Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

NFLRC NetWork #6

CAN PRAGMATIC COMPETENCE BE TAUGHT?


Gabriele Kasper
University of Hawai`i
Please cite as...
1997 Second Language Teaching & Curriculum Center

'Can Pragmatic Competence Be Taught?' The simple answer to the question as formulated
is "no". Competence, whether linguistic or pragmatic, is not teachable. Competence is a
type of knowledge that learners possess, develop, acquire, use or lose. The challenge for
foreign or second language teaching is whether we can arrange learning opportunities in
such a way that they benefit the development of pragmatic competence in L2. This, then, is
the issue I will address in this paper.

The pragmatic component in models of communicative competence


There are many definitions of pragmatics around. One I find particularly useful has been
proposed by David Crystal. According to him, "Pragmatics is the study of language from the
point of view of users, especially of the choices they make, the constraints they encounter in
using language in social interaction and the effects their use of language has on other
participants in the act of communication" (Crystal 1985, p. 240). In other words, pragmatics
is the study of communicative action in its sociocultural context. Communicative action
includes not only speech acts - such as requesting, greeting, and so on - but also participation
in conversation, engaging in different types of discourse, and sustaining interaction in
complex speech events. Following Leech (1983), I will focus on pragmatics as interpersonal
rhetoric - the way speakers and writers accomplish goals as social actors who do not just need
to get things done but attend to their interpersonal relationships with other participants at the
same time.
Leech (1983) and his colleague Jenny Thomas (1983) proposed to subdivide pragmatics into a
pragmalinguistic and sociopragmatic component. Pragmalinguistics refers to the resources
for conveying communicative acts and relational or interpersonal meanings. Such resources
include pragmatic strategies like directness and indirectness, routines, and a large range of
linguistic forms which can intensify or soften communicative acts. For one example, compare
these two versions of apology - the terse 'I'm sorry' and the Wildean 'I'm absolutely
devastated. Can you possibly forgive me?' In both versions, the speaker apologizes, but she
indexes a very different attitude and social relationship in each of the apologies (e.g., Fraser,
1980; House & Kasper, 1981; Brown & Levinson, 1987; Blum-Kulka, House, & Kasper,
1989).
Sociopragmatics was described by Leech (1983, p. 10) as 'the sociological interface of
pragmatics', referring to the social perceptions underlying participants' interpretation and
performance of communicative action. Speech communities differ in their assessment of
speaker's and hearer's social distance and social power, their rights and obligations, and the
degree of imposition involved in particular communicative acts (Takahashi & Beebe, 1993;

1 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

Blum-Kulka & House, 1989; Olshtain, 1989). The values of context factors are negotiable; they
can change through the dynamics of conversational interaction, as captured in Fraser's (1990)
notion of the 'conversational contract' and in Myers-Scotton's Markedness Model (1993).
Pragmatic ability in a second or foreign language is part of a nonnative speakers (NNS)
communicative competence and therefore has to be located in a model of communicative
ability (Savignon, (1991, for overview). In Bachman's model (1990, p. 87ff), 'language
competence' is subdivided into two components, 'organizational competence' and 'pragmatic
competence'. Organizational competence comprises knowledge of linguistic units and the
rules of joining them together at the levels of sentence ('grammatical competence') and
discourse ('textual competence'). Pragmatic competence subdivides into 'illocutionary
competence' and 'sociolinguistic competence'. 'Illocutionary competence' can be glossed as
'knowledge of communicative action and how to carry it out'. The term 'communicative action'
is often more accurate than the more familiar term 'speech act' because communicative action
is neutral between the spoken and written mode, and the term acknowledges the fact that
communicative action can also be implemented by silence or non-verbally. 'Sociolinguistic
competence' comprises the ability to use language appropriately according to context. It thus
includes the ability to select communicative acts and appropriate strategies to implement
them depending on the current status of the 'conversational contract' (Fraser, 1990).

Need L2 pragmatics be taught?


As Bachman's model makes clear, pragmatic competence is not extra or ornamental, like the
icing on the cake. It is not subordinated to knowledge of grammar and text organization but
co-ordinated to formal linguistic and textual knowledge and interacts with 'organizational
competence' in complex ways. In order to communicate successfully in a target language,
pragmatic competence in L2 must be reasonably well developed. But adopting pragmatic
competence as one of the goals for L2 learning does not necessarily imply that pragmatic
ability requires any special attention in language teaching. Before turning to the central
question of my talk, i.e., whether L2 pragmatics can be taught, I will therefore address the
logically prior question of whether L2 pragmatics needs to be taught. Because perhaps
pragmatic knowledge simply develops alongside lexical and grammatical knowledge, without
requiring any pedagogic intervention.
Indeed, adult NNS do get a considerable amount of L2 pragmatic knowledge for free. This is
because some pragmatic knowledge is universal, and other aspects may be successfully
transferred from the learners' L1. To start with the pragmatic universals, learners know that
conversations follow particular organizational principles - participants have to take turns at
talk, and conversations and other speech events have specific internal structures. Learners
know that pragmatic intent can be indirectly conveyed, and they can use context information
and various knowledge sources to understand indirectly conveyed meaning. They know that
recurrent speech situations are managed by means of conversational routines (Coulmas, 1981;
Nattinger & DeCarrico, 1992) rather than by newly created utterances. They know that
strategies of communicative actions vary according to context (Blum-Kulka, 1991); specifically,
along such factors as social power, social and psychological distance, and the degree of
imposition involved in a communicative act, as established in politeness theory (Brown &
Levinson, 1987; Brown & Gilman, 1989). Learners have demonstrated knowledge of the
directive and expressive speech acts that have been most frequently studied in cross-cultural
and interlanguage pragmatics, such as requests and apologies, and they have been shown to
understand and use the major realization strategies for such speech acts. For instance, in

2 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

requesting, users of any language studied thus far distinguish different levels of directness;
direct, as in 'feed the cat', conventionally indirect, as in 'can/could/would you feed the cat?',
and indirect, as in 'the cat's complaining.' Furthermore, language users know that requests
can be softened or intensified in various ways, as in 'I was wondering if you would terribly
mind feeding the cat', and that requests can be externally modified through various supportive
moves, for instance justifications, as in 'I have to go to a conference', or imposition
minimizers, as in 'She only needs food once a day'. Studies document that these strategies of
requesting are available to ESL or EFL learners who are NS of such diverse languages as
Chinese (Johnston, Kasper, & Ross, 1994), Danish (Frch & Kasper, 1989), German (House
& Kasper, 1987), Hebrew (Blum-Kulka & Olshtain, 1986), Japanese (Takahashi & DuFon,
1989), Malay (Piirainen-Marsh, 1995), and Spanish (Rintell & Mitchell, 1989). In their early
learning stages, learners may not be able to use such strategies because they have not yet
acquired the necessary linguistic means, but when their linguistic knowledge permits it,
learners will use the main strategies for requesting without instruction.
Learners may also get very specific pragmalinguistic knowledge for free if there is a
corresponding form-function mapping between L1 and L2, and the forms can be used in
corresponding L2 contexts with corresponding effects. For instance, the English modal past as
in the modal verbs could or would has formal, functional and distributional equivalents in
other Germanic languages such as Danish and German - the Danish modal past kunne/ville
and the German subjunctive knntest and wrdest. And sure enough, Danish and German
learners of English transfer ability questions from L1 Danish (kunne/ville du lne mig dine
noter) and L1 German (knntest/ wrdest Du mir Deine Aufzeichnungen leihen) to L2
English (could/would you lend me your notes) (House & Kasper, 1987; Frch & Kasper,
1989), and they do this without the benefit of instruction.
Positive transfer can also facilitate learners' task in acquiring sociopragmatic knowledge.
When distributions of participants' rights and obligations, their relative social power and the
demands on their resources are equivalent in their original and target community, learners
may only need to make small adjustments in their social categorizations (Mir, 1995).
Unfortunately, learners do not always make use of their free ride. It is well known from
educational psychology that students do not always transfer available knowledge and
strategies to new tasks. This is also true for some aspects of learners' universal or L1-based
pragmatic knowledge. L2 recipients often tend towards literal interpretation, taking
utterances at face value rather than inferring what is meant from what is said and underusing
context information. Learners frequently underuse politeness marking in L2 even though they
regularly mark their utterances for politeness in L1 (Kasper, 1981). Although highly contextsensitive in selecting pragmatic strategies in their own language, learners may
underdifferentiate such context variables as social distance and social power in L2
(Fukushima, 1990; Tanaka, 1988).
So, the good news is that there is a lot of pragmatic information that adult learners possess,
and the bad news is that they don't always use what they know. There is thus a clear role for
pedagogic intervention here, not with the purpose of providing learners with new information
but to make them aware of what they know already and encourage them to use their universal
or transferable L1 pragmatic knowledge in L2 contexts.
The most compelling evidence that instruction in pragmatics is necessary comes from
learners whose L2 proficiency is advanced and whose unsuccessful pragmatic performance is

3 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

not likely to be the result of cultural resistance or disidentification strategies (Kasper, 1995,
for discussion). In a study of a large sample of advanced ESL learners, Bouton (1988)
examined how well these students understood different types of indirect responses, or
implicature, as in the following dialog:
Sue: How was your dinner last night?
Anne: Well, the food was nicely presented.
Bouton found that in 27% of the cases, implicatures were understood differently by native
speakers (NS) and NNS. A re-test of 30 students after 4 1/2 years demonstrated that their
comprehension now showed a success rate of over 90%. But some implicature types resisted
improvement through exposure alone. These included the Pope question (as in Is the Pope
Catholic?) and indirect criticism as in the Sue & Anne dialogue. Students' comprehension of
implicature may thus profit from instruction, and as we will see shortly, this has indeed
proved to be the case.
Turning to production, candidates for pedagogic intervention can be sorted in four groups: (1)
choice of communicative acts, (2) the strategies by which an act is realized, (3) its content, and
(4) its linguistic form. Drawing on her and Beverly Hartford's data from academic advising
sessions (Bardovi-Harlig & Hartford 1990, 1993), Bardovi-Harlig (1996) noted that NNS
students tended to leave suggestions about their coursework to their advisor and then react to
them. Consequently, the NNS performed more rejections of advisor suggestions than the NS
students, who were more initiative in making suggestions and thereby avoided rejections.
Both NS and NNS regularly offered explanations when they rejected their advisor's course
suggestion, but the NS would also suggest alternatives ('how about I take x course instead'),
something the NNS never did. For their rejections, the NNS sometimes used inappropriate
content, such as claiming the course suggested by their advisor was either too easy or too
difficult, or even evaluating their advisor's course as 'uninteresting'. Finally, even at the end of
the observation period, the NNS had not learnt how to mitigate their suggestions and
rejections appropriately. By using mitigating forms such as 'I was thinking' or 'I have an idea...
I dont' know how it would work out, but...', the NS would cast their suggestions in tentative
terms. By contrast, the NNS tended to formulate their suggestions much more assertively, as
in 'I will take language testing' or 'I've just decided on taking the language structure' (all
examples from Bardovi-Harlig, 1996, 22f.).
Two things need to be emphasized in assessing the implications of Bouton's and BardoviHarlig and Hartford's studies. First, the participating advanced students were ESL learners,
yet the target environment either did not provide students with the input they needed, or they
did not notice it. Secondly, the recorded differences in NS and NNS pragmatic comprehension
and production may lead to serious miscommunication and compromise the NNS's goals.
Bardovi-Harlig and Hartford (1990) found that when students' contributions were
pragmatically inappropriate, they were less successful in obtaining their advisor's consent for
taking the courses they preferred.
A further aspect of students' pragmatic competence is their awareness of what is and is not
appropriate in given contexts. Bardovi-Harlig and Drnyei (1997) reported that Hungarian
and Italian EFL learners recognized grammatically incorrect but pragmatically appropriate
utterances more readily than pragmatically inappropriate but grammatically correct
utterances, and this was true for learners of all proficiency levels. This finding strongly
suggests that without a pragmatic focus, foreign language teaching raises students'
metalinguistic awareness, but it does not contribute much to develop their metapragmatic
consciousness in L2.

4 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

Can L2 pragmatics be taught?


As we have seen, then, without some form of instruction, many aspects of pragmatic
competence do not develop sufficiently. We therefore need to know what pragmatic aspects
can be taught and which instructional approaches may be most effective. Table 1 summarizes
the data-based research on pragmatic instruction.
Table 1: Studies examining the effect of pragmatic instruction
proficiency

languages

research goal

design

assessment/
procedure/
instrument

discourse
markers &
strategies

advanced

L1 German FL
English

explicit vs
implicit

pre-test/
post-test
control group
L2 baseline

roleplay

pragmatic
routines

intermediate

pre-test/
post-test
control group

roleplay

Billmyer 1990

compliment

high
intermediate

L1 Japanese
SL English

+/-instruction

pre-test/
post-test
control group
L2 baseline

elicited
conversation

Olshtain &
Cohen 1990

apology

advanced

L1 Hebrew FL
English

teachability

pre-test/
post-test L2
baseline

discourse
completion
question.

WildnerBassett 1994

pragmatic
routines &
strategies

beginning

L1 English SL
German

teachability to
beginning FL
students

pre-test/
post-test

questionnaires roleplay

Bouton 1994

implicature

advanced

L1 mixed SL
English

+/-instruction

pre-test/
post-test
control group

multiple
choice
question

pre-test/
post-test/
delayed
post-test
control group

multiple
choice &
sentence
combining
question

study

teaching goal

House &
Kasper 1981
WildnerBassett 1984,
1986

L1 German FL
eclectic vs
English
suggesto-pedia

Kubota 1995

implicature

intermediate

L1 Japanese
FL English

deductive vs
inductive vs
zero

House 1996

pragmatic
fluency

advanced

L1 German FL
English

explicit vs
implicit

pre-test/
post-test
control group

roleplay

roleplay
holistic ratings

multi-method

Morrow 1996

complaint &
refusal

intermediate

L1 mixed SL
English

teachability/
explicit

pre-test/
post-test/
delayed
post-test L2
baseline

Tateyama et
al. 1997

pragmatic
routines

beginning

L1 English FL
Japanese

explicit vs
implicit

pre-test/
post-test
control group

All of the 10 studies report on classroom-based research on pragmatics. I excluded studies


conducted in a lab type situation because I wanted to make sure that the chosen approaches
are ecologically valid in actual L2 classrooms.
As you can see from the second column to the left, the teaching goals in these studies extend

5 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

over a large range of pragmatic features and abilities. Some studies examine the discourse
markers and strategies by which conversationalists get in and out of conversations, introduce,
sustain, and change topics, organize turn-taking and keep the conversation going by listener
activities such as backchanneling. Many of these conversational activities are implemented by
pragmatic routines which regularly occur in spoken discourse, yet foreign language learners
may have little exposure to them. A number of discourse markers and strategies are illustrated
in the following conversational sequence.
A telephone conversation (Sacks, 1995, vol. II, p. 201f; transcript slightly modified)
A: Hello.
B: Vera?
A: Ye:s.
B: Well you know, I had a little difficulty getting you. (1.0) First I got the wrong number, and
then I got Operator, [A: Well.] And uhm (1.0) I wonder why.
A: Well, I wonder too. It uh just rung now about uh three ti//mes.
B: Yeah, well Operator got it for me.
A: She did.
B: Uh huh. So //uh
A: Well.
B: When I- after I got her twice, why she [A: telephoned] tried it for me. Isn't that funny?
A: Well it certainly is.
B: Must be some little cross of lines someplace hh
A: Guess so.
B: Uh huh,uh, am I taking you away from yer dinner?
A: No::. No, I haven't even started tuh get it yet.
B: Oh, you have//n't.
A: hhheh heh
B: Well I- I never am certain, I didn't know whether I'd be m too early or too late // or riA: No::. No, well I guess uh with us uhm there isn't any - [B: Yeah.] p'ticular time.
Another group of studies explores whether students benefit from instruction in specific speech
acts. So far, speech acts examined are compliments, apologies, complaints, and refusals. There
is a research literature on all of these speech acts, documenting how they are performed by
native speakers of English in different social contexts. Based on this literature, students were
taught the strategies and linguistic forms by which the speech acts are realized and how these
strategies are used in different contexts. As one example, consider the realization strategies
(or 'speech act set') for apologies (adapted from Blum-Kulka, House, & Kasper, 1989):
Apologetic formula: I'm sorry, I apologize, I'm afraid
Assuming Responsibility: I haven't read your paper yet.
Account: I had to prepare my TESOL plenary.
Offer of Repair: But I'll get it done by Wednesday.
Appeaser: Believe me, you're not the only one.
Promise of forbearance: I'll do better after TESOL.
Intensifier: I'm terribly sorry, I really tried to squeeze it in.
Bringing together the ability to carry out speech acts and manage ongoing conversation,
House (1996) examined instructional effects on what she calls pragmatic fluency - the extend
to which students' conversational contributions are relevant, polite, and overall effective. And
finally, while most studies focus on aspects of production, two studies examined pragmatic

6 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

comprehension: in Bouton (1994), students were taught different types of implicatures, as in


the Sue & Anne dialogue quoted earlier, and Kubota (1995) replicated Bouton's study in an
EFL context.
Whereas most of these pragmatic features were taught to intermediate or advanced learners,
participants in Wildner-Bassett (1994) and Tateyama et al. (1997) were beginning learners.
These two studies thus address the important question of whether pragmatics is teachable to
beginners or whether there needs to be some threshold of linguistic L2 competence first.
Wildner-Bassett's (1994) and Tateyama et al.'s studies are also the only ones in which the
target language is not English - in Wildner-Bassett's study, the L2 is German, in Tateyama et
al., it is Japanese. Note that in some studies, the target language is a foreign language whereas
in others, it is a second language. This has consequences for the learning outcomes, as I will
show a bit later.
The studies differed in their research goals. Olshtain and Cohen (1990), Wildner-Bassett
(1994) and Morrow (1996) explored whether the features under investigation were teachable
at all. These studies did not employ control groups but compared students' test performance
before and after instruction to that of NS of the target language, referred to as 'L2 baseline' in
the 'design' column in Table 1. Billmyer (1990) and Bouton (1994) examined whether students
who received instruction in complimenting and implicature did better than controls who did
not. Yet another group explored the effectiveness of specific teaching approaches. In these
studies, two or more student groups received different types of instruction. House and Kasper
(1981), House (1996), and Tateyama et al. (1997) compared explicit with implicit approaches.
Explicit teaching involved description, explanation, and discussion of the pragmatic feature in
addition to input and practice, whereas implicit teaching included input and practice without
the metapragmatic component. Wildner-Bassett (1984, 1986) compared an eclectic approach
with a modified version of suggestopedia, and Kubota (1995) compared an inductive
approach, where students had to figure out in groups how implicatures in English work, to a
teacher-directed deductive approach and zero instruction in implicature. Information about
the designs and assessment procedures and instruments is provided in the two rightmost
columns in Table 1, but I'm not going to comment on those. Instead, let's proceed to the
findings of the studies.
First of all, the studies that examined whether the selected pragmatic features were teachable
found this indeed to be the case, and comparisons of instructed students with uninstructed
controls reported an advantage for the instructed learners. Secondly, the studies comparing
the relative effect of explicit and implicit instruction found that students' pragmatic abilities
improved regardless of the adopted approach, but the explicitly taught students did better
than the implicit groups. Thirdly, with respect to other teaching approaches, Wildner-Bassett
(1984, 1986) found that both the eclectively taught students and the suggestopedic group
improved their use of conversational routines considerably, however the eclectic group
outperformed the suggestopedic group. Kubota (1995) reported an advantage for students
receiving either deductive or inductive instruction over the uninstructed group, with a
superior effect for the inductive approach, this initial difference had evaporated by the time a
delayed post-test was administered.
Wildner-Bassett (1994) and Tateyama et al. (1997) demonstrated that pragmatic routines
are teachable to beginning foreign language learners. This finding is important in terms of
curriculum and syllabus design because it dispels the myth that pragmatics can only be taught

7 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

after students have developed a solid foundation in L2 grammar and vocabulary. As we know
from uninstructed first and second language acquisition research, most language development
is function-driven - i.e., the need to understand and express messages propels the learning of
linguistic form. Just as in uninstructed acquisition, students can start out by learning
pragmatic routines which they cannot yet analyze but which help them cope with recurrent,
standardized communicative events right from the beginning.
There is little evidence for aspects of L2 pragmatics that resist development through teaching,
but the few documented cases are instructive. One such study is Kubota's replication of
Bouton's (1994) research on the teaching of implicature. Kubota's Japanese EFL learners were
able to understand the exact implicatures that were repeated from the training materials but
were unable to generalize inferencing strategies to new instances of implicature. However,
these students' English proficiency was much less advanced than that of the learners in
Bouton's studies, and with more time, occasion for practice, and increased L2 input, the
students' success rate might have improved.
The other study that suggests limitations to teachability in L2 pragmatics is House's (1996)
investigation on improving the pragmatic fluency of advanced German EFL students. All but
one feature of pragmatic fluency gained from consciousness raising and conversational
practice; the resistent aspect was to provide appropriate rejoinders, or second pair parts, to an
interlocutor's preceding contribution, as in this exchange:
NS: Oh I tell you what we go shopping together and buy all the things [we need]
NNS: [Of course] of course
NS: Okay then and you try and call Anja and ask her if she knows somebody who owns a grill
NNS: Yes of course (House, 1996, p. 242)
More appropriate acceptances of the NS' suggestions would have been 'ok/good idea/let's do
it that way then' or the like. Why would inappropriate rejoinders persist in these advanced
learners' discourse despite instruction? A plausible explanation is Bialystok's (e.g., 1993)
notion of control of processing: fluent and appropriate conversational responses require high
degrees of processing control in utterance comprehension and production, and such complex
skills may be very hard to develop through the few occasions for practice that foreign language
classroom learning provides.
But despite those few limitations, the research supports the view that pragmatic ability can
indeed be systematically developed through planful classroom activities. In order to address
the next question -

How can language instruction help develop pragmatic competence?


- we need to consider for a moment what opportunities for pragmatic learning are offered by
traditional forms of language teaching.
L2 classrooms as impoverished learning environments
It is a well-documented fact that in teacher-fronted teaching, the person doing most of the
talking is the teacher (e.g., Chaudron, 1988, for various analyses of teacher talk). This is to the
detriment of students' speaking opportunities, but it could be argued that through the sheer
quantity of teacher talk, students are provided with the input they need for pragmatic
development. However, studies show that compared to conversation outside instructional

8 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

settings, teacher-fronted classroom discourse displays


a more narrow range of speech acts (Long, Adams, McLean, & Castaos, 1976)
a lack of politeness marking (Lrscher & Schulze, 1988)
shorter and less complex openings and closings (Lrscher, 1986; Kasper, 1989)
monopolization of discourse organization and management by the teacher (Lrscher,
1986; Ellis, 1990), and consequently,
a limited range of discourse markers (Kasper, 1989).
The reason for such differences is not that classroom discourse is 'artificial'. Classroom
discourse is just as authentic as any other kind of discourse. Rather, classroom interaction is
an institutional activity in which participants' roles are asymmetrically distributed (Nunan,
1989), and the social relationships in this unequal power encounter are reflected and
re-affirmed at the level of discourse. Teacher's and students' rights and obligations, and the
activities associated with them, are epitomized in the basic interactional pattern of traditional
teacher-fronted teaching - the (in)famous pedagogical exchange of elicitation (by the teacher)
- response (by a student) - feedback (by the teacher) (cf. discussion in Chaudron, 1988, p. 37).
The classic scenario is consistent with a knowledge-transmission model of teaching, according
to which the teacher imparts new information to students, helps them process such
information and controls whether the new information has become part of students'
knowledge. Such functions can be implemented through a very limited range of
communicative acts.
If we map the communicative actions in classic language classroom discourse against the
pragmatic competence that nonnative speakers need to communicate in the world outside, it
becomes immediately obvious that the language classroom in its classical format does not
offer students what they need - not in terms of teacher's input, nor in terms of students'
productive language use. In a comparison of teacher-fronted teaching and small group work,
Long et al. (1976) demonstrated over 20 years ago that student participation increases
dramatically in student-centered activities. Importantly, student-centered activities do more
than just extend students' speaking time: they also give them opportunities to practice
conversational management, perform a larger range of communicative acts, and interact with
other participants in completing a task.
But despite its unique structure, even teacher-fronted classroom discourse offers some
opportunities for pragmatic learning. One important learning resource is classroom
management, because in this activity language does not function as an object for analysis and
practice but as a means for communication. If classroom management is performed in the
students' L1, they miss a valuable opportunity for experiencing the L2 as a genuine means of
communication. In a recent call for a role of students' native language in ESL teaching,
Auerbach (1993) proposed that classroom management is one of the activities that could be
carried out in students' L1 rather than the L2. Auerbach argues that using minority students'
native language for classroom management is one way of validating the students'
ethnolinguistic identity in an ESL classroom. In my view, Auerbach's call against English Only
classrooms in ESL settings for immigrant minorities is valid and necessary, but I want to
caution against extending it to EFL situations or any other foreign language classrooms, for
that matter. For students of English in Continental Europe or Asia, or students of Japanese
and French in the US, the FL classroom may be the only regular opportunity for using the FL
for communication. These opportunities should not be curtailed, and certainly not when it
comes to routinized activities such as classroom management discourse. In a recent study of

9 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

his learning of Japanese as a Foreign Language, Cohen (1997) reports:


"Classroom talk was focused primarily on completing a series of planned
transactions, such as making introductions, buying stamps or postcards at a post
office, buying clothes in a department store, telling the doctor about our illness,
and the like. There was little non-transactional social conversation in class, other
than asides in English. In addition, spoken language tended to be focused on
structures that we were to learn (...). Toward the end of the second month, we
would start the class off with teacher-directed questions and answers, usually
inquiring about what we had done the previous day or weekend, or what we
intended to do - usually with the purpose of practicing some structure or other."
Because little genuinly communicative interchange was conducted in Japanese, students had
not much exposure to authentic input in this classroom.
From the studies reviewed earlier and from other theory and research of SL learning, we can
distill a number of activities that are useful for pragmatic development. Such activities can be
classified into two main types: activities aiming at raising students' pragmatic awareness, and
activities offering opportunities for communicative practice.
Awareness-raising
Through awareness-raising activities, students acquire sociopragmatic and pragmalinguistic
information - for instance, what function complimenting has in mainstream American culture,
what appropriate topics for complimenting are, and by what linguistic formulae compliments
are given and received. Students can observe particular pragmatic features in various sources
of oral or written 'data', ranging from native speaker 'classroom guests' (Bardovi-Harlig, et
al., 1991) to videos of authentic interaction, feature films (Rose, 1997), and other fictional and
non-fictional written and audiovisual sources.
Observation tasks
Especially in a second language context, students can be given a variety of observation
assignments outside the classroom. Such observation tasks can focus on sociopragmatic or
pragmalinguistic features.
A sociopragmatic task could be to observe under what conditions native speakers of American
English express gratitude - when, for what kinds of goods or services, and to whom (cf.
Eisenstein & Bodman, 1993). Depending on the student population and available time, such
observations may be open or structured. Open observations leave it to the students to detect
what the important context factors may be. For structured observations, students are provided
with an observation sheet which specifies the categories to look out for - for instance,
speaker's and hearer's status and familiarity, the cost of the good or service to the giver, and
the degree to which the giver is obliged to provide the good or service. A useful model for such
an observation sheet is the one proposed by Rose (1994) for requests.
A pragmalinguistic task focuses on the strategies and linguistic means by which thanking is
accomplished - what formulae are used, and what additional means of expressing appreciation
are employed, such as expressing pleasure about the giver's thoughtfulness or the received
gift, asking questions about it, and so forth. Finally, by examining in which contexts the
various ways of expressing gratitude are used, sociopragmatic and pragmalinguistic aspects
are combined. By focusing students' attention on relevant features of the input, such

10 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

observation tasks help students make connections between linguistic forms, pragmatic
functions, their occurrence in different social contexts, and their cultural meanings. Students
are thus guided to notice the information they need in order to develop their pragmatic
competence in L2 (Schmidt, 1993). The observations made outside the classroom will be
reported back to class, compared with those of other students, and perhaps commented and
explained by the teacher. These discussion can take on any kind of small group of whole class
format.
Whether gathered through out-of-class observation or brought into the classroom through
audiovisual media, authentic native speaker input is indispensible for pragmatic learning. This
is not because students should imitate native speakers' action patterns but in order to build
their own pragmatic knowledge on the right kind of input. Comparisons of textbook dialogues
and authentic discourse show that there is often a mismatch between the two. For instance,
Bardovi-Harlig, et al. (1991) examined conversational closings in 20 textbooks for American
English and found that few of them represented closing phases accurately. Myers-Scotton and
Bernstein (1988) discovered similar discrepancies between the representation of many other
conversational features in authentic discourse and textbook dialogues. The reason for such
inaccurate textbook representations is that native speakers are only partially aware of their
pragmatic competence (the same is true of their language competence generally). As Wolfson
(1989) noted, most of native speakers' pragmatic knowledge is tacit, or implicit knowledge: it
underlies their communicative action, but they cannot describe it. Even the most proficient
conversationalist has little conscious awareness about turn-taking procedures and politeness
marking. Miscommunication or pragmatic failure is often vaguely diagnosed as 'impolite'
behavior on the part of the other person, whereas the specific source of the irritation remains
unclear. Because native speaker intuition is a notoriously unreliable source of information
about the communicative practices of their own community, it is vital that teaching materials
on L2 pragmatics are research-based (Myers-Scotton & Bernstein, 1988; Wolfson, 1989;
Olshtain & Cohen, 1991; Bardovi-Harlig, et al., 1991).
Authentic L2 input is essential for pragmatic learning, but it does not secure successful
pragmatic development. When students' observe L2 communicative practices, their minds
don't simply record what they hear and see like a videocamera does. Students' experiences are
interpretive rather than just registering. Cognitive psychology (e.g., Sanford & Garrod, 1981)
as well as radical constructivism (e.g., von Glaserfeld, 1995) emphasize the importance of
prior knowledge for comprehension and learning. In our attempt to understand the practices
of an unfamiliar community, we tend to view such practices through the lenses of our own
customs. We tend to classify experiences into 'familiar' and thus not requiring further
reflection or analysis, and 'unfamiliar', i.e., peculiar, enigmatic, inviting explanation, and
attracting evaluation. Mller (1981) referred to this interpretive strategy as cultural
isomorphism. As a strategy for the acquisition of everyday knowledge, cultural isomorphism is
a combination of assimilation and spot-the-difference. L2 practices are subjected to the same
social evaluations as the apparently equivalent L1 practices. The resulting perspective is that
of a tourist who sorts experiences in the visited country into 'just like home' and 'strange'. As
Elbeshausen and Wagner (1985) comment, "Tourism is not educational but it dramatically
increases our repertoire of anecdotes" (p. 49), and this is because through the assimilative and
contrastive strategy of isomorphism, stereotypical evaluations of L2 practices emerge.
Language teaching therefore has the important task to help students situate L2
communicative practices in their sociocultural context and appreciate their meanings and
functions within the L2 community. The research literature on cross-cultural pragmatics
documents the rich intracultural variation of communicative action patterns and thus offers

11 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

compelling counter-evidence against unhelpful and often mutual stereotypes. For example, a
stereotype held by some Japanese learners of English is that Americans have a very direct
style of communication (Tanaka, 1988; Robinson, 1992); however, research on requests
(Blum-Kulka & House, 1989; Blum-Kulka, 1991) and refusals (Beebe, Takahashi, & UlissWeltz,, 1990; Beebe & Cummings, 1996) provides evidence to the contrary.

Practicing L2 pragmatic abilities


Turning to students options for practicing their L2 pragmatic abilities, such practice requires
student-centered interaction. In their books on tasks for language learning, Nunan (1989) and
Crookes and Gass (1993a, b) explain the rationale underlying a task-based approach from the
perspectives of second language acquisition and pedagogy. Most small group interaction
requires that students take alternating discourse roles as speaker and hearer, yet different
types of task may engage students in different speech events and communicative actions. It is
therefore important to identify very specifically which pragmatic abilities are called upon by
different tasks. A useful distinction can be made between referential and interpersonal
communication tasks. In referential communication tasks (Yule, in press), students have to
refer to concepts for which they lack necessary L2 words. Such tasks expand students'
vocabulary and develop their strategic competence. Interpersonal communication tasks are
more concerned with participants' social relationships and include such communicative acts
as opening and closing conversations, expressing emotive responses as in thanking and
apologizing, or influencing the other person's course of action as in requesting, suggesting,
inviting, and offering. Activities such as roleplay, simulation, and drama engage students in
different social roles and speech events. Such activities provide opportunities to practice the
wide range of pragmatic and sociolinguistic abilities (Crookall & Saunders, 1989; Crookall &
Oxford, 1990; Olshtain & Cohen, 1991) that students need in interpersonal encounters
outside the classroom.

Reconsidering pragmatic ability as a teaching goal


The purpose of the proposed learning activities is to help students become more effective and
successful communicators in L2. But what exactly does 'effective' and 'successful' mean? In
conclusion of this paper, I will briefly re-examine the goals that instruction in pragmatics
should aim for.
First, it may be useful to remind ourselves that NS are no ideal communicators. As Coupland,
Wiemann, and Giles, (1991, p. 3) comment, "language use and communication are (...)
pervasively and even intrinsically flawed, partial, and problematic". And yet, by and large NS
communication succeeds more than it fails - not because it is perfect but because it is good
enough for the purpose at hand. It would be unreasonable and unrealistic to place higher
demands on L2 learners' communicative abilities than on those of NS. Therefore, there is a
continued need for studies examining how NS and NNS communicate effectively in different
contexts.
Secondly, there often appears to be an implicit understanding that effective and successful
NNSs have the same or very similar pragmatic ability as NS. On this view, pragmatic
competence as a learning objective should be based on a NS model. However, as Siegal (1996)
points out, "Second language learners do not merely model native speakers with a desire to
emulate, but rather actively create both a new interlanguage and an accompanying identity in
the learning process" (1996, p. 362ff) Second language learners' desire for convergence with

12 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Can Pragmatic Competence be Taught?

http://www.nflrc.hawaii.edu/networks/NW06/

NS pragmatics or divergence from NS practices is shaped by learners' views of themselves,


their social position in the target community and in different contexts within the wider L2
environment, and by their experience with NS in various encounters.
Thirdly, members of the target community may perceive NNS's total convergence to L2
pragmatics as intrusive and inconsistent with the NNS's role as outsider to the L2 community,
whereas they may appreciate some measure of divergence as a disclaimer to membership.
Giles, Coupland, and Coupland (1991) documented that in many ethnolinguistic contact
situations, successful communication is a matter of optimal rather than total convergence.
Optimal convergence is a dynamic, negotiable construct that defies hard-and-fast definition. It
refers to pragmatic and sociolinguistic choices which are consistent with participants'
subjectivities and social claims, and recognizes that such claims may be in conflict between
participants.
Fourthly, as Peirce (1995) noted, language classrooms provide an ideal arena for exploring the
relationship between learners' subjectivity and L2 use. Classrooms afford second language
learners the opportunity to reflect on their communicative encounters and to experiment with
different pragmatic options. For foreign language learners, the classroom may be the only
available environment where they can try out what using the L2 feels like, and how more or
less comfortable they are with different aspects of L2 pragmatics. The sheltered environment
of the L2 classroom will thus prepare and support learners to communicate effectively in L2.
But more than that, by encouraging students to explore and reflect their experiences,
observations, and interpretations of L2 communicative practices and their own stances
towards them, L2 teaching will expand its role from that of language instruction to that of
language education.
Go to References.

13 of 13

6/25/14 10:49 PM

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen