Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

Stages of Change and the Transtheoretical Model

(James O. Prochaska, 1985)

Purpose
The transtheoretical model (TTM) of health behavior seeks to bridge the cognitive and the behaviorist
approaches by positing a series of stages in modifying behavior; in only some of these are cognitive
processes pertinent. (See diagram).

Conceptual Basis
Prochaska based his approach on a set of assumptions that formed a critique of earlier approaches to
behavior change. He observed, first, that a majority of at-risk people are not ready to change their
behavior, and consequently will not be helped by traditional action-oriented prevention programs.
A major problem in many efforts to impact upon the smoking population is the failure to
take into account the readiness of the individual smoker with respect to changing his/her
smoking behavior. Most past smoking-cessation programs and techniques were
designed with an explicit action-oriented strategy that assumed the individual smoker
was ready to embark on the smoking-cessation venture... (1, p189)
Indeed, no single theoretical approach could address the complexities of behavioral change, and hence
the notion of a ‘transtheoretical’ approach. While behavior change is complex, it does, however,
unfold in a stable sequence of stages, and these form the basis for the TTM (2, p104). Furthermore,
people typically apply different processes of change according to the progress they have made in
changing their behavior.
The transtheoretical model drew concepts from a number of theories to develop a combined
model of behavioral change, originally in the context of smoking cessation. Prochaska et al. argued that
no single theory can account for the complexities of behavior change; they drew concepts from Freud,
from Skinner, from Rogers and others to identify processes of behavioral change over time. In place of
viewing change as a discrete event, it approached change as a process that occurs in a series of
identifiable stages. At each of these stages different types of intervention are appropriate to move the
person to the next stage in the process (2, p104).

Description
The model includes four main constructs: the sequential stages of change; processes which people
typically use to facilitate change; decisional balance, which predicts whether change will occur; and self-
efficacy, the person’s confidence they can make changes.
The six stages of change represent ‘motivational postures’ and include:

Stage Description
Precontemplation The person has no intention of changing the behavior, usually within the next
(PC) six months. This may be due to a lack of information, or to lack of confidence.
The person is typically unmotivated and will resist discussing or thinking about
making the change. They are not ready for traditional health promotion
interventions.

Contemplation (C) The person expresses an intention to take action within six months. They are
aware of both benefits and costs of making the change, and this balance may
keep them in this phase for a long time: “chronic contemplation or behavioral
procrastination” (2, p100). They are not ready for an intervention that expects
immediate action.

Preparation (P) The person intends to take action in the immediate future (commonly defined
as 30 days). They typically have a plan of action (such as joining a fitness
class, etc.) and have taken some preparatory action (such as obtaining
information). They are ready for traditional action interventions.

Action (A) The person has made specified changes to their lifestyle within the past six
months.

Maintenance (M) The person works to prevent relapse; during this phase confidence increases
that they can continue their new lifestyle. The duration of this phase varies
according to the behavior; for smoking, abstinence for five years may be
required before the risk of relapse becomes insignificant.

Termination (T) In principle, the maintenance stage will lead to a phase in which the person is
no longer tempted to revert to the former behavior and the change is
complete.

Classifying People According to Stage. People are classified into stage via questionnaires that
generally provide a simple algorithm. Applied to current smokers, for example, a question might read
“Are you seriously considering quitting smoking?” and the answers would include “No” (PC); “Within
the next six months” (C), “Within the next month, and have quit for at least 24 hours within the past 12
months” (P). For ex-smokers, “Have quit within the past 6 months” (A); “Have quit for more than 6
months” (M) (3, p64). Staging algorithms have been proposed for exercise readiness (4), adolescent
alcohol use (5), and mammography (2, p113).
The six stages were initially proposed as homogeneous groupings but empirical analyses
subsequently suggested that variants exist. Velicer et al. described distinct subtypes within each stage
of change. For precontemplation, they identified ‘progressing,’ ‘disengaged’ and ‘immotive’ sub-
groups. For contemplation and preparation, four subgroups were identified: ‘classic contemplators,’
‘early,’ ‘progressing,’ and ‘disengaged.’ The action stage was divided into ‘classic action,’
‘progressing,’ ‘disengaged,’ and ‘risk for relapse.’ (6, pp305-309). These groups employed different
processes of change. Anatchkova et al. have identified Classic, Progressing, Early Preparation, and
Disengaged subtypes for the Preparation stage, based on different patterns of pros and cons (7).
Likewise, Contemplators could be divided into Classic Contemplators, Progressing, Early
Contemplators, and Disengaged (8).

The processes of change include the activities or processes that help the person in progressing through
the stages of change. Prochaska et al. list ten processes, but there may be more:

Process Description
Experiential processes (cognitive and emotional activities)

Consciousness raising Finding facts and information that support the change in behavior; these
include information on how to change and awareness of the adverse
consequences of the behavior. Information comes from the therapist, the
media or from reading

Dramatic relief Experiencing the negative emotions that accompany the behavior, and
recognizing the relief that accompanies changing the behavior. People
may be moved emotionally by personal testimonies of people who have
changed, or through psychodrama.

Self-evaluation Coming to recognize the change as a significant part of one’s identity by


contrasting the image before and after the change (e.g., switching from
couch potato to active person)

Environmental evaluation The person’s recognition of how their behavior affects those around
them. This has a cognitive and an affective component and can include
an understanding that they can become a role model for others.

Self-liberation Believing that one can change and making a firm commitment to change.
Making New Year’s resolutions and public commitments can represent
this process.
Behavioral processes

Helping relationships Seeking social support for making the change; benefitting from the trust
and acceptance of others. This may occur through a therapeutic alliance,
buddy systems, or support from a partner.

Counterconditioning Substituting healthier alternatives or safer substitutes for the problem


behavior. An example would be nicotine replacement therapy
Contingency This implies creating consequences that will encourage the person to
management initiate change in a particular direction. This mainly involves reinforcing
positive behavior, group recognition and reducing rewards for the
negative behavior.
Stimulus control Removing cues to the unhealthy behavior and adding cues and reminders
for the desired behavior. Avoiding social situations that encourage a
person to over-eat would be an example.

Social liberation Policies and social activism is required to create environments in which
the healthy alternative appear as the social norm. This is especially
relevant in poor neighborhoods and with disenfranchised groups. Salad
bars, smoke-free zones are examples.

These processes were then linked by Prochaska to the stages of change, suggesting that, as one
progresses across the stages of change, there is a shift from internal to external influences:

Progression across the stages of change

Precontemplation 6 Contemplation 6 Preparation 6 Action 6 Maintenance

Consciousness raising;
Dramatic relief;
Environmental re-
evaluation

Self-
reevaluation
Self-liberation

Counter-
conditioning;
Supportive
relationships;
Reinforcement;
Stimulus control

Decisional balance refers to the subjective weighting a person makes of the pros (positive images,
values and beliefs) and cons of the recommended action. An initial model proposed four categories of
pros and four of cons, but a simple two component model was found adequate (2, p102). The theory
held that the balance between perceived pros and cons should vary across the stages of change: people
in PC will judge the pros to outweigh the cons of the behavior, while the reverse will hold for those in
maintenance. The valence of pros and cons will cross in the C or P stage of change (9, p41).
Self-efficacy refers to the person’s level of confidence that he or she can manage temptations to revert
to the unhealthy behavior. Temptations chiefly arise in times of emotional distress, or in certain social
situations, or as a result of craving (2, p103).
Prochaska is careful to point out that the stages represent a construct and not a theory. A
theory requires clear relationships between constructs, but with the TTM this has only been achieved
for the stages of change and the perceived pros and cons of change (2, p105). For people in
precontemplation, pros consistently outweigh cons; in the action stage, cons are consistently lower than
in the contemplation stage. “These relationships suggest that to progress from precontemplation, the
pros of changing must increase; to progress from contemplation, the cons must decrease; to progress to
action, the pros must be higher than the cons” (2, p105). Prochaska has presented such relationships
as general mathematical relationships. A relatively consistent finding is that progress from
precontemplation to action involves approximately one standard deviation increase in the pros of
changing. A weaker relationship holds that the same progress requires a half SD reduction in cons; in
effect, progression requires twice as much change in pros as in cons (10).

Measurements. Numerous questionnaires have been developed to rate people on the characteristics
relevant to the TTM. Velicer et al. have described a 20-item Temptations to Smoke inventory that
records the intensity of different triggers for smoking (11, p277); a Confidence Inventory (12), and a
20-item Smoking Decisional Balance inventory that assesses the pros and cons of smoking (13).
DiClemente et al. developed a 20-item Smoking Abstinence Self-Efficacy scale which records the
smoker’s confidence at being able to avoid smoking in 20 challenging situations (14). A 40-item
Smoking Processes of Change scale covers the frequency with which the person uses each of the ten
change processes listed above (11; 15; 16, p278).
Measures of Decisional Balance have been proposed for adult smoking (13), for adolescent
alcohol use (5), and for children, along with a Situational Temptations Questionnaire (17).

Validation
Conceptual Basis. Several studies have assembled empirical evidence concerning the assumptions that
underlie the TTM. The existence of distinct stages of change has been widely confirmed: in an early
study of smoking (18), in the adoption of HIV prevention practices (19), of contraceptive use (20), and
in 12 different health behaviors (9).
Justifying the claim that a majority of people are not ready to make changes, Velicer et al.
reported on three large samples of smokers, of whom 40% were in precontemplation, 40% in
contemplation and 20% in preparation. The proportion in precontemplation fell with higher education
(21). A similar study by Fava et al. reported 42% of smokers in precontemplation, 40% in
contemplation and 18% in preparation (1, p196). In a full population sample, Nigg et al. observed that
the majority of people are stably placed, either in precontemplation or in maintenance (22). In a
European study, approximately 70% of smokers were in precontemplation, 20% were in contemplation
and 10% were in the action stage (23). Laforge et al. reported on stages of readiness for five health
behaviors in Australia and the United States. The samples included ex-smokers, and found 50% (U.S.)
and 53% (Australia) to be in maintenance, and 20% (U.S.) and 24% (Australia) to be in
precontemplation (3, Table 2). Figures for adopting a low-fat diet, regular exercise and for stress
reduction were similar, while figures for weight loss (applied to the whole population, including those
who did not need to lose weight) were 39% (U.S.) and 49% (Australia) in precontemplation, and 31%
(both samples) in maintenance.
Staging algorithms were compared for exercise behavior, and showed that algorithms which
used more precise definitions of exercise classified more people in the precontemplation and
contemplation stage, while shorter definitions tended to stage subjects in preparation and action.
Maintenance was the most consistently described stage across algorithms (4).
The validity of the association between decisional balance and the stages of change was
examined by Prochaska et al. in a comparison of studies of 12 different health behaviors. Based on
cross-sectional (rather than longitudinal) comparisons, the results suggest that as people progress from
PC to C their appraisal of the pros of making the change rises steeply; this is followed by a decline in
perceived cons as people progress from C to the P and A stages such that the pros outweigh the cons
by the time that the action stage is reached (9, Figure 1). This result held across almost all of the 12
behaviors, and for a wide variety of samples of people.
Construct validation. Several studies have compared smokers in the different stages of change and
show significant contrasts between people in each stage that correspond to the theoretical model.
Significant differences exist between smokers in the first three stages in terms of their previous attempts
to quit, their confidence in being able to stop smoking, and the number of processes they use to support
change (18, Tables 2-4); (1, Tables 3, 4). Six months after being classification by stage, 8% of the
precontemplation group had quit smoking, 12% of the contemplation group, and 21% of the
preparation group (18, Table 5).
Velicer et al. tested a set of 40 construct predictions concerning the association between
profiles of decisional balance and situational temptations to smoke and the likelihood of progression
from one stage to another over 12 months (24). The analyses supported 36 of 40 predictions,
showing that outcomes in terms of transition across stages were predicted by habit strength, and
positive and negative evaluation strengths (24).
The processes of change have been widely examined. Factor analyses suggest these cluster into
patterns such as a finding of ten main factors and two second-order factors distinguishing behavioral
from experiential processes. Hoeppner et al. confirmed the fit of this model on school children and
showed that the use of all ten of the processes increased across the stages of change (25). However,
an unpublished report by Rossi gave only limited support to the idea that the same set of processes
applies across different behaviors: for some fewer were used while for others, additional processes not
normally included in the TTM list were employed (2, p106).
Outcome evaluation. Interventions designed on the basis of the TTM have been tested in randomized
trials (26-28). For smoking, typical results are that between 23% to 30% of patients progress to action
or maintenance stages over a one-year period. A two-year trial of smoking cessation among
adolescents achieved a two-year quit rate of 23.9% in the stage-matched intervention group, and
11.4% in the control group (29, Table 3). An 18-month trial compared ten intervention formats for
delivering stage-matched interventions, against a no-intervention control group. The results confirmed
that abstinence rates at 18 months were consistently higher for those in the preparation stage than those
in contemplation or precontemplation. Abstinence rates for those in preparation varied from 21.6% to
35.3%. However, the abstinence rate in the control group was 31.6%: the fourth highest of the 11
groups (28, Table 1). There did not seem to be any dose-response gradient between the different
intervention groups.
In a study of the uptake of mammography screening, 63.6% of a TTM stage-matched
intervention group complied, compared to 58.5% of a standard intervention group, and 54.9% of a no-
intervention group (30).

Alternative Forms
A computer-based expert system called the Pathways to Change system provides individually tailored
advice to smokers in the form of a computer-generated written report, based on their replies to an
attitude and behavioral questionnaire. It guides them through the stages of change according to their
current TTM stage and provides written guidance to the person on the next step in achieving change
(11; 31). It has been tested and shown to provide long-term (e.g., 18-month) smoking quit rates of
roughly 20 to 25% (11; 26; 32). Equivalent efficacy results for intensive smoking-cessation clinics is
about 25 to 30% at 12 months; quit rates for self-help manuals are about 15%, and about 10% for
brief physician advice combined with nicotine replacement therapy (31, pp119-120).
Prochaska’s team applied the expert system to smoking, diet and sun exposure behaviors
simultaneously. Smoking abstinence rates at 24 months were 22% for the expert system group, slightly
higher than the 16% for the no-intervention control group (27, p508). The sun exposure outcomes
were 29.7% in the intervention group and 18.1% in the control group; dietary fat reduction figures were
33.5% and 25.9% (27, Table 5). In a subsequent and much larger study, the smoking figures were
25.4% quit rate at 24 months in the expert system group and 18.5% in the control group (26, p411).
Adoption of sun protection behavior was 23.4% in the experimental group and 12.5% in the control.
The TTM was extended to a nine-stage model of adolescent smoking acquisition and cessation
by Pallonen et al. (33), and a similar nine-stage classification has been developed for adolescent
drinking (5). Pallonen’s nine stages include three acquisition stages for non-smokers: acquisition pre-
contemplation, contemplation (never smokers who were thinking about trying) and acquisition
preparation stages. Current smokers are divided into four groups: recent acquisition smokers, who had
been smoking for less than six months; those who had smoked for more than six months are divided
into the conventional groups of PC, C and P. Former smokers are divided into the action group, who
had quit within six months and maintenance group, who had quit for more than six months (33, Table
1). The perceived pros and cons of smoking differed systematically across these various groups (33,
Figure 2).
Various other authors have modified the general idea of the stages of change. Dijkstra et al.,
for example, sub-divided precontemplators into “immotives,” who are not anticipating changing within
the next five years (if ever) and “precontemplators” (34). The immotives were significantly less likely to
make a quit attempt over the coming 14 months (34, Table 5).

Commentary
The TTM has had an immense impact on thinking about health behavior change: the familiar stages have
been widely adopted into the conventional wisdom in clinical practice teaching and into experimental
trials. Perhaps the main contribution of the TTM has been to point out the serious over-simplification of
traditional public health approaches that delivered essentially the same message to everyone in the
population. The simple recognition that only a small minority of people are ready to alter their current
behavior, and yet there are effective ways to move them toward an interest in change, has been both
realistic and reassuring to clinicians. And it need not be too complex to be practical: the expert system
offers a cost-effective way to deliver tailored interventions that provide significant, although modest,
gains over no intervention. Velicer and Prochaska argued that, because the output from the expert
system can me mailed to participants, it can potentially have a greater effectiveness than other
approaches that may have higher efficacy but reach smaller numbers of clients because they require the
smoker to seek out a cessation program. By contrast, the expert system can be administered
proactively to smokers who are not yet seeking assistance to quit (31).
The TTM idea has stimulated many similar models. A simple way of assessing readiness for
change was the Contemplation Ladder proposed by Biener and Abrams in 1991 (35). Ten rungs are
identified, running from “taking action to quit (e.g., cutting down, enrolling in a program)” at the top to
“No thought about quitting” at rung 1. Rung 8 is labeled “Starting to think about how to change my
smoking patterns”, and rung 5 is “Think I should quit but not quite ready”; Rung 2 is “Think I need to
consider quitting someday.” Respondents are told: “Each rung on this ladder represents where various
smokers are in their thinking about quitting. Circle the number that indicates where you are now.”
The results of the randomized trials seem to suggest strong construct validity of the stages: the
2004 report of Prochaska et al. suggest a 74% higher probability of quitting for people in contemplation
compared to precontemplation (28, pp211-212). However, the evidence that the interventions
can actually move people across the stages seems less convincing. In that same study, for example,
which compared 10 formats for delivering the change materials, the no-intervention control group
ranked fourth in terms of quit rates at 18 months and furthermore, the gradient in quit rates across
stages was stronger for the control group than in almost all of the intervention groups (28, Table 2).
Thus it seems that the stages have strong predictive validity, but that the smoking cessation materials
based on these stages are less impressive. Evidence of effectiveness for other health behaviors appears
somewhat better.
Some of the limitations of the TTM are its use of a cognitive model, which may have limited
application to some addictive behaviors in which the chemical addiction outweighs reasoning or will.
The original development focused on extinctive behavior changes such as smoking cessation; the
application to adoptive behaviors such as eating fruit or taking exercise is less well studied. Rosen, for
example, noted that the typical sequence in which cognitive and behavioral change processes differ
across health behaviors (36).

References
(1) Fava JL, Velicer WF, Prochaska JO. Applying the transtheoretical model to a representative
sample of smokers. Addict Behav 1995; 20:189-203.

(2) Prochaska JO, Redding CA, Evers KE. The transtheoretical model and stages of change. In:
Glanz K, Rimer BK, Lewis FM, editors. Health behavior and health education: theory,
research, and practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 2002: 99-120.

(3) Laforge RG, Velicer WF, Richmond RL, Owen N. Stage distributions for five health behaviors
in the United States and Australia. Prev Med 1999; 28:61-74.

(4) Reed GR, Velicer WF, Prochaska JO, Rossi JS, Marcus BH. What makes a good staging
algorithm: examples from regular exercise. Am J Health Promot 1997; 12:57-66.

(5) Migneault JP, Pallonen UE, Velicer WF. Decisional balance and stage of change for adolescent
drinking. Addict Behav 1997; 22:339-351.

(6) Velicer WF, Hughes SL, Fava JL, Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. An empirical typology of
subjects within stage of change. Addict Behav 1995; 20:299-320.

(7) Anatchkova MD, Velicer WF, Prochaska JO. Replication of subtypes for smoking cessation
within the Preparation stage of change. Addict Behav 2006; 31:359-366.

(8) Anatchkova MD, Velicer WF, Prochaska JO. Replication of subtypes for smoking cessation
within the contemplation stage of change. Addict Behav 2005; 30:915-927.

(9) Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Rossi JS, Goldstein MG, Marcus BH, Rakowski W et al. Stages
of change and decisional balance for 12 problem behaviors. Health Psychol 1994; 13:39-46.

(10) Prochaska JO. Strong and weak principles for progressing from precontemplation to action
based on twelve problem behaviors. Health Psychol 1994; 13:47-51.

(11) Velicer WF, Prochaska JO, Bellis JM, DiClemente CC, Rossi JS, Fava JL et al. An expert
system intervention for smoking cessation. Addict Behav 1993; 18:269-290.

(12) Velicer WF, DiClemente CC, Rossi JS, Prochaska JO. Relapse situations and self-efficacy: an
integrative model. Addict Behav 1990; 15:271-283.
(13) Velicer WF, DiClemente CC, Prochaska JO, Brandenburg N. A decisional balance measure
for assessing and predicting smoking cessation. J Pers Soc Psychol 1985; 48:1279-1289.

(14) DiClemente CC, Prochaska JO, Gibertini M. Self-efficacy and the stages of self-change of
smoking. Cognitive Ther Res 1985; 9:181-200.

(15) Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, DiClemente CC, Fava JL. Measuring processes of change:
aplication to the cessation of smoking. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988; 56:520-528.

(16) Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: toward an
integrative model of change. J Consult Clin Psychol 1983; 51:390-395.

(17) Rossi SR, Greene GW, Rossi JS, Plummer BA, Benisovich SV, Keller S et al. Validation of
decisional balance and situational temptations measures for dietary fat reduction in a large
school-based population of adolescents. Eat Behav 2001; 2:1-18.

(18) DiClemente CC, Prochaska JO, Fairhurst SK, Velicer WF, Velasquez MM, Rossi JS. The
process of smoking cessation: an analysis of precontemplation, contemplation, and preparation
stages of change. J Consult Clin Psychol 1991; 59:295-304.

(19) Prochaska JO, Redding CA, Harlow LL, Rossi JS, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of
change and HIV prevention: a review. Health Educ Q 1994; 21:471-486.

(20) Grimley DM, Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Prochaska GE. Contraceptive and condom use
adoption and maintenance: a stage paradigm approach. Health Educ Q 1995; 22:20-35.

(21) Velicer WF, Fava JL, Prochaska JO, Abrams DB, Emmons KM, Pierce JP. Distribution of
smokers by stage in three representative samples. Prev Med 1995; 24:401-411.

(22) Nigg CR, Burbank PM, Padula C, Dufresne R, Rossi JS, Velicer WF et al. Stages of change
across ten health risk behaviors for older adults. Gerontologist 1999; 39:473-482.

(23) Etter J-F, Perneger TV, Ronchi A. Distributions of smokers by stage: international comparison
and association with smoking prevalence. Prev Med 1997; 26:580-585.

(24) Velicer WF, Norman GJ, Fava JL, Prochaska JO. Testing 40 predictions from the
transtheoretical model. Addict Behav 1999; 24:455-469.

(25) Hoeppner BB, Velicer WF, Redding CA, Rossi JS, Prochaska JO, Pallonen UE et al.
Psychometric evaluation of the smoking cessation Processes of Change scale in an adolescent
sample. Addict Behav 2006.

(26) Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Redding C, Rossi JS, Goldstein M, DePue J et al. Stage-based
expert systems to guide a population of primary care patients to quit smoking, eat healthier,
prevent skin cancer, and receive regular mammograms. Prev Med 2005; 41:406-416.
(27) Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Rossi JS, Redding CA, Greene GW, Rossi SR et al. Multiple risk
expert systems interventions: impact of simultaneous stage-matched expert system interventions
for smoking, high-fat diet, and sun exposure in a population of parents. Health Psychol 2004;
23:503-516.

(28) Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Prochaska JM, Johnson JL. Size, consistency, and stability of
stage effects for smoking cessation. Addict Behav 2004; 29:207-213.

(29) Hollis JF, Polen MR, Whitlock EP, Lichtenstein E, Mullooly JP, Velicer WF et al. Teen reach:
outcomes from a randomized, controlled trial of a tobacco reduction program for teens seen in
primary medical care. Pediatrics 2005; 115:981-989.

(30) Rakowski W, Ehrich B, Goldstein MG, Rimer BK, Pearlman DN, Clark MA et al. Increasing
mammography among women aged 40-74 by use of a stage-matched, tailored intervention.
Prev Med 1998; 27:748-756.

(31) Velicer WF, Prochaska JO. An expert system intervention for smoking cessation. Patient Educ
Couns 1999; 36:119-129.

(32) Velicer WF, Prochaska JO, Redding CA. Tailored communications for smoking cessation: past
successes and future directions. Drug Alcohol Rev 2006; 25:49-57.

(33) Pallonen UE, Prochaska JO, Velicer WF, Prokhorov AV, Smith NF. Stages of acquisition and
cessation for adolescent smoking: an empirical integration. Addict Behav 1998; 23:303-324.

(34) Dijkstra A, Roijackers J, DeVries H. Smokers in four stages of readiness to change. Addict
Behav 1998; 23:339-350.

(35) Biener L, Abrams DB. The contemplation ladder: validation of a measure of readiness to
consider smoking cessation. Health Psychol 9916; 10:360-365.

(36) Rosen CS. Is the sequencing of change processes by stage consistent across health problems?
A meta-analysis. Health Psychol 2000; 19:593-604.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen