Sie sind auf Seite 1von 26

Differential argument marking: an introduction

Ilja A. Serant (University of Konstanz)


Alena Witzlack-Makarevich (University of Kiel)1
1. Introduction
In this introductory article we would like to provide an overview of the range of the
phenomena that can be referred to as differential argument marking (DAM). We begin this
introduction with an overview of the existing terminology (this section). We than proceed by
considering individual aspects of the phenomenon which figured prominently in studies of
DAM in various traditions (Sections 2 and 3).
The term differential marking or to be historically precise, differential object
marking was first used by Bossong (1982, 1985) in his investigations of the phenomenon in
Sardinian and New Iranian languages. Somewhat older than this term is the term split (as in
split ergativity) used in the line of research focusing primarily on the differential marking of
the agent argument. It has been in use since Silverstein (1976) and was popularized by Dixon
(1979, 1994).
Recent years have been marked by a growing interest in differential marking and as a
result numerous related terms were coined to refer to individual roles marked differentially
and particular types of differential marking. De Hoop & de Swart (2008) were the first to
systematically discuss differential subject marking (DSM), Fauconnier (2011) introduced the
term differential agent marking, whereas Kittil (2008) explores differential goal marking or
differential R marking (DRM in Haspelmath 2005). Another notion that refers to the
phenomenon of DAM is optional ergative marking (cf. among others McGregor 1992, 1998,
2006, 2010; Chelliah 2009 (referred to as agentive case); Gaby 2010; Rumsey 2010;
Fauconnier 2011). As these authors show, in addition to the semantic function to encode
agents, ergative case is sometimes also employed to mark focal, unexpected or contrastive Ss
and As in a number of languages of Australia (e.g. Warrwa or Umpithamu), Papua New
Guinea (Ku Waru (Papuan)) or in Himalaya (e.g. Meithei or Lhasa (Tibeto-Burman)). Apart
from these role-specific terms Iemmolo (2011) has introduced the term differential object
indexing (DOI) to refer to the cases of differential argument marking on the verb in contrast to
case marking on the noun phrase. Finally, in the traditions of the DAM research in individual
language families and languages many more language-, role- or marking-specific labels have
been used, for instance, prepositional accusative in Romance linguistics (cf. inter alia,
Torrego Salcedo 1999) or bi-absolutive construction in the Nakh-Daghestanian languages
(cf. Forker 2012).
As is obvious from the plethora of terms listed above, DAM takes many forms. Given
that our investigation is aimed at diachronic processes it is obvious that we cannot a priori
focus on a subset of cases for something that we treat here as being in flux thus leaving out
phenomena that have the potential to develop into DAM in a more accepted sense (or in fact
have been attested to undergo this development), as well as those phenomena that arguably
originate from DAM but exhibit somewhat deviating properties due to a later development.
For this reason we keep the definition as wide as possible. We will use the term DAM broadly
defining it as in (1) (drawing on Woolford 2008, Iemmolo & Schikowki 2014+):

Names are ordered alphabetically; both authors contributed equally.

(1)

The broad definition of DAM:


Any kind of situation where an argument of a predicate bearing the same generalized
semantic role (or macrorole) may be coded in different ways, depending on factors
other than the argument role itself.

As follows from this definition and some of the listed terms point at, DAM is not restricted to
case marking (also called dependent marking or flagging, cf. Haspelmath 2005) but also
includes differential agreement (or head marking or indexing).2 Whereas some linguists
assume that the two types of differential marking share commonalities (e.g. Dalrymple &
Nikolaeva 2011: 12), others claim that they are different in terms of their functions and
triggers and may emerge from different diachronic processes (de Hoop & de Swart 2008: 5;
Iemmolo & Schikowki 2014+). While we agree with this second view, we are open as to the
possibility that there might nevertheless be plenty of overlap in both diachrony and
synchrony.
To capture different kinds of DAM systems we first provide a brief overview of
factors that are subject to variation in DAM cross-linguistically. Then, we put forward a
coordinate system in which we highlight the aspects that we consider central for the
understanding of DAM and give a more narrow definition of DAM in (15), both definitions
will be used in the present volume.
In what follows we will first provide an overview of the properties staking out the
phenomenon of DAM. We begin with an overview of the synchronic variation of the
phenomenon and first consider the argument-triggered DAM systems (Section 2.1). In
particular, we discuss both inherent lexical argument properties (Section 2.1.1) and noninherent discourse based argument properties (Section 2.1.2) and proceed with the argument
properties of the larger syntactic environment (Section 2.1.3). Section 2.2 covers DAM cases
triggered by various predicate properties. Section 2.3 provides a brief summary of the various
triggers for DAM. In Section 2.4 we introduce various restrictions that constrain the
occurrence of DAM cross-linguistically. Section 3 is devoted to realization properties of
DAM. Section 3.1 discusses the morphological distinction between symmetric vs. asymmetric
DAM types. Then, we contrast different loci of realization of DAM: head-marking and
dependent-marking (Section 3.2). Section 3.3 aims at highlighting differences in syntactic
(behavioral) properties found with DAM. The distinction between obligatory vs. optional is
introduced in Section 3.4. Section 3.5 provides a brief summary of the factors involved in
variation. Finally, we discuss a few functional explanations (Section 4) and list a number of
questions we invite you to address in this volume (Section 5).
2. Synchronic variation of DAM
As defined above, DAM encompasses a range of phenomena sharing the trait of encoding the
same argument role in different ways. However, apart from this shared property DAM
systems vary from language to language and to allow for the comparison of DAM systems
and their diachronic development paths we decompose this phenomenon into a number of
characteristics building up on the attested synchronic variation and suggestions made in the
literature on the topic.
In what follows we introduce two orthogonal distinctions of DAM systems: argumenttriggered DAM (Section 2.1) vs. predicate-triggered DAM (Section 2.2) and restricted DAM
vs. unrestricted DAM (Section 2.4). We begin with considering those DAM systems where
the differential argument marking may be found with one and the same form of the predicate
(henceforth: argument-triggered DAM). For this type of DAM a number of variables are
2

Some authors go even further and consider inverse systems and voice alternations as instances of DAM (e.g. de
Hoop & de Swart 2008: 1).

needed to account for the attested variation. These are various properties of arguments
(Sections 2.1.1, 2.1.2) and event semantics (Section 2.1.3). In Section 2.2, in turn, we will
turn to the predicate-triggered DAM which all have in common that the differential argument
marking there alternates with some change in the form of the predicate involved.
2.1 Argument-triggered DAM
The properties of arguments can determine DAM in two ways: either the properties of the
differentially marked argument alone are responsible for a particular marking (Sections 2.1.1
2.1.2) or the properties of more than one argument in a clause i.e. the whole constellation of
arguments, also referred to as scenario determine a particular marking (Section 2.1.3). In
both cases, the relevant argument properties include a wide range of inherent lexical
(semantic and formal, Section 2.1.1), as well as non-inherent, first of all pragmatic
characteristics of arguments (Section 2.1.2). In what follows we provide a preliminary
typology of these characteristics.
2.1.1 Inherent lexical argument properties
Many of the properties we cover in this section are often represented as integrated into various
implicational hierarchies or scales. One of the most cited versions of such hierarchies is given
in (2). It was introduced by Dixon (1979) as potentiality of agency scale and was based on
Silversteins (1976) hierarchy of inherent lexical content.3 It was widely popularized by Croft
(2003) as extended animacy hierarchy, which also include definiteness. Another common
version of the hierarchy is Aissens (1999) prominence hierarchy given in (3) or indexability
hierarchy in Bickel & Nichols (2007).
(2)

(3)

first person pronoun > second person pronoun > third person pronoun > proper nouns
> human common noun > animate common noun > inanimate common noun
(Dixon 1979: 85)
Local person > Pronoun 3rd > Proper noun 3rd > Human 3rd > Animate 3rd >
Inanimate 3rd
(Aissen 1999: 674)

These and similar complex hierarchies involve a range of distinct dimensions (cf. Croft 2003:
130) and individual dimensions might be more or less prominent in shaping DAM systems in
individual languages (see Aissen 1999 for some examples). The major reason for the
suggestion of extended versions of hierarchies is the fact that individual dimensions such as
animacy or definiteness are not entirely orthogonal. Personal pronouns are not only inherently
animate (except for the third person, cf. English it), they are also inherently definite and
highly accessible referents. Therefore, they are highest ranked also on hierarchies based on
definiteness (see Section 2.1.2) or on the accessibility hierarchy (cf. Ariel 1988, 2001). Since
different languages may involve slightly different variants of the extended hierarchies, we
prefer to operate with individual dimensions contributing to the complex hierarchies. In what
follows we first provide an overview thereof and then present some examples.4
We begin this overview with the inherent lexical argument properties which have a
semantic component. These properties and the relevant levels are listed in Table 1. These are
probably the most frequently discussed factors behind DAM and examples of their effects on
case marking or agreement can be easily found in the literature (e.g. Silverstein 1976, Aissen
1999, Dixon 1994):
3

For a more extensive overview of the history of research on the effects of referential hierarchies on differential
marking see Filimonova (2005).
4
These dimensions are still inherently complex in the sense that they can be further decomposed into a range of
binary features as in Silversteins (1976) original proposal (e.g. [animate], [human], [ego]).

Table 1: Inherent semantic argument properties


Person
First & Second person > Third person > (Obviative / Fourth person)
Animacy/Humanness Animate humans > Animate non-humans (animals) > Inanimate
Uniqueness
Proper nouns > Common nouns
Number
Singular vs. Plural vs. Dual
The individual levels in Table 1 are ordered where possible in an implicational hierarchy.
With respect to argument marking these hierarchies are meant to reflect either universal
constrains on possible splits in alignment or, at least, the cross-linguistic frequency of actual
language types (cf. Croft 2003: 123). For instance, according to one reading, the types on the
top of the hierarchies tend to show accusative alignment, whereas the ones at the bottom of
the hierarchy tend to align ergatively (cf. Silverstein 1976, see also Bickel et al. 2014+ for the
testing of the effects of various hierarchies on alignment against a large sample of over 370
case systems worldwide).
Not all of the properties listed in Table 1 apply to both DSM and DOM to the same
extent. For instance, animacy is sometimes claimed to be a relevant parameter rather for
DOM, while DSM/Differential Agent Marking systems along exclusively animacy scale are
rare (Fauconnier 2011). Moreover, Fauconnier (2011) demonstrates that independently acting
inanimates may pattern with animates as regards Differential Agent Marking, while being
distinct from the dependently (via human instigation) acting inanimates. In turn, West Slavic
languages (more specifically Polish, but also South Middle Russian) represent the rare,
animacy-only-driven case of DAM in the S/A position. Thus, in Polish three distinct
nominative suffixes which distinguish between virile animates NOM1 -i, -owie and
everything else NOM2 -y/-i are found (Kucaa 1971, 1978).
By listing individually the dimensions in Table 1 we do not imply that one can find
attestations of languages in which every single one is the only relevant trigger of DAM.
Rather, in the vast majority of languages these and further dimensions to be introduced later
interact in an intricate fashion. For instance, we do not know of any language where number
is the only relevant dimension, however, there are many synchronic cases where a
combination of person and number provides the exact characterization of the split in marking,
this is particularly common within pronouns. Also diachronically cases are known where
number plays a role. For instance, in Old Russian primarily animacy-driven DOM has started
out in singulars and spread further to plurals. Here, the DOM (ACC vs. GEN) is attested with
singular masculine proper names and anthroponyms from the earliest original Old Russian
sources on, i.e. from 11th c. representing the Common Slavic inheritance. At the same time,
the animacy-driven DOM has spread onto plurals during 1315th cc. and with nouns referring
to animals in the 16th c. (inter alia, Krysko 1994: 61). The dual forms equally develop
animacy-driven DOM from 1214th c. (Krysko 1994: 98). There is evidence that plural has
acquired DOM approximately during the same time period as dual in Old Russian. Thus, the
number hierarchy works here. However, there are counterexamples: animacy-driven DSM is
found only in the plural in Polish, while the singular has never developed the animacy-driven
DSM here.
Apart from the inherent semantic properties of arguments discussed above, very often
differences in argument marking may be better captured in terms of inherent formal properties
of the relevant arguments. The latter include the part of speech distinction (pronouns vs.
nouns) and much less frequently discussed gender/inflectional class distinctions. The
pronouns vs. nouns distinction is one of the most common lines of split in case marking
worldwide (cf. Bickel et al. 2014+). For instance, in Jingulu all pronominal patient-like
arguments are marked with the accusative suffix -u, as in (4), whereas all nominal patients are

in the unmarked nominative case, no matter whether they are animate, as in (5c) and (5d),
human, as in (5d) or definite, as in (5b)(5d):
(4) Jingulu (Mirndi, Australia)
a.
Angkurla
larrinka-nga-ju
ngank-u.
NEG
understand-1SG-do 2SG-ACC
I didn't understand you. (Pensalfini 1997: 247)
b.
Ngiji-ngirri-nyu-nu
kunyaku.
see-lPL.EXCL-2OBJ-did
2DU.ACC
We saw you two. (Pensalfini 1997: 102)
c.
Jaja-mi
ngarr-u!
wait-IRR
1SG-ACC
Wait for me! (Pensalfini 1997: 160)
(5) Jingulu (Mirndi, Australia)
a.
Ngangarra ngaja-nga-ju.
wild.rice
see-1SG-do
I can see wild rice. (Pensalfini 1997: 100)
b.
Jani madayi-rni
ngaja-nya-ju?
Q
cloud.NOM-FOC
see-2SG-do
Can you see the cloud? (Pensalfini 1997: 198)
c.
Wiwimi-darra-rni
warlaku
ngaja-ju.
girl-PL-ERG
dog.NOM
see-do
The girls see the dog. (Pensalfini 1997: 275)
d.
Ngaja-nga-ju niyi-rnini
nayurni.
see-1SG-do 3SG.GEN-F woman.NOM
I can see his wife. (Pensalfini 1997: 249)
As Filimonova (2005) points out, pronouns belong to the most archaic parts of the lexicon and
might be more stable and resistant to morphological changes and syntactic realignments than
nouns. This might also be part of the explanation for why pronouns especially those
referring to the speech act participants represent the most notorious hierarchy offenders.
The second group of inherent formal argument properties which can trigger DAM are
gender and inflectional classes. This type of DAM is hardly ever discussed, probably due to
the fact that inflectional-class assignments in many languages are only partly semantically
conditioned (by the sex of the extensions), partly formally (e.g. deverbal nouns which
typically uniformly receive particular inflectional class) and partly idiosyncratically (various
exceptions). An exception in the case of typological studies is Bickel et al. (2014+) and a few
discussions of DAM in individual languages, e.g. Karatsareas (2011) on Cappadocian Greek.
Moreover, the inflectional classes themselves often exhibit differential marking of one
and the same argument. We consider that the differences between inflectional classes might
be viewed as a diachronic effect of morphologization of a previously
(morpho-)syntactically constrained DAM. Many Slavic languages seem to undergo this
process whereby the former accusative case (zero) vs. genitive case animacy-driven DOM is
now becoming just one heterogeneous accusative case with two allomorphs ex-accusative and
ex-genitive according to various syntactic and substitution tests.5 The latter may be considered
5

The syntactic behavior of the ex-genitive accusatives is exactly the same as the one of the ex-accusative
accusatives with regard to various tests such as passivization; the ex-genitive accusatives can be coordinated or
even combined with ex-accusative accusatives within one NP; the ex-genitive accusatives and ex-accusative
accusatives are mutually substitutable with no restrictions as to grammaticality.

as being distributed according to different inflectional classes (originally animate vs.


inanimate) which take different accusative allomorphs: the ex-genitive and the ex-accusative
one, respectively.
2.1.2 Non-inherent discourse based argument properties
Apart from the inherent semantic and formal lexical argument properties discussed above, a
range of further characteristics related to how referents are used in discourse are known to
interact with DAM. On the one hand, these properties include such semantic dimensions as
(in)definiteness and specificity, on the other hand, they include pragmatic categories
considered under the umbrella term of information structure.
The two notions of definiteness and specificity are notoriously difficult to define. A
common proxy for definiteness is the semantic-pragmatic notion of identifiability. Thus, a
definite argument is the one for which the hearer can identify the referent (Lyons 1999: 25).
In a similar approach, Lambrecht defines identifiability as reflecting a speakers assessment
of whether a discourse representation of a particular referent is already stored in the hearers
mind or not (Lambrecht 1994: 76). In contrast to definiteness, which depends both on the
speaker and the hearer, specificity only depends on the speaker and is given whenever the
speaker has a particular referent in mind (Lyons 1999:35).6 As the two phenomena of
definiteness and specificity closely interact, they are frequently integrated into one hierarchy,
as in (6) (see e.g. Croft 2003: 132):
(6)

Definite > Specific (indefinite) > Non-specific (indefinite)

The interaction of DAM with another type of discourse-based properties of arguments viz.
information structure properties became particularly prominent in some recent studies on
DAM including McGregor (1998, 2006), Iemmolo (2010) and Dalrymple & Nikolaeva
(2011). Thus, Dalrymple & Nikolaeva (2011: 14) claim that many seemingly unpredictable
cases of variation in DOM can be accounted for by considering information structure
understood as that level of sentence grammar where propositions (i.e. conceptual states of
affairs) are structured in accordance with the information-structure role of sentence elements.
Specifically, they propose that topicality plays a critical role in many cases of DOM, such that
marked objects are often secondary topics (Dalrymple & Nikolaeva 2011: 14) or topics
(Iemmolo 2010), while unmarked ones are non-topical (Iemmolo 2010; Dalrymple &
Nikolaeva 2011). In line with the distinction between inherent and non-inherent properties of
arguments relevant for DAM made in the present paper, Dalrymple & Nikolaeva (2011: 15)
emphasize the non-inherent, contextual nature of topicality.
Apart from topicality, focality also figures as a demarcation line for DAM, particularly
in cases of differential agent marking (occasionally called split or optional ergativity). This
type of DAM can be exemplified with Literary Central (Lhasa) Tibetan. In this language,
unmarked agent arguments are associated with unmarked information distribution, whereas
the use of the ergative marker yields the reading with emphasis (focus) on either the identity
or the agency of the agent (cf. Tournadre 1991). McGregor (2006: 399) states that NPs are
marked with one ergative exponent in Warrwa (Kimberley, Western Australia) when they are
unexpected, unpredictable, or surprising in terms of their identity and agentivity, otherwise
they are marked with a different ergative exponent. He formulates the following rules:
(a) IDENTITY: the identity of the Agent is unpredictable and/or surprising; and
(b) AGENTIVITY: the Agent-referent shows an exceptional or unexpected degree
6

For an overview of the history of research on specificity and other approaches to specificity, see von Heusinger
(2011).

of agentivity. (McGregor 2006: 399)


Consider the following two consecutive sentences from a narrative illustrating this difference:
(7)

Warrwa (Kimberley, Western Australia)


nyinka jurrb -ji-na-yina
kinya wanyji kwiina iri,
this
jump 3minNOM-say-PA-3minOBL this
later big
woman
ka-na-ngka-ndi-
-ji-na,
kinya-na
wuba,
1minNOM-TR-FUT-get-3minACC
3minNOM-say-PST
this-ERG
small
The little one jumped at her then, at the big woman, and tried to get her.
(McGregor 2006: 402)

(8)

kinya kwiina-nma iri


marlu laj
-ji-na-
this
big-fERG
woman
not
throw 3minNOM-say-PST-3minACC
kinya wuba, laj,
marlu laj
-ji-na-,
this
little throw not
throw 3minNOM-say-PST-3minACC
But no, the big woman threw the little man away. (McGregor 2006: 402)

In (7), the little (scil. woman) is continuous topic in the second clause. It is thus expected and
also important in McGregors (2006) account not able to affect the P participant (the big
woman), hence, less agentive or controlling. In turn, in (8), the big woman is both unexpected
(by virtue of contrastive topic / topic shift) and high in potency/agentivity being able to affect
the P argument entirely. Further examples can be found in McGregor (2010). While it is
somewhat difficult to define and operationalize the notion of emphasis/focality, related
notions of unexpectedness, surprise or unpredictability of the referent might be better terms in
describing individual DAM systems. For instance, Schikowski (2013) uses the term
unexpectedness in addition to various other inherent (animacy) and context-dependent
(specificity) properties to explain DOM in Nepali.
As with inherent properties of arguments, non-inherent discourse based properties
often do not determine the individual DAM systems on their own but interact with other
properties. For instance, according to Escandell-Vidal (2009) in Balearic Catalan pronominal
objects are always case-marked, thus, inherent part-of-speech characteristic is at work,
whereas with non-pronominal objects case marking is partly determined by topicality.
To summarize, the information-structure roles that are typically coded by DAM are
foci with subjects and topics with objects. Sometimes, these roles are additionally supplied
with some kind of emphasis/contrast, or with unexpectedness, and encode either contrastive
topics (as opposed to continuative topics) or contrastive foci (e.g. the corrective focus).
2.1.3 Properties of scenario and global vs. local DAM systems
In Sections 2.1.12.1.2 we discussed how various inherent and discourse-based properties of
arguments affect argument marking. This type of DAM conditioned by argument-internal
properties is sometimes referred to as local (Silverstein 1976: 178; Malchukov 2008: 213,
passim). However, not only the properties of differentially marked arguments themselves
might be relevant: In some languages, argument marking is sensitive to the properties of other
arguments of the same clause, i.e. to the nature of the co-arguments. In other words, not only
one argument on its own, but the whole configuration of who is acting on whom can shape
DAM systems. This type of DAM has been called global by Silverstein (1976: 178), because
the assignment of case-marking is regulated on the global level of the event involving all
arguments. Following Bickel (1995, 2011) and Ziga (2006), such argument configurations
are referred to as scenarios. Parallels are found in indexing where such systems are well-

known under the notion of hierarchical agreement (cf. Siewierska 2003, 2004: 5156).
However, also case marking can be affected, though such cases are less common.
Effects of scenarios on case marking can be illustrated with object marking in
Aguaruna. In this language, the object argument is marked in one of two ways. First, it can be
in the unmarked nominative, such as the nominal argument yawa dog.NOM in (9a) and the
pronominal arguments, such as n 3sNOM in (9b), and yawa 1pNOM in (9c):
(9) Aguaruna (Jivaroan, Peru)
a.
yawa
ii-nau
maa-ta-ma-ka-um?
dog.NOM
1PL-POSS
kill.HIAF-NEG-REC.PST-INT-2sgPST
Have you killed our dog? (Overall 2007: 155)
b.
n
ma-ta.
3SG.NOM
carry.PFV-IMP
You(sg.) carry him! (Overall 2007: 443)
c.
hutii
ainau-ti
atum
wai-hatu-ina-hum-i.
1PL.NOM
PL-SAP
2.PL.NOM see-1PL.OBJ-PL.IPFV-2PL.-DECL
You(pl.) see us. (Overall 2007: 444)
Second, objects can be marked with the accusative case suffix -na, such as biika-na beansACC in (10a), ii-na 1p-ACC in (10b), and ami-na 2s-ACC in (10c):
(10) Aguaruna (Jivaroan, Peru)
a.
ima
biika-na-kI
yu-a-ma-ha-i.
INTENS
bean-ACC-RESTR eat-HIAF-REC.PST-1SG-DECL
I only ate beans. (Overall 2007: 146)
b.
n
ii-na
antu-hu-tama-ka-aha-tata-wa-i.
3SG.NOM
1PL-ACC
listen-APPL-1PL.OBJ-INTS-PL-FUT-3-DEC
He will listen to us. (Overall 2007: 326)
c.
hutii
a-ina-u-ti
daka-sa-tata-ham-i
1PL.NOM
COP-PL.IPFV-REL-SAP
wait-ATT-FUT-1SG>2SG.OBJ-DECL
ami-na.
2SG-ACC
We will wait for you. (Overall 2007: 444)
As (10c) and (10b) demonstrate, an object with identical referential properties (first person
plural pronoun) can be either in the nominative or in the accusative case. Thus, the internal
properties of arguments cannot be the trigger of DOM in Aguaruna. Instead, the distribution
of the two types of object marking is determined by the configuration of the referential
properties of both the transitive subject and object arguments and is summarized as follows:
Object NPs are marked with the accusative suffix -na, with some exceptions that are
conditioned by the relative positions of subject and object on the following person
hierarchy:
1sg > 2sg > 1pl/2pl > 3
First person singular and third person subjects trigger accusative case marking on any
object NP, but second person singular, second person plural, and first person plural only
trigger marking on higher-ranked object NPs. (Overall 2009: 168f.)
Similar cases have been reported from other languages. Thus, Malchukov (2008: 213) states
that differently from Hindi where the DOM is purely locally constrained, in the genetically

related language Kashmir, the DOM underlies global considerations: P takes an object
(ACC/DAT) case if A is lower than P on the Animacy/Person Hierarchy (Malchukov 2008:
213 relying on Wali & Koul 1997: 155). Thus, as Malchukov points out, the global vs. local
distinction may be observed even with genetically the same DAM system.
Not only inherent argument properties of more than one argument can trigger DAM, as
in the examples above, also non-inherent discourse-related argument properties of the whole
scenario are known to trigger DAM. The well-known examples include proximative
vs. obviative case marking in the Algonquian languages (see, for instance, Dahlstrom 1986 on
Plains Cree).
2.1.4. Properties dependent on event semantics
In some languages DAM is not directly triggered by the inherent or discourse-related
properties of arguments but rather by the way these arguments are involved into an event. The
relevant aspects include among others volitionality/control or agentivity and affectedness
(for discussion, see Nss 2004, McGregor 2006, Fauconnier 2012: 4) and other instances
related. DAM is used here to differentiate between different degrees of transitivity in various
ways. While manipulating the degrees of agentivity/control/volitionality is typically done by
means of differential agent (or subject) marking, various degrees of affectedness (pertaining
to objects) and resultativity (pertaining to the verbal domain) are expressed via differential P
or object marking. This division of labor is, of course, expected, because such semantic
entailments as volitionality or affectedness are typically realized on the A and P arguments,
respectively. The following subsections give an overview of these two subtypes.
2.1.4.1 Agentivity-related DAM/DSM
Tsova-Tush provides an example of differential S marking triggered by volitionality: when
the argument is volitionally involved and/or in control of the event the S argument is in the
ergative, as in (11a), whereas when the involvement of the argument lacks volition or control,
it is in the nominative case, as in (11b):
(11) Tsova-Tush (Nakh-Daghestanian; Georgia; Holisky 1987: 105)
a.
(as)
vui-n-as.
1sERG
fall-AOR-1sERG
I fell. (It was my own fault that I fell down.)
b.
(so)
vo-en-sO.
1sNOM
fall-AOR-1sNOM
I fell down, by accident.
The feature distinguishing (11a) from (11b) can be described as the presence of the control
over the prestage of the event (Serant 2013) which is somewhat different from volitionality
and control, because the subject referent does not have control over the very event (to fall in
(11) or to get cold in (12) below). At the same time, the more agentive marking implies that
the subject referent had the opportunity to avoid the situation to come about, but it failed to
exercise its control at the stage before the event took place. Thus, in Lithuanian, both (12a)
and (12b) are grammatical in isolation, but given the context provided by the sentence with
the doctor, only (12a) is allowed:
(12) Lithuanian (Baltic, Indo-European, Serant 2013: 289)
Gydytojas
ant
skaudanio pirto udjo led,
doctor
on
aching
finger put
ice

ir
and

po
after

deimties
ten

minui
minutes

man pirt-as
visai atal-o
I.DAT finger-NOM fully get.cold-PST.3
(b)
*a
pirt-
visai atal-a-u
I.NOM finger-ACC
fully get.cold-PST-1.SG
The doctor put ice on [my] aching finger and after 10 minutes my finger got cold (lit. to
me the finger got cold). [Elicited]
(a)

Crucially, in both examples, there is no direct control over the event on the part of the
experiencer; to denote full control, the respective causative form of the verb to get cold has
to be used in Lithuanian.
2.1.4.2 Affectedness and resultativity related DAM/DOM
This subtype has mostly been discussed in relation to particular areas, most prominently with
respect to the total vs. partitive alternation in the Finnic and some neighboring Indo-European
languages. One of the reasons is perhaps because languages of the eastern Circum-Baltic area
(term coined in Dahl & Koptjevskaja-Tamm 2001) show remarkable degree of productivity of
this type of DAM (Serant, forthc.):
(13) Lithuanian (Baltic, Indo-European, from Serant 2014)
a. Jis i-gr-
vanden-
he TELIC-drink-PST.3 water-ACC.SG
He drank (up) (okthe/oksome) water / water.
b. Jis i-gr-
vanden-s
he TELIC-drink-PST.3
water.GEN.SG
He drank (*the/oksome) water / water.
The verb to drink subcategorizes for an accusative object in Lithuanian, as in (13a), which is
the default option in this language and may have both definite (or exhaustive) and indefinite
(weak/some) interpretation, since this languages does not have grammaticalized articles and
bare NPs are generally ambiguous as to (in)definiteness. However, the regular accusative
marking may be overridden by the genitive case, as in (13b) where the exhaustive or definite
reading is no longer available. The genitive option found with indefinite-quantification
reading in (13b) is straightforwardly related to non-specificity, which is one of the prominent
non-inherent parameters with differential object marking (e.g. in Spanish, see 2.1.2 above).
On the other hand, the indefinite-quantity reading yields the verbal phrase in (13b)
atelic (non-resultative in the Finnish tradition, cf. Huumo 2010), the whole event of drinking
water becomes an activity predicate and not an accomplishment one as in (13a). While this
effect is found mostly with incremental-theme verbs (term coined in Dowty 1991) in
Lithuanian, Finnic languages allow basically any accomplishment verb to have this
transformation from an accomplishment verb into an activity verb by means of DOM, cf. to
open (a non-incremental-theme verb):
(14)

Finnish (Finnic, Finno-Ugric, from Kiparsky 1998)


a.
Hn avasi
ikkunan
he
open.PST.3SG window.ACC.SG

b.

He opened the window.


Hn avasi
ikkunaa
he
open.PST.3SG window.PART.SG
(i) He was opening the window.
(ii) He opened the window (partly).
(iii) He opened the window for a while.
(iv) He opened the window again and again.

Crucially, all four readings in (14b) indicate a construal of an event in the past that is not
committal as to the achievement of an inherent end point. In turn, only (14a) with the
accusative marking7 of the object indicates that the inherent end point of the process of
window opening has been achieved. At the same time, in contrast to (13b), there is no weak
quantification of the object referent only the verbal action is quantified while the object is
affected holistically. Note that there is no relation to the viewpoint (or even progressive)
aspect here as is sometimes assumed in the literature (Serant, forthc.). The non-resultativity
or only partial result in the action in (14b) entails, of course, that the object referent has not
been affected to the extend it has been in (14a).
2.1.5 Argument-triggered DAM: summary
Section 2.1 considers only those cases of DAM where various argument properties function as
a trigger and the form of the predicate remains the same. This type has been in the focus of
the study of DAM from its very beginning and arguably represents the consensus examples of
DAM. We follow this tradition and consider this type of DAM as more central. The following
is thus our narrow definition of DAM:
(15)

The narrow definition of DAM:


Any kind of situation where an argument of a predicate bearing the same generalized
semantic role (or macrorole) may be coded in different ways, depending on factors
other than the argument role itself and/or the clausal properties of the predicate such as
polarity, TAM, embeddedness, etc.

2.2 Predicate-triggered DAM


We now turn to the discussion of the other type of DAM, namely, the predicate-triggered
DAM. The cases of DAM to be discussed in this section involve a broader understanding of
the phenomenon according to the definition in (1) but not to the definition in (15). Here, there
is no differential marking with one and the same form of the predicate. Instead, different
though paradigmatically related forms of the predicate require differential marking of its
argument. Neither inherent, nor discourse-related properties of the noun phrase play any role
in this type of DAM. Nevertheless, we think that such DAM systems are of no lesser interest
than the systems discussed in Section 2.1 and may diachronically be related to them.
2.2.1 Clause-type-based differential marking
A very common, but not very frequently discussed kind of DAM is the one in which a
particular kind of argument marking is found in one type of clause, whereas in some other
type of clause the relevant argument is marked differently (cf. main versus subordinate
clause split in Dixon 1994: 101 or split according to construction in McGregor 2009).
7

The Finnish accusative case is highly syncretic: it is homonymous to the genitive in the singular and
nominative in the plural and has dedicated morphology only with personal pronouns. This is why it is sometimes
(somewhat misleadingly) referred to as genitive in the traditional Finnish literature.

This type of DAM can be illustrated by the comparison of the main clause with
different types of dependent clauses in Maithili. In the main clause, the sole argument of oneargument clauses and the more agent-like arguments of two-argument clauses are in the
nominative, as in (16a) and (16b) respectively:
(16) Maithili (Indo-European; India, Nepal)
a.
o
hs-l-aith.
3hREM.NOM laugh-PST-3hNOM
He(hREM) laughed. (Bickel & Ydava 2000: 346)
b.

o
okra
ch-ait
ch-aith.
3hREM
3nhREM.DAT
like-IPFV.PTCP
AUX-3hNOM
S/he(hREM) likes him/her(nh.REM). (Bickel & Ydava 2000: 347)

However, in various types of dependent clauses, for instance, in converbial clauses, as in


(17a), and infinitival clauses as in (17b), these arguments are in the dative case:
(17) Maithili (Indo-European; India, Nepal)
a.
[hamr
(*ham) ghar ib-k]
1DAT
1NOM home come-CVB
pit-j
khu he-t-h.
father-hNOM happy be(come)-FUT-3hNOM
When I come home, father will be happy. (Bickel & Ydava 2000: 353)
b.

[Rm-k
(*Rm)
sut-b-k
lel]
Ram-DAT
Ram.NOM sleep-INF:OBL-GEN for
ham yah hm-s
uh-ge-l-ah.
1NOM here place-ABL rise-TEL-PST-1NOM
I got up from this place in order for Ram to (be able to) sleep. (Bickel & Ydava
2000: 358)

Note that at no place there is differential marking possible with one and the same form of the
predicate. Instead, the marking is complementary distributed according to the matrix vs.
embedded status of the predicate.
2.2.2 TAM-based differential marking
Tense, aspect, and mood of the clause present an often-discussed trigger of DAM in particular
in differential agent marking when discussing the so-called split ergativity (cf. Comrie 1978;
Dixon 1994: 97101; Malchukov & de Hoop 2007).
The distribution of case markers in Georgian illustrates this type of DAM. In the
Georgian Present the agent argument is in the nominative case, such as deda mother.NOM
in (18a). In Aorist the agent argument is in the narrative case (sometimes also called ergative),
such as deda-m mother-NARR in (18b):
(18) Georgian (Kartvelian; Georgia; Harris 1981: 27, 42)
a.
deda
bans
tavis
vil-s.
mother.NOM she.bathes.him.PRS self.GEN
child-DAT
The mother is bathing her child.

b.

deda-m
dabana
tavis-i
mother-NARR
she.bathed.him.AOR self.GEN-NOM
The mother bathed her child.

vil-i.
child-NOM

A number of functional explanations and predictions on possible systems of marking


have been proposed with respect to the effects of tense and aspect properties of the clause (see
Dixon 1994: 97100; DeLancey 1981, 1982). For instance, Dixon (1994: 99) predicts that if a
language shows differential agent marking conditioned by tense or aspect, the ergative
marking pattern is always found either in the past tense or in the perfective aspect. Such
functional explanations of alleged correlation of marking and TAM are often presented as
textbook knowledge (cf. Song 2001: 174). However, they are not unproblematic, cf. the
discussion in Creissels (2008) and Witzlack-Makarevich (2011: 143144). One of the
problems lies in the following: languages frequently used to illustrate effects of the tenseaspect properties of the clause on DAM include a number of Indo-Aryan and Iranian
languages (e.g. Dixon 1994: 100, Malchukov & de Hoop 2007). However, it has often been
noted that though tense-aspect values of the clause might superficially seem to condition
particular argument marking in these languages, the distribution of case markers is actually
determined by certain morphological verb forms (for instance, a special participle or a
converb) and not by TAM as such and this distribution has an etymological motivation
(for examples, see Witzlack-Makarevich 2011: 144).
2.2.3 Polarity-based differential marking
Polarity is another predicate-related feature that is long known to interact with argument
marking (cf. Dixon 1994: 101). Its effects can be illustrated with Finnish examples in (17).
Whereas in affirmative clauses the P argument can be either in the accusative or partitive
case, as in (17a), in negative clause only the partitive case marking of the P argument is
grammatical, as in (17b):
(19) Finnish (Uralic; Finland; Sulkala & Karjalainen 1992: 115)
a.
sin
omena-n
/
omena-a.
eat.1sIPFV apple-ACC /
apple-PART
I ate/was eating an apple.
b.
en
synyt
omena-a.
NEG-1s
eat-2PTCP
apple-PART
I didnt eat/was not eating an apple.
2.3 Summary of DAM triggers
Sections 2.12.2 covered the whole range of DAM triggers. We identified two major types of
DAM systems. On the one hand, we distinguish between the DAM systems found with the
arguments with no direct dependency on the predicate form, such systems can be triggered by
various argument properties and event semantics and are in accordance with both our narrow
definition in (15) and broad definition in (1). On the other hand, there is a whole range of
DAM systems where the same argument role is marked differently in different predicates
subparadigms. Table 2 summarizes this typology and provides references to the respective
examples.
Table 2. DAM systems according to the trigger
arguments of the same predicate form
Trigger

properties of the

properties of the whole

event

arguments of different
predicate forms
TAM, polarity, clause

of DAM

Examples

argument itself
(local DAM)
inherent
discourse
properties properties
Jingulu
(4), (5)

Warrwa
(7), (8)

scenario (global DAM)


inherent
properties
Aguaruna
(9), (10)

discourse
properties
3PROX >
3OBV (not
in the text)

semantics

type, etc.

TsovaTush
(11)

Maithili (14), (15);


Georgian (16); (Finnish
(17)

2.4 The scope of DAM: restricted and unrestricted DAM systems


Whereas in some languages DAM seems to apply through the whole language system, in
many languages its range is restricted in various ways, e.g. to particular predicates or
individual clause types or to particular inflectional classes. Thus, one can distinguish between
restricted DAM systems (to be illustrated in this section) and apparently unrestricted systems
(the examples were given in Sections 2.12.3, though admittedly we are not always certain
whether DAM indeed applies without any restrictions in these languages).
In Latvian the NOM/ACC split patient marking is restricted to a very limited domain,
namely, to the debitive construction denoting necessity. The construction is marked by an
auxiliary and the prefix j- on the verb, as in (20):
(20)

Latvian (Baltic, Indo-European)


a.
Tev
(ir)
j-ciena
mani/*es
you.DAT
(AUX.PRS.3) DEB-respect I.ACC/*I.NOM
b.
Tev
(ir)
j-ciena
vi/mte/valsts
you.DAT
(AUX.PRS.3) DEB-respect he.NOM/mother.NOM/state.NOM
a. You have to be respectful towards me (ACC)
b. You have to be respectful towards him (NOM) / [your] mother (NOM) / [the]
country (NOM). [Constructed example]

In this construction the patient argument realized with speech-act-participant personal and
reflexive pronouns are obligatorily marked with the accusative case, while other NP types are
marked with the nominative case in the standard language. Elsewhere, Latvian does not show
any DAM. The debitive construction in (20) is thus the only domain in Latvian within which
one finds DAM.
Another type of cross-linguistically common domains within which one finds DAM
are subordinate clauses. For instance, in Turkish, the domains for the differential subject
marking is the nominalized (argument) subordinate clause in which the subordinate subject
must either bear the nominative case which is a morphological zero or be marked overtly
by the genitive case. In the former case the subject has a generic, non-specific interpretation
(21b), in the latter case, it is specific indefinite (21a) (Kornfilt 2008: 8384):
(21)
a.

Turkish (Turkic)
[ky-
bir
haydut-un
bas-t-n]-
duy-du-m
village-ACC a
robber-GEN raid-FN-3SG-ACC
hear-PST-1SG
I heard that a (certain) robber raided the village. (specific) (Kornfilt 2008: 84)

b.

[ky-
haydut
bas-t-n]-
duy-du-m
village-ACC robber
raid-FN-3SG-ACC
hear-PST-1SG
I heard that robbers raided the village. (non-specific, generic) (Kornfilt 2008: 84)

Crucially, the NOM vs. GEN differential subject marking is found only in the subordinate
clauses, while the main clauses do not allow this DSM.
In addition to syntactically restricted domains, as in (20) and (21), DAM systems may
also be restricted lexically. Thus, the range of DAM may be limited by a particular class of
verbs semantically or otherwise motivated. For instance, a small number of one-argument
predicates in Hindi/Urdu allow for differential marking of its sole argument in correlation
with volitionality, e.g. bhk- bark, khs- cough, chk- sneeze, hs- lough, etc. (see
Davison 1999 for an exhaustive list). This is illustrated in (22): whereas in (22a) the sole
argument is in the unmarked nominative case and the events of coughing is understood as
being unintentional, in (22b) the sole argument is in the ergative case to reflect the intentional
nature of the coughing event:
(22)
a.

Hindi/Urdu (Indo-Aryan; India, Pakistan; Tuite et al. 1985: 264)


Ram
khs-a.
Ram.NOM cough-PRF.M
Ram coughed.

b.

Ram=ne
khs-a.
Ram=ERG cough-PRF.M
Ram coughed (purposefully).

We discussed similar cases in Section 2.1.4.1 under properties dependent on event


semantics. The major difference between these examples and the examples in Section 2.1.4.1
lies in the fact that the range of DAM on the basis of intentionality in Hindi/Urdu does not
apply to every sole argument, instead, its domain is limited to a very small set of verbs.
To summarize, the range of DAM can be restricted in various ways by the properties
of the predicate: by various verbal grammatical categories (such as tense, aspect or mood), by
the syntactic position (e.g. embedded vs. matrix) or by lexical restrictions (particular verb
classes only). The categories which restrict the range of DAM are often similar to the ones
discussed in Section 2.2, but their effect on DAM is different: whereas in restricted systems
we find DAM triggered mostly by the familiar inherent or discourse-based properties of
arguments but limited to particular contexts, e.g. to particular types of clauses, the predicatebased DAM systems in Section 2.2 are directly triggered by a particular form of the predicate.
Note that the restricted argument-triggered DAM systems (Section 2.1) still adhere to the
narrow definition of DAM in (15) alongside the unrestricted argument-triggered ones.
Another way to put it is as follows: if one knows that the DAM system is restricted, one can
identify the domain where one finds alternating argument marking. However, to predict what
kind of marking an argument takes, one has to consider the triggers of DAM. The crosstabulation of the scope variable of DAM system and the familiar trigger variable yields four
subtypes of DAM systems summarized in Table 3:

Table 3. Typological variation of DAM systems


Trigger
argument properties
predicate properties
unrestricted argument- unrestricted predicateunrestricted
triggered DAM
triggered DAM
Scope
restricted argument
restricted predicaterestricted
triggered DAM
triggered DAM

3 Morphological and syntactic properties of DAM


In this section we provide a survey of the variation in DAM related to its morphological and
syntactic properties. We first discuss the dichotomy between symmetric and asymmetric
DAM systems (Section 3.1) and then proceed to the locus of marking and give a short
overview of the research on differential flagging in contrast to differential indexing (Section
3.2). In Section 3.3 we briefly consider the syntactic properties of DAM. Finally, Section 3.4
touches upon the issues of obligatoriness of DAM.
3.1 Symmetric vs. asymmetric DAM
Most of the studies on DAM focused on an asymmetric morphological alternation between an
overt morphological exponent and a morphological zero. Recently, however, also symmetric
DAM systems i.e. systems where both alternatives receive an overt morphological marking
became the focus of attention in several studies (e.g. de Hoop & Malchukov 2008; Iemmolo
2013). Some researchers have argued that symmetric and asymmetric DAM systems are
regulated by different parameters (Dalrymple & Nikolaeva 2011: 19; Iemmolo 2013). For
instance, Iemmolos (2013) study restricted to differential object marking shows that
symmetric DOM systems respond to parameters related to verbal semantics, e.g. polarity and
quantification, affectedness or boundedness (aspectuality), whereas asymmetric systems
reflect various argument-internal properties, most prominently information-structure role,
animacy, identifiability, etc. (cf. Section 2.1). While functional correlations to the
morphological realization of DAM as those put forward by Iemmolo (2013) do indeed find
significant cross-linguistic support, we think that they must be viewed rather as tendencies.
Thus, the DAM found, for instance, in Warrwa (Kimberley, Western Australia) as described
by McGregor (2006) involves alternations between two different ergatives and is thereby
symmetric by definition but is inherently argument-related (see also Fauconnier 2011;
Arkadiev 2014).
3.2 Differential flagging vs. differential indexing
Differential marking of arguments may be realized as head- or as dependent-marking a
difference that is largely constrained by the strategy the language uses to mark the core
arguments (i.e. indexing only, indexing and flagging or flagging only). Thus, inter alia,
Dalrymple & Nikolaeva (2011) treat both as different aspects of the same phenomenon.
At the same time, indexing and flagging often have different functions not only
synchronically but also diachronically (cf., inter alia, Croft 1988: 167168). While agreement
or indexing is a topic related phenomenon as Givn (1976: 185) puts it (cf. also Kibrik
2011), flagging, at least initially, is not related to topichood or information-structure in
general, but rather to semantic roles and various dependency relations between a head and its
dependent, historically stemming from some local expressions (cf. Iemmolo, forthc. b).
Semantic roles and various dependency relations constitute the most frequent function of
cases (cf. Blake 1994). At the same time, case marking can and does sometimes end up being

employed for rather pragmatic than semantic purposes, cf., inter alia, the optional ergative
marking illustrated in Section 2.1.2, where one of the ergative markers is associated with
continuous topichood while the other with some contrast. Iemmolo (forthc. a) is an important
attempt to delineate the distinction between differential object marking (DOM) or rather
differential case marking (DCM), as in Malchukov (2008), on the one hand, and differential
object indexing (DOI), on the other. Iemmolo (forthc. a) claims that the main distinction
between the two is that DOI is related to topic continuities while DOM is employed to encode
topic discontinuities. This also naturally follows from the fact that independent argument
expressions (such as full NPs) are more related to topic discontinuities while verb affixes or
bound pronouns are typically employed for expected situations such as continuous topics. It
might thus be the case that the effects found in Iemmolo (forthc. a) are due to the distinction
between different referential expressions, namely, independent (e.g. full NPs) vs. bound
pronouns/ affixes which have different locus of marking.
3.3 Syntactic properties of DAM
In the previous section we have discussed morphological properties of DAM systems. Yet, the
syntactic or behavioral properties (to use the term from Keenan 1976) of arguments in general
may be heavily constrained by the morphological marking involved an issue that
notoriously has been neglected in the discussion of various DAM systems. It is tacitly
assumed and perhaps correctly for the many but not all instances of DAM that
concomitant to a shift in marking of an argument, the syntactic role and, hence, the syntactic
properties of that argument do not change. However, there are many instances in which this is
not the case. For example, one of the markings of the object may block passivization or other
syntactic derivations in the given language, cf. Russian:
(23) Russian (Slavic, Indo-European)
a.
Ja
vypil
sok
I.NOM drink.PST.M.SG
juice.ACC.SG/NOM.SG
I drank (up) the/some juice. [Elicited]
b.

Ja
vypil
sok-a
I.NOM drink.PST.M.SG
juice.GEN.SG
I drank some juice. [Elicited]

While (23a) can easily be passivized, there is no passive construction in Standard Russian that
would match the meaning in (23b) inducing weak quantification of the object referent.
Ideally, according to definition of DAM in (1) and (15), there should be no change in
the syntactic behavior for an alternation to qualify as DAM. In case of dislocation, there
should be no resumptive pronoun and, more generally, no other factors that would suggest
rather extra-clausal status of the marked option such as rather more adjunct-like status of the
marked constituent.
3.4 Obligatory vs. optional DAM
Systems of DAM vary in terms of obligatoriness of a particular marking. Whereas in some
DAM systems a particular marking applies in predictable and consistent fashion with certain
types of NPs or in certain grammatical contexts, other systems seems to be more flexible (cf.
McGregors 2009 split case marking on the one hand, and optional case marking on the
other). Thus, de Swart (2006) reports that the definiteness may but need not be marked in
Hindi. It is only if the speaker commits himself to the definite interpretation it is marked by
case. In Russian, in turn, the marking of animacy is obligatory and is not subject to speakers

construal of the event. Obligatoriness also implies that the alternative option is equally
committal.
3.5 Summary
So far we have observed various DAM systems and their properties. In Section 1 we have
given a broad definition of DAM (1) which we recapitulate here for convenience: the term
DAM broadly refers to any kind of the situation where an argument of a predicate bearing the
same generalized semantic role (or macrorole) may be coded in different ways, depending on
factors other than the argument role itself (comparably to the way it is defined in Woolford
2008, Iemmolo & Schikowki 2014+). However, it has to be acknowledged that the consensus
examples such as DOM in Spanish may be given much more narrow definition. Thus, as is
summarized in Table 2, predicate-triggered DAM systems are quite different in many
respects. Here, DAM alternations are distributively governed by two distinct forms of the
predicate or its distinct syntactic status and is not straightforwardly related to such factors as
NP-internal properties, scenario or event semantics crucial for the other types of DAM. To
capture these differences, we have provided also the narrow definition of DAM in (15) above,
recapitulated here for convenience:
(15)

The narrow definition of DAM:


Any kind of situation where an argument of a predicate bearing the same generalized
semantic role (or macrorole) may be coded in different ways, depending on factors
other than the argument role itself and/or the clausal properties of the predicate such as
polarity, TAM, embeddedness, etc.

The narrow definition successfully excludes cases in which differential marking is found to be
triggered by various diatheses marked on the verb such as, e.g., the differential marking of the
object argument governed by the applicative vs. basic form of the predicate, active vs. middle,
etc.
Having said this, different predicate forms expressing, for example, different aspectual
properties (such as perfective vs. imperfective) are indeed interrelated with such factors as
event semantics, but, crucially, only meditatively (in terms of Hopper & Thompson 1980). In
diachronic terms, predicate-triggered DAM systems may develop into argument-triggered
ones which suggests that these two types are not totally distinct. To capture potential
diachronic and synchronic relations we have introduced the distinction between the broad
definition of DAM and the narrow one.

4 Functional explanations for DAM


In this section we will briefly survey a few common explanations of DAM. These
explanations are directly linked to the understanding of what functions morphological
marking plays, in particular, to the functions of case marking.
The two most frequently mentioned functions of case marking are the distinguishing
(discriminatory/disambiguating) function and the identifying (also called highlighting or
coding) function (cf. Mallinson & Blake 1981, Comrie 1989, Song 2001, de Hoop &
Malchukov 2008, Dalrymple & Nikolaeva 2011: 38). The distinguishing function is
available if case marking serves the purposes of disambiguation of NPs argument roles in
clauses with two or more arguments. Case marking fulfills the identifying function in that it
codes some internal semantic or pragmatic argument properties, such as animacy or

definiteness. In what follows, these two functions are presented in further detailed and linked
to particular configurations of argument marking.
The distinguishing function of case marking is based on the understanding of what
represent a most natural (that is, most frequent in actual discourse) two-argument clause in
terms of its arguments. Comrie (1989) summarizes this intuition as follows:
[], the most natural kind of transitive construction is one where the A is high in
animacy and definiteness, and the P is lower in animacy and definiteness; and any
deviation from this pattern leads to a more marked construction. Comrie (1989:128)
The configuration involving an argument that is more marked in terms of actual discourse
frequency is expected to be morphologically marked. This account thus predicts that animate
and/or definite objects, which represent a less natural (i.e. marked) combination of role and
semantic features, should be marked formally, e.g. with an overt case marker (or by some
other means, e.g. passive or inverse construction), while inanimate an/or indefinite objects,
which manifest a natural combination, need not be marked overtly (cf. Comrie 1989: 128,
Malchukov 2008). For obvious reasons, the distinguishing function can only play a role in
clauses with more than one argument.
It should be pointed out, however, that systems of case marking fulfilling the purely
distinguishing function is extremely infrequent synchronically. These are the systems of the
kind described in Section 2.1.3 under scenario, apart from Aguaruna, other known examples
are Awtuw (Feldman 1986) and Hua (Haiman 1979). Contrary to what one would expect from
the perspective of the discriminatory function, in the majority of DAM systems a particular
argument marking applies mechanically across the board and is not restricted to marking
arguments only in contexts of actual ambiguity (cf. Malchukov 2008: 213).
In the indexing approach to the function of case marking, the presence of a marker on
an argument is independent of the relationship between the argument of a clause. Instead, a
particular marker is viewed as a device to emphasize either the finer semantic role of the
argument in an event or various properties of an argument (Hopper & Thompson 1980,
Iemmolo 2010, Dalrymple & Nikolaeva 2011). For instance, for Nss (2004: 1206), the
relevant properties triggering overt object marking is affectedness of the argument.
Affectedness is in turn defined by employing two other concepts: the concept of part-whole
relations and of salience. In terms of the part-whole relations, an entity of which only a
subpart is affected is in general less affected than the ones affected as a whole. The concept of
saliency relies on the assumption that to humans some types of effects are more easily
perceptible and of greater interest than others (Nss 2004: 1202).
Finally, there have been attempts at a unified account that integrates the two functions
mentioned above as well as add additional principle, e.g. that a case marking system should
also aim at economy (de Swart 2006, 2007; de Hoop & Malchukov 2008).
5 Questions to be addressed
As the goal of this volume is to document the development or particular steps in the
development of DAM systems, we are primarily interested in well-described changes with
regard to any of the properties and features mentioned in Section 2 or, alternatively, in more
general diachronic treatments. We emphasize that diachrony is understood here in its direct
sense, namely, as a variation between A and B whereby A is the earlier state for B; we do not
imply therefore that the studies necessarily should be historical and involving ancient texts. A
lot of synchronic variation may and should also be captured in terms of an ongoing diachronic
shift, be this either dialectal variation or variation between different registers, different
speakers generations, etc.

I. Questions on the rise of DAM


What is the etymology of the individual morphological markers involved in DAM?
How does the functional shift of a case marker to an alternating DAM marker take
place, e.g. why do dative-like cases intrudes into the domain of direct objects where
the accusative case is used, what is the semantic/function motivation for that (e.g. in
Spanish or Indo-Aryan)?
How does it come that a case enters functional domains which are rather atypical for
case marking, such as information-structure roles, animacy, (in)definiteness, etc.? Can
you describe the relevant process that took place in your language or, at least, parts of
the process?
How can one explain the development from a solid, valence-driven case frame of a
predicate into a sort of labile predicate with a DAM-driven case frame? In other
words, how is the predicate affected when it starts subcategorizing for two alternative
argument markings instead of a previous rigid one?
Does the DAM system in your language stem from an originally biclausal construction
(e.g. with clefting)? What kind of diachronic path does the change from a biclausal
construction into monoclausal undergo in relation to DAM emergence?
Is there some gradience as to the different predicates that start allowing DAM? Are
there some specific semantic or other verb classes that are affected first? Or are there
initially some formal constraints on the predicates that are affected first?
Is there some gradience with respect to different NP types that are affected?
If you observe effects on a DAM system from various argument properties such as
animacy, information-structure or (in)definiteness, can you tell whether their relevance
in determining your DAM system has changed/is changing?
How inflectional classes interact with the rise of DAM? (E.g. animacy clearly pertains
to DAM but may be realized via gender/inflectional-classes distinctions.)
Is the syntactic status of the argument the same with both differential marking
strategies or are there some syntactic (e.g. behavioral) differences to begin with and, if
so, how would you describe them? (E.g. behavioral subject/object tests could be
applied at different diachronic stages to determine these differences.)
II.

Questions on further development of DAM


What is the diachronic relation between various argument-related triggers of DAM:
animacy, definiteness/specificity, information-structure-driven (in addition to
Iemmolo 2010, Dalrymple & Nikolaeva 2011), are there any transitions?
If you have NP-internal case agreement (e.g. adjective and head-noun agreement), is
there any gradience in how differential marking affects this agreement?
Do you observe some changes in the syntactic properties in your DAM system?
Do you observe some changes in the functions of your DAM system?
What is the relative chronology of the lexical input restrictions in the rise of DAM?
Which NP types acquire DAM first and which last?
How can DAM be transferred or copied via language contact?

Questions on the demise of DAM


How do DAM systems disappear in favor of a unified pattern?
What are the motivations for the DAM disappearance?

III.

IV.

What is the resulting system with respect to the previous DAM (e.g. which casemarking option has been selected/generalized and why)?
How stable are DAM systems in general? Can they be viewed as an intermediate step
in a change from one case-marking into another one (in terms of a renewal)?

General questions
Are there further dimensions not listed in Section 24 (syntactic, semantic,
morphological, distributional) that might be relevant diachronically?
How can characteristics pertaining to such distinctions as global vs. local (scenario),
argument-triggered vs. predicate-triggered DAMs affect the changes you observe (is
there, e.g. a change from global to local or a change from argument-triggered to
predicate-triggered DAM)?
Are there any changes that lead to the shift from a symmetric into an asymmetric
DAM system or vice versa?

Abbreviations/Glossings (not in Leipzig glossing rules):


AOR
aorist
ATT
attenuative Aktionsart
DEB
debitive (necessity) mood
H
honorific
HIAF
high affectedness Aktionsart
INT
interrogative
INTENS
intensifier
fERG
focal ergative (as opposed to the non-focal ergative)
min
minimal number
NARR
narrative case
REL
subject relativizer
REM
remote
RESTR
restrictive
SAP
speech act participant
TR
transitive conjugation marker

References
Aissen, Judith. 1999. Markedness and subject choice in Optimality Theory. Natural Language
and Linguistic Theory 17. 673711.
Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language
and Linguistic Theory 21. 435483.
Ariel, Mira. 1990. Accessing noun-phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.
Ariel, Mira. 2001. Accessibility Theory: An overview. In Ted Sanders, Joost Schilperoord & Wilbert
Spooren (eds.), Text representation: linguistic and psycholinguistic aspects, 2987. Amsterdam:
John Benjamins.
Arkadiev, Peter 2014: Multiple Ergatives: from Allomorphy to Differential Agent Marking. Talk
given at Max-Planck-Institut fr Evolutionre Anthropologie, Leipzig, 29 October 2014.
Bickel, Balthasar. 1995. The vestibule of meaning: Transitivity inversion as a morphological
phenomenon. Studies in Language 19(1). 73127.
Bickel, Balthasar 2011. Grammatical relations typology. In Jae Jung Song (ed.), The Oxford

handbook of language typology, 399444. Oxford: Oxford University Press.


Bickel, Balthasar & Johanna Nichols. 2007. Inflectional morphology. In Timothy Shopen
(ed.), Language typology and syntactic description, vol 3, 169240. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Bickel, Balthasar & Yogendra P. Ydava. 2000. A fresh look at grammatical relations in
Indo-Aryan. Lingua 110. 343373.
Bickel, Balthasar, Alena Witzlack-Makarevich & Taras Zakharko. 2014+. Typological
evidence against universal effects of referential scales on case alignment. In Ina
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, Andrej Malchukov & Marc Richards (eds.), Scales: a crossdisciplinary perspective on referential hierarchies. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Blake, Barry J. 1994. Case. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Second Edn.
Bossong, Georg. 1982. Der prpositionale Akkusativ im Sardischen. In Otto Winkelmann &
Maria Braisch (eds.), Festschrift fr Johannes Hubschmid zum 65. Geburtstag. Bern:
Francke.
Bossong, Georg. 1998. Le marquage diffrentiel de lobjet dans les langues dEurope. In Jack
Feuillet (ed.), Actance et valence dans les language de lEurope. 193258. Berlin: Mouton
de Gryuter.
Brown, Jason & Tyler Peterson. 2008. Grammaticalization and strategies in resolving subject
marking paradoxes: The case of Tsimshianic. In de Hoop, Helen & Peter de Swart (eds.),
Differential subject marking, 223246. Dordrecht: Springer.
Chelliah, Shobhana L. 2009. Semantic role to new information in Meithei. In Jhanna Bardal
& Shobhana L. Chelliah (eds.), The role of semantic. pragmatic. and discourse factors in
the development of case, 377400. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Comrie, Bernard. 1978. Ergativity. In Winfred Philipp Lehmann (ed.), Syntactic typology:
studies in the phenomenology of language, 329394. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Comrie, Bernard. 1989. Language universals and linguistic typology, 2nd ed. Oxford:
Blackwell.
Creissel, Denis. 2008. Direct and indirect explanations of typological regularities: the case of
alignment variations. Folia Linguistica 42. 138.
Croft, William. 1988. Agreement vs. case marking and direct objects. In Michael Barlow &
Charles A. Ferguson (eds.), Agreement in natural language: Approaches. Theories.
Descriptions, 159179. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
Croft, William. 2003. Typology and universals. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Dahlstrom, Amy. 1986. Plains Cree morphosyntax. Berkeley: University of California
dissertation.

Dalrymple, Mary & Irina Nikolaeva. 2011. Objects and information structure. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Davison, Alice 1999. Ergativity: functional and formal issues. In Michael Darnell (ed.),
Functionalism and formalism in linguistics. 177208. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Dahl, sten and Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm (eds.). 2001. The Circum-Baltic languages.
Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
de Hoop, Helen & Peter de Swart. 2008. Differential subject marking. Dordrecht: Springer.
DeLancey, Scott. 1981. An interpretation of split ergativity and related patterns. Language 57.
626657.
DeLancey, Scott. 1982. Aspect, transitivity, and viewpoint. In Paul J. Hopper (ed.), Tenseaspect. Between semantics and pragmatics, 167184. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
de Swart, Peter. 2006. Case markedness. In Leonid Kulikov, Andrej Malchukov & Peter de
Swart (eds.), Case, valency and transitivity, 249267. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Dixon. Robert M. W. 1979. Ergativity. Language 55. 59138.
Dixon, Robert M. W. 1994. Ergativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Dowty, D.R. 1991. Thematic Proto-Roles and Argument Selection, Language 67. 547-619.
Escandell-Vidal, M. Victoria. 2009. Differential object marking and topicality: The case of
Balearic Catalan. Studies in Language 33(4). 832884
Fauconnier, Stefanie. 2011. Differential agent marking and animacy. Lingua 121. 533547.
Feldman, Harry, 1986. A grammar of Awtuw. Canberra: Australian National University
dissertation.
Filimonova, Elena. 2005. The noun phrase hierarchy and relational marking: Problems and
counterevidence. Linguistic Typology 9. 77113.
Forker, Diana. 2012. The bi-absolutive construction in Nakh-Daghestanian. Folia Linguistica
46(1). 1286.
Frey, Werner 2010. -Movement and conventional implicatures: about the grammatical
encoding of emphasis in German. Lingua 120. 14161435.
Gaby, Alice 2010. From discourse to syntax and back: The lifecycle of Kuuk Thaayorre
ergative morphology. Lingua 120. 16771692.
Givn, Talmy. 1976. Topic, pronoun, and grammatical agreement. In Charles N. Li (ed.),
Subject and topic, 149188. New York: Academic Press.
Haiman, John. 1979. Hua: a Papuan language of New Guinea. In Timothy Shopen (ed.),
Languages and their status, 3489. Cambridge, MA: Winthrop Publishers.
Haspelmath, Martin. 2005. Argument marking in ditransitive alignment types. Linguistic
Discovery 3(1). 121.
Harris, Alice C. 1981. Georgian syntax: a study in Relational Grammar. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Holisky, Dee Ann. 1987. The case of the intransitive subject in Tsova-Tush (Batsbi). Lingua
71. 103132.
Hopper, Paul J. & Sandra A. Thompson. 1980. Transitivity in grammar and discourse.
Language 56. 251299.
Huumo, T. 2010. Nominal aspect, quantity, and time: The case of the Finnish object, Journal
of Linguistics 46. 83125.
Iemmolo, Giorgio. 2010. Topicality and differential object marking: Evidence from Romance
and beyond. Studies in Language 34(2). 239272.
Iemmolo, Giorgio. 2011. Towards a typological study of Differential Object Marking and
Differential Object Indexing. Pavia: Universit degli studi di Pavia dissertation.
Iemmolo, Giorgio. 2013. Symmetric and asymmetric alternations in direct object encoding.
STUF - Language Typology and Universals 66(4). 378403.

Iemmolo, Giorgio. Forthcoming a. Differential object marking: an overview. (a paper


submitted).
Iemmolo, Giorgio. Forthcoming b. On the polysemy of object markers and topic markers: a
study in diachronic typology. Language.
Iemmolo & Schikowki 2014+. Differential object coding. University of Zurich Ms.
Itkonen, Erkki. 1972. ber das Objekt in den finnisch-wolgaischen Sprachen. Finnischugrische Forschungen 39. 153213.
Itkonen, Erkki. 1973. Zur Geschichte des Partitivs. Finnisch-Ugrische Forschungen 40. 278
339.
Karatsareas, Petros. 2011. A study of Cappadocian Greek nominal morphology from a
diachronic and dialectological perspective. Cambridge: University of Cambridge
dissertation.
Kibrik, Andrej A. 2011. Reference in discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kiparsky, P. 1998: Partitive Case and Aspect. In Butt, M. & W. Geuder (eds.), The Projection
of Arguments. Lexical and Compositional Factors. 265-307. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
Kittil, Seppo T. 2006. Object-, animacy- and role-based strategies: A typology of object
marking. Studies in Language 32(1). 132.
Kittil. Seppo 2008. Animacy effects on differential Goal Marking, Linguistic Typology 12.
245-268.
Kornfilt, Jaklin. 2008. DOM and two types of DSM in Turkish. In Helen de Hoop & Peter de
Swart (eds.), Differential subject marking, 79112. Springer: Kluwer.
Krysko. Vadim B. 1994. Razvitie kategorii oduevlennosti v istorii russkogo jazyka.
Moscow: Lyceum.
Kucaa, Marian (1971): Zanikanie kategorii ywotnoci-nieywotnoci w jednej z gwar
polskich. Studia z Filologii Polskiej i Sowiaskiej 10, 43-57.
Kucaa, Marian (1978): Rodzaj gramatyczny w historii polszczyzny. Wrocaw etc.:
Ossolineum
Lambrecht. Knud. 1994. Information structure and sentence form: Topic, focus, and the
mental representation of discourse referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lyons, Christopher. 1999. Definiteness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Malchukov, Andrej L. 2008. Animacy and asymmetries in differential case marking. Lingua
118. 203221.
Malchukov, Andrej L. & Helen de Hoop Tense. 2007. On fluid differential case marking: A
bidirectional OT approach. Lingua 117. 16361656.
Malchukov, Andrej L. and Helen de Hoop. 2010. Tense-, aspect- and mood-based differential
case marking. Lingua 121(1). 3547.
Mallinson, Graham & Barry Blake. 1981. Language typology. Cross-linguistic studies in
syntax. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
McGregor, William B. 1992. The semantics of ergative marking in Gooniyandi. Linguistics
30. 275318.
McGregor, William B. 1998. Optional ergative marking in Gooniyandi revisited: Implications
to the theory of marking. Leuvens contributions in linguistics and philology 87(3-4). 491571.
McGregor, William B. 2006. Focal and optional ergative marking in Warrwa (Kimberley.
Western Australia). Lingua 116(4). 393423.
McGregor, William B. 2006. Focal and optional ergative marking in Warrwa (Kimberley,
Western Australia). in: Lingua 116(4). 393423.
McGregor, William B. 2009. Typology of ergativity. Language and Linguistics Compass 3.
480508.

McGregor, William B. 2010. Optional ergative case marking systems in a typologicalsemiotic perspective. Lingua 120. 16101636.
Nss. shild. 2004. What markedness marks: The markedness problem with direct objects.
Lingua 114(910). 11861212.
Nss, shild. 2007. Prototypical transitivity. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Overall, Simon E. 2007. A grammar of Aguaruna. Bundoora, Victoria: La Trobe University
dissertation.
Pensalfini, Robert. 1997. Jingulu grammar, dictionary and texts. Cambridge, MA:
Massachusetts Institute of Technology dissertation.
Rumsey, Alan. 2010. Optional ergativity and the framing of reported speech. Lingua 120.
16521676.
Schikowski, Robert. 2013. Object-conditioned differential marking in Chintang and Nepali.
Zurich: University of Zurich dissertation.
Serant, Ilja A. 2013. Rise of canonical subjecthood. In Ilja A. Serant & Leonid Kulikov
(eds.), The diachronic typology of non-prototypical subjects, 283310. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Serant, Ilja A. 2014. Denotational Properties of the Independent Partitive Gentive in
Lithuanian. In: Holvoet, Axel and Nicole Nau (eds.), Grammatical Relations and their
Non-Canonical Encoding in Baltic. [Valency, Argument Realization and Grammatical
Relations in Baltic, 1]. 257-299. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
Serant, Ilja A. forthcoming. Independent partitive as a Circum-Baltic isogloss, Journal
Language Contact. (accepted)
Siewierska, Anna. 2003. Person agreement and the determination of alignment. Transactions
of the Philological Society 101. 339370.
Siewierska, Anna. 2004. Person. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Silverstein, Michael. 1976. Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In Robert M.W. Dixon (ed.),
Grammatical categories in Australian languages, 112171. New Jersey: Humanities
Press.
Song, Jae Jung. 2001. Linguistic typology: morphology and syntax. Harlow: Longman.
Sulkala, Helena & Merja Karjalainen. 1992. Finnish. London: Routledge.
Torrego Salcedo, Esther. 1999. El complemento directo preposicional. In I. Bosque & V.
Demonte (eds.), Gramtica descriptiva de la lengua espaola. Vol. 2. Las construcciones
sintcticas fundamentales.Relaciones temporales. aceptuales y modales, 17791805.
Madrid: Espasa Calpe.
Tournadre, Nicolas. 1996. Ergativit en tibtain approche morphosyntaxique de la langue
parle. Paris and Leuven: Peteers.
Tournadre, Nicolas. 1991. The rhetorical use of the Tibetan ergative. Linguistics of the TibetoBurma Area 14(1). 93-108.
Tuite, Kevin J., Asif Agha & Randolph Graczyk. 1985. Agentivity. Transitivity and the
question of active typology. Papers from the annual regional meeting of the Chicago
Linguistic Society 21(2). 252270.
von Heusinger, Klaus. 2003. Cross-linguistic implementations of specificity. In Katarzyna
Jaszczolt & Ken Turner (eds.), Meaning through language contrast. Vol. 2. 405421.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
von Heusinger, Klaus. 2011. Specificity. In Klaus von Heusinger, Claudia Maienborn & Paul
Portner (eds.), Semantics. An international handbook of natural language meaning.
Vol. 2. 10251058. Berlin: de Gruyter.
Wali, Kashi & Omkar Nath Koul. 1997. Kashmiri: a cognitive-descriptive grammar. London:
Routledge.

Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena. 2011. Typological variation in grammatical relations. Leipzig:


University of Leipzig dissertation.
Woolford, Ellen. 2008. Differential subject marking at argument structure. Syntax and PF. In
Helen de Hoop & Peter de Swart (eds.), Differential subject marking, 1740. Springer:
Kluwer.
Zeevat, Henk & Gerhard Jger. 2002. A reinterpretation of syntactic alignment. In Dick de
Jongh, Henk Zeevat & Maria Nilsenov (eds.), Proceedings of the third and fourth
international Tbilisi symposium on language. Logic and computation. Amsterdam: ILLC
Scientific Publications.
Ziga, Fernando. 2006. Deixis and alignment: inverse systems in indigenous languages of
the Americas. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen