Sie sind auf Seite 1von 55

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability: Can an artificial

auditory environment utilising music and sound enhance


the process of hypno-psychotherapy?

By Peter Bryant

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Table of Contents
Introduction ............................................................................................................................................ 4
Section 1: Terminology, Definitions and Assumptions. .......................................................................... 6
Suggestion and Suggestibility. ............................................................................................................ 6
Clarification of Terms: Suggestion and Suggestibility ..................................................................... 6
Suggestion ....................................................................................................................................... 6
Suggestibility ................................................................................................................................... 8
General Characteristics ................................................................................................................. 10
Suggestion ..................................................................................................................................... 10
Suggestibility ................................................................................................................................. 10
Hypnotic Susceptibility and Hypnotisability. .................................................................................... 11
Clarification of Terms: Hypnotisability.......................................................................................... 12
General Characteristics of Hypnotisability.................................................................................... 12
Trance ............................................................................................................................................... 13
Clarification of Terms: Trance ....................................................................................................... 13
General Characteristics: Trance .................................................................................................... 13
Absorption & Dissociation ................................................................................................................ 14
Clarification of Terms: Absorption and Dissociation .................................................................... 14
Absorption .................................................................................................................................... 14
General Characteristics: Absorption and Dissociation.................................................................. 14
Absorption .................................................................................................................................... 14
Dissociation ................................................................................................................................... 14
General Characteristics: Dissociation ........................................................................................... 15
Assumptions...................................................................................................................................... 15
Section 2: Music and the Mind. ............................................................................................................ 16
Therapeutic Properties of Music....................................................................................................... 16
Music, Mind and Brain ...................................................................................................................... 18
A Socio-cultural History of Music and Hypnosis ............................................................................... 21
The History of Therapeutic Music and Sound ................................................................................... 27
Sound and Brainwave Manipulation ............................................................................................. 27
Music Therapy ............................................................................................................................... 28
Section Three: Music and Hypnosis Research. ..................................................................................... 32
Procedure.......................................................................................................................................... 32
Scopus ........................................................................................................................................... 32
Web of Science.............................................................................................................................. 33

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

PubMed ......................................................................................................................................... 33
PsycINFO ....................................................................................................................................... 33
Other Publications ........................................................................................................................ 33
Section 4: General Overview of the Literature. .................................................................................... 35
H1. Can sound manipulate an individuals level of suggestibility? .................................................... 35
H2. Can sound manipulate an individuals hypnotic susceptibility? ................................................. 38
H3. Are these manipulations temporal or permanent? .................................................................... 40
H4. Which characteristics of sound are most likely to alter a listeners hypnotisability? ................. 41
H5. Could music or sound enhance the process of hypno-psychotherapy and if so, how?.............. 42
H6. How may these findings be best utilised by therapy practitioners? ........................................... 43
H7. Does being musically sophisticated increase a persons hypnotisability? .................................. 43
Section 5: Conclusion and considerations for further research. .......................................................... 44
Bibliography .......................................................................................................................................... 45

ABSTRACT: Music and Hypnosis have many similar qualities and properties. This
paper aims to investigate the methods by which music could aid the process of
hypno-psychotherapy. A systematic literature review of the topic is conducted.
Eighty-four relevant papers are discussed and analysed in order to establish an
empirical research base for the topic. It is found that the psychological properties
of absorption, dissociation, empathy, expectancy and rapport development are
common to both fields and can be used to assist clients with the
psychotherapeutic process in multiple ways, as discussed. Recommendations for
further research are provided alongside guidance and support for therapists
wishing to utilise music within their hypnotherapy and psychotherapy practices.

Main text word count = 11,990.

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Introduction
The psychological state which has been labelled hypnosis by scholars, practitioners and
researchers for most of its modern existence, is usually, and most effectively instigated via the use of
sound (American Psychological Association, 2014). This sound has traditionally been the words of a
therapist or a qualified professional who has delivered suggestions to the listener within the context
of a therapeutic intervention, to manage and assist with and alleviate unpleasant symptoms of a
particular mental disorder, health condition or habitual issue. Throughout the history of hypnosis,
there have been a number of questions raised by scholars as to the ability of music to produce
hypnotic phenomena and specifically musics capabilities to compliment and enhance a talkingtherapy session (Kennaway, 2011). This may be thanks to hypnosis and music enjoying a complex
and interconnected relationship due to their broadly similar psychological features such as
absorption, dissociation and expectancy (Rouget, 1985). In many cultures around the world, the idea
of utilising music for the benefit of health is relatively commonplace and is now gaining acceptance
in the western world (Saarikallio, 2012; Campbell & Doman, 2011).
With the rise in modern audio technology, it is now a common occurrence for talkingtherapists to utilise music regularly within their practices, both within the actual sessions themselves
and within their therapeutic recordings for client distribution (Brookhouse, 2013). Just how
widespread the utilisation of music within a therapeutic context is, does not currently seem to be
known, however, the majority of practitioners who utilise music within their sessions qualify it to
drown out extraneous noise or to fill the otherwise empty silence for the reassurance of their
subjects (and perhaps, if they are completely honest, also themselves) (Brookhouse, 2013).
This is one way of using music to benefit the process of therapy, however, I plan to argue
that the use of music can be exploited further as a tool to aid talking-therapists by heightening a
clients suggestibility and willingness to engage within the therapeutic process. This strategy, it is

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

hoped, would thereby improve the efficacy of psychotherapeutic procedures in a cost effective
manner, and without the negative side-effects of other solutions.
This paper will present a systematic literature review of the current and historical thinking
on this topic with the intention that these investigations promote future primary research within this
area.
I have a number of hypotheses that I wish to investigate during this literature review which
are as follows:
1) Does music or sound allow individuals to become more suggestible?
2) Can music or sound manipulate a persons level of hypnotisability?
3) If H1 and/or H2 are correct then are these alterations temporal or permanent?
4) If H2 is true then, which characteristics of sound are most likely to alter the listeners
hypnotisability and why might these musical characteristics adapt client hypnotisability?
5) How could these findings enhance the process of hypno-psychotherapy?
6) How best may these findings be utilised by practitioners in their psychotherapy practices
in order to achieve greater results with clients?
7) Does being musically sophisticated increase a persons ability to be hypnotised?

At the close of this paper, I shall recommend additional areas of research in order to
elaborate upon these initial hypotheses in order to improve the perspective on this topic for future
researchers. This paper is an overview of how sound and music can affect the mind. How music can
improve client-therapist relationships and sound can improve hypnotic and psychotherapeutic
interventions for the benefit of clients. I believe that music is a vitally important, cost-effective, sideeffect free, largely untapped source of potential for improving the lives of many people.

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Section 1: Terminology, Definitions and Assumptions.


Writing on topics such as Music, Mental Health, Medicine, Psychology, Hypnosis and
Suggestion unavoidably requires the use of jargon. Unfortunately, there is a disparity on the use of
some terminology throughout the literature, particularly with regard to hypnosis, which has suffered
many problems with the definition of its terms over the course of its history (Waterfield, 2004). I
have, therefore, dedicated this section to providing my working definitions of several terms that I
shall use throughout this paper in favour of ensuring that my meaning and use of each term is clear.

Suggestion and Suggestibility.


Suggestion and suggestibility have become largely synonymous within popular culture
thanks to the topic of hypnosis being prevalent in mainstream popular culture1 (Tasso & Perez,
2008). This is an issue for researchers and academics as there are several important distinctions
between the two definitions and their related phenomena which are relevant to hypnosis and their
associated mental processes.

Clarification of Terms: Suggestion and Suggestibility


Suggestion
Suggestion has seven definitions, according to the Oxford English Dictionary, only one of
which is within the remit of hypnosis:
The insinuation of a belief or impulse into the mind of a subject by words, gestures, or the
like; the impulse or idea thus suggested.
The six accompanying definitions are more common everyday definitions or niche uses of
the word (Oxford English Dictionary, 2014). The phenomenon of suggestion is pervasive in everyday
life; a young child injures himself, picks himself up and dusts himself down. Only then does he

See A Socio-cultural History of Music and Hypnosis on page 21.

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

witness his mothers expression of panic which prompts him to cry. It has therefore been suggested
to him that he is expected to react emotionally to this situation by his anxious mother (Gass & Seiter,
1999; Gwynn & Spanos, 1996; Tasso & Perez, 2008).
The above scenario is an example of everyday or non-hypnotic suggestion. Suggestion is not
a purely hypnotic phenomenon as some individuals believe; it is not a requirement for suggestion to
be within a therapeutic or even a so-called hypnotic context in order for it to be effective in
communicating ideas and beliefs. Thus slow down is a suggestion along with a whole plethora of
everyday statements that we regularly encounter in our lives within normal communication. The lack
of distinction between these different types of suggestion has caused some confusion within
academic texts which, as a result, now make a deliberate effort to explicitly categorise suggestions
as nonhypnotic suggestion (hurry up) and hypnotic suggestion. The only difference between these
two types of suggestion is that hypnotic suggestions are non-hypnotic suggestions conducted in a socalled trance state, or - at least - within the context of hypnosis (which may in itself, be a suggestion)
(Tasso & Perez, 2008). Accordingly, modern authors have argued that there are an almost limitless
number of types of suggestion depending on their context, whilst others have been more reserved
and stated that the ability to respond to both hypnotic and non-hypnotic suggestions is a global and
universal trait with similar properties to intelligence which has multiple facets and qualities (Tasso &
Perez, 2008, p. 284; Gardner, 2006).
It is generally agreed that there are three distinct components of suggestion (Bernheim,
1889):
1. The introduction of an idea (to the individual).
2. The acceptance of that idea (by the individual).
3. The realisation of that idea (by the behaviour, performance or attitude of the individual).
These three properties are vital for any suggestion to be effective. Everyday suggestion has a
pivotal role for our communicative abilities, which arguably, make us human. There is strong
7

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

evidence that our innate capacity for suggestion has directly contributed to our evolutionary success
as a species (Brown, 1991; Waterfield, 2004). It has been found that everyone of normal cognitive
ability is born with the psychological mechanisms and traits which enable them to be capable of
accepting suggestions. Despite its importance, the literature on the topic of suggestibility
(particularly non-hypnotic suggestibility) is still poor, due to it being side-lined in favour of research
on hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility which is arguably easier to objectify and empirically measure
(Tasso & Perez, 2008).

Suggestibility
Suggestibility is characterised as to how readily an individual will accept statements and
ideas (suggestions2) from others and subsequently act upon them. There is no requirement for the
presence of a hypnotic context which is a similar property for the use of the term suggestion.
However, in recent popular culture, hypnosis and suggestibility have become fused in the public
consciousness (along with hypnotic susceptibility or hypnotisability3). It is for this reason that the
term suggestibility is commonly used to refer to the psychological state of hypnosis, as suggestion is
arguably a central and vital facet and requirement of effective hypnosis (Kihlstrom, 2008, p. 26).
However, the literature posits that suggestibility and hypnotisability are separate psychological
constructs and should be treated as separate, mutually exclusive entities (Hilgard, 1973; 1991;
Bowers, 1983; De Pascalis, 1989; Edmonston, 1989; Evans, 1989; De Pascalis, et al., 1998; Tasso &
Perez, 2008). After all, suggestions form a pivotal role in hypnotic procedures in order to inspire
positive change. The only qualifier that they are hypnotic is that these suggestions are delivered to
the client during a so-called state or psychological framework of trance. However, in this paper, I
shall not use the term suggestibility to refer to an individual's capacity to enter a so-called
psychological state of hypnosis. Instead, I shall regard it as a term to describe how effectively both
2
3

See page 6-8 for a definition.


See page 11 for a definition.

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

hypnotic and non-hypnotic suggestions enact on the behaviour and attitude of the listener.
Unfortunately, for the purposes of clarity, highly suggestible individuals are usually ascribed to be
very competent hypnotic subjects. Highly hypnotisible individuals are seen to have an improved
ability to comply with verbal suggestions and thus, the efficacy of the therapeutic procedure is
enhanced4. This trend has lead to a confusion of the terms suggestibility and hypnotic susceptibility
or hypnotisability5. These pseudo-interchangeable terms even appear in dictionaries as the word
suggestibility, according to the Oxford English Dictionary, is synonymous with hypnotisability6:
Susceptibility to (hypnotic or other) suggestion (Oxford English Dictionary, 2014).
These definitions are somewhat unhelpful as the research literature has, correctly, begun to
separate the psychological train of suggestibility from that of hypnotisability.
An individuals level of suggestibility dictates how successful the second and third
components are from the three facets of suggestion7 and thus how readily a new idea is accepted by
that individual and to what extent it affects their life. This is where hypnosis (along with other
mechanisms) can be used as a tool in order to increase both hypnotisability and suggestibility using a
hypnotic and psychological environment. Research has indicated that suggestibility can be modestly
increased using a wide variety of factors from hypnosis to pharmacological products and other
psychological tools (Faymonville, et al., 2000). However, the ability of music or sound to enhance
suggestibility (with or without the use of hypnosis) is conspicuous only by its absence from the
majority of empirical research literature. For the purposes of this paper, I shall work according to the
definition that suggestibility is the degree to which an individual can accept non-hypnotic
suggestions. This is important, as it can be argued that music is a form of non-hypnotic suggestion

See the list of assumptions on page 15 for more on this.


For the sake of argument (and my word count) I shall use the term hypnotisability to mean hypnotic
susceptibility for the remainder of this paper.
6
Hypnotisability itself is not referenced within the OED. This is surprising as hypnotisability does feature
heavily within the literature. For example Lynn & Snodgrass Hypnotizability and music involvement (1986), or
Hypnotizability and blink rate by (Lichtenberg, et al., 2008) among others.
7
See the three distinct components of suggestion (Bernheim, 1889), summarised on page 7 of this paper.
5

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

which may be more effective when combined with the context and psychological attributes of
hypnosis. Music can be seen a parallel to linguistic suggestion; an idea can be communicated to an
individual either explicitly and consciously with language or unconsciously with sound and music.

General Characteristics
Suggestion
The general characteristics of suggestion are a deliberate instruction, idea or theme
communicated to an individual with the intent to alter their perspectives, beliefs, or attitudes in
order to invoke a paradigm shift. Thus, all forms of communication could be labelled as suggestive.
This includes written and verbal language, music, literature and art. Suggestion is capable of
permeating all of the senses including visual, olfactory, auditory, proprioceptive (Kinaesthetic),
somatosensory and gustatory perception. Suggestion is a facet of everyday life; we encounter it on
an almost continual basis in everyone we communicate with (Tasso & Perez, 2008).

Suggestibility
Suggestibility is the degree to which we accept these ubiquitous suggestions. Evidence has
suggested that a persons general level of suggestibility decreases with their age (Hilgard, 1965). This
may be due to a number of psychological biases which generally increase in power with our
cumulative life experience. However, there are as yet no general tests of suggestibility8. It has been
argued that there are three distinct types of suggestibility; Emotional suggestibility9, Physical
suggestibility and Intellectual suggestibility, but there may be as many types of suggestibility as there

Suggestibility tests of which there are eighteen common varieties, which were developed during the mid-toth
late 20 century such as the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibly Scales (SHSS) (Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959),
actually measure hypnotisability factors such as hypnotic quality and hypnotic depth (Laurence, et al., 2008;
Waterfield, 2004). For more information on these scales, including their potential in clinical practice see
(Weitzenhoffer, 2000, pp. 250-306)
9
This type may be particularly suitable for musical hypnosis thanks to music being a very effective mechanism
for conveying and manipulating emotional states (Juslin & Sloboda, 2001).

10

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

are types of suggestion with each type having its own factor (and measure) of suggestibility
(Edmonston, 1989).

Hypnotic Susceptibility and Hypnotisability.


Hypnotisability10 is a measure of an individuals capacity to complete a hypnotic experience
in terms of depth and performance (Kihlstrom, 2008). It can be defined as an individuals readiness
to enter a state of hypnosis whatever that may be - and subsequently the amount (and quality) of
hypnotic phenomena which can seemingly be achieved by the individual when experiencing
suggestions when under the psychological influence of hypnosis (Tasso & Perez, 2008). As we have
already witnessed, hypnotisability is commonly confused with suggestibility. In academic
environments, there are important requirements for clear distinctions between the two
psychological traits (Tasso & Perez, 2008). Hypnotisability has had a reasonably large interest from
academics throughout the 20th century. Clarke Hull made a pioneering attempt at quantifying the
quality of a hypnotic state with the use of scales, in order to assess an individuals ability to enter a
trance state (Hull, 1933). Subsequent development by other scholars and academics has enabled a
wealth of psychometric tests to be implemented in order to assess the quality of an individuals
hypnotic experience11 (Woody & Barnier, 2008). Researchers can now utilise a wide variety of tools
to assess the quality of an individuals hypnotic experience, but further work is needed from a
research perspective in specific areas. For example, the majority of scales designed to assess
hypnotisability aim to assess the depth of a trance, however, the existence of this depth and its
clinical methodology is contested by some researchers and practitioners in modern literature
(Kihlstrom, 2008).

10
11

Hypnotizability in American English.


See footnote 8 on page 10 for more information.

11

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Clarification of Terms: Hypnotisability


Hypnotisability can be subjectively measured using psychometric tests12. When an individual
reaches a point where they can no longer manifest a subsequent phenomenon that the researcher
or therapist is requesting, they are said to have reached their current level of hypnotic susceptibility.
Tests of hypnotic susceptibility tend to be used primarily in research environments rather than as an
aid to therapeutic intervention as most therapists deem them to be an unnecessary hindrance to the
therapeutic process with the exception of the most sceptical of clients (Brookhouse, 2013).

General Characteristics of Hypnotisability


Unlike suggestibility, hypnotisability has been shown to remain relatively constant
throughout an individuals life (Piccione, et al., 1989). Research has suggested that there are
individual differences in hypnotisability with around 5-10% of the population finding a hypnotic state
easy to engage with and 5-10% finding hypnosis extremely difficult to achieve with a broad spectrum
in-between these two extremes. This spectrum (generally considered to be identical to many other
personality and psychological trait) is represented as a Gaussian bell curve. However, hypnotisability
can be affected by personality traits such as an individuals capacity for absorption13 and certain
types of intelligence (Geiger, et al., 2014; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2007). Other research has
demonstrated that small alterations and enhancements in hypnotisability can be made depending
upon the individuals environment, personal experiences and genetic makeup (Gorassini, 2004).
Strong correlates which do appear to be related to measures of hypnotisability are absorption
(Tellegen & Atkinson, 1974; de Groh, 1989), fantasy proneness (Lynn & Rhue, 1988), empathy
(Wickramasekera & Szlyk, 2003) and self-directedness14 (Laidlaw, et al., 2005). It has been noted that
children have higher hypnotic susceptibility scores than adults, possibly due to their innate capacity
for imaginative exercises. There are no physiological traits or identifiers of hypnotisability.

12

See page 9, footnote 7 Suggestibility tests.


See definition on page 14.
14
Individuals with an internal locus of control had lower hypnotisability scores than individuals with an
external locus of control.
13

12

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Furthermore, an individuals hypnotisability can be modestly adjusted with drugs, abnormal


circumstances or environments and with repeat exposure to hypnosis (Hilgard, 1965).

Trance
A trance is an altered state of consciousness from normal consciousness (Wier, 2007, p. 58).
The debate on whether hypnosis is a trance state has been a point of contention for the majority of
the 20th century (Waterfield, 2004). However, for the purposes of this paper, the distinction is
unimportant. Few scholars would argue that the act of hypnosis causes no psychological change for
any individual. In this essay, I choose to call this state a trance. Whether it be caused by social
pressure to conform or by a different set of brainwave patterns on an EEG machine is not a crucial
distinction for my argument to stand. The term trance has been used throughout the majority of
music literature to reference rituals of demonic possession, absorption, dissociation and other
psychological constructs. I shall refer to them all as a state of trance and explicitly state which type I
am discussing at the relevant time.

Clarification of Terms: Trance


As discussed above, throughout this essay, I shall use the term trace to indicate the state of
hypnosis. I am defining the state of trance according to Beckers definition as:
A cognitive, emotional, and behavioural state characterized by the loss of sense of self, the
cessation of inner languaging [sic], and an extraordinary ability to withstand fatigue or pain (Becker,
2004).
I will utilise the assumption that hypnosis (both hetero and solo) results in a successful state
of trance throughout this paper.

General Characteristics: Trance


The characteristics of trance have been widely documented and have been subject to
exaggeration. Unlike hypnotic susceptibility, there are physiological and behavioural patterns to the

13

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

trance state; however, these cannot indicate for certain that an individual is in trance and should be
used with caution (Barber, 1961).

Absorption & Dissociation


The interrelated psychological traits of absorption and dissociation have provided a possible
psychological mechanism for trance, hypnosis and other phenomena associated with an altered
state of consciousness involving music to occur (Herbert, 2011).

Clarification of Terms: Absorption and Dissociation


Absorption
Absorption is defined as
an effortless, non-volitional quality of deep involvement with the objects of consciousness
(Jamieson, 2005).
This is in contrast to goal-directed attention, which is effortful, rational and progress
directed (Herbert, 2011, p. 5).

General Characteristics: Absorption and Dissociation


Absorption
Absorption has been identified as a contributing factor to strong or peak musical
experiences (Lowis, 1998).

Dissociation
The treatment of dissociation will be purely non-pathological for the entirety of this paper.
Non-pathological dissociation is defined as:
an altered state of consciousness that is not organically induced, and that involves the
temporary alteration or separation of normally integrated mental processes in conscious awareness
(Butler & Palesh, 2004).

14

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

General Characteristics: Dissociation


Dissociation involves a temporary detachment from immediate surroundings and reality. It is
commonly displayed on a continuum from everyday experiences such as daydreaming to severe
pathological breakdowns (Dell & O'Neil, 2009, p. 18). It is worth noting, that, despite its similarities
with hypnosis (indeed, dissociation is considered to be the hallmark psychological quality of the
hypnotic experience15), several studies have shown that it does not appear to predict hypnotic
susceptibility or hypnotic responsiveness (Faith & Ray, 1994; Silva & Kirsch, 1992). Although, it has
also been found that individuals diagnosed with dissociative identity disorder have the highest
hypnotisability scores of any clinical group (Spiegel, et al., 2011).

Assumptions
Throughout this paper, I adhere to several assumptions (for the sake of argument) which are
not yet ready to be clarified by the research literature. None of these assumptions are controversial,
and although general acceptance is no proof of the statement being correct, all my assumptions are
commonly held beliefs among the majority of empirically minded hypnotherapists and scholars.
1. Better quality hypnotic experiences lead to higher efficacy of effective interventions.
2. The higher a persons hypnotisability, the more likely they are to have a higher quality
hypnotic experience.

15

See (Orne, 1959; 1966; Bowers, 1983; Frankel, 1976; Hilgard, 1986; Hilgard & Hilgard, 1994) for more on this.

15

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Section 2: Music and the Mind.


Therapeutic Properties of Music
Music is inherently suggestive, although its ability to explicitly communicate or express
anything with a similar quality to language is highly lacking (Patel, 2008, p. 301). In modern times,
there have been many debates between scholars as to musics ability to communicate concepts,
ideas and objects in a similar way to language (Levitin, 2006; Patel, 2008). These debates are still
ongoing, but aside from a strong argument as to the development of language itself (Mithen, 2007),
music seems to be essentially redundant in modern communicative processes (Pinker, 1997). Music
scholars and composers have debated musics ability to communicate messages throughout the 20th
century. Ivor Stravinsky stated his controversial opinion in 1936 in his autobiography that:
music is, by its very nature, essentially powerless to express anything at all If, as is nearly
always the case, music appears to express something, this is only an illusion and not a reality. It is
simply an additional attribute which, by tacit and inveterate agreement, we have lent it, thrust upon
it, as a label, a convention in short, an aspect which, unconsciously or by force of habit, we have
come to confuse with its essential being (Stravinsky, 1936, pp. 53-54).
Having said this, this additional attribute is extremely powerful in television, films and in
our lives. Anecdotal evidence of the power of music to stimulate memory, improve performance at
cognitive tasks (such as creativity and athletic ability) and to contribute to emotional and mood
management are beginning to be supported by a large amount of empirical evidence (Whaley, et al.,
2009)16.
The academic study of music and its potential to alter the mind has been small compared
with the references from popular culture. Rumours of musics alleged ability to brainwash the mind
have been relatively commonplace throughout history; over one thousand years ago, musics ability
16

For more on the topic of music, healing and its power to accelerate and improve cognitive development see
(Campbell & Doman, 2011).

16

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

to control the emotive state of listeners deemed it an unattractive addition to church services as
religious leaders feared that music had the capacity to influence the ethos of listeners, for good or
for ill and feared its power over the human psyche (Burkholer, et al., 2010, p. 25). There have even
been specific composers whose music has been associated with hypnosis and hypnotic phenomena
the most prolific of which is Richard Wagner whose music has been perceived to induce hypnotic like
properties in its listeners, primarily through the use of highly emotive passages (Warthin, 1894)17.
The understanding of music as a psychological tool to aid our comprehension of the mind
has increased in recent times thanks to a greater recognition of the power of our own psychological
processes and new technology. Unfortunately, this technological development has fed a wave of
reports of musics alleged contribution to so-called suggestion based suicides, murders and other
similar incidents (Kennaway, 2012). On the other side of the spectrum, there is the rapidly expanding
field of music therapy which is increasingly becoming a highly popular therapy modality18. All these
findings can assist other therapists in how to use music constructively and responsibly with their
own clients in order to improve their therapeutic results.

17

There is an extremely recent experiment (currently in press), where eight volunteers were asked to listen to
the entirety of Wagners Ring Cycle whilst their heart rate, sweat level and micro-movements were monitored.
Preliminary results suggest that individual psychological responses can be prompted by musical techniques
such as the use of leitmotifs in order to manipulate the audiences reaction (Goldsmiths, University of London,
2014).
18
See page 28 for more information.

17

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Music, Mind and Brain


Musics ability to influence the mind has been shown in a wide variety of domains
throughout history. Examples include playing tempo-calibrated music in supermarkets and
restaurants in order to manipulate how long individuals spend within these establishments. Sonic
techniques have been promoted to businesses as a potential method of increasing turnover by
controlling how much money individuals spend within these respective environments (Gueguen, et
al., 2007).
Music has been used in commercial settings, media and by individuals themselves to suggest
everything from moods, ideas and products throughout history. Research into this area has
uncovered a wealth of surprising findings which have influenced business practices. For example,
supermarkets can suggest to customers which wines to purchase using music from different
(stereotypical) nationalities and different associated musical instruments. The presence of French
music in a wine isle (as indicated with the traditional French instrument; the accordion) can lead to
an increase in sales of French wine by a factor of five. A similar result has been found with the use of
the Germanic oompah band music increasing German wine sales by a factor of two (North, et al.,
1997). The music was acting (perhaps unconsciously) as a suggestion as to the nationality of the
wine to be chosen by the customers. It is this type of musical suggestion that I am aiming to
investigate and whether it can be applied for more than purely commercial means.
One of the most obvious and powerful mechanisms of the capacity of sound to manipulate
the mind is called Pavlovian conditioning. The work of Ivan Pavlov (1849 1936) successfully
demonstrates that concepts and ideas can be associated with external objects such as dogs
associating food with the sound of a tuning fork, bell or metronome (Pavlov, 1927). This conditioned
response could then, with repeated exposure and training, be instigated even when there was no
food present. Since these studies, it has been shown that conditioning affects humans on many

18

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

levels and can be extremely powerful as a therapeutic tool19. There have been a number of cases
where musical instruments have been used in order to aid hypnotherapy interventions, in particular
the use of a gong is favoured by some practitioners as an exotic and dramatic instrument to aid
therapeutic change (Livnay, 2014).
Is that all there is to music as a tool for therapeutic and cognitive change? Can there be a
mechanism for aiding the process of hypnosis using music besides standard Pavlovian conditioning?
Further examples of the power of music include noise and sound control in the world of gambling
and in other entertainment media such as television shows and films where we are provided with an
auditory accompaniment which enables the viewer to gain a better understanding of the context of
the visual information being presented. One only has to watch a horror film or a comedy with the
sound turned off to appreciate the value of a soundtrack, score or canned laughter track. Sound and
music convey highly-emotive messages in an extremely effective and efficient manner. Architects
have long exploited the auditory properties of the environments which they create, particularly
within the entertainment industry. Greek amphitheatres are renowned for their spectacular auditory
properties. This is mainly due to their circular, tiered design and the specific properties of the type of
limestone that went into their construction. These considerations helped to dampen unwanted lowfrequency crowd noise and boosted the actors voices at higher frequencies (Declercq & Dekeyser,
2007). Modern examples include casino design where hard, sonically reflective surfaces are
maximised in order to allow a higher degree of sound reflection and reverberation. These reflections
allow the casino to sound and seem busier than using soft surfaces. Busy casinos promote and
motivate gamblers to bet more money, more frequently and increase impulsivity due to the
psychology of social proof and social conformity (Friedman, 2000, pp. 135-137). Another example,
from modern warfare, features the playlists of soldiers stationed in territories overseas, which
contain an extremely high amount of heavy metal and other violent music. Soldiers readily admit to

19

This school of thought is broadly referred to as Behaviourism.

19

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

using music in order to alter their mindset in order to allow them to become monsters and commit
what they commonly refer to as inhumane acts in the name of war (Gilman, 2010).
The benefit of music has not gone unnoticed in a variety of domains related to psychology
and performance improvement. For example, the use of music during cardio-vascular exercise can
improve performance by as much as 15% (Edworthy & Waring, 2006). Other transference effects
have been noted, for example, an enhanced capability to complete spatial tasks (Rauscher, et al.,
1993), increased work productivity and enhanced reading comprehension, among other
psychological improvements (Music Works, 2014)20.
Music has been isolated as a key factor in rapidly transforming our sense of self via an
altered state of consciousness known as a trance. Music is used throughout the world in order to
enhance trance or ecstatic rituals (Becker & Penman, 2009). However, the relationship between
music and trance is complex and there is, as yet, no evidence that music alone can place a listener in
a state of trance without the correct context and the listeners implied consent (Heap, 2013). The
effects demonstrated with trance states and music require both language and music working in situ
in order to have their full transformative effect. However, there is not currently enough research to
rule out whether instrumental music alone could, in the right circumstances, have a comparable
effect purely of its own merit (Patel, 2008, pp. 324-325).

20

For more on this please see Victoria Williamsons excellent book You are the Music (2014). Also see the
fascinating Music Works project (www.musicworksforyou.com).

20

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

A Socio-cultural History of Music and Hypnosis


Among the scattered and disparate literature on the topic of music and hypnosis are a
number of key articles which attempt to address basic questions about the links and contrasts
between the two fields. An understanding of the historical and current attitudes towards music and
hypnosis in popular culture is important, as personal expectation, which depends and is influenced
by the attitudes of popular culture, is a trait shared by both music and hypnosis (Lynn, et al., 2008, p.
122; Stevens & Byron, 2011). Presently, the most prominent of these articles is by James Kennaway
who discusses the history of music, hypnosis and trance states from their earliest beginnings to the
present day (Kennaway, 2011). Kennaway begins by tracing the history of music and trance back to
the late eighteenth century where the early development of hypnosis then referred to as animal
magnetism occurred (Waterfield, 2004). Music had obviously existed long before these hypnotic
effects were popularised by Franz Mesmer. Mesmer not only utilised music and sound to
therapeutic effect, (referred to by him as sympathetic vibration) but also developed a theory that
auditory phenomena could be used to communicate, propagate and reinforce what came to be
understood as modern-day hypnotic suggestion (Mesmer, 1779, p. 78; Goldschmidt, 1983;
Kennaway, 2010). Indeed, it has been heavily speculated that pianos, violins and harps in particular
the glass armonica which Mesmer frequently used in his treatments are directly responsible for
the successes and triumphs of his bogus therapy of magnetically aligning human universal fluid
(Polter, 1934, p. 15; Franklin, 1785, p. 23; Gallo & Finger, 2000; Hadlock, 2000; Hyatt King, 1945).
This hypothesis gains more validity when we consider that many of Mesmers clients had very strong
musical experiences throughout their lives, such as auditory hallucinations, cures achieved with the
aid of music and stories of tone-deaf patients suddenly developing musical talent whilst in a
magnetized sleep (Wirth, 1836, p. 180; Kretschmar, 1824, pp. 41-42; Spiritus, 1819, pp. 83-4; Baehr
& Kohlschtter, 1843, p. ix). Today, music has a longstanding relationship with healthcare thanks to

21

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

the development of Music Therapy21 an intervention which is specifically designed around the
modality of music and its capacity to improve the mind (The American Music Therapy Association,
2014). Unfortunately, the public opinion of hypnosis has not progressed as much as the opinion of
music

therapy

within

western

culture22.

Other practitioners of hypnosis began to see the benefits of using sound and music within
their performances. The beginnings of hypnosis were usually displays of women reaching a crisis
seemingly caused by the techniques of a male hypnotist. The sexualised nature of this practice has
been discussed at length23 (Braid, 1843; Kaplan, 1974). Of particular note are Jean-Martin Charcots
use of gongs, tuning forks and other percussive instruments in order to provoke cataleptic fits or
hysterical hypnosis (Regnard, 1887). Other individuals such as Paul Regnard, Paul Richer, Alfred
Binet and Charles Fr also used percussive instruments in addition to simple melodies and
childrens lullabies with similar results to Charcot (Regnard, 1887, pp. 261-2; Richer, 1881, pp. 599600; Binet & Fr, 1887, pp. 88-89, 93). From the perspective of clients, it may have seemed as if the
sound itself was contributing something to these performances by allowing them to be more
outlandish and incredible than performances without the aid of music.
Moving into the 19th century, musical hypnosis became of interest to mainstream
composers, in particular, Wagner. I have already discussed that he was suspected of using hypnotic
techniques to aid his compositions and, as a direct result of his new compositional style, was labelled
a mesmerist and a master of hypnosis. These terms were intended to be highly derogatory
implying that he could control and manipulate his audience without their prior consent (Nietzsche,
1888, pp. 104, 99). Wagners personal notes are full of references to the current thinking on the
topic of musical hypnosis, and he was quite obviously fascinated by the concept (von Herrmann,
21

See page 28.


Today, music therapy is effectively a chartered profession whilst hypnotherapy and even psychotherapy is
still largely unregulated.
23
It is interesting that music was chosen as a means of enhancing hypnotic technique as music performance
th
was a predominantly female pastime during the 18 century (Burkholer, et al., 2010). The use of music in
therapeutic sessions may have enhanced the rapport between Mesmer and his female clients thereby
improving the alleged performance of animal magnetism.
22

22

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

2007). At the time, popular culture contained frequent references to hypnosis, indeed a novel
entitled Trilby was published in 1894 where musical hypnosis features as a major plot mechanism.
The novels antagonist Svengali hypnotises the protagonist (a young, innocent and nave teenage girl
called Trilby) into performing hypnosis around Europe before marrying her (du Maurier, 1985).
If anyone suspected that the anxieties surrounding music and hypnosis would abate during
the 20th century - when the frequently wild claims could be examined according to the scrutiny of
the scientific method, rather than supported by anecdotal evidence - they were to be disappointed.
Alongside the huge technological advances promoted by the first and second world wars came new
fears about music and its ability to influence the mind, in particular, its use as a tool for
brainwashing. Music could now be recorded, manipulated and then distributed in processes that
would have been unimaginable to composers and performers during the 18th century (Rouget, 1985,
p. 121). Once again, popular culture was ahead of academic interest as anxieties about the Cold War
fed into mainstream media including manipulation by music. Cinema (once itself considered to be a
form of hypnotism (Andriopoulos, 2008)) contributed to this by publicising films such as The
Manchurian Candidate and A Clockwork Orange which depicted the Ludovico Technique a fictitious
form of aversion therapy which involved watching atrocious images of violence whilst listening to
music (Burgess, 1966).
Academic interest in using auditory cues in order to influence the mind took a revolutionary
step when Ivan Pavlov started conducting experiments with dogs (Pavlov, 1928)24. This, now famous
research, sparked the academic debate into whether humans could be primed to behave in a similar
fashion to the dogs in Pavlovs experiments; namely, to respond to auditory stimuli completely
unconsciously auditory hypnosis (Thomas, 1994).
The development of popular music brought with it further anxieties about control. Genres
such as rock and heavy metal (potentially a straw man approach by lawyers thanks to its general
reliance on satanic themes and evil iconography) drawing strong criticism from governments for

24

To save me repeating myself, please see my previous discussion of Pavlovian conditioning on page 18.

23

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

their themes of rebellion, evil and death (Kennaway, 2011). This criticism was initially centred at the
sheer popularity of the music; for example, right-wing Christian David Noebel has argued that rock
music is literally a communist plot orchestrated by Russian researchers in order to take control of
Americas youth. He cites that the widespread popularity of the Beatles during the 1970s and 80s
as proof of his argument (Ostendorf, 1997; Noebel, 1974; Noebel, 1982).
It is not just the music which has come under attack but also the lyrics. There have been a
number of violent incidents and suicides allegedly caused by music that the perpetrators were
listening to prior to the incidents taking place. Concern arose about the phenomenon of subliminal
messages encoded into the lyrics of tracks which, it was argued, could instruct people to become
violent, impulsive or act more impulsively. The first alleged instance of subliminal messaging was not
in auditory form but in film. In 1957, James Vicary claimed to have developed a new way of
marketing confectionary to cinema audiences using subliminal messages such as drink coke and
eat popcorn at extremely short durations whilst the film was being performed (Packard, 1957).
Subsequent research has shown that this technique is not as profitable or as reliable as Vicary
claimed in his research, in fact, he had made the whole thing up in an effort to revive his consulting
business (Moore, 1992; Pratkanis, 1992). Despite this, the auditory equivalent of subliminal
messaging called backmasking gained even more public attention than its visual (likely fictitious)
analogy. A popular claim was that bands such as Judas Priest were deliberately inserting barely
audible, reversed, satanic messages into recordings, which, it was argued, could affect the
unconscious minds of listeners. It was claimed that these messages could unconsciously alter the
behaviour, beliefs and attitudes of the listeners without conscious awareness, resulting in increased
suicidal tendencies and other violent behaviours (Lilienfeld, et al., 2010). Experts have often
disagreed about precisely what is being said in these messages as they are, by definition, almost
impossible to understand (Kennaway, 2011). Nevertheless, this has not held back high profile
litigation as artists such as Ozzie Osborne who was (unsuccessfully, as the case was thrown out on
the grounds of free speech) sued in 1985 based on the accusation that his song suicide solution had
24

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

caused a listener to attempt suicide. This suicide, it was argued, had been instigated via the process
of musical brainwashing. Another case, again in 1985, saw two teenagers shoot themselves. Their
parents blamed the heavy metal band Judas Priest, claiming that satanic incantations are revealed
when the music is played backwards (Wright, 2000).
Further examples of the harm of alleged musical brainwashing can be found in the 1990s
after Richard Kuntz committed suicide whilst listening to Marilyn Manson and media reports blamed
Manson for the Columbine school massacre in 1999 (Wright, 2000). In all of these cases, individuals
have testified in court that it was the music that was the primary culprit of these atrocities. The
arguments for the method of use is remarkably similar to one of the theories regarding how
hypnosis may operate (i.e. the critical factor theory25) in that if someone were to hear the phrase
God is Satan, the brain would reject it straight away. However, if someone were to hear the phrase
dog si natas or Satan is God backwards then it would be decoded by the right (or creative) part
of the brain and stored as a fact (Aranza, 1983). The academic interest in this area is continuing but
does not currently support the idea that musical brainwashing is a large contributing factor in
teenage suicides (Scheel & Westefeld, 1999; Rosenbaum & Prinsky, 1991).
Although the idea of music and hypnosis has had an influential and notorious history, the
idea of musical hypnosis for brainwashing has significant weaknesses. Firstly, there is scant evidence
to suggest that people can be hypnotised against their will or without their knowledge an almost
essential requirement for any hypnotic procedure (Heap & Aravind, 2001). Furthermore, the quasihypnotic states which have been associated with the history of this topic may be more accurately
described as a voluntary, self-controlled, learned change of self-consciousness (Erlmann, 1982).
This is important, as these findings do not rule out that music can contribute to a trance experience
when an individual is adequately informed about what is taking place and consents to it.
Furthermore, the effects of music upon any trance state are, at best, minimal from a brainwashing

25

This theory states that suggestions in hypnotised individuals do not undergo the same level of critical
analysis as suggestions in non-hypnotised individuals see (Zarren & Eimer, 2002, pp. 6-7).

25

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

perspective, although they may a much more significant benefit for psychotherapeutic outcomes
and interventions (Fachner, 2006; Schipkowensky, 1977).
An ethical therapist then may be able to use music in order to enhance therapy and
accomplish greater therapeutic achievements than with standard silent therapy alone. That being
said, it has been found that music may only have a limited effect in the practice of hypnosis, and it
has been shown that the participants have to be in some way complicit with the way in which the
music is being used (Critchley & Henson, 1977). This is not a problem for therapists and other
professions who would be glad of any method which can be seen to improve the lives and
therapeutic results of their clients. I would argue that the therapeutic use of hypnosis, which has so
far been pushed to the sidelines of research and implementation due to the fears and anxieties of
brainwashing deserves to be investigated to the fullest extent. In the next section, I aim to amass all
the available primary research literature on the therapeutic use of musical hypnosis.

26

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

The History of Therapeutic Music and Sound


Sound and Brainwave Manipulation
One of the first academic papers to address the topic of direct musical manipulation of the
mind was written by Heinrich Wilhelm Dove, a physicist and meteorologist who discovered the
phenomenon of binaural beats in the beginning of the 19th century (Dove, 1839; Oster, 1973). Ever
since the invention of the Electroencephalogram (EEG) it has been claimed that sound may be able
to alter brainwave patterns. Indeed, this is the most popular hypothesis of musical manipulation of
the mind. Binaural beats have become the central mechanism for these claims which posit that they
are able to adjust conscious awareness by altering the firing rates of neurones by a process called
entrainment. Binaural beats are constructed by the superposition of two different tones. The degree
to which these tones differ in frequency is directly related to the frequency of the binaural beat. For
example, if I performed two tones, one with a frequency of four-hundred and forty hertz to your left
ear and one with a frequency of four-hundred and sixty hertz to your right ear via headphones, then
you would perceive a binaural beat of twenty hertz. It is claimed, that a binaural beat of twenty
hertz (that is, the difference between the left and right ears) is capable of manipulating your
neurones to fire at this rate (i.e. twenty times per second) and thus affect conscious experience. It is
generally agreed that normal consciousness has a neuron frequency of ten to fourteen hertz
whereas an individual who is asleep (or in a state of trance) would display brainwave activity around
four to five hertz or lower. However, evidence behind this theory has been lacking and has proven
difficult to validate.
A theory of brainwave entrainment was first postulated as a supporting theory for evidence
for a similar effect to binaural beats in the visual domain (Herrmann, 2001; Budzynski, 2006). This
theory was extrapolated into the auditory domain and applied to the phenomena of binaural beats
(Foster, 1990). Modern studies on binaural beats were conducted in the latter half of the 20th
century, possibly due to their demand for synthetic audio technology (the phenomena, owing to the

27

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

use of pure sinusoidal tones, do not normally exist in the natural environment (Oster, 1973). The
results of these studies have been mixed. Some show effects in the application of binaural beat
technology for stress management, pain reduction, and behavioural improvement (Huang &
Charyton, 2008) along with affect vigilance (attention) and improvement of mood (Lane, et al.,
1998). Other research has demonstrated no or very little effect, for example, with pain perception
(Wahbeh, et al., 2007; Stevens, et al., 2003). However, this murky research landscape on the topic
has not stopped claims by practitioners of being able to induce alterations in listener consciousness,
along with a large amount of other desirable (and marketable) mental effects. Binaural beats have
even been marketed and used as a form of auditory drug (gethightnow.com, 2013). This is
anecdotally regarded as being a feature of brainwave entrainment, although the supporting
evidence and research for brainwave entrainment, contrary to popular opinion, is severely lacking
(Turow & Lane, 2011). Sadly, the empirical research literature on binaural beats has proven to be a
lot less exciting than the claims made by the retailers of binaural beat audio products with very little
evidence of their efficacy in treating psychological conditions such as stress, anxiety or pain
perception (Bryant, 2012). This is not to say that music itself may be ineffective in assisting with
psychological ailments as binaural beats are unusually harsh, unnatural sounds and the mechanism
by which they are claimed to work is dubious, at best. Other types of music, along with other
auditory interventions, have shown a lot more potential to be therapeutically viable and effective.

Music Therapy
Despite the lack of evidence for sound directly altering brain states via binaural beats and
entrainment, to claim that music has no place in altering the mind or provide a therapeutic influence
would be highly incorrect. The therapeutic properties of music have demonstrated highly positive
results under empirical examination. Inherent musical properties such as the social, communicative

28

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

and cognitive aspects of musical performance and improvisation play a vital role in music therapy
which is:
An evidence-based clinical intervention utilising music and sound to address physical,
emotional, cognitive and social needs of individuals (American Music Therapy Association, 2014).
There is a huge amount of research demonstrating the positive effects of performing,
listening to and improvising music for a wide variety of medical and psychological conditions
including depression (Maratos, et al., 2008), assisting individuals with Autism and other learning
disabilities (Geretsegger, et al., 2014), mental health disorders such as Schizophrenia or
Schizophrenia-like disorders (Mossler, et al., 2013), and acquired brain injury (Bradt, et al., 2010).
This research landscape has tended to focus on music as an interactive and social activity rather than
directly on the psychological qualities of music (such as the type and genre of the music and what it
means for the listener). However, some important findings can be taken from the research literature
of music therapy and applied to other therapies (such as hypnotherapy and psychotherapy) in order
to improve client care. A number of scholars have drawn parallels between the techniques of
hypnosis and those used in music therapy. For example, music can capture the attention of a listener
in much the same way that a hypnotic suggestion by seeming to attract the conscious awareness of
the listener wholly and fully (Vas, 2013). Furthermore, argues the Hungarian scholar Jzsef Vas:
Hypnosis can be seen as an inductive method expressing its effect from outside to inside the
client or participant, however, music can be used as a deductive therapeutic tool, effecting from
inside to outside (Vas, 2013).
Other scholars have also noted similarities between hypnosis procedures and music.
Rhythmic practices which lead to:
phase locking when two or more objects are pulsing at nearly the same time tend to lock
in and begin pulsing at the same rate...the rhythm can bring the two (therapist and client) together

29

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

on a level which is distinct from verbal communication...even more basic and significant." (Moreno,
1988, p. 271)
This is not just an apt description of a musical style and technique, but also the hypnotic
practice of pacing and leading which is an inherent aspect of modern hypno-psychotherapeutic
practice (Livnay, 2014).
Hypnosis has also been a highly effective tool in studying auditory hallucinations. Hypnosis
has been used in several studies to create auditory hallucinations which could be studied in a PET
scanner. Highly hypnotisable participants were not able to differentiate between a recording of a
voice and their own auditory hallucination of a voice in their head allowing researchers to gain a
valuable insight into the neuroscience of psychosis (Szechtman, et al., 1998).
Clearly the overlap between music therapy and other modalities is only just beginning to be
investigated. The use of music within psychotherapy in general has demonstrated several important
properties which can assist in the therapy intervention such as: increasing levels of arousal (Berlyne,
1971; Madsen, et al., 1975), evoking curiosity (Berlyne & Borsa, 1968), improving the ability to focus
and diverting attention away from other potentially distracting senses (Hernandez-Peon, 1961;
Marteniuk, 1976), providing a positive emotional experience (Berlyne, 1971), and enticing an
emotive response (Juslin & Sloboda, 2001). However, both hypnotic and musical interventions have
their uses in facilitating an improved performance in the psychotherapy process, for example,
listening to music has been shown to decrease levels of arousal due to stress (Pelletier, 2004);
increase client self-disclosure during highly emotive procedures (Jensen, 2001); assist therapist with
client resistance by allowing the client to seemingly control the session utilising music (Stanton,
1980); and assist with relaxation techniques (including, crucially, progressive muscle relaxation)
(Robb, 2000). Music can even assist and improve induction procedures (Morris, 1986); alongside
client support strategies such as self-hypnosis (Walker, 1990); and assist with therapeutic deepening
techniques (Walker & Diment, 1979) alongside other factors which assist therapy clients by

30

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

contributing directly to the therapeutic process (Granone, 1973). Furthermore, music can benefit
from hypnotic procedures (for example, by combating performance anxiety) in order to produce
enhanced performances (as judged by experimentally blinded music critics) (Mellgren, 1979). Music
can also be used as an adjunct to hypno-psychotherapy in the care of cancer patients, by focusing
the mind, and by allowing emotions to be triggered and controlled. These powerful strategies,
alongside other therapeutic techniques such as anchoring, assisting with hypnotic imagery and
enabling a greater sense of focus enable hypnosis and music to be powerful allies in combating
complex mental health and other psychological issues (Esser, 2007).

31

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Section Three: Music and Hypnosis Research.


Procedure
This paper shall use the following research databases in order to provide a systematic
literature review of the topic of Music, Suggestion and Hypnosis. To perform this review, several
databases will be used to assess the range of literature that currently exists. Multiple databases are
used in order to gain a complete overview of the present research landscape. These databases will
include Scopus26, Web of Science27, PubMed28, PsycINFO29, and Google Scholar30. Articles that were
unavailable online were sourced from the British Library or via International Inter-Library Loan. All
articles will be assessed for their quality during the search process by their relevance to this papers
hypotheses. Primary research findings and data will not be collated in this paper as there are too
many research variables and not enough studies to perform a fair and comprehensive meta-analysis
at this time. All searches were performed on the 29th of September 2014 using the search terms
Hypnosis, Music, Suggestibility, and Hypnotizability (the z/s spelling is automatically redundant
in all databases). Searches were conducted by Music followed by either Hypnosis, Suggestibility
or Hypnotisability. Furthermore, there was no time specification provided for the search resulting
in a complete publication history for these search terms. The methodology of the search process is
broken down only in the Scorpus search in order to save words, but is identical for all the included
databases.

Scopus
Scorpus is the largest up-to-date collection of global, unbiased and expertly sourced
research data in the world (Elsevier, 2014). Music and Hypnosis yielded five results (based on title
26

http://www.scopus.com/ (Requires university login).


www.webofknowledge.com (Requires university login).
28
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed
29
http://www.apa.org/pubs/databases/psycinfo/
30
http://scholar.google.co.uk/
27

32

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

only a strict search) with 289 results when the search included the article title, abstract and
keywords (a loose search). Of these 289 results, only 38 were relevant to this paper ranging from
1956 to 2014. Music and Hypnotisability yielded 10 results in the loose search; it was, therefore,
not deemed necessary to include the results of a strict search (1 result, present in loose search).
Music and Suggestibility yielded 6 results for a strict search. A further search of Music and
Suggestion produced 654 results of which only 8 were relevant for this paper. Upon collating the
results of this database search and removing, duplicate papers, a total of 51 papers were extracted
from the Scorpus search for inclusion in this paper.

Web of Science
The total number of articles from this database, after elimination of repeated papers, is 13
resulting in a cumulative total of 64 papers.

PubMed
This search produced 7 unique papers. A total of 71 papers had now been discovered.

PsycINFO
A total of 15 new articles were discovered in this database resulting in a total of 84 papers
for inclusion in this paper.

Other Publications
Included within this literature review will be a number of other publications which do not
form part of a research database. These include books, magazine articles, newspaper clippings and
other sources which will be documented in the bibliography at the end of this paper alongside the
33

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

articles I have sourced from the research databases above. The online academic research tool
Questia31 was used in order to discover the majority of these publications with others sourced from
the bibliographies of papers which have been included in this document.

31

https://www.questia.com/

34

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Section 4: General Overview of the Literature.


The topic of Music and Hypnosis, much like the topic of hypnosis itself, has waxed and
waned in interest for its entire existence (Kennaway, 2011). The research literature has reflected this
fact. Whilst the literature is not as sensational as the popular culture narrative32, it is still extremely
interesting. In this section, my aim is collate the disparate research literature from a number of
sources and provide an overview of the topic so far and attempt to answer the eight hypotheses set
out in my introduction. There have been remarkably few studies conducted in this field when
compared to other interventions33 and the studies which have been conducted are of limited quality.
I do believe that the research which has been conducted demonstrates a highly positive outlook for
the application of music and hypnosis research. Ideally, this paper would be a meta-analysis of the
field, but the research literature, as it currently stands, is nowhere near this ideal. A meta-analysis of
music and hypnosis cannot yet be conducted. It is my hope that this paper will inspire more research
in this area in order for a meta-analysis to be completed in the near future. I will conclude this
section with recommendations for further research in order to implement this.

H1. Can sound manipulate an individuals level of suggestibility?


We have already witnessed the power of music in commercial and business settings34.
Purchasing decisions can be influenced by sound, for example, it has been found that individuals
purchase more expensive wine when classical music is played rather than top 40 popular music due
to the inherent suggestions and stereotypes of sophistication and affluence associated with classical
music (Areni & Kim, 1993). Further research has indicated that even the taste of wine can be
influenced by the music being played at the time of consumption (North, 2011). Research on
whether repeated exposure to these auditory cues can increase the likelihood of a favourable
response for the initiator is scarce. However, research from other domains has generally shown that
32

See A Socio-cultural History of Music and Hypnosis on page 21.


For example, Hypnosis compared against Cognitive Behavioural Therapy.
34
See page 18.
33

35

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

repeated exposure to suggestions increases their power and effectiveness and can even, in extreme
circumstances, lead to the creation of false memories (Zaragoza & Mitchell, 1996). Using music and
sound is a highly indirect mechanism to influence individuals and can, therefore, have powerful
ramifications when used to increase everyday non-hypnotic suggestibility. However, for whatever
reason, the mechanisms behind these impressive observations are not usually discussed by the
researchers. A notable exception can be found in research which has shown that music combined
with suggestion is a highly prevalent way of decreasing driver stress and anger. Drivers who were
tested within an education class performed significantly better after instrumental music preceded by
a verbal suggestion (intended to relax the driver) was presented to them, rather than silence in
subsequent driving tests on a simulator (Groene & Barrett, 2012).
Music can also be used to aid suggestive and associative memory recall. Techniques have
been developed where music can assist learning a foreign language. Learners are instructed to listen
to the music and allow rhythmically stated foreign words to wash over them in a remarkably similar
way to some hypnotic procedures. This procedure has proven remarkably successful and supports
the idea that the unconscious mind can absorb and retain information presented whilst the
conscious mind is distracted with a different, unrelated task (Stanton, 1984).
Further examples of how music can affect or influence our choices are highly prolific. There
have been a number of studies which have examined the use of music on telephone call centre
waiting times. It has been found that customer expectations, along with familiar music, are highly
effective in keeping people waiting on the telephone (Sherwin, 2013). There has even been an entire
company Muzak Holdings - established in order to produce music to influence customers in order
to make them calmer on the telephone, increase impulsivity with regard to purchasing decisions or
to make them more receptive to advertising. This practice appears to be in high demand and worth a
considerable amount of money and investment as the company Mood Media purchased Muzak
holdings in 2011 for 215 million (National Post, 2011).

36

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

The suggestion-based effects of music have also been felt in the education sector with
background music boosting the behaviour and mathematical performance of school children with
emotional and behavioural difficulties (Hallam & Price, 1998). Music can also be used to dramatically
increase the atmosphere in social spaces such as banks. Classical music performed in a banking hall
caused a two hundred and thirty-three percent increase in a customers perception of the bank as
inspiring against the same environment having only ambient noise (North, et al., 2000). Further to
this, the type of music, volume of music and time of day all had significant interactions on
customers estimates of the maximum sum they would spend for products on sale at a bar (North, et
al., 2000).
It has been suggested that low-frequency sound called infrasound, and strong vibrations are
a key contributor to alleged ghost and other supernatural sightings. The psychological effects of
infrasound have been investigated (anecdotally) perhaps most notably by Vic Tandy who developed
a reputation for debunking paranormal myths in the late 20th century when he experienced a
ghostly apparition whilst working in a research laboratory for a medical manufacturing firm. Tandy
then experienced a ghostly apparition in his peripheral vision which promptly vanished when he
turned to observe it properly. The experience was later attributed to imperceptible low frequency
noise emanating from a recently installed extractor fan that activated the physiological fight or
flight response, this generated tension, unease and caused Vics eyes to vibrate in their sockets
leading to him seeing the ghostly apparition (Lyster, 2001). These infrasound vibrations have also
provided an explanation for reports of the London underground being haunted with extremely high
levels around areas that are frequently reported bizarre experiences (Ghosts of the Underground,
2006).
All this research literature, alongside anecdotal support from films, songs and other media
demonstrate that sound, music and noise can have a high influence over our thoughts, feelings and
emotions and can successfully persuade and suggest concepts, ideas and behaviours to individuals at

37

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

an unconscious level. Music and sound is a very powerful and highly effective method of delivering
suggestions.
Despite this, there is as yet, no studies on whether music can increase a persons general
level of suggestibility. This may be due to the fact that there are no objective measures of
suggestibility itself, let alone studies which examine the correlates to this psychological trait. I am
therefore not in a position to answer my hypothesis in full, but can state that music is a highly
effective method of delivering suggestion and the more it is utilised, the more effective it seems to
become (from an anecdotal perspective).

H2. Can sound manipulate an individuals hypnotic susceptibility?


The limited research literature on this topic makes it difficult to reach a direct conclusion.
Researchers have found that listening to ten minutes of music produced a slight increase in hypnotic
responsiveness, but ten music of listening to silence failed to generate any increase at all (Talone, et
al., 1975). Indeed, sensory deprivation seems to be a highly effective method of increasing hypnotic
susceptibility alongside drugs and other methods which have extreme psychological and
physiological side effects which have no place in a modern therapy practice. In contrast to this, it has
been found that listening to highly rhythmical music for fifteen minutes prior to a hypnotic induction
significantly increases the effectiveness of the induction procedure (Maurer Sr, et al., 1997).
Furthermore, highly hypnotisable participants in this study, were more likely to subjectively report
relaxed feelings and shamanic-type experiences. Research has found that individuals who have high
hypnotisability scores show more absorption in classical music than individuals with low
hypnotisability scores (Snodgrass & Lynn, 1989). Music may also be able to assist the clients
willingness to enter a hypnotic state by allowing the client the control of the progress of the
intervention by adjusting music via a volume control or other device. There have been a number of
cases where this approach has assisted clients who were initially reluctant to utilise hypnotic

38

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

techniques (Stanton, 1980). Furthermore, music has been demonstrated to be effective with
increasing hypnotic depth (Walker, 1992) and with induction procedures (Morris, 1986).
It is interesting to note that musicians are usually highly hypnotisable subjects. Research has
shown that musicians (as measured by the psychometric test, GoldMSI35) tend to have a higher pain
tolerance, as a group, than non-musicians when participating in a cold pressor pain task (Bryant,
2012). It has been shown that the psychological traits of absorption, imagination and dissociation
are a common feature shared by both music and hypnosis (Herbert, 2011; Tellegen & Atkinson,
1974; Snodgrass & Lynn, 1989). Musical absorption, in particular, is highly correlated with hypnotic
susceptibility (Snodgrass & Lynn, 1989). It has been shown that there are links between musical
enjoyment and absorption indicating that it may be worthwhile allowing clients to select their own
music for their therapeutic intervention in the same way they would select the modality of therapy
they wish to use (Rhodes, et al., 1988). Physiologically, it has been found that music can produce
similar bodily responses to that of hypnosis, not just in terms of relaxation but also that individuals
who are attending to music produce fewer eye movements, probably as a result of an inwardly
directed shift of attention. Once again this research uncovered that absorption ratings were much
higher in the participant group who provided their own music against unknown music or no music
while viewing a visual stimulus (Schfer & Fachner, 2014).
In more recent research it has been demonstrated that individuals who have a high degree
of musical involvement, so a strong emotional response to music (as measured by a Musical
Involvement Scale (MIS)36) are more likely to experience trance states (Nagy, 2003). Further research
has shown that hypnotic susceptibility shows a moderate correlation with musical involvement and
that hypnotic induction is effective for improving the musical experience of low musical involvers.

35

This is the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Mllensiefen, 2012). It provides a quantitative measure
of an individuals level of musical sophistication via a psychometric assessment. See more at
http://www.gold.ac.uk/music-mind-brain/gold-msi/.
36
For more information please see Dixon (1980).

39

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

This same piece of research found that music serves as a hypnotic induction for high involvers, but
not for low involvers (Nagy & Szab, 2006).
The research literature indicates that hypnotic susceptibility and music are linked, perhaps
not directly, but via the cognitive and psychological processes which are common to each domain
namely absorption, dissociation, emotion and trance.

H3. Are these manipulations temporal or permanent?


Very little research has been conducted on this area. Musical traits, thanks to their training
via a method of repetition and neural plasticity seem to be permanent in the majority of cases (for
example, perfect or absolute pitch or the technical ability of playing an instrument). Furthermore,
music and memory are highly interlinked with a number of studies showing the impressive ability of
music in rehabilitating patients with dementia and other neurocognitive disorders (Vink, et al.,
2004). Research has suggested that the effects of any hypnotic intervention are temporary, provided
no post-hypnotic suggestion was delivered. However, music therapy interventions (alongside other
psychotherapeutic interventions) are more permanent.
From the research literature I have presented; it is apparent that any alteration in hypnotic
susceptibility or suggestibility via the medium of sound is temporary and is only in effect during the
presentation of the stimulus. This is by no means an issue for therapists, who are used to
interventions (such as hypnosis) only lasting for the length of the session, but the results permeating
into the lives and personalities of their clients. A permanent residue of the mechanism of
intervention is not a requirement for a therapeutic intervention to be effective.

40

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

H4. Which characteristics of sound are most likely to alter a listeners hypnotisability?
Across all studies, it has been the element of rhythm which has been emphasised as being a
very important aspect of music trance. This is clearly apparent when looking at trance from a wider
cultural context than therapeutic interventions. However, it may not be musical qualities which are
the most important factors in determining the success of a hypnotic intervention. As we have seen,
the emotional response to music, the level of absorption, dissociation, expectation and empathy
generated by the music are far more important than any of the pure musical characteristics such as
instrumentation, orchestration or genre. Allowing the participant to choose their own music appears
to be vitally important for a successful therapy intervention as it heightens the chances of the
participant engaging with the music (Walker, 1990). It is in this respect that the genre of music and
other taste qualities are important. However, I would advise that music without lyrics is prioritised
in order not to distort the talking therapy aspect of the intervention (Waterfield, 2004).
Having said this, there are a number of individuals who place a great deal of stress on which
instruments are used in order to ensure a quality trance. These include the use of a gong, the sound
of which is said to possess the essential qualities of rhythm (a beating effect, similar to binaural
beats), resonance (the resulting overtones coalesce to produce a hypnotic effect), arousing and
then relaxing when the sound fades into nothing (Livnay, 2014).
There has been also speculation that the use of musical instruments in order to administer
and guide trance allows the participant to enter a deeper (and presumably more effective) trance
state, but there have been no conclusive empirical studies conducted on this hypothesis (Livnay,
2014).

41

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

H5. Could music or sound enhance the process of hypno-psychotherapy and if so,
how?
There have been a number of clinical studies investigating the use of music with the delivery
of suggestions. A study in 1973 found that hospitalised chronic schizophrenic patients, on a fourweek treatment plan showed more improvement when music was performed alongside relaxing
suggestions than music alone or music accompanying encouraging suggestions. All these
interventions were provided on audio tape, with both the patients and nurses blinded to the
experimental nature of the intervention (Ihalainen & Rosberg, 1976).
Classical music has been shown to increase and influence client self-disclosure in therapeutic
circumstances. In a study of eighty-five undergraduates who were asked to reveal the most
significant event of experience of their lives, participants who listened to classical music in the
background whilst completing the self-disclosure task, chose more emotive memories,
demonstrated more expression and increased their liking of musical enjoyment. Consequently, the
amount and validity of information revealed was significantly improved in the music group than in
the group who completed the exercise in silence (Jensen, 2001). Music and hypnosis has also been
used successfully in a number of other therapeutic case studies; as an aid to the induction
procedures (Morris, 1986); in self-hypnosis (Walker, 1990); as a deepening technique (Walker &
Diment, 1979); and as an tool for assisting with client compliance and combatting resistance,
allowing the patient to lead the therapy via the use of sound (Walker, 1992).
Music has been shown to increase the performance of hypno-psychotherapy in cancer
patients via several mechanisms previously mentioned37. Critically, music can increase the level of
endorphins and lower cortisol levels (which, when elevated for an extended period of stress can
inhibit immune response) which is a property unique to music (Bartlett, et al., 1993).

37

See the music therapy section in this paper on page 28.

42

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Overall then, it is clear that music can enhance the process of hypnosis and psychotherapy
far more than being used purely to drown out extraneous noise. It can be considered an intervention
in its own right and assist and compliment the practice of hypno-psychotherapy to make it more
effective.

H6. How may these findings be best utilised by therapy practitioners?


Music, in general, can assist a practitioner in a wide variety of procedures. At the very basic
level, the use of music could improve the relationship between a client and a therapist which has
been shown to be one of the most important aspects of the therapy process (Rogers, 1959). At a
higher level, the therapist can utilise the knowledge of music in planning, or pacing their therapeutic
intervention appropriately on a client-by-client basis. For example, if a client is having issues with
self-disclosure, a session aided by classical music may enable them to disclose more information
than a standard session of hypno-psychotherapy. By asking the client to bring their own music to a
subsequent session, we can help to engage the client further with the therapeutic process and inturn boost the rapport between the practitioner and the client.

H7. Does being musically sophisticated increase a persons hypnotisability?


Based on the available research literature, this certainly seems to be the case. Being
musically sophisticated either by listening to music or being involved in music-related tasks increases
the participants capacity for absorptive experiences and thus their hypnotisability. Musical activities
usually involve a high degree of empathy, social cohesion, improvement in reaction times, alongside
imagination and fantasy proneness. All of these traits correlate with hypnotic susceptibility and can
therefore to be seen to be contributory towards an individuals capability to produce a high-quality
hypnotic and therapeutic experience (Barnier & McConkey, 2004).

43

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Section 5: Conclusion and considerations for further research.


Further research is needed in order to enhance and clarify several issues with the research
landscape of Music and Hypnosis. The field as a whole does show some highly useful
implementations for music in aiding suggestion for a variety of purposes. But there are still large
gaps in our knowledge regarding the effective use of music and hypnosis in aiding psychological
difficulties. For example, the ability of music to alter our level of non-hypnotic suggestibility (and the
field of suggestibility in general) is lacking in quality research literature. Another prosperous avenue
of research would be to conduct a clinical trial where hypnosis and music in combination were
assessed against hypnosis and music therapy on an individual basis for a variety of psychological
issues. This would be an ideal situation. A lot of the literature in this document is over twenty years
old and therefore out of date. It would be highly beneficial if some of the research identified within
this paper were to be replicated in the 21st century in order to ensure validity of the arguments that
are presented in modern times.
The literature identifies that hypnosis and music share common factors such as Absorption,
Dissociation, Trance, Expectancy and establishing rapport between a therapist and their clients.
Music does not have the popular culture stigmatism of hypnosis and may, therefore, be a powerful
tool in aiding a difficult or challenging hypno-psychotherapeutic process. All in all, the research
landscape looks to be positive regarding the use of music to aid hypnosis and psychotherapeutic
procedures, provided that the therapist is mindful of the use of music within each therapeutic
session and the client is prepared to suggest suitable music and consent for its use within the
therapeutic context.

44

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Bibliography
American Music Therapy Association, 2014. What is Music Therapy. [Online]
Available at: http://www.musictherapy.org/about/musictherapy/
[Accessed 23 10 2014].
American Psychological Association, 2014. The Official Division 30 Definition and Description of
Hypnosis. [Online]
Available at: http://psychologicalhypnosis.com/info/the-official-division-30-definition-anddescription-of-hypnosis/
[Accessed 24 09 2014].
Andriopoulos, S., 2008. Possessed: Hypnotic Crimes, Corporate Fiction, and the Invention of Cinema.
1st ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Aranza, J., 1983. Backward Masking Unmasked. 1st ed. Shreveport: Huntington House.
Areni, C. S. & Kim, D., 1993. The influence of background music on shopping behavior: classical
versus top-forty music in a wine store. Advances in Consumer Research, Volume 20, pp. 336-340.
Baehr, J. K. & Kohlschtter, R., 1843. Mittheilungen aus dem Schlafleben der Somnamble Auguste K.
in Dresden. 1st ed. Dresden: Arnold.
Barber, T. X., 1961. Physiological effects of hypnosis. Psychological Bulletin, 58(5), pp. 390-419.
Barnier, A. M. & McConkey, K. M., 2004. Defining and identifying the highly hypnotizable person. In:
M. Heap, R. J. Brown & D. A. Oakley, eds. The Highly Hypnotizable Person. New York: BrunnerRoutledge., pp. 30-61.
Bartlett, D., Kaufman, D. & Smeltekop, R., 1993. The Effects of Music listening on Perceived Sensory
Experiences on the Immune System as Measured by Interleukin-1 and Cortisol. Journal of Music
Therapy, 30(1), pp. 194-209.
Becker, J., 2004. Deep Listeners: Music, Emotion and Trancing. 1st ed. Bloomington: Indiana
University Press.
Becker, J. & Penman, J., 2009. Religious Ecstatics, Deep Listeners, and Musical Emotion. Empirical
Musicology Review, 4(2), pp. 49-70.
Berlyne, D., 1971. Aesthetics and psychobiology. 1st ed. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.
Berlyne, D. & Borsa, D., 1968. Uncertainty and the orientation reaction. Perception and
psychophysics, 3(1), pp. 77-79.
Bernheim, H., 1889. Automatisme et Suggestion. Paris, Congress for Physiological Psychology.
Binet, A. & Fr, C., 1887. Animal Magnetism. 1st ed. London: Kegan and Paul.
Bowers, K. S., 1983. Hypnosis for the Seriously Curious. 1st ed. New York: Norton.
Bradt, J. et al., 2010. Music therapy for acquired brain injury. Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews, Issue 7.
Braid, J., 1843. Neurypnology. 1st ed. London: John Churchill.
Brookhouse, S., 2013. NCHP Training. Stage 2, week 6. [Sound Recording] (NCHP).

45

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Brown, P., 1991. The Hypnotic Brain. 1st ed. New York: Yale University Press.
Bryant, P. D., 2012. Binaural Beats and Pain Endurance: A double-blind, randomised study into the
effectiveness of an auditory phenomenon on cold pain endurance., London: Goldsmiths, University of
London.
Budzynski, T., 2006. The Clinical Guide to Sound and Light. Stanford, USA: Standford University Press.
Burgess, A., 1966. A Clockwork Orange. 1st ed. Harmondsworth: Penguin.
Burkholer, P. J., Grout, D. J. & Palisca, C. V., 2010. A History of Western Music. 8th ed. New York:
W.W. Norton & Company.
Butler, L. D. & Palesh, O., 2004. Spellbound: Dissociation in the Movies. Journal of Trauma &
Dissociation, 5(2), pp. 61-87.
Campbell, D. & Doman, A., 2011. Healing at the Speed of Sound: How What We Hear Transforms Our
Brains and Our Lives. 1st ed. New York: Hudson Street Press.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T. & Furnham, A., 2007. Personality and music: Can traits explain how people
use music in everyday life?. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), pp. 175-185.
Critchley, M. & Henson, R. A., 1977. Music and the Brain. 1st ed. London: William Heinemann.
de Groh, M., 1989. Correlates of hypnotic susceptibility. In: N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves, eds.
Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral perspective. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books, pp. 32-63.
De Pascalis, V., 1989. Hypnotic Susceptibility, alpha waves and 40-Hz EEG Rhythm, and Personality.
In: V. A. Gheorghiu, P. Netter, M. Eysenck & R. Rosenthal, eds. Suggestion and Suggestibility: Theory
and Research. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 221-239.
De Pascalis, V., Ray, W. J., Tranquillo, I. & D' Amico, D., 1998. EEG activity and heart rate during recall
of emotional events in hypnosis: relationships with hypnotizability and suggestibility. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, Volume 29, pp. 255-275.
Declercq, N. F. & Dekeyser, S. A., 2007. Acoustic diffraction effects at the Hellenistic amphitheater of
Epidaurus: Seat rows responsible for the marvelous acoustics. Journal of the Acoustical Society of
America, 121(4), pp. 2011-2022.
Dell, P. F. & O'Neil, J. A., 2009. Dissociation and the Dissociative Disorders: DSM-V and Beyond. 1st
ed. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Dixon, R. D., 1980. Suggested scales for the measurement of musical involvement and genre tastes.
Popular Music and Society, 7(4), pp. 223-244.
Dove, H. W., 1839. Akustik, Theoretische Optik, Meteorologie. In: Repertorium der Physik:
Enthaltend eine vollstndige Zusammenstellung. Lausanne, Switzerland: Veit & Comp, pp. 404 408.
du Maurier, G., 1985. Trilby. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Edmonston, W. E., 1989. Conceptual clarification of hypnosis and its relationship to suggestibility. In:
V. A. Gheorghiu, P. Netter, M. Eysenck & R. Rosenthal, eds. Suggestion and Suggestibility: Theory
and Research. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 69-78.

46

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Edworthy, J. & Waring, H., 2006. The effects of music tempo and loudness level on treadmill
exercise. Ergonomics, 49(15), pp. 1597-1610.
Elsevier, 2014. Content Overview. [Online]
Available at: http://www.elsevier.com/online-tools/scopus/content-overview
[Accessed 29 09 2014].
Erlmann, V., 1982. Trance and Music in the Hausa Boori Spirit Possession Cult in Niger.
Ethnomusicology, Volume 26, pp. 49-58.
Esser, L., 2007. Hypno-psychotherapy and its connection with music in complementary cancer
support, London: National College of Hypnosis, Psychotherapy and Mindfulness.
Evans, E. J., 1989. The independence of suggestibility placebo response, and hypnotizability. In: V. A.
Gheorghiu, P. Netter, M. Eysenck & R. Rosenthal, eds. Suggestion and Suggestibility: Theory and
Research. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 145-154.
Fachner, J., 2006. Music and Altered States of Consciousness. In: D. Aldridge & J. Facher, eds. Music
and Altered States. London: Jessica Kingsley, pp. 15-37.
Faith, M. & Ray, W. J., 1994. Hypnotizability and dissociation in a college age population: Orthogonal
individual differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 17(2), pp. 211-216.
Faymonville, M. E. et al., 2000. Neural mechanisms of antinociceptive effects of hypnosis.
Anesthesiology, Volume 92, pp. 1257-1267.
Foster, D. S., 1990. EEG and Subjective Correlates of Alpha-Frequency Binaural-Beat Stimulation
Combined with Alpha Biofeedback. s.l.:Ann Arbor.
Frankel, F. H., 1976. Hypnosis: Trance as a Coping Mechanism. 1st ed. New York: Springer.
Franklin, B., 1785. Report of Dr Benjamin Franklin, and other Commissioners, charged by the King of
France with the Examination of the Animal Magneticism. 1st ed. London: J. Johnson.
Friedman, B., 2000. Designing casinos to dominate the competition: The Friedman international
standards of casino design. 1st ed. Reno, Nevada: Institute for the Study of Gambling and
Commercial Gaming.
Gallo, D. A. & Finger, S., 2000. The Power of a Musical Instrument: Franklin, the Mozarts, Mesmer
and the Glass Armonica. History of Psychology, Volume 3, pp. 326-43.
Gardner, H. E., 2006. Multiple Intelligences: New Horizons in Theory and Practice. 2nd ed. New York:
Basic Books.
Gass, R. H. & Seiter, J. S., 1999. Persuasion, Social Influence and Complience Gaining. 1st ed. MA:
Allyn and Bacon.
Geiger, E., Peter, B., Prade, T. & Piesbergen, C., 2014. Intelligence and hypnotizability: Is there a
connection?. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 62(3), pp. 310-329.
Geretsegger, M., Elefant, C., Mssler, K. A. & Gold, C., 2014. Music therapy for people with autism
spectrum disorder. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 6.
gethightnow.com, 2013. Binaural Beats. [Online]
Available at: http://gethighnow.com/binaural-beats/

47

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Ghosts of the Underground. 2006. [Film] Directed by Joe Kane. s.l.: Channel 5.
Gilman, L., 2010. An American Soldiers iPod: Layers of Identity and Situated Listening in Iraq. Music
and Politics, 4(2).
Goldschmidt, H., 1983. Schubert und kein Ende. Beitrge zur Musikwissenschaft, Volume 25, pp.
228-92.
Goldsmiths, University of London, 2014. Wagner experiment helps psychologists understand how we
experience music. [Online]
Available at: http://www.gold.ac.uk/news/homepagenews/wagnerexperimenthelpspsychologistsunderstandhowweexperiencemusic.php
[Accessed 23 11 2014].
Gorassini, D. R., 2004. Enhancing hypnotizability. In: M. Heap, R. J. Brown & D. A. Oakley, eds. The
Highly Hypnotizable Person. New York: Brunner-Routledge, pp. 213-240.
Granone, F., 1973. Music as a facilitating factor in psychotherapy processes. Minerva Medica, 64(7),
pp. 320-2.
Groene, R. & Barrett, S., 2012. The Effect of Music and Suggestion on Defensive Driving Responses of
High School Students: Implications for Music Therapy. Music Therapy Perspectives, 30(1), pp. 56-64.
Gueguen, N., Jacob, C., Lourel, M. & Guellec, H. L., 2007. Effect of Background Music on Consumer's
Behaviour: A Field Experiment in a Open-Air Market. European Journal of Scientific Research, 16(2),
pp. 268-272.
Gwynn, M. I. & Spanos, N. P., 1996. Hypnotic Responsiveness, nonhypnotic suggestibility, and
responsiveness to social influence. In: R. G. Kunzendorf, N. P. Spanos & B. Wallace, eds. Hypnosis and
Imagination. Amityville, New York: Baywood Publishing, pp. 147-175.
Hadlock, H., 2000. Mad Loves. 1st ed. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Hallam, S. & Price, J., 1998. Research Section: Can the use of background music improve the
behaviour and academic performance of children with emotional and behavioural difficulties?.
British Journal of Special Education, Volume 25, pp. 88-91.
Heap, M., 2013. Personal Correspondence. London: s.n.
Heap, M. & Aravind, K. K., 2001. Hartland's Medical and Dental Hypnosis. 4th ed. London: Churchill
Livingstone.
Herbert, R., 2011. Everyday Music Listening: Absorption, Dissociation and Trancing. 1st ed. Oxford:
Ashgate.
Hernandez-Peon, R., 1961. The efferent control of afferent signals entering the central nervous
system. Annals of New York Academy of Science, 8(1), pp. 866-882.
Herrmann, C. S., 2001. Human EEG responses to 1100 Hz flicker: resonance phenomena in visual
cortex and their potential correlation to cognitive phenomena. Experimental Brain Research, pp.
346-353.
Hilgard, E. R., 1965. Hypnotic susceptibility. 1st ed. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

48

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Hilgard, E. R., 1973. The domain of hypnosis, with some comments on alternative paradigms.
American Psychologist, Volume 28, pp. 972-982.
Hilgard, E. R., 1986. Divided Consciousness: Multiple Controls in Human Thought and Action.
Expanded Edition ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Hilgard, E. R., 1991. Suggestibility and suggestions as related to hypnosis. In: J. F. Schumaker, ed.
Human Suggestibility. New York: Routledge, pp. 37-58.
Hilgard, E. R. & Hilgard, J. R., 1994. Hypnosis In The Relief Of Pain. 2nd ed. New York: Routledge.
Huang, T. L. & Charyton, C., 2008. A Comprehensive review of the psychological effects of brainwave
entrainment. Alternative Therapies, 14(5), pp. 38 - 50.
Hull, C. L., 1933. Hypnosis and Suggestibility. 1st ed. USA: Appleton-Century Crofts Inc.
Hyatt King, A., 1945. The Musical Glasses and Glass Harmonica. Proceedings of the Royal Musical
Association, Volume 72, pp. 97-122.
Ihalainen, O. & Rosberg, G., 1976. Relaxation and encouraging suggestions given to hospitalized
chronic schizophrenics. The international journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 24(1), pp.
228-237.
Jamieson, G. A., 2005. The Modified Tellegen Absorption Scale: A clearer window on the structure
and meaning of absorption. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 33(2), pp. 119139.
Jensen, K. L., 2001. The effects of selected classical music on self-disclosure. Journal of Music
Therapy, 38(1), pp. 2-27.
Juslin, P. N. & Sloboda, J. A., 2001. Music and Emotion: Theory and Research. 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Kaernbach, C., Schroger, E. & Gunter, T. C., 1998. Human event-related brain potentials to auditory
periodic noise stimuli. Neuroscience Letters, pp. 17-20.
Kaplan, F., 1974. The Mesmeric ManiaThe Early Victorians and Animal Magnetism. Journal of the
History of Ideas, Volume 35, pp. 691-702.
Kennaway, J., 2010. From Sensibility to Pathology: The Origins of the Idea of Nervous Music around
1800. Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences, Volume 65, pp. 396-426.
Kennaway, J., 2011. Musical Hypnosis: Sound and Selfhood from Mesmerism to Brainwashing. Social
History of Medicine, 25(2), pp. 271-289.
Kennaway, J., 2012. Musical Hypnosis: Sound and Selfhood from Mesmerism to Brainwashing. Social
History of Medicine, Volume 25, pp. 271-289.
Kihlstrom, J. F., 2008. Chapter 2: The Domain of Hypnosis, revisited. In: M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier,
eds. The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Practice. Oxford: Oxford University
Press, p. 26.
Kihlstrom, J. F., 2008. The domain of hypnosis revisited. In: M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier, eds. The
Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp.
21-52.

49

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Kretschmar, 1824. Geschichte eines mit merkwrdigen Erscheinungen verbundenden Idiosomnambulismus. Archiv fr den thierischen Magnetismus, Volume 12, pp. 1-58.
Laidlaw, T. M., Dwivedi, P., Naito, A. & Gruzelier, J. H., 2005. Low self-directedness (TCI), mood,
schizotypy and hypnotic susceptibility. Personality and Individual Differences, 39(2), pp. 469-480.
Lane, J. D., Kasian, S. J., Owens, J. E. & Marsh, G. R., 1998. Binaural Auditory Beats Affect Vigilance
Performance and Mood. Physiology & Behavior, 63(2), pp. 249-252.
Laurence, J., Beaulieu-Prevost, D. & du Chene, T., 2008. Measuring and understanding individual
differences in hypnotizability. In: M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis:
Theory, Research and Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 225-253.
Levitin, D. J., 2006. This is Your Brain on Music. 2nd ed. London: Atlantic Books.
Lichtenberg, P. et al., 2008. Hypnotizability and blink rate: A test of the dopamine hypothesis.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 56(3), pp. 243-254.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., Ruscio, J. & Beyerstein, B. L., 2010. Myth #5 Subliminal Messages Can
Persuade People to Purchase Products. In: 50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology. Singapore:
Blackwell Publishing, pp. 36-44.
Livnay, J. S., 2014. The Issues Involved in Using a Gong in Psychotherapy and Hypnotherapy. [Online]
Available at:
http://www.livnay.ws/en/Publications/Publication/smid/1136/ArticleID/269/reftab/581/t/TheIssues-Involved-in-Using-a-Gong-in-Psychotherapy-and-Hypnotherapy
[Accessed 03 11 2014].
Lowis, M. J., 1998. Music and Peak Experiences: An Empirical Study. Mankind Quarterly, 39(2), pp.
203-224.
Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I. & Hallquist, M. N., 2008. Social Cognitive Theories of Hypnosis. In: M. R. Nash &
A. J. Barnier, eds. Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Practice. Oxford: Oxford
University Press, pp. 111-139.
Lynn, S. J. & Rhue, J. W., 1988. Fantasy proneness: Hypnosis, developmental antecedents, and
psychopathology. American Psychologist, 43(1), pp. 35-44.
Lynn, S. J. & Snodgrass, M., 1986. Hypnotizability and Music Involvement. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 34(3), pp. 264-264.
Lyster, S., 2001. Inventor hopes device will give up the ghost. Birmingham Post & Mail, 12 February.
Madsen, C., Greer, R. & Madsen, C., 1975. Research in music behavior. Modifying music behavior in
the classroom. 1st ed. New York: Teachers College Press.
Maratos, A., Gold, C., Wang, X. & Crawford, M., 2008. Music therapy for depression. Cochrane
Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 1.
Marteniuk, R., 1976. Information processing in motor skills. 1st ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Winston.
Maurer Sr, R. L., Kumar, V. K., Woodside, L. & Pekala, R. J., 1997. Phenomenological experience in
response to monotonous drumming and hypnotizability. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 40(2),
pp. 130-145.
50

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Mellgren, A., 1979. Hypnotherapy and art (vocalists and musicians). The Journal of the American
Society of Psychosomatic Dentistry and Medicine, 26(4), pp. 152-155.
Mesmer, F. A., 1779. Mmoire sur la Dcouverte du Magnetisme Animal. 1st ed. Geneva: Didot.
Mithen, S., 2007. The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. 1st ed.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Moore, T. E., 1992. Subliminal perception: Facts and fallacies. Skeptical Inquirer, Volume 16, pp. 273281.
Moreno, J., 1988. The Music Therapist: Creative arts therapist and contemporary Shaman. The Arts
in Psychotherapy, 15(1), pp. 271-280.
Morris, R. E., 1986. The effect of music on hypnotic induction, Melbourne, Florida: ProQuest
Information & Learning.
Mossler, K., Chen, X., Hedal, T. O. & Gold, C., 2013. Music therapy for people with schizophrenia and
schizophrenia-like disorders. The Cochrane Collaboration, pp. 1-24.
Mllensiefen, D. G. B. S. L. &. M. J., 2012. The Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI):
Technical Report and Documentation v1.0.. London: Goldsmiths, University of London..
Music Works, 2014. [Music and] Productivity. [Online]
Available at: http://www.musicworksforyou.com/research/research-topics/8-productivity
[Accessed 23 11 2014].
Nagy, K., 2003. The influence of intensity of musical involvement and type of music on musical
experiences. Hanover, Germany, Hanover University of Music and Drama.
Nagy, K. & Szab, C., 2006. The effect of hypnotic induction on music listening experience of high and
low musical involvers. Bologna, Studiorum University of Bologna.
National Post, 2011. Torontos Mood Media buys Muzak. [Online]
Available at: http://news.nationalpost.com/2011/03/25/torontos-mood-media-buys-muzak/
[Accessed 22 10 2014].
Nietzsche, F., 1888. Der Fall Wagner. 1st ed. Stuttgart: Reclam.
Noebel, D., 1974. The Marxist Minstrels: A Handbook on Communist Subversion of Music. 1st ed.
Tulsa: American Christian College Press.
Noebel, D., 1982. The Legacy of John Lennon. 1st ed. Nashville: Thomas Nelson.
North, A. C., 2011. The Effect of Background Music on the Taste of Wine. British Journal of
Psychology, 103(3), pp. 293-301.
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J. & McKendrick, J., 1997. In-store music affects product choice. Nature,
Volume 390, p. 132.
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J. & Mckendrick, J., 2000. The Effects of Music on Atmosphere in a Bank
and a Bar. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30(7), p. 15041522.
Orne, M. T., 1959. The nature of hypnosis: Artifact and essence. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology, Volume 58, pp. 277-299.

51

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Orne, M. T., 1966. On the mechanism of posthypnotic amnesia. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, Volume 15, pp. 121-134.
Ostendorf, B., 1997. Rhythm, Riots and Revolution. In: R. Fiebig-Hase & U. Lehmkuhl, eds. Enemy
Images in American History. Providence, RI: Berghahn, p. 15979.
Oster, G., 1973. Auditory Beats in the Brain. Scientific American, pp. 94-102.
Oster, G., 1973. Auditory Beats in the Brain. Scientific American, 229(4), pp. 94 - 102.
Oxford English Dictionary, 2014. Suggestibility. [Online]
Available at: http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/193663?redirectedFrom=Suggestibility#eid
[Accessed 2 10 2014].
Oxford English Dictionary, 2014. Suggestion. [Online]
Available at: http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/193668?redirectedFrom=Suggestion#eid
[Accessed 02 10 2014].
Packard, V., 1957. The Hidden Persuaders. 1st ed. London: Longman, Geen and Co.
Patel, A. D., 2008. Music, Language and the Brain. 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pavlov, I. P., 1927. Conditional Reflexes. 1st ed. New York: Dover Publications.
Pavlov, P., 1928. Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes. 1st ed. New York: International Publishers.
Pelletier, C. L., 2004. The effect of music on decreasing arousal due to stress: A meta-analysis.
Journal of Music Therapy, 41(3), pp. 192-214.
Piccione, C., Hilgard, E. R. & Zimbardo, P. G., 1989. On the degree of stability and measured
hypnotizability over a 25-year period. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), pp. 289295.
Pinker, S., 1997. How the Mind Works. 1 ed. New York: W. W. Norton.
Polter, K. H., 1934. Musik als Heilmitte. 1st ed. Dsseldorf: G. H. Nolte.
Pratkanis, A. R., 1992. The cargo-cult science of Subliminal Persuasion. The Skeptical Inquirer, pp.
260-272.
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L. & Ky, C. N., 1993. Music and spatial task performance. Nature,
365(6447), p. 611.
Regnard, P., 1887. Les Maladies pidemiques de lesprit. 1st ed. Paris: Plon-Nourri.
Rhodes, L. A., David, D. C. & Combs, A. L., 1988. Absorption and enjoyment of music. Perceptual and
Motor Skills, 66(3), pp. 737-738.
Richer, P., 1881. tudes Cliniques sur Lhystro-pilepsie. 1st ed. Paris: Delahaye & Lecrosnier.
Robb, S. L., 2000. Music assisted progressive muscle relaxation, progressive muscle relaxation, music
listening, and silence: A comparison of relaxation techniques. Journal of Music Therapy, 37(1), pp. 221.
Rogers, C. R., 1959. A theory of Therapy, Personality, and Interpersonal Relationships, as developed
in the Client-Centred Framework. In: S. Koch, ed. Psychology: A Study of a Science. Study 1, Volume
3: Formulations of the Person and the Social Context. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 184-256.
52

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Rosenbaum, J. L. & Prinsky, L., 1991. The Heavy Metal Subculture and Suicide. Suicide and Lifethreatening Behavior, Volume 24, pp. 15-23.
Rouget, G., 1985. Music and Trance. 1st ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Rouget, G., 1985. Music and Trance. 1st ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Saarikallio, S., 2012. Cross-Cultural Approaches to Music and Health. In: R. MacDonald, G. Kreutz & L.
Mitchell, eds. Music, health & wellbeing. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 477-490.
Schfer, T. & Fachner, J., 2014. Listening to music reduces eye movements. Attention, Perception, &
Psychophysics, p. In Press.
Scheel, K. & Westefeld, J. S., 1999. Heavy Metal Music and Adolescent Suicidality. Adolescence,
Volume 34, pp. 253-73.
Schipkowensky, N., 1977. Music and the Brain. In: M. Critchley & R. A. Henson, eds. Musical Therapy
in the Field of Psychiatry and Neurology. London: William Heinemann, pp. 433-47.
Sherwin, A., 2013. Stuck on hold to your local council? Your spirits will be lifted by Lighthouse Family
and Simply Red. [Online]
Available at: http://www.independent.co.uk/arts-entertainment/music/news/stuck-on-hold-toyour-local-council-your-spirits-will-be-lifted-by-lighthouse-family-and-simply-red-8835182.html
[Accessed 12 11 2014].
Silva, C. E. & Kirsch, I., 1992. Interpretive sets, expectancy, fantasy proneness, and dissociation as
predictors of hypnotic response. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(5), pp. 847-856.
Snodgrass, M. & Lynn, S. J., 1989. Music absorption and hypnotizability. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 37(1), pp. 41-54.
Spiegel, D. et al., 2011. Dissociative disorders in DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety, 28(9), pp. 824-852.
Spiritus, D., 1819. Beobachtungen ber die Heilkraft des animalischen Magnetismus. Archiv fr den
thierischen Magnetismus, Volume 5, pp. 73-107.
Stanton, H. E., 1980. Patient-controlled hypnotherapy: One answer to resistance?. Australian Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 8(2), pp. 91-94.
Stanton, H. E., 1984. A comparison of the effects of an hypnotic procedure and music on anxiety
level. Australian Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 12(2), pp. 127-132.
Stevens, C. & Byron, T., 2011. Universals in Music Processing. In: S. Hallam & I. Cross, eds. Oxford
Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 14-24.
Stevens, L. et al., 2003. Binaural Beat Induced Theta EEG Activity and Hypnotic Susceptibility:
Contradictory Results and Technical Considerations. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 45(4), pp.
295-309.
Stravinsky, I. F., 1936. An Autobiography. 1st ed. New York: Norton.
Szechtman, H., Woody, E., Bowers, K. S. & Nahmias, C., 1998. Where the imaginal appears real: a
positron emission tomography study of auditory hallucinations. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 95(1), pp. 1956-60.

53

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Talone, J. M., Diamond, M. J. & Steadman, C., 1975. Modifying hypnotic performance by means of
brief sensory experiences. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental hypnosis, Volume 23,
pp. 190-9.
Tasso, A. F. & Perez, N. A., 2008. Parsing everyday suggestibility: what does it tell us about
hypnosis?. In: M. R. Nash & A. J. Barnier, eds. The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research
and Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 283.
Tellegen, A. & Atkinson, G., 1974. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences
("absorption"), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83(3), pp.
268-277.
The American Music Therapy Association, 2014. AMTA Press Release on Music Therapy - Jan. 2014.
[Online]
Available at: http://www.musictherapy.org/amta_press_release_on_music_therapy_-_jan_2014/
[Accessed 29 09 2014].
Thomas, R., 1994. Pavlovs Dogs Dripped Saliva at the Sound of a Bell. Psycoloquy, Volume 5, p. 80.
Turow, G. & Lane, J. D., 2011. Binaural Beat Stimulation. In: Music, Science and the Rhythmic Brain.
New York: Routledge, p. 122.
Vas, J. P., 2013. Tones and being tuned. Suggestions for the common origins of music therapy and
hypnotherapy. Psychiatr Hungary, 28(2), pp. 159-64.
Vas, P. J., 2013. Life paths and motifs. Meeting points of hypnotherapy and music therapy. Psychiatr
Hung, 28(3), pp. 291-9.
Vink, A. C., Birks, J. S., Bruinsma, M. S. & Scholten, R. J., 2004. Music therapy for people with
dementia. The Cochrane database of systematic reviews, Volume 3.
von Herrmann, H. C., 2007. Induction psycho-motrice. In: M. Franz, W. Schffner, B. Siegert & R.
Stockhammer, eds. Electric Laokoon. Berlin: Akademie Verlag, pp. 82-96.
Wahbeh, H., Calabrese, C., Zwickey, H. & Zajde, D., 2007. Binaural Beat Technology in Humans: A
Pilot Study to Assess Neuropsychologic, Physiologic, And Electroencephalographic Effects. The
Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 13(2), pp. 199-206.
Walker, W.-L., 1990. Music as a pathway for self-hypnosis. Australian Journal of Clinical &
Experimental Hypnosis, 18(1), pp. 57-59.
Walker, W.-L., 1992. Combining music and words as a pathway through hypnosis: Practical
guidelines. Australian Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 20(2), pp. 117-132.
Walker, W.-L. & Diment, A. D., 1979. Music as a deepening technique. Australian Journal of Clinical &
Experimental Hypnosis, 7(1), pp. 35-36.
Warthin, A. S., 1894. Some Physiologic Effects of Music in Hypnotized Subjects. The Medical News,
Volume 45, pp. 89-92.
Waterfield, R., 2004. Hidden Depths: The Story of Hypnosis. 1st ed. London: Pan Books.
Weitzenhoffer, A. M., 2000. The Practice of Hypnotism. 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

54

Sound, Suggestibility and Hypnotisability

Peter Bryant

Weitzenhoffer, A. M. & Hilgard, E. R., 1959. Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scales, Forms A & B. 1st
ed. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Whaley, J., Sloboda, J. & Gabrielsson, A., 2009. Peak experiences in music. In: S. Hallam, I. Cross & M.
Haut, eds. Oxford Handbook of Music Psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 455.
Wickramasekera, I. E. & Szlyk, J. P., 2003. Could Empathy Be a Predictor of Hypnotic Ability?.
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 51(4), pp. 390-399.
Wier, D. R., 2007. The Way of Trance. 1st ed. Laytonville, California: Trance Research Foundation.
Williamson, V., 2014. You Are the Music: How Music Reveals What it Means to be Human. 1st ed.
London: Icon Books Ltd.
Will, U. & Berg, E., 2007. Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli.
Neuroscience Letters, pp. 55-60.
Wirth, J. U., 1836. Theorie des Somnabulismus oder des thierischen Magnetismus. 1st ed. Leipzig:
Scheible.
Woody, E. Z. & Barnier, A. J., 2008. Hypnosis scales for the twenty-first century: what do we need
and how should we use them?. In: 1st, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and
Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press, p. 256.
Wright, R., 2000. Ill Sell you Suicide: Pop Music and Moral Panic in the Age of Marilyn Manson.
Popular Music, Volume 19, pp. 365-85.
Zaragoza, M. S. & Mitchell, K. J., 1996. Repeated exposure to suggestion and the creation of false
memories. Psychological Science, 7(5), pp. 294-300.
Zarren, J. I. & Eimer, B. N., 2002. Brief Cognitive Hypnosis: facilitating the change of dysfunctional.
1st ed. New York: Springer Publishing Company.

55

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen