation has its share of music exploring unconventional, antiauthoritarian, or controversial themes that appeal to adolescents, but is of concern to many adults, parents, politicians, and healthcare professionals (North & Hargreaves, 2008). Externalising problem behaviour Most research on music and adolescent development examined the premise that songs that are considered more problematic (e.g. anti-authoritarian, obscene, degrading, antisocial, prejudicial) in a given culture and at a given time may constitute a risk factor for externalising problem behaviours (e.g. antisocial behaviour, violence, theft). North and Hargreaves (2008) reviewed this literature and concluded that so-called problem music (e.g., heavy metal, hip-hop, Goth) can be linked to more externalising behaviours in adolescence. However, they pointed out many research caveats, such as that most evidence is correlational and cross-sectional and thereby precludes any demonstration of causality and even prediction or risk. Baker and Bor (2008) also reviewed this literature and concluded that controversial music tastes are not causes of mental health issues, but rather (or perhaps) markers of emotional vulnerability. On the other hand, studies using experimental designs indicate that antisocial songs seem to inuence antisocial thoughts and feelings (Anderson, Carnagey, & Eubanks, 2003). Can this extend to actual behaviours? A recent longitudinal study utilised crosslagged structural equation modelling to conrm that music genres that explore and express more controversial themes (heavy metal and hip-hop) can indeed predict more externalising problem behaviours during adolescence (Selfhout, Delsing, ter Bogt, & Meeus, 2008). These longitudinal ndings are provocative, robust, and they can be grounded in well-known theoretical mechanisms (e.g. cognitive priming, excitation

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

16

D. Miranda

transfer, social cognition, and desensitisation; Brown & Bobkowski, 2011), which can predict that repeated exposure to aggressive songs would reinforce and prime aggressive behaviours in some vulnerable adolescents. Lastly, it should be underscored that, unfortunately, research has focused almost exclusively on music as a risk factor rather than as a protective factor against externalising problem behaviour in youth.

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

Internalising problem behaviour There is a gradual increase of studies on music and internalising problem behaviour (e.g. mood dysregulation, depression, anxiety) in adolescence. Miranda, Gaudreau, Debrosse, Morizot, and Kirmayer (in press) reviewed this literature and suggested that listening to music could potentially inuence (reduce or increase) subclinical internalising symptoms in as much as music involves emotion regulation and coping, social cognitive inuences, as well as psychotherapeutic effects. They noticed that cross-sectional studies found concurrent links between music preferences (e.g. heavy metal) and internalising symptoms. However, they also pointed out that longitudinal evidence revealed that music preferences (including heavy metal) are not risk factors for internalising symptoms among adolescents (for example, Miranda & Claes, 2008). Nonetheless, Primack, Swanier, Georgiopoulos, Land, and Fine (2009) conducted a seven-year longitudinal study which found that adolescents excessive exposure to media (television, videocassettes, computer games, and radio) was a risk factor for depression symptoms in youth. According to the authors, among various mechanisms, this risk factor may be explained by young individuals self-comparison with unattainable images, anxiety-provoking content, or displacement of protective experiences. Given that music is at the core of multi-tasking, one may posit that certain negative songs or videos may have a compounding effect on the overall impact of media on depression. However, researchers should remember that adolescents favourite songs also convey plenty of positive messages. Accordingly, there is indication that some of todays most popular music can act as a protective factor against internalising symptoms. The six-month longitudinal study of Miranda and Claes (2008) demonstrated that listening to soul music (e.g. hip-hop, R&B) acted as a protective factor against depression symptoms among adolescents. This may be explained to some extent by the fact that African American music can often convey positive lyrics, powerful narratives, and vivid imagery, which elegantly explore themes of resilience, self-esteem, coping, self-determination, relatedness, and cultural pride.

Risky health behaviours The potential inuence of music on adolescents risky behaviours (e.g. substance use, risky sexuality, self-harm) is also receiving a lot of attention. Popular songs can convey great amounts of messages about drugs; and adolescents exposure to such lyrics is associated to their actual substance use (Primack, Douglas, & Kraemer, 2009). The inuence of musical subcultures on adolescents substance use also seems to be partially mediated by their socialisation with substance using peers (Mulder, ter Bogt, Raaijmakers, Gabhainn, Monshouwer, & Vollebergh, 2009). Nonetheless, Miranda, Gaudreau, Morizot, and Fallu (2012) used a sociometric design (peer nomination) to show that fantasising while listening to music may act as a protective factor against the inuence of peer substance use on individual substance use in adolescence.

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

17

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

In terms of risky sexuality, a two-year follow-up study found that adolescents exposure to music with degrading sexual lyrics predicted early sexual activities and intercourse (Martino et al., 2006). Another 12-month longitudinal study showed that greater exposure to gangsta rap music predicted risky sexual behaviours among African American female adolescents living in lower socio-economic-status neighbourhoods (Wingood et al., 2003). North and Hargreaves (2008) pointed out that the potential links between music preferences and self-harm are obviously most alarming. But they conclude that preferences for so-called problem music is probably a marker of vulnerability rather than a causal factor. Young, Sweeting, and West (2006) conducted an eight-year longitudinal study with adolescents. Their ndings showed that fans of the Goth music subculture were at greater risks for self-harm and suicidal behaviour, but possibly due to mechanisms of peer selection rather than peer modelling. Finally, it should also be mentioned that adolescents who often used MP3 players engage in more risky listening behaviours (e.g. listening with earbud-style earphones at high volume) that can lead to hearing loss (Vogel, Verschuure, van der Ploeg, Brug, & Raat, 2009). Music can represent an adjunct component in prevention and intervention Prevention and intervention based on music can be useful among adolescents because such initiatives use developmentally relevant elements of everyday youth culture. Prevention Music can be used for the purpose of prevention in adolescence. Brown and Bobkowski (2011) underscored that meta-analytic estimates of health promotion through media reveal modest effects that may nevertheless be interesting when scaled on large populations. There are indications that music-based prevention initiatives can be useful in youth, because adolescents recognise relevant elements of their youth culture within it. For instance, Lemieux, Fisher, and Pratto (2008) asked musically talented adolescents (who were also opinion leaders among their peers) to write, record, and distribute music about HIV prevention to their peers. This strategy revealed effective as it integrated evidence-based practices from the information, motivation, and behavioural skills model. The programmes positive outcomes regarded many aspects of HIV prevention, including motivation, behavioural skills, condom use, and testing behaviours. But what about preventing the adverse consequences of media itself? Educational strategies that promote adolescents reective/critical thinking about the meanings and motives of media products (e.g. advertisements) have shown promising outcomes (Brown & Bobkowski, 2011). Hence, sensitive and respectful educational interventions, such as `media literacy, may help adolescents to maintain (or develop) intelligent behaviours vis-a vis their favourite songs. Intervention A recent meta-analysis, of prospective evaluation studies, indicated a dose effect in which the numbers of music therapy sessions (with emotional, social, and motivational ger, & components) predicted less depressive symptoms among clients (Gold, Solli, Kru Lie, 2009). Interestingly, in an earlier meta-analysis, Gold, Voracek, and Wigram (2004)

18

D. Miranda

found that music therapy had a medium to large positive effect among children and adolescents with psychopathology. In terms of clinical practice, McFerran (2010) suggests that music therapies can judiciously address the developmental needs of adolescents by intervening on their identity formation, resilience, connectedness, and competence. Moreover, it seems that music therapists also need to develop competence in addressing cultural diversity among their young clients for whom music can have similar and different cultural meanings (for example, Jones, Baker, & Day, 2004). Conclusion This literature review offered three arguments in favour of increasing developmental research on music in adolescent psychology. First, music can inuence key aspects of adolescent development for instance, aesthetics; identity; socialisation; emotion regulation and coping; personality and motivation; gender roles; and positive youth development. This list of musics developmental impacts is not exhaustive, but it nonetheless serves as a sound basis. Second, music can act as a protective and a risk factor during adolescence. These factors are complex and require considering the interplay among the person, the context, and the music. Third, music can serve as an adjunct component in prevention and intervention for adolescents. This is encouraging as adolescents may benet more from preventive and clinical initiatives that reect and respect their youth culture. There are prospects for this burgeoning area of research. First, it is theoretically grounded in everyday youth culture within which music holds a key role. Second, it builds on and extends theories of development by delving into an everyday phenomenon that is ubiquitous in adolescence, namely music. Third, it uses sophisticated data analysis (e.g. structural equation modelling), statistical controls, longitudinal designs, large samples, experimental and sociometric designs to support the various hypotheses regarding the role of music during adolescence. Fourth, the studies are being conducted in different parts of the world (e.g. Australia, Canada, Netherlands, Sweden, the United Kingdom, the United States), thereby reminding that music is important for many adolescents (and researchers) across many societies. Finally, it can generate developmentally attuned practices for supporting and improving prevention and psychotherapy among adolescents who listen to music. However, there are also major caveats. First, there is no nomenclature in which the developmental correlates of music can be situated within an integrative roadmap. Second, there is a agrant lack of cultural studies, especially among ethnic-minority adolescents for whom music can be a cross-cultural and inter-generational resource. Third, music is usually studied in a vacuum, as though no other social media (e.g. social networks on the Internet) or physical activities (e.g. dancing to music) are involved. Fourth, there seems to be limited communication between researchers and therapists, despite their common interest in music. Lastly, there is a scarcity of programmatic mixed-method research that attempts to replicate ndings by combining different factors (biological, psychological, social, and cultural) and methods (e.g. quantitative, qualitative, experimental, longitudinal). To conclude, music can represent a developmental resource and psychological research on music opens up a scientic window to the psychological, social, and cultural needs of contemporary adolescents. Therefore, building bridges between the psychology of music and developmental psychology is an attuned initiative to better listen to the everyday sounds of adolescence.

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth Acknowledgements

19

lie Morinville, and The author would like to thank Laura Mitchell, Tom ter Bogt, Muna Osman, Ame Karole Vaugon for their thoughtful comments on an earlier version of this article.

Notes on contributor
Dave Miranda, PhD, is assistant professor at the School of Psychology of the University of Ottawa (Canada). He is director of the Laboratory of Personality and Social Psychology (LPSP), in which research projects are focusing on the social psychology of music; development of personality and coping in adolescence and youth; developmental psychopathology and social networks; crosscultural psychology; and social psychology of media.

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

References
Anderson, C.A., Carnagey, N.L., & Eubanks, J. (2003). Exposure to violent media: The effects of songs with violent lyrics on aggressive thoughts and feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 960 971. Arnett, J.J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist, 54, 317326. Bakagiannis, S., & Tarrant, M. (2006). Can music bring people together? Effects of shared musical preference on intergroup bias in adolescence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 129 136. Baker, F., & Bor, W. (2008). Can music preference indicate mental health status in young people? Australasian Psychiatry, 16, 284 288. Berns, G.S., Capra, C.M., Moore, S., & Noussair, C. (2010). Neural mechanisms of the inuence of popularity on adolescent ratings of music. NeuroImage, 49, 2687 2696. Brown, J.D., & Bobkowski, P.S. (2011). Older and newer media: Patterns of use and effects on adolescents health and well-being. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 95 113. Brown, B.B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in adolescence. In R.M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology, Volume 2: Contextual inuences on adolescent development (3rd ed., pp. 74 103). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Cattell, R.B., & Saunders, D.R. (1954). Musical preferences and personality diagnosis: A factorization of one hundred and twenty themes. Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 3 24. ` -i-Freixanet, M., Furnham, A., & Muro, A. (2009). Personality, selfChamorro-Premuzic, T., Goma estimated intelligence, and uses of music: A Spanish replication and extension using structural equation modeling. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 149 155. Compas, B.E. (2009). Coping, regulation, and development during childhood and adolescence. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 124, 87 99. te , J. (2009). Identity formation and self development in adolescence. In R.M. Lerner & Co L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology, Volume 1: Individual bases of adolescent development (3rd ed., pp. 266 304). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex (Vol. 2). London: John Murray. Delsing, M.J.M.H., Ter Bogt, T.F.M., Engels, R.C.M.E., & Meeus, W.H.J. (2008). Adolescents music preferences and personality characteristics. European Journal of Personality, 22, 109 130. s, L. (1989). A comparison between young peoples privately and publicly expressed musical Finna preferences. Psychology of Music, 17, 132 145. Fitchen, M. (1931). Speech and music development of a one year old child. Child Development, 2, 324 326. Galambos, N.L., Berenbaum, S.A., & McHale, S.M. (2009). Gender development in adolescence. In R.M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed., pp. 305 357). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Gantz, W., Gartenberg, H.M., Pearson, M.L., & Shiller, S.O. (1978). Gratications and expectations associated with pop music among adolescents. Popular Music and Society, 6, 81 89. ger, V., & Lie, S.A. (2009). Dose response relationship in music therapy Gold, C., Solli, H.P., Kru for people with serious mental disorders: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 193 207.

20

D. Miranda

Gold, C., Voracek, M., & Wigram, T. (2004). Effects of music therapy for children and adolescents with psychopathology: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1054 1063. Greenwood, D.N., & Long, C.R. (2009). Mood specic media use and emotion regulation: Patterns and individual differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 616 621. Hargreaves, D.J. (1986). The developmental psychology of music. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hargreaves, D.J., & North, A.C. (1997). The social psychology of music. In D.J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 1 21). New York: Oxford University Press. Hargreaves, D.J., North, A.C., & Tarrant, M. (2006). Musical preference and taste in childhood and adolescence. In G.E. McPherson (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 135 154). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Huron, D. (2003). Is music an evolutionary adaptation? In I. Peretz & R. Zatorre (Eds.), The cognitive neuroscience of music (pp. 57 75). New York: Oxford University Press. James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Dover Publications. Jones, C., Baker, F., & Day, T. (2004). From healing rituals to music therapy: Bridging the cultural divide between therapist and young Sudanese refugees. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 31, 89 100. m, S., Va stfja ll, D., Barradas, G., & Silva, A. (2008). An experience sampling Juslin, P.N., Liljestro study of emotional reactions to music: Listener, music, and situation. Emotion, 8, 668 683. Juslin, P.N., & Sloboda, J.A. (2010). Handbook of music and emotion: Theory, research, applications. New York: Oxford University Press. stfja ll, D. (2008). Emotional responses to music: The need to consider underlying Juslin, P.N., & Va mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 559 621. Khalfa, S., Dalla Bella, S., Roy, M., Peretz, I., & Lupien, S.J. (2003). Effects of relaxing music on salivary cortisol level after psychological stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 999, 374 376. Kistler, M., Rodgers, K.B., Power, T., Austin, E.W., & Hill, L.G. (2010). Adolescents and music media: Toward an involvement-mediational model of consumption and self-concept. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20, 616 630. Larson, R. (1995). Secrets in the bedroom: Adolescents private use of media. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24, 535 550. Laursen, B., & Collins, W.A. (2009). Parent child relationships during adolescence. In R.M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology, Volume 2: Contextual inuences on adolescent development (3rd ed., pp. 3 42). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Lemieux, A.F., Fisher, J.D., & Pratto, F. (2008). A music-based HIV prevention intervention for urban adolescents. Health Psychology, 27, 349 357. Lerner, R.M., & Steinberg, L. (2009a). Handbook of adolescent psychology, Volume 1: Individual bases of adolescent development (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Lerner, R.M., & Steinberg, L. (2009b). Handbook of adolescent psychology, Volume 2: Contextual inuences on adolescent development (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Levitin, D.J. (2008). The world in six songs. Toronto: Penguin Group. Lewis, P.A. (2002). Musical minds. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 364 366. Lin, S.F., & Thomas, G.V. (2002). Development of understanding of popular graphic art: A study of everyday aesthetics in children, adolescents, and young adults. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 278 287. Lonsdale, A.J., & North, A.C. (2011). Why do we listen to music? A uses and gratications analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 108 134. MacDonald, R., Byrne, C., & Carlton, L. (2006). Creativity and ow in musical composition: An empirical investigation. Psychology of Music, 34, 292 306. Martino, S.C., Collins, R.L., Elliott, M.N., Strachman, A., Kanouse, D.E., & Berry, S.H. (2006). Exposure to degrading versus nondegrading music lyrics and sexual behavior among youth. Pediatrics, 118, e430 e441. Masten, A.S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 491 495. McDermott, J., & Hauser, M. (2005). The origins of music: Innateness, uniqueness, and evolution. Music Perception, 23, 29 59. McFerran, K. (2010). Adolescents, music and music therapy: Methods and techniques for clinicians, educators, and students. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

International Journal of Adolescence and Youth

21

McPherson, G.E. (2006). The child as musician: A handbook of musical development. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Menon, V., & Levitin, D.J. (2005). The rewards of music listening: Response and physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. NeuroImage, 28, 175 184. Milligan, K., Atkinson, L., Trehub, S.E., Benoit, D., & Poulton, L. (2003). Maternal attachment and the communication of emotion through song. Infant Behavior and Development, 26, 1 13. Miranda, D., & Claes, M. (2008). Personality traits, music preferences and depression in adolescence. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth, 14, 277 298. Miranda, D., & Claes, M. (2009). Music listening, coping, peer afliation and depression in adolescence. Psychology of Music, 37, 215 233. Miranda, D., & Gaudreau, P. (2011). Music listening and emotional well-being in adolescence: A person- and variable-oriented study. European Review of Applied Psychology, 61, 1 11. Miranda, D., Gaudreau, P., Debrosse, R., Morizot, J., & Kirmayer, L.J. (In press). Music listening and mental health: Variations on internalizing psychopathology. In R. MacDonald, G. Kreutz, & L. Mitchell (Eds.), Music, health and well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Miranda, D., Gaudreau, P., & Morizot, J. (2010). Blue notes: Coping by music listening predicts Neuroticism changes in adolescence. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 247 253. Miranda, D., Gaudreau, P., Morizot, J., & Fallu, J.-S. (2012). Can fantasizing while listening to music play a protective role against the inuences of sensation seeking and peers on adolescents substance use? Substance Use & Misuse, 47, 166 179. Mitchell, L.A., & MacDonald, R.A.R. (2006). An experimental investigation of the effects of preferred and relaxing music listening on pain perception. Journal of Music Therapy, 43, 295 316. Mulder, J., ter Bogt, T.F.M., Raaijmakers, Q.A.W., Gabhainn, S.N., Monshouwer, K., & Vollebergh, W.A.M. (2009). The soundtrack of substance use: Music preference and adolescent smoking and drinking. Substance Use & Misuse, 44, 514 531. Mulder, J., ter Bogt, T.F.M., Raaijmakers, Q.A.W., Gabhainn, S.N., & Sikkema, P. (2010). From death metal to R&B? Consistency of music preferences among Dutch adolescents and young adults. Psychology of Music, 38, 67 83. Mursell, J.L. (1937). The psychology of music. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. North, A.C., & Hargreaves, D.J. (2008). The social and applied psychology of music. Oxford: Oxford University Press. North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J., & ONeill, S.A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 255 272. k, E., Brattico, E., Tervaniemi, M., & Huotilainen, M. (2011). The development of Nieminen, S., Isto aesthetic responses to music and their underlying neural and psychological mechanisms. Cortex, 47, 1138 1146. Nilsson, U. (2009). Soothing music can increase oxytocin levels during bed rest after open-heart surgery: A randomised control trial. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 18, 2153 2161. ONeill, S.A. (1997). Gender and music. In J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 46 63). New York: Oxford University Press. Oxford English Dictionary (1989)., Vol. X, (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Peretz, I., & Zatorre, R. (2003). The cognitive neuroscience of music. New York: Oxford University Press. Peretz, I., & Zatorre, R. (2005). Brain organization for music processing. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 89 114. Phinney, J.S. (1990). Ethnic identity in adolescents and adults: Review of research. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 499 514. Primack, B.A., Douglas, E.L., & Kraemer, K.L. (2009). Exposure to cannabis in popular music and cannabis use among adolescents. Addiction, 105, 515 523. Primack, B.A., Swanier, B., Georgiopoulos, A.M., Land, S.R., & Fine, M.J. (2009). Association between media use in adolescence and depression in young adulthood: A longitudinal study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 181188. Raviv, A., Bar-Tal, D., Raviv, A., & Ben-Horin, A. (1996). Adolescent idolization of pop singers: Causes, expressions, and reliance. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 25, 631 650.

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

22

D. Miranda

Rentfrow, P.J., & Gosling, S.D. (2003). The Do Re Mis of everyday life: The structure and personality correlates of music preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1236 1256. Rentfrow, P.J., & Gosling, S.D. (2006). Message in a ballad: The role of music preferences in interpersonal perception. Psychological Science, 17, 236 242. Rentfrow, P.J., McDonald, J.A., & Oldmeadow, J.A. (2009). You are what you listen to: Young peoples stereotypes about music fans. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 12, 329344. Roberts, K.R., Dimsdale, J., East, P., & Friedman, L. (1998). Adolescent emotional response to music and its relationship to risk-taking behaviors. Journal of Adolescent Health, 23, 49 54. Roberts, D.F., Henriksen, L., & Foehr, U.G. (2009). Adolescence, adolescents, and media. In R.M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology, Volume 2: Contextual inuences on adolescent development (3rd ed., pp. 314 344). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. , J. (2007). The role of music in adolescents mood regulation. Psychology Saarikallio, A., & Erkkila of Music, 35, 88 109. Sacks, O. (2007). Musicophilia: Tales of music and the brain. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Saether, E. (2008). When minorities are the majority: Voices from a teacher/researcher project in a multicultural school in Sweden. Research Studies in Music Education, 30, 25 42. Santiago, L.P. (1969). The lyrical expression of adolescent conict in the Beatles songs. Adolescence, 4, 199 210. Schellenberg, E.G. (2006). Exposure to music: The truth about the consequences. In G.E. McPherson (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 111 134). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Schwartz, K.D., & Fouts, G.T. (2003). Music preferences, personality style, and developmental issues of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 205 213. Selfhout, M.H.W., Branje, S.J.T., ter Bogt, T.F.M., & Meeus, W.H.J. (2009). The role of music preferences in early adolescents friendship formation and stability. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 95 107. Selfhout, M.H.W., Delsing, M.J.M.H., ter Bogt, T.F.M., & Meeus, W.H.J. (2008). Heavy metal and hip-hop style preferences and externalizing problem behavior: A two-wave longitudinal study. Youth & Society, 39, 435 452. Seligman, E.P.M., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5 14. Steinberg, L. (2011). Adolescence (9th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83 110. Tarrant, M., North, A.C., & Hargreaves, D.J. (2000). English and American adolescents reasons for listening to music. Psychology of Music, 28, 166 173. ter Bogt, T.F.M., Engels, R.C.M.E., Bogers, S., & Kloosterman, M. (2010). Shake it baby, shake it: Media preferences, sexual attitudes and gender stereotypes among adolescents. Sex Roles, 63, 844 859. ter Bogt, T.F.M., Mulder, J., Raaijmakers, Q.A.W., & Gabhainn, S.N. (2011). Moved by music: A typology of music listeners. Psychology of Music, 39, 147 163. Vogel, I., Verschuure, H., van der Ploeg, C.P.B., Brug, J., & Raat, H. (2009). Adolescents and MP3 players: Too many risks, too few precautions. Pediatrics, 123, e953 e958. Wingood, G.M., DiClemente, R.J., Bernhardt, J.M., Harrington, K., Davies, S.L., Robillard, A., & Hook, E.W. III (2003). Prospective study of exposure to rap music videos and African American female adolescents health. American Journal of Public Health, 93, 437 439. Young, R., Sweeting, H., & West, P. (2006). Prevalence of deliberate self harm and attempted suicide within contemporary Goth youth subculture: Longitudinal cohort study. British Medical Journal, 332, 1058 1061. Zillmann, D., & Gan, S. (1997). Musical taste in adolescence. In J. Hargreaves & A.C. North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 161 187). New York: Oxford University Press. Zimmer-Gembeck, M.J., & Skinner, E.A. (2011). Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 1 17.

Downloaded by [University Library Utrecht] at 08:46 18 November 2013

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen