Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Running head: ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT

The Influence of Adjustment Factors to Human Figure Drawing Test (HFDT)


Indicators of Low SES Filipinos

Daniella Morga
Doreen Flores
Tyrone Reden Sy
Ateneo de Manila University
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 2


Abstract
The purpose of this study is to discover the relationship between adjustment factors and
the drawing characteristics of human figure drawings of lower SES Filipinos. There were 65
Filipino workers who participated in this study, majority of which came from a provincial bus
company. A questionnaire composed of demographic questions, and scales for the adjustment
factors, and a blank sheet of paper for the human figure drawing test (HFDT) was given to
respondents. The HFDT outputs of respondents were evaluated by the three researchers.
Interrater reliability for global quality of drawings (0.68), bizarreness (0.67) and specific
indicators (0.79) were within acceptable ranges. No significant correlations were found for
quality, bizarreness and specific indicators. However, it was found that those who had only
elementary education had higher mean scores for depression and they were also more anxious.
Counterintuitively, those who had a monthly family income of 1,000 to 5,000 had better quality
and less bizarre drawings despite being more anxious than the rest of the respondents. When only
the data for bus company workers were analyzed (n=41), it was found that those who were more
depressed tend to have lower quality of drawings and those who had more bizarre drawings tend
to have more intrusive thoughts and were more depressed.
Keywords: HFDT, adjustment factors, low SES Filipinos
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 3

The Influence of Adjustment Factors to Human Figure Drawing Test (HFDT)
Indicators of Low SES Filipinos
Even in childhood, the human figure is one of the first and most often drawn among all
subjects (Cox, 1993; McCarty, 1924). Initially used by psychologists and educationists to assess
a childs development and intelligence, the drawing of the human figure evolved into being used
as a tool to diagnose personality disorders as well as emotional maladjustments in both children
and adults and has become one of the most frequently used assessment instrument (Yama, 1990;
Koppitz, 1968; Handler, &Reyher, 1964; Machover, 1949).
In the Philippines, the demand for psychological evaluations in different situations is
increasing, especially for the Philippine government (Dans-Lopez & Tarroja, 2010). More and
more Overseas Filipino Workers (OFWs) that have been deployed are reported to experience
psychological breakdown, commit illegal acts and encounter problems in adjustment resulting
for repatriation due to premature contract termination and consequently, becoming additional
burdens to their respective families (Dans-Lopez & Tarroja, 2010). Against this backdrop, there
are calls to find ways to minimize these occurrences through psychological evaluation before
deployment (Dans-Lopez & Tarroja, 2010).
According to Machover (1949), deep and often unconscious feelings and motives may
be accessed through various means of self-expression. Further, she saw that an effective
instrument for this expression is the drawing of human figures. The human figure is suggested to
be a representation of the self in the environment, indicating desires, defects and deficiencies,
compensation, or a combination of all (Handler, 1985, p. 177). The nonthreatening nature of
drawing human figures allows for the inner feelings and conflicts to be expressed nonverbally
(McNeish, 1993). Drawings have been used to raise awareness on potential problems in
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 4

psychological functioning among and between different populations (Dans-Lopez & Tarroja,
2010; Zalsman, Netanel, Fischel, Freudenstein, Landau, et al., 2000; Lev-Wiesel, 1999;
McNeish, 1993; Naglieri & Pfeiffer, 1992; Marsh, Linberg, and Smeltzer, 1991).
However, studies on the relevance and effect of culture on human figure drawings have
mixed findings. Some studies found that there were differences in drawings of children among
different cultural groups, such as in the different tribes of Native Americans as compared to
white children, among Maori children in New Zealand, among Tallensi children, among Turkish
children and between Japanese and U.K. children (Cox, Koyasu, Hiranuma, and Perara, 2001;
Cox, 1993). Contrary to the mentioned studies, Yama (1990) found that global characteristics of
the drawings of his Vietnamese sample suggests that some aspect of personality does not depend
of culture, that is, there are aspects of the drawings that reflects something within the
respondents that goes beyond differences in language and culture. Given the ease in its
administration and the simplicity of its instructions, the human figure drawing may be suitable,
especially for cultures that do not speak or are not fluent in English, and where translation of
other verbal tests may result in breakdown in communication (Dans-Lopez & Tarroja, 2010;
Gustafson & Waehler, 1992; Yama, 1990).
Empirical studies on human figure drawings in the context of Filipino adult experiences
are scant. There are only few, if any at all, that studied human figure drawings among Filipinos
with lower socio-economic status and lower educational attainment. As such, empirical studies
using the human figure drawing test within the Filipino context and related to adjustment
experiences may prove useful for psychologists in the field of assessment of individuals in the
same economic status, such as those applying to be OFWs.
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 5

The purpose of this study is to discover the relationship between adjustment factors
(intrusive thoughts, avoidance, failure to adapt, depression and anxiety) and the characteristics
(quality, bizarreness and specific indicators) of the human figure drawings of lower SES
Filipinos.
Method
The sites of this study were a military camp in Quezon City, a bus station in Recto,
Manila, a bus station in Angeles, Pampanga, and a professional school in Makati.
Participants of this were a heterogeneous mixture of respondents from those different
sites. Among the 65 participants, 72.3% were male, 76.9% were married, 40% were able to enter
college, 63.1% were bus company workers, and 38.9% had a monthly family income of P5,001
to P10,000.
Table 1
Sociodemographic Characteristics of Respondents (n=65)
Valid Percent
Gender
Male
Female

72.3
24.6
Civil Status
Single
Married
Widower/Widow
Separated

13.8
76.9
4.6
1.5
Occupation
Bus Company
Armed Forces - Reserved
Household Helpers
Security Guards

8.8
9.6
23.5
10.3

ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 6

Educational Attainment
Elementary
High School
College
Vocational


13.8
35.4
4.0
7.7
Monthly Family Income
P1,000 to P5,000
P5,001 to P10,000
P10,001 to P15,000
P15,001 to P20,000
P20,001 to P30,000
More than P30,000

22.2
38.9
22.2
11.1
5.6
0.0

The study was comprised of a questionnaire about demographics, a questionnaire on
Mga Reaksyon sa Stress o Problema, a Draw-a-Person sheet, and a consent form. The Mga
Reaksyon sa Stress o Problema questionnaire was composed of 21 items that were rated using a
five point Likert scale ranging from 1 hindi (never) to 5 parati (always). Participants were
also asked questions about gender, educational attainment, marital status and children, and
household income. The sheet for the draw-a-person instructed participants to draw a whole
person specifying not to draw a stick to the best of their ability.
The reliability of the factors for Mga Reaksyon sa Stress o Problema were established
where the items for intrusive thoughts had Cronbachs = .82; items for avoidance had
Cronbachs = .70; items for failure to adapt had Cronbachs = .67; items for depression had
Cronbachs = .60; and items for anxiety had Cronbachs =.68.
Data collection occurred between the November 30, 2013 and the January 7, 2014.
Researchers went to the location sites previously stated. Participants were purposively selected,
for example, bus drivers and conductors arriving in the bus terminal were invited to take a survey
in the dispatchers office where it is conducive to take the survey. They were told that the survey
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 7

will not have any effect on their jobs. After each participant finished, they were given a small
food token.
Researchers used an HFDT Microsoft Excel template to rate the characteristics of each
drawing. After which, the researchers had brownbag sessions where they discussed and
reconciled the differences in their ratings of respondents drawings. The answers to the
demographics and the Mga Reaksyon sa Stress o Problema questionnaire were encoded in
SPSS together with the ratings for global characteristics quality and bizarreness and the sum of
the indicators. Data were cleaned and analyzed using descriptive and correlational statistics
(Pearsons R) as well as t-test and One-way ANOVA.
Results

Interrater reliability for global quality (0.68), bizarreness (0.67) and specific indicators
(0.79) were within acceptable ranges. The mean score for Quality is M = 2.67 (SD = .89), for
Bizarreness M = 2.64 (SD = .88), and for Specific Indicators M = 8.46 (SD = 4.75).
No significant correlations were found among all adjustment factors and quality,
bizarreness and specific indicators. There were also no significant differences between males and
females as well as those who have and do not have children for all variables. Likewise, one-way
ANOVA revealed no significant difference across Civil Status for all variables.
However, there were significant differences across educational attainment for depression
(F (3,59) = 5.57, p<0.05, and anxiety (F (3,59) = 3.41, p< 0.05). Those who had only elementary
education had higher mean scores for depression (M = 1.91, SD = .47) and they were also more
anxious (M = 2.36 SD = .92) than all the other respondents who had higher educational
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 8

attainment. There were no significant differences for quality, bizarreness and specific indicators
across educational attainment.
Likewise, a one-way ANOVA was conducted to determine whether there were significant
differences for quality, bizarreness, specific indicators and adjustment factors across family
income. There were significant differences for quality (F (4,54) = 5.01, p< 0.05), bizarreness (F
(4,54) = 5.38, p< 0.05) and anxiety(F (4,54) = 3.22, p< 0.05). Counterintuitively, those who had
a monthly family income of 1,000 to 5,000 had better quality (M = 3.38, SD = .64) and less
bizarre drawings (M = 3.40, SD = .67) despite being more anxious (M = 2.36, SD = .76) than the
rest of the respondents.
We hypothesized that the non-significance of the correlation between the adjustment
factors and the HFDT indicators could be due to the heterogeneity of our sample. Hence, we
decided to analyze only the respondents from the bus line company (n = 41), making our sample
a relatively homogenous population (e.g. they come from the same working environment).
Among bus company workers, quality of drawings was weakly and negatively correlated
with depression such that those who were more depressed had lower quality of drawings
(r = -.33, p < 0.05). It was also found that bizarreness was inversely and weakly correlated with
intrusive thoughts (r = -.33, p < 0.05) and depression (r = -.32, p < 0.05); those who had more
bizarre drawings tend to have more intrusive thoughts and were more depressed (Table 2).
There were also differences for quality (F (4,31) = 4.71, p< 0.05) and bizarreness
(F (4,31) = 4.66, p<0.05) across monthly family income levels. Again, respondents who had an
income of 1,000 to 5,000 had higher quality (M = 3.54, SD = .75) and less bizarre drawings
(M = 3.58, SD = .81).
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 9

Table 2
Pearsons Product Moment Correlations among Bus Company Workers (n = 41)
Intrusive Thoughts Depression
Quality -.28 -.33*
Bizarreness -.33* -.32*
* Significant at = 0.05

Discussion
Educational Attainment and Depression
Poor psychological health is among the leading chronic health problems worldwide with
depression-related symptoms being the most prevalent. Comparing the results of those who
reached High school and those who took vocational courses, scores on the depression scale were
higher among those who reached high school. Furthermore, those who only reached Elementary
were more depressed and anxious than the rest. This is concurrent with the findings A public
health survey by Lindstrom, et.al in Scandinavia (2006) wherein they conducted across
Scandinavia, revealed that respondents who have low education have higher tendencies of poor
psychological health. This may be attributed to the fact that those who only reached elementary
have narrower opportunities to land on a decent paying job. They also mentioned that based on
the Swedish Public Health Report, people who experience economic stress is more prone to
mental illness than the others. (Lindstrm, Moghaddassi,& Merlo, 2006).
Family Income
In the study of Dans-Lopez and Tarroja (2010), respondents with only high school or
elementary education had poorer quality and more bizarre drawings. It was mentioned that those
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 10

with high school and elementary education mostly belonged to lower income groups. This
finding seems to be inconsistent with the results of our present study which shows the opposite.
As in previous studies conducted elsewhere, the idea is that people who has lower family income
suffer from economic stress, thereby poorer psychological health, leading to poorer quality and
more bizarre drawings in the HFDT. In contrast however, our study found that those who have
lower family income actually have better quality of drawings hence it could be inferred that they
are more psychologically healthy. What could have been the mediating variables that may have
led more anxious and more depressed Filipinos who come from lower-income brackets in our
study to be more psychologically healthy as evidenced by their HFDT?
One hypothesis is that the strong spirituality and prayerfulness of Filipinos acted as a
mediating variable a buffer that improved psychological health despite them suffering from
depression and anxiety. For example, as the researchers were talking to one respondent who
earns less than P400.00 a day, he fervently said that despite all of this sufferings, we should stay
strong be and dedicate all our efforts and sufferings to God.
Indeed, in a study of Pargament et al (2005), prayer coping was shown to promote
physical and mental health. Religious coping as a multidimensional construct has been related to
physical health, psychological well-being, health behaviors, and feelings of efficacy (Harrison,
Koenig, Hays, Eme-Akwari, & Pargament, 2001). Faith may offer motivation, a source of value
and significance and coping. Prayer coping is meant more than a ritual because it indicates the
intention of survival. Filipinos in our study may be using religiosity coping as their mechanism
as they face economic stressors.
Adjusting preferences and goals in line with experienced constraints and limitations or
also known as adaptive coping can also explain the obtained results. Adaptive coping has been
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 11

described as appropriate for a good psychological adjustment to the unchangeable (Brandstdter,
& Rothermund, 2002). Within this coping strategy, acceptance is considered a key variable.
When people in the low socio economic status accept their situation, it is assumed that they will
adjust their life goals towards more achievable goals by integrating this difficult life event
(Poppe, et.al, 2013).
The findings of our study provides a counterevidence to the stereotype that people
from the lower income brackets tend to be psychologically unhealthy succumbing to the stressors
of daily life. Instead we add to the literature on positive psychology, specifically on resilience
defined as the flexibility in responding to changing situational demands, and the ability to
bounce back from negative emotional experiences (Block & Block, 1980). We suggest that the
economic constraints of the lower income Filipino respondents have actually made them
psychologically-fit resilient individuals who experience positive emotions even in the midst of
stressful events and are able to rebound successfully despite adversity. We conclude that lower
income Filipinos are capable of understanding the benefits associated with positive emotions and
using this knowledge to their advantage when coping with negative emotional stress or events
(Tugade & Fredrickson, 2002).
Limitations and Recommendations
There were several limitations associated with this study. The study was designed with
reading level as a primary consideration, but we found that there were individuals who were
guided in answering the questionnaire because they do not know how to read. The test
administration was different with them because the researchers have to read the whole
questionnaire. Hence, the researchers might have also affected the way the respondents answered
the questions.
ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 12

Another factor will be the testing conditions. Since there are different populations, testing
conditions were not consistent. The researchers suggests that for future studies use a
homogenous population and similar settings be used as much as possible.
Findings from this present study highlight different results from majority of the studies
conducted on HFDT. Future studies may benefit in increasing the sample size so that it can be
more representative of the low SES group. We also suggest the examination of religiosity and
spirituality as a mediating variable between demographic variables, adjustment factors and
HFDT indicators.

ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 13

References

Ai, A. L., Peterson, C., Bolling, S. F., & Koenig, H. (2002). Private prayer and the optimism of
middle-age and older patients awaiting cardiac surgery. The Gerontologist, 42, 7081.

Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1980). The role of ego-control and ego-resiliency in the origination of
behavior. In W. A. Collings (Ed.), The Minnesota Symposia on
Child Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 39101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.


Brandstdter J, Rothermund K. (2002) The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal
adjustment: a two-process framework. Dev Rev 2002; 22: 117150.


Cox, M., Koyasu, M., Hiranuma, H., & Perara, J. (2001). Childrens human figure drawings in
the UK and Japan: The effects of age, sex, and culture. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 19, 275292.

Cox, M. V. (1993). Children's drawings of the human figure. Hove [England] : Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates
Dans-Lopez, G., &Tarroja, M. (2010).Exploring Human Figure Drawings as an assessment tool
for departing OFW domestic helpers and caregivers.Philippine Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 12(1), pp. 36-62

Gustafson, J., &Waehler, C. (1992).Assessing abstract and concrete thinking with the draw-a-
person technique.Journal of Personality Assessment, 59, 439-447.

Handler, L., &Reyher, J. (1964). The effects of stress on the draw a person test. Journal of
Counsulting Psychology, 28, 259-264
Handler, L. (1985). The clinical use of the draw-a-person test (DAP). In C.S. Newmark (Ed.),
Major psychological assessment instruments (pp. 165-216). Boston: Allyn& Bacon.
Harrison, M. O., Koenig, H. G., Hays, J. C., Eme-Akwari, A. G., & Pargament, K. I. (2001).
The epidemiology of religious coping: A review of recent literature. International Review
of Psychiatry, 13, 86-93.

Koppitz, E. M. (1968). Psychological evaluation of children's human figure drawings. New
York: Grune& Stratton.

Lev-Wiesel, R. (1999). The Use of the Machover Draw-a-Person test in detecting adult
survivors of sexual abuse: A pilot study. American Journal of Art Therapy, 37(4), 106-
113.

ADJUSTMENT FACTORS ON HFDT 14

Lindstrm, M., Moghaddassi, M., & Merlo, J. (2006). Individual and contextual determinants of
self reported poor psychological health: A population based multilevel analysis in
southern Sweden. Scandinavian Journal Of Public Health, 34(4), 397-405.


Machover, K. (1949). Personality projection in the drawing of the human figure. Springfield, IL:
Thom
Marsh, D., Linberg, L., Smeltzer, J. (1991). Human figure drawings of adjudicated and
nonadjudicated adolescents.Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 77-86.

McCarty, S. A. (1924). Childrens drawings. Baltimore, MD: Williams and Wilkins.
McNeish, T. (1993). Identification of individuals with serious emotional disturbance using the
Draw A Person Screening Procedure for Emotional Disturbance. The Journal of Special
Education, 27(1), 115-121.

Naglieri, J. & Pfeiffer, S. (1992). Performance of disruptive behavior disordered and normal
samples on the Draw A Person: Screening Procedure for Emotional Disturbance.
PsychologicalAssessment, 4, 156-159.

Pargament, K. I., Magyar-Russell, G. M., & Murray-Swank, N. A. (2005). The sacred and the
search for significance: Religion as a unique process. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 665
687.

Poppe, C., Crombez, G., Hanoulle, I., Vogelaers, D., & Petrovic, M. (2013). Improving quality
of life in patients with chronic kidney disease: influence of acceptance and personality.
Nephrology, Dialysis, Transplantation: Official Publication Of The European Dialysis
And Transplant Association - European Renal Association, 28(1), 116-121.

Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B L. (2002). Positive emotions and emotional intelligence. In
Barrett L. Feldman & P. Salovey (Eds.). The Wisdom of Feelings (pp. 319340). New
York: Guilford.

Yama, M. F. (1990). The usefulness of human figure drawings as an index of overall
adjustment.Journal of Personality Assessment, 54(1-2), 78-86
Zalsman, G., Netanel, R., Fischel, T., Freudenstein, O., Landau, E., Orbach, I., Wiezman, A.,
Pfeffer, C., Apter, A. (2000). Human figuredrawings in the evaluation of severe
adolescent suicidalbehavior (abstract). Journal of the American Academy of Child
&Adolescent Psychiatry, 3, 1024-1031

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen