Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

Developmental Psychology © 2010 American Psychological Association

2010, Vol. 46, No. 1, 46 –56 0012-1649/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017420

Adolescent Peer Relationships and Behavior Problems Predict Young


Adults’ Communication on Social Networking Websites

Amori Yee Mikami, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen, Meredyth A. Evans, and Amanda L. Hare
University of Virginia

This study examined online communication on social networking web pages in a longitudinal sample of 92
youths (39 male, 53 female). Participants’ social and behavioral adjustment was assessed when they were ages
13–14 years and again at ages 20 –22 years. At ages 20 –22 years, participants’ social networking website use
and indicators of friendship quality on their web pages were coded by observers. Results suggested that youths
who had been better adjusted at ages 13–14 years were more likely to be using social networking web pages
at ages 20 –22 years, after statistically controlling for age, gender, ethnicity, and parental income. Overall,
youths’ patterns of peer relationships, friendship quality, and behavioral adjustment at ages 13–14 years and
at ages 20 –22 years predicted similar qualities of interaction and problem behavior on their social networking
websites at ages 20 –22 years. Findings are consistent with developmental theory asserting that youths display
cross-situational continuity in their social behaviors and suggest that the conceptualization of continuity may
be extended into the online domain.

Keywords: online, social networking, adolescents, friendship, peer relationships

Explosive growth has occurred in online social communication Developmental Context of Social Communication
(Madden, 2006), with youths disproportionately affected by this
Characterizations of youths’ social relationships in the internet
new technology (Pew Internet and American Life Project, 2009).
medium, as well as the investigation of continuity between face-
As online use increases, so too do debates about how internet-
to-face and online social behaviors, carry high relevance for de-
based interaction may compare with historical face-to-face ways of
velopmental psychology. It is during the adolescent period that
communicating (Bargh & McKenna, 2004; Tyler, 2002). One
peer interactions arguably hold the greatest importance for indi-
argument posits that internet interaction is often of lower quality
viduals’ social and behavioral functioning (Berscheid, 2003; Col-
than is face-to-face interaction, because constraints inherent in the
lins, 1997; Gifford-Smith & Brownell, 2003). The quantity of peer
online medium hinder relationships. Furthermore, use of online
interactions and the intimacy in friendships rise dramatically dur-
communication may be positively correlated with adjustment
ing this time (Berndt, 1999; Furman & Buhrmester, 1992); this
problems because (a) socially inept youths are drawn to online
increase in emotional closeness may be especially relevant for the
interaction and (b) the almost inevitably poor quality of online
friendships of girls (Maccoby, 1998). The correlation between
communication increases maladjustment. An alternative argument
friendship quality and adjustment (Buhrmester, 1990), as well as
postulates that the internet is merely a new medium for youths to
the influence of peers’ behavior on youths’ own behavior through
display the same long-standing patterns as they do using modes
contagion effects (Dishion & Owen, 2002; Harris, 1995), is also
other than online forms of communication, such that there is
suggested to peak during adolescence. Increasingly, a significant
correspondence between face-to-face and online interaction styles
proportion of these important peer interactions occurs online for
and friendship quality. In contrast to the first argument, use of
many adolescents (Bargh & McKenna, 2004), making them rele-
online communication may be negatively correlated with adjust-
vant for researchers to characterize.
ment problems, because socially competent youths treat the online
The templates that peer relationships establish in early adoles-
environment as yet another place in which to interact with existing
cence may further become critical in early adulthood as peers
friends and broaden their social circle.
become primary sources of support. Existing research suggests
continuity in patterns of interpersonal communication and relation-
Amori Yee Mikami, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen, Meredyth A. ship quality over time and across social contexts. Several long-
Evans, and Amanda L. Hare, Department of Psychology, University of term, longitudinal studies find that characteristics of peer interac-
Virginia. tions in childhood and early adolescence are repeated in young
This study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants adult relationships with romantic partners (Sroufe, Egeland, Carl-
1R01MH44934 and 1R01MH58066. We thank the participants and their son, & Collins, 2005; Stocker & Richmond, 2007) and friends
friends and families. We are grateful to the many individuals who helped
(Bagwell, Newcomb, & Bukowski, 1998; Eisenberg et al., 2002).
collect data for this study, in particular Anne Dawson, Joanna Chango,
Megan Schad, April Reeves, Claire Stephenson, and Jessica VanAtta.
Similarly, externalizing and internalizing behavior problems dis-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Amori played in social relationships at one time point have been shown to
Yee Mikami, 102 Gilmer Hall Box 400400, Department of Psychology, repeat themselves in relationships several years later (e.g., Ped-
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904-4400. E-mail: ersen, Vitaro, Barker, & Borge, 2007; Stocker & Richmond,
Mikami@virginia.edu 2007). Given this evidence for continuity in social interaction,

46
ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKING 47

early adolescents’ patterns of face-to-face communication with Becker-Blease, 2007; Modayil et al., 2003). Similarly, youths with
peers may be replicated in an online medium as young adults. strong, positive face-to-face relationships may be those most fre-
quently using internet social communication as an additional
Quality of Internet-Based Relative to Face-to-Face venue in which to interact. In support of this argument, work has
Interaction found youths’ self-report of internet use to be cross-sectionally
correlated with self-reports of extraversion (Peter, Valkenburg, &
Existing research about online social communication remains Schouten, 2005), low social anxiety (Valkenburg & Peter, 2007),
limited. To our knowledge, all published, empirical studies to date and sociability in face-to-face interactions (Birnie & Horvath,
have correlated individuals’ self-reports of their online relation- 2002). Willoughby (2008) found trends that adolescents’ self-
ships and adjustment at a single time point, with the exception of reports of better relationships with peers, though not with parents,
Kraut et al. (1998, 2002), who used objective measures of internet predicted increases in internet use over a 21-month period.
use and self-report measures of adjustment in a 3-year longitudinal Social networking websites, the most popular of which are
design, and Willoughby (2008), who used self-report measures in Facebook and MySpace, exemplify online social communication.
a 21-month design. Whether earlier patterns of peer interactions Because of their recency, few studies have specifically investi-
show continuity with behavior in the online medium remains gated these explicitly socially oriented websites as opposed to the
largely unknown. Further, observational data about friendship research reviewed studying other types of internet use. Notably,
quality online is completely lacking. Herein, we review support unlike other forms of internet communication, these sites encour-
for the first argument, that internet-based interaction is of poorer age nonanonymous interactions and the recognition of connections
quality than face-to-face interaction, and then review support for between friends. Ellison, Steinfield, and Lampe (2007) found that
the second argument, that the internet represents a new medium for the amount of time college students reported using Facebook was
youths to enact their long-standing patterns of social communica- positively correlated with their self-reported face-to-face involve-
tion displayed face-to-face. ment in the college community; this relationship held after statis-
Theoretical reasons have been proposed stating that because tical control of total internet use, suggesting a unique function of
nonverbal cues and personalizing information are limited online, Facebook to enhance social communication. In a study of a Dutch
internet-based interactions result in lower quality relationships social networking website, Valkenburg, Peter, and Schouten
than do face-to-face interactions (Keisler, Seigel, & McGuire, (2006) found that adolescents who self-reported receiving positive
1984). Two studies that compared participants’ self-report of their comments from friends posted on their page also self-reported
internet and face-to-face relationships found that youths rarely good adjustment. Walther, Van Der Heide, Kim, Westerman, and
become as close to online friends as they do to in-person friends Tong (2008) found that participants judged Facebook page owners
(Mesch & Talmud, 2007; Parks & Roberts, 1998). Further argu- on the basis of characteristics of the friends on the owners’ pages,
ments have been proposed that internet use contributes to poor suggesting that youths view Facebook interactions as reflecting the
adjustment because youths’ online social interactions do not sub- quality of owners’ face-to-face relationships.
stitute for (or potentially take time away from) intimate face-to-
face relationships. Some research has found youths’ self-report of Demographic Moderators of Online Social
internet use to be cross-sectionally correlated with self-report of
Communication
adjustment problems (Modayil, Thompson, & Varnhagen, 2003;
Morahan-Martin & Schumacher, 2003; Ybarra, Alexander, & Although historically adolescent boys have adopted internet
Mitchell, 2005) and poor face-to-face relationships (Papacharissi technology faster than girls, current research suggests gender
& Rubin, 2000; Sanders, Field, Diego, & Kaplan, 2000). However, equivalence in most types of internet use (Gross, 2004; Joiner et
the direction of effects in these studies is unknown. It may be that al., 2005; Subrahmanyam, Greenfield, Kraut, & Gross, 2001;
poorly adjusted youths are drawn to internet communication be- Willoughby, 2008) and potentially female predominance in the
cause of their failure in face-to-face relationships (Wolak, Mitch- social communication functions of the internet (Jackson, Ervin,
ell, & Finkelhor, 2003) and that such youths would have difficulty Gardner, & Schmitt, 2001a). Females may make more emotionally
establishing positive social ties in any venue. However, consistent supportive comments online than do males (Baron, 2004; Hartsell,
with the hypothesized causal pathway, Kraut et al. (1998, 2002) 2005), which has been postulated to reflect gender differences also
found that adolescents’ objectively assessed internet use predicted displayed in face-to-face relationships. White and high-income
self-reports of decreased social support and increased psychopa- individuals may be more likely to use the internet than their
thology over a 1- to 2-year period, although these effects disap- low-income or minority peers (Jackson, Ervin, Gardner, &
peared at a subsequent follow-up. Schmitt, 2001b; Pew Internet and American Life Project, 2008),
In support of the alternative viewpoint, some research indicates but it is unknown how income or ethnicity might affect commu-
that the characteristics of individuals’ face-to-face relationships nication or relationship quality online.
play out again in the online medium. Adolescents who described
themselves as having internalizing symptoms (Gross, Juvonen, & Study Aims
Gable, 2002; Ybarra et al., 2005) were more likely to communicate
over the internet with people they knew less well, and to talk about The current study utilized observational measures and multiple
superficial topics, than were adolescents without internalizing informants in a longitudinal design to examine predictors of young
symptoms. In retrospective self-report designs, individuals’ prob- adults’ communication on the social networking websites Face-
lematic internet interactions have appeared to be extensions of book and MySpace. We first hypothesized that youths with social
difficulties that predated their internet use (Mitchell, Finkelhor, & networking websites at ages 20 –22 years relative to youths with-
48 MIKAMI, SZWEDO, ALLEN, EVANS, AND HARE

out such websites would be those who were best adjusted (a) at Coie, Dodge, and Coppotelli (1982) and modified for adolescents
ages 13–14 years and (b) concurrently at ages 20 –22 years. (Franzoi, Davis, & Vasquez-Suson, 1994). Each participant nom-
Among youths with web pages, we next hypothesized that youths’ inated up to 10 peers in their grade with whom they would most
adjustment, both at ages 13–14 years and at ages 20 –22 years, like to spend free time outside of school and 10 peers with whom
would predict similar patterns of communication on their web they would least like to spend such free time. Because the entire
pages. Specifically, popularity and friendship quality at 13–14 and sample attended the same school, each adolescent’s nominations
20 –22 years of age were expected to predict social competence on came from 72 to 146 peers, depending on the adolescent’s grade
web pages, whereas problem behaviors at ages 13–14 and 20 –22 cohort. Sociometric status was calculated for each participant by
years would also be displayed online. We made this prediction on taking the number of “most liked” nominations received minus the
the basis of developmental theory showing consistency in social number of “least liked” nominations received, divided by the
behaviors across medium and time. On the basis of established number of peers making nominations. This procedure has yielded
gender differences in communication (Baron, 2004; Hartsell, stability over a 1-year period (r ⫽ .78 for positive and r ⫽ .66 for
2005), we further hypothesized that girls would show more emo- negative nominations), strong links to friendship and attachment se-
tionally supportive interactions on web pages than would boys. In curity, and longitudinal links to decreasing levels of hostility over
sum, we expected that data would support the theory that the time (Allen et al., 2005; Allen, Porter, McFarland, McElhaney, &
internet represents a new medium to display the same patterns of Marsh, 2007).
social interaction as have been displayed in other venues. Observed positivity and negativity in peer interaction.
Each adolescent and his or her best friend (nominated by the
Method adolescent) participated in a dyadic interaction in which the pair
decided together who, out of a hypothetical cast of characters,
Participants were 92 (39 male, 53 female) youths taking part in should be selected to be rescued from a desert island. The task was
an ongoing longitudinal study of adolescent and young adult intended to produce discussion and conflict. Coders, unaware of
development. The youths entered the study in 1998 –1999 when other data about the participants, rated the positivity (smiling
they were in the seventh and eighth grades (mean age ⫽ 13.30 genuinely toward one another, validating the other person’s ideas,
years, SD ⫽ 0.62) and were reassessed in 2006 –2008 as young making truly friendly jokes, and listening to the other person) and
adults (mean age ⫽ 20.92 years; SD ⫽ 1.11). These youths are the negativity (interrupting the other person, criticizing the other per-
individuals for whom their social networking web pages could be son’s ideas, and appearing uninterested in what the other person
coded, and they represent a subset of the full sample of 184 was saying) in the dyadic interaction. Positive and negative indi-
participants assessed at baseline and 172 participants assessed at cators were considered separately as has been recommended by
follow-up. other researchers (Hartup, 1995). This standardized coding system
At baseline, adolescents were recruited from a single public has demonstrated validity (Allen, Porter, & McFarland, 2006;
middle school; the students had attended the same school as an Allen et al., 2007). Interrater reliability between raters was accept-
intact group since the fifth grade. Racial/ethnic diversity was as able (intraclass correlation coefficients [ICCs]: .65–.86).
follows: 58% White, 29% African American, and 13% other or Depressive symptoms. Adolescents completed the Child De-
mixed. Annual family income was distributed as follows: 18% pression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs & Beck, 1977), a widely used
reported incomes under $19,999; 28% had incomes of $20,000 – self-report measure of depressive symptoms (Twenge & Nolen-
$39,999; 22% reported incomes of $40,000 –$59,000; and 33% Hoeksema, 2002). We chose a self-report measure because of
had an income of $60,000 or more. Active, written informed consensus that adolescents are the most valid reporters of their
consent was obtained from participants at every assessment point; own internalizing problems (Bird, Gould, & Staghezza, 1992). The
if participants were minors, they provided assent, and their parents CDI has been documented to relate to clinical depression among
provided consent. Study procedures were approved by a university adolescents (Craighead, Curry, & Ilardi, 1995; Timbremont &
review board. For further details about the sample, please see Braet, 2004). The scale contains 27 items (sample item: “I am sad
Allen, Porter, McFarland, Marsh, and McElhaney (2005). once in a while”), each rated on a metric from 0 to 2 to indicate the
youth’s feelings within the past 2 weeks. Alpha level in our sample
Baseline Measures (Ages 13–14 Years) was .86.
Delinquent behavior. Primary caregivers (98% of whom
Using a multimethod procedure, we assessed adolescents’ peer were mothers) completed the short form of the Delinquency scale
status, friendship quality, and problem behaviors. These particular of the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991), which as-
peer relationship measures were selected because sociometric sta- sesses the adolescent’s delinquent and acting out behavior (sample
tus and friendship quality are believed to be distinct, yet important items: “She destroys things belonging to her family or others”;
ways of characterizing youths’ social competence (Parker & “She is disobedient at school”). Each item is rated on a metric from
Asher, 1993). Similarly, the adjustment measures chosen were 0 to 2 and summed to produce a composite score. Reliability and
thought to reflect the central areas of problem behavior— both validity of this form in relation to the full scale has been docu-
internalizing and externalizing domains— known to relate to social mented (Lizotte, Chard-Wierschem, Loeber, & Stern, 1992). All
functioning (Asarnow, 1988). We note that baseline data collection adolescents were living with the primary caregiver who completed
predated the advent of social networking websites, and therefore the report. We relied on parental report to assess these behaviors
social networking information was not available at this time point. because of evidence that adults are the most valid informants of
Peer sociometric status. We assessed peer sociometric status disruptive behavior (Loeber, Green, Lahey, & Stouthamer-Loeber,
using a limited nomination sociometric procedure adapted from 1991). Alpha in our sample for the six-item scale was .69.
ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKING 49

Online Social Communication Follow-up Measures such as “what’s up?” were not counted as connection, as they were
not considered to demonstrate a close relationship between the
When participants were young adults, we assessed online social friend and the participant (ICC ⫽ .84). Friends’ connection posts
networking web page use and friendship quality on these web were positively correlated with participants’ self-report on the
pages, as well as indicators of adjustment. Of the 172 participants Online Friendships Questionnaire that they “talk with people on-
reached at the follow-up assessment point, 147 reported that they line often seen in person” (r ⫽ .29; p ⫽ .03) and were negatively
had a social networking web page; of these, 118 gave consent to correlated with self-report that they “have a close friendship with
code their page, and we successfully coded 92 of these pages. The someone they met online” (r ⫽ ⫺.35; p ⬍ .01), questions used and
pages we could not code (a) were on a social networking website, validated in previous research (Morahan-Martin & Schumacher,
such as Friendster, with a scheme that was too different from 2003).
Facebook or MySpace for us to apply the coding; (b) could not be Support of friends on web page. Again in the most recent 20
located; or (c) were not accessible because the participant con- posts from friends on the web page, coders recorded the number of
sented during the visit but failed to accept our “friend request” on different friends making posts characterized by strong words of en-
the website (which is needed to view the page). There were no couragement, compliments, understanding, caring, or validation. This
significant differences between youths who gave consent for us to construct indicated supportive relationship quality on the website,
code their web pages versus youths with web pages who did not similar to ways in which emotionally supportive comments are coded
give consent on any baseline measure: age, t(145) ⫽ ⫺0.10; in face-to-face interaction tasks. Posts in which the friend said “I miss
family income, t(145) ⫽ ⫺0.75; gender, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 147) ⫽ 0.07; you so much,” “I love you,” or “you’re truly my inspiration” would
ethnic minority status, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 147) ⫽ 1.21; peer sociometric exemplify support. Posts that were mildly positive in tone, such as
status, t(145) ⫽ ⫺0.85; observed positivity, t(145) ⫽ 0.47, or “take care” or “have a good weekend,” were not considered support
negativity, t(145) ⫽ ⫺0.81, in peer interaction; delinquent behav- because these comments seem plausible in a casual relationship.
ior, t(145) ⫽ 0.36; or depressive symptoms, t(145) ⫽ ⫺1.57, all ps Comments without a validation, such as “I’m so happy to be going
⬎.05. Additionally, there were no differences between the youths home this weekend” or “did you do the lab homework?” were not
whose pages we successfully coded and the youths with web pages considered support, although the question about homework would be
that we did not code. considered connection (ICC ⫽ .83).
Facebook and MySpace are by far the most popular social Hostility in “About Me” section. Participants’ descriptions
networking websites (TechCrunch, 2008), which is why we chose of themselves on the website were coded for the presence of
them for coding. Of the pages coded in our sample, 66% were on aggression, hostility, or negativity on a dichotomous metric. Cod-
Facebook and 34% were on MySpace. On these websites, users ers assigned a 1 if there was no indication of these themes. A 2 was
create their own page where they typically include their name, coded if there was any mention of anger or hostility toward the
photos, and information about themselves. Individuals are linked world, criticism or annoyance expressed, or explicit mention of
to friends in a social network, and friends post comments on each any threat of violence. For example, the statement “I live in the
others’ pages that may be viewed by all network members. For worst town you could ever imagine, not only is it boring here, but
coding, we selected indicators reflecting the most prominent fea- it is consumed by pricks” would be counted for the presence of
tures of these web pages: the description of the page owner hostility (␬ ⫽ .63).
(hostility in “About Me” sections, inappropriateness in photos Inappropriate photos posted. After looking at all photos on
posted by the owner) and the characteristics of the friends (number the website posted by the participant, coders assigned a rating of
of friends and connection and support in friends’ posts on the 1–3 to indicate the degree to which any picture showed behavior
page). Seventy pages were selected at random to be double coded that might be considered inappropriate if viewed by an authority
to provide an estimate of consistency among raters. figure. To establish this construct, we researched what type of
Number of friends on web page. Coders recorded the total photos posted on social networking websites lead employers to not
number of friends in the participant’s social network, which is hire applicants (Finder, 2006). All participants had at least one
listed on the web page (ICC ⫽ .97). photo on their web page. Coders assigned a 1 if there were no
Connection with friends on web page. Coders examined the inappropriate photos displayed. Coders assigned a 2 for the pres-
most recent 20 posts from friends displayed on the participant’s ence of minor inappropriate behavior and a 3 for the presence of
web page. This number was chosen because most participants had significant inappropriate behavior in any photo. Benign photos of
many more than 20 posts, and the most recent 20 are automatically the participant, including most photos of the participant drinking
displayed on a participant’s web page when someone clicks on the beer, would be scored 1. A score of 2 was given for any picture of
link to view posts. Coders counted the number of different friends downing shots of alcohol or the participant in provocative clothing.
who had posted comments among these 20 that indicated that the
If there was any photo of the participant naked, making obscene
friend and the participant shared an actual relationship. This con-
gestures, or engaging in vandalism, the construct was scored 3
struct captured whether the participant was primarily communi-
(ICC ⫽ .76).
cating online with existing friends or communicating with strang-
ers or distant acquaintances, as has been found in previous research
to characterize youths with psychopathology (Wolak et al., 2003). Social and Behavioral Adjustment Follow-up Measures
Posts in which the friend said “see you at dinner” or “wasn’t that
lecture today hilarious?” suggested a connected relationship be- We selected measures of social and behavioral adjustment in
tween individuals who communicate with one another outside of face-to-face relationships at follow-up that were conceptually the
the online realm. Posts that appeared to be chain letters or posts, most similar to those included at baseline.
50 MIKAMI, SZWEDO, ALLEN, EVANS, AND HARE

Friendship quality. The age of the participants at the measures of depressive symptoms and delinquent behavior pre-
follow-up assessment point precluded assessment of peer socio- dicted hostility in the “About Me” section and inappropriate pho-
metric status, and observational measures of friendship quality tos, then the hypothesis regarding continuity of behavior problems
were not available. However, participants and their best friends into the online domain would be confirmed.
(nominated by the participant) each independently reported on the Because participants who had social networking web pages did
quality of their friendship using the Network of Relationships not randomly represent the full sample of youths, we used the
Inventory (NRI; Furman & Buhrmester, 1985), a 45-item scale Heckman two-stage procedure to correct for selection bias (Heck-
commonly used in this age group. We considered positive features man, 1979). In the Heckman correction, in the first stage, we used
(e.g., companionship, intimacy, nurturance, affection) and nega- probit regression to estimate the likelihood of having a web page
tive features (e.g., conflict, antagonism, criticism, punishment) of and, in the second stage, incorporated parameter estimates from
friendship quality separately. Reliability and validity have been the selection equation to predict the relationship between early
documented (Furman, 1996). adolescent adjustment and online communication. This procedure
Depressive symptoms. Participants self-reported their de- reduced the likelihood of a bias in regression estimates resulting
pressive symptoms on the Beck Depression Inventory–II (Beck, from unmeasured differences between youths who have web pages
Steer, & Brown, 1996), a widely used, 21-item scale with good and youths who do not.
psychometric properties (Steer, Ball, Ranieri, & Beck, 1999) in We entered all possible interactions between gender and base-
which participants endorse depressive symptoms experienced in line social adjustment at Step 3 in each regression. Out of 20
the past 2 weeks. Alpha in our sample was .85. possible interactions, two were significant. Probing revealed op-
Rule-breaking behavior. Parental report was no longer ap- posite effects such that the relationship between one measure of
propriate for this age group. We collected best friend reports of the adjustment and online communication was stronger for girls, but
youths’ behavior on the Young Adult Behavior Checklist (Achen- the relationship between the other measure and online communi-
bach, 1997) Rule-Breaking subscale. This subscale contains 14 cation was stronger for boys. Because the number of significant
items. Alpha in our sample was .86. interactions was close to what would be expected by chance, and
because the directions of the two effects were inconsistent, we do
Data Analytic Plan not report these findings in the current article.
Next, regarding correspondence between concurrent indicators
We used binary logistic regression to test our first hypothesis of adjustment and patterns of online social communication at
that positive adjustment (a) in early adolescence and (b) concur- 20 –22 years of age, we conducted correlations between these
rently in young adulthood would predict having a social network- measures. If social adjustment measures were correlated with a
ing web page in young adulthood. The presence versus absence of greater number of friends, friends’ connection, and friends’ sup-
a social networking web page was the criterion variable, and the port online, and behavior problem measures were correlated with
measures of friendship quality and adjustment at each age period hostility and inappropriate photos online, then this hypothesis
were the predictors. We included the covariates of participant would be confirmed.
gender, age, and ethnic minority status as well as baseline family
income. Gender and minority status were dichotomous, dummy- Results
coded variables (1 ⫽ male, 2 ⫽ female; 1 ⫽ White, 2 ⫽ minority).
We next tested the hypotheses that among the group of youths
Preliminary Analyses
with social networking web pages, adjustment at 13–14 and 20 –22
years of age would be reflected in the youths’ web pages at 20 –22 All continuous study variables were roughly normally distrib-
years of age and that females would display more supportive uted, with the exception of number of friends on the web page,
relationships on their web pages than would males. First, regarding which was positively skewed. We calculated the square root of this
predictive relationships between early adolescent adjustment and variable, which yielded a normal distribution; we used the trans-
young adult online relationships, we conducted ordinary least formed variable in analyses. We next examined the z-score distri-
squares hierarchical multiple regressions for the continuous vari- butions of all variables. For no variable did any participant have a
ables of (a) number of friends in the network, (b) friends posting z score greater than ⫾3.0. Descriptive statistics and correlations
connection comments, and (c) friends posting support comments; among the baseline indicators of adjustment and follow-up social
binary logistic regression for (d) hostility in the “About Me” networking variables are presented in Table 1. Variables within
section; and ordinal logistic regression for (e) inappropriate pho- each time period were modestly or moderately (and not highly)
tos. At Step 1, we entered the demographic covariates of baseline correlated with one another, justifying the inclusion of each of
family income, gender, age, and ethnic minority status. At Step 2, these separate indicators.
we entered the measures of baseline social adjustment together: (a)
peer sociometric status, (b) observed positivity in peer interaction, Factors Differentiating Youths With and Without
(c) observed negativity in peer interaction, (d) depressive symp-
Social Networking Web Pages
toms, and (e) delinquent behavior. If the baseline measures of
sociometric status, positivity, and negativity in peer interaction Among the youths who completed follow-up measures (n ⫽
predicted the number of friends in network, friends posting con- 172), 147 (86%) reported that they had a personal social network-
nection comments, and friends posting support comments, then the ing web page, and 25 (14%) reported that they did not. We used
hypothesis regarding continuity of social status and friendship the full sample of participants with web pages for these analyses
quality into the online domain would be confirmed. If the baseline (as opposed to the proportion of individuals with web pages who
ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKING 51

Table 1
Correlations Between Adjustment at 13–14 Years of Age and Online Communication at 20 –22 Years of Age

Variable Descriptive statistics 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

1. Sociometric status —
M 0.35
(SD) (1.65)
2. Observed positivity .12 —
M 2.36
(SD) (0.63)
3. Observed negativity ⫺.06 ⫺.05 —
M 0.78
(SD) (0.52)
4. Depressive symptoms ⫺.08 ⫺.07 .04 —
M 5.07
(SD) (4.28)
5. Delinquent behavior ⫺.27ⴱⴱ ⫺.18ⴱ .09 .18ⴱ —
M 1.52
(SD) (1.82)
6. Number of friends .25ⴱ .29ⴱⴱ ⫺.18 ⫺.11 ⫺.30ⴱⴱ —
M 298.60
(SD) (247.6)
7. Friends’ connection .27ⴱⴱ .41ⴱⴱ .10 ⫺.14 ⫺.37ⴱⴱ .42ⴱⴱ —
M 5.74
(SD) (2.64)
8. Friends’ support .28ⴱⴱ .10 ⫺.15 ⫺.14 ⫺.21ⴱ .29ⴱⴱ .24ⴱ —
M 1.63
(SD) (1.35)
9. Hostility in about me 1 ⫽ 79, 2 ⫽ 13 .00 ⫺.27ⴱⴱ .04 ⫺.16 .21ⴱ .04 ⫺.04 ⫺.01 —
(86%) (14%)
10. Inappropriate photos 1 ⫽ 66, 2 ⫽ 16, 3 ⫽ 10 .04 .08 ⫺.08 .21ⴱ .01 .02 .13 .01 ⫺.12 —
(72%) (17%) (11%)

Note. Means (with standard deviations in parentheses) are presented for raw scores on continuous variables. Pearson product moment correlations are
presented for continuous variables, point biserial correlations were presented for continuous with categorical variables, and phi coefficients were calculated
for categorical variables. Constructs 1–5 were assessed at baseline (13–14 years of age); N ⫽ 172. Constructs 6 –10 were assessed at follow-up (20 –22 years
of age); N ⫽ 92.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

gave permission for us to code them) as a more conservative test ethnic minority group. However there were no main effects for
of the hypothesis that better adjusted youths have web pages. gender, age, or baseline parental income. At Step 2, we found
Having a social networking web page was not significantly significant effects for two of the baseline social adjustment mea-
associated with the demographic factors of participant age, odds sures in predicting the number of friends the participant listed on
ratio (OR) ⫽ 0.64, 95% confidence interval (CI) ⫽ 0.41–1.01, p ⫽ their web page at 20 –22 years of age. Youths with higher early
.06; ethnic minority status, OR ⫽ 0.72, CI ⫽ 0.25–2.11, p ⬎ .05; adolescent positivity in their peer dyadic interaction had more
gender, OR ⫽ 1.26, CI ⫽ 0.51–3.14, p ⬎ .05; or baseline family friends in their online network. Youths with more negativity in
income, OR ⫽ 1.51, CI ⫽ 0.93–2.45, p ⬎ .05. However, after their peer dyadic interaction, however, had fewer friends in their
statistical control of demographic factors, early adolescent youths online networks (see Table 3).
who had displayed greater observed negativity in their face-to-face Connection of friends on web page. As displayed in Table 3,
peer interactions and more depressive symptoms were signifi- at Step 1 we controlled for demographic variables, none of which
cantly less likely to have a social networking webpage in young
was associated with the criterion variable of number of friends
adulthood. With regard to concurrent adjustment, young adults
posting comments on the web page indicating connection (out of
who self-reported more positive features in their close friendships
the most recent 20 posts). At Step 2, observed positivity in peer
were also more likely to have a social networking webpage (see
interactions predicted a larger number of individuals posting con-
Table 2).
nection comments. Parent-reported delinquency, however, pre-
dicted a smaller number of individuals posting connection com-
Longitudinal Associations Between Early Adolescent ments.
Adjustment and Web Page Indicators High support of friends on web page. As displayed in Table
We conducted analyses limited to the subsample of youths with 3, at Step 1 participant gender was significant, such that female
social networking web pages that we coded (n ⫽ 92). participants, in comparison with male participants, had more
Number of friends on web page. At Step 1, we found that friends posting highly supportive comments on their web page.
having more friends was correlated with being a member of an However no other demographic variable predicted support. After
52 MIKAMI, SZWEDO, ALLEN, EVANS, AND HARE

Table 2
Early Adolescent Adjustment of Youths With Social Networking Web Pages in Young Adulthood

Has web page (n ⫽ 147) No web page (n ⫽ 25)


at 20–22 years of age at 20–22 years of age
95% Confidence
Variable M (SD) M (SD) Odds ratio interval

Ages 13–14 years


1. Sociometric status 0.47 (0.96) 0.07 (1.56) 1.31 0.78–2.20
2. Observed positivity 2.38 (0.63) 2.22 (0.51) 1.12 0.71–1.79
3. Observed negativity 0.74 (0.47) 0.99 (0.67) 0.68ⴱ 0.45–0.99
4. Depressive symptoms 4.68 (3.84) 7.44 (6.28) 0.63ⴱ 0.43–0.93
5. Delinquent behavior 1.35 (1.81) 2.38 (1.70) 0.72 0.47–1.10
Ages 20–22 years
1. Self-report positivity 11.88 (2.05) 10.24 (2.41) 2.05ⴱⴱ 1.26–3.35
2. Self-report negativity 4.31 (1.64) 4.47 (2.03) 1.10 0.70–1.71
3. Peer-report positivity 11.42 (1.91) 11.03 (2.40) 1.17 0.74–1.86
4. Peer-report negativity 4.46 (1.71) 4.34 (1.32) 1.12 0.64–1.96
5. Depressive symptoms 5.32 (6.08) 6.13 (7.34) 0.90 0.57–1.43
6. Rule breaking 3.68 (4.26) 3.93 (4.04) 0.95 0.58–1.54

Note. Presented means are raw scores (unadjusted for covariates), with standard deviations in parentheses. Odds ratios, however, were calculated by
logistic regression with statistical control of participant age at baseline, gender, ethnic minority status, and baseline family income.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

statistical control of income, gender, age, and ethnic minority at 13–14 years of age significantly predicted the presence of
status, at Step 2 having higher sociometric status at 13–14 years of inappropriate photos on web pages (see Table 4).
age predicted more friends posting supportive comments on the
web page. Concurrent Associations Between Young Adult
Hostility in “About Me” section. No demographic variable at Adjustment and Online Social Communication
Step 1 predicted the presence of hostility in the participant’s “About
Me” section. At Step 2, participants who had higher parent-rated Table 5 presents correlations between concurrent indicators of
delinquent behavior at 13–14 years of age were more likely to display social adjustment and problem behavior, on the one hand, and
hostility in their “About Me” section (see Table 4). measures of online communication in young adulthood, on the
Inappropriate photos posted. No demographic variable at other. Youths’ self-report of positivity in their friendship was
Step 1 was associated with the presence of inappropriate photos on positively correlated with number of friends and support of friends
the web page. At Step 2, more self-reported depressive symptoms on the web page. Youths’ self-report of negativity in their friend-

Table 3
Social Adjustment at 13–14 Years of Age Predicts Friendship on Social Networking Web Pages
at 20 –22 Years of Age

Predictor Number of Friends’ Friends’


(ages 13–14 years) friends ␤ connection ␤ support ␤

Step 1
Family income .23 .09 .14
Female gender ⫺.02 .09 .33ⴱⴱ
Ethnic minority .28ⴱ ⫺.14 ⫺.07
Age at baseline ⫺.12 ⫺.02 .02
Summary statistics
Total R2 .20ⴱ .20ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ
⌬R2 .20ⴱ .20ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ
Step 2
Sociometric status .18 .09 .24ⴱ
Observed positivity .32ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ ⫺.04
Observed negativity ⫺.27ⴱ .20 ⫺.14
Delinquent behavior ⫺.13 ⫺.23ⴱ ⫺.03
Depressive symptoms ⫺.23 ⫺.02 ⫺.03
Summary statistics
Total R2 .32ⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱ .29ⴱⴱ
⌬R2 .12ⴱ .16ⴱⴱ .05

Note. ⌬R2 effect size conventions: .01 ⫽ small; .06 ⫽ medium; .14 ⫽ large.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKING 53

Table 4
Behavioral Adjustment at 13–14 Years of Age Predicts Problem Behaviors on Social Networking
Web Pages at 20 –22 Years of Age

Hostility Inappropriate photos

Predictor 95% Confidence 95% Confidence


(13–14 years of age) Odds ratio interval Odds ratio interval

Step 1
Family income 0.53 0.22–1.29 0.79 0.51–1.23
Female gender 0.77 0.19–3.08 0.80 0.25–2.56
Ethnic minority 0.56 0.29–1.10 0.80 0.15–4.29
Age at baseline 0.60 0.27–1.36 0.80 0.43–1.49
Step 2
Sociometric status 1.08 0.40–2.94 0.98 0.65–1.49
Observed positivity 0.48 0.21–1.10 1.05 0.44–2.54
Observed negativity 2.07 0.49–8.71 1.02 0.20–5.10
Delinquent behavior 2.73ⴱ 1.05–7.13 0.97 0.65–1.44
Depressive symptoms 1.16 0.24–5.66 1.27ⴱ 1.02–1.57

p ⬍ .05.

ship was negatively correlated with connection of friends on the ship quality was concurrently associated with more friends
web page. Best friend reports of positivity and negativity were posting supportive comments online; self-reported positivity
associated, positively and negatively, respectively, with observed was also associated with a larger number of friends. Self-
support on the web page. With regard to problem behaviors, reported negativity in the friendship predicted fewer friends
concurrent rule-breaking behavior, but not depression, was corre- posting comments indicating connection; peer-reported nega-
lated with inappropriate photos. Neither problem behavior was tivity predicted fewer friends posting supportive comments.
significantly correlated with hostility on the web pages. Concurrent peer reports of the participants’ rule-breaking be-
havior was associated with more posted pictures of the partic-
Discussion ipant engaging in inappropriate actions.
Taken together, these results are consistent with existing devel-
Findings from this study suggest that youths may use social
opmental theory that youths display cross-situational continuity in
networking websites to enact their long-standing face-to-face pat-
their interpersonal interactions and suggest that the conceptualiza-
terns of interaction. After accounting for demographic factors, we
tion of continuity may be extended into the online domain. Find-
found that youths who were better adjusted at 13–14 years of age,
ings also highlight the potential importance of establishing positive
as indicated by having less observed negativity in face-to-face peer
peer relationships in the developmental period between early ad-
interactions and fewer self-reported depressive symptoms, were
olescence and young adulthood, suggesting that sociometric status
more likely to be using social networking websites at 20 –22 years
and friendship quality at 13–14 years of age may set the stage for
of age. In addition, youths at age 20 –22 who self-reported more
positivity in their closest friendship were more likely to be using youths’ relationship quality at 20 –22 years of age. Further, indi-
social networking websites. cators of delinquent and depressive psychopathology in early
Among the group of youths with social networking web adolescence may manifest themselves in young adult relationships,
pages, the presence of higher positivity and lower negativity in suggesting that adjustment patterns in early adolescence can carry
a dyadic peer interaction in early adolescence each predicted a long-lasting effects.
greater number of friends on the website. Higher observed Our findings diverge from previous studies suggesting low
positivity in the peer interaction and lower parent-reported correspondence between online and face-to-face relationships and
delinquent behaviors predicted a greater number of friends that maladjusted youths are drawn to the internet. However, pre-
posting comments indicating connection. Higher peer sociomet- vious studies were conducted when far fewer households had
ric status at baseline predicted a greater number of friends access to online technology. Given the high degree to which social
posting comments indicating support. Early adolescent delin- networking web pages are used by youths today, it is more likely
quent behaviors predicted the presence of hostility in the par- that this represents a normative way of communicating. Social
ticipant’s “About Me” section, and depressive symptoms pre- networking web pages also differ from previous internet commu-
dicted the presence of photos on the website that might be nication tools (such as instant messaging and e-mail) in that they
considered inappropriate. Females were more likely to have support communication with many friends and encourage users to
friends posting supportive comments than were males, but the recognize connections between individuals. The graphical inter-
predictive relationships between early adolescent adjustment face of the web page allows users to share information with friends
and young adult online communication did not differ by gender. in a vivid, easily accessible manner. In short, the web has changed
Analyses further suggested that concurrent social and behav- rapidly enough that studies based on usage from the year 2000 may
ioral adjustment was also associated with similar patterns of not reflect the impact of sites explicitly designed to facilitate social
communication online. Self- and peer-reported positive friend- networking on youths’ interactions.
54 MIKAMI, SZWEDO, ALLEN, EVANS, AND HARE

Table 5
Correlations Between Concurrent Adjustment and Online Communication at 20 –22 Years of Age

Variable Descriptive statistics 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Youth self-report positivity —


M 11.61
(SD) (2.28)
2. Youth self-report negativity .06 —
M 4.36
(SD) (1.69)
3. Peer report positivity .31ⴱⴱ ⫺.02 —
M 11.39
(SD) (1.97)
4. Peer report negativity .13 .36ⴱⴱ ⫺.09 —
M 4.47
(SD) (1.69)
5. Youth self-report depressive
symptoms ⫺.04 .12 .00 ⫺.04 —
M 5.36
(SD) (6.22)
6. Peer report rule breaking ⫺.03 .02 ⫺.14 .46ⴱⴱ .07 —
M 3.72
(SD) (4.21)
7. Number of friends .28ⴱⴱ ⫺.07 .06 ⫺.02 ⫺.12 ⫺.05 —
M 298.60
(SD) (247.6)
8. Friends’ connection .15 ⫺.28ⴱⴱ .04 ⫺.06 ⫺.03 .02 .34ⴱⴱ —
M 5.74
(SD) (2.64)
9. Friends’ support .27ⴱⴱ ⫺.06 .28ⴱⴱ ⫺.27ⴱⴱ .12 ⫺.11 .27ⴱⴱ .24ⴱⴱ —
M 1.63
(SD) (1.35)
10. Hostility in about me 1 ⫽ 79, 2 ⫽ 13 .16 .17 .00 .14 .08 ⫺.05 .03 ⫺.04 ⫺.01 —
(86%) (14%)
11. Inappropriate photos 1 ⫽ 66, 2 ⫽ 16, 3 ⫽ 10 .02 ⫺.15 .16 .02 ⫺.11 .33ⴱⴱ .01 .14 .01 ⫺.10 —
(72%) (17%) (11%)

Note. Means (with standard deviations in parentheses) are presented for raw scores on continuous variables. Pearson correlations are presented for
continuous variables, point biserial correlations are presented for continuous with categorical variables, and phi coefficients are presented for categorical
variables. All constructs were assessed at follow-up (20 –22 years of age). N ⫽ 92.
ⴱⴱ
p ⬍ .01.

Nonetheless, our finding that the best adjusted youths (both in A significant limitation of this study is the lack of online relation-
early adolescence and in young adulthood) were those using online ship measures at the early adolescent assessment point. Because social
social communication as young adults may specifically pertain to networking websites were started very recently (Facebook and
social networking websites and not other online activities. Perhaps MySpace, e.g., were both founded in 2004 but did not achieve
youths with psychopathology were spending large amounts of time widespread adoption until later), it was not possible to assess this type
online, but in other media not assessed as part of this study. We of online social communication when the participants entered the
speculate that well-adjusted youths may use social networking study in 1998 –1999. In addition, in 1998 –1999, it was uncommon for
websites in which visible communication is prominent, whereas youths in early adolescence, the age of the participants in this study,
inept youths may prefer online activities where they have greater to be using e-mail, instant messaging, or chat rooms; at that time,
anonymity. This hypothesis is supported by findings that people these types of online communication tools were predominantly used
with large face-to-face social networks are more likely to use the by college students. Nonetheless, our study was not able to control for
internet to communicate with friends and family and less likely to online communication use at baseline, which limits the conclusions
use the internet to communicate with strangers (Bessiere, Kiesler, that may be drawn. In the future, we will be able to use indicators of
Kraut, & Boneva, 2008). online social communication in the important objectives of prospec-
Strengths of this study include the longitudinal design and tively predicting changes in youths’ social and behavioral adjustment.
observational measures of friendship quality, both in face-to-face We speculate that online use in and of itself will not contribute to
relationships and online. There is nearly complete independence of adjustment but rather that positive social communication and friend-
method variance in primary analyses. The use of peer nominations ship quality online will further enhance social– emotional functioning,
to assess sociometric status is rare, particularly in a study with whereas negative online relationships will predict increasing adjust-
adolescent participants. Further, social networking websites repre- ment problems.
sent a novel, observational medium for assessing peer relationships Another limitation of the present study was the relatively small
that has high ecological validity. sample size for the analyses limited to the participants with coded
ONLINE SOCIAL NETWORKING 55

web pages. The high percentage of participants who did not give Bessiere, K., Kiesler, S., Kraut, R., & Boneva, B. (2008). Effects of
permission to code their page raises the concern that the sample internet use and social resources on changes in depression. Information,
may not reflect the population of social networking web page Communication, and Society, 11, 47–70.
users, although we did not find any evidence that individuals who Bird, H. R., Gould, M. S., & Staghezza, B. M. (1992). Aggregating data
consented differed from those who did not consent on any of the from multiple informants in child psychiatry epidemiological research.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
nine baseline measures of demographics and adjustment. A further
31, 78 – 84.
limitation is that the baseline sociometric measure, modified for Birnie, S. A., & Horvath, P. (2002). Psychological predictors of internet
use with an adolescent sample, may not fully capture the peer social communication. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,
acceptance of youths in the way that such measures do for ele- 7(4).
mentary school–aged children. Buhrmester, D. (1990). Intimacy of friendship, interpersonal competence,
In sum, although earlier studies concluded that online social and adjustment in preadolescence and adolescence. Child Development,
relationships were nearly always of poorer quality than face-to- 61, 1101–1111.
face relationships, the results from the current study instead sug- Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types
gest continuity between youths’ face-to-face and online commu- of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18,
nication patterns, friendship quality, and behavioral adjustment. In 557–570.
Collins, W. A. (1997). Relationships and development during adolescence:
a well-known cartoon for The New Yorker (Steiner, 1993), a dog in
Interpersonal adaptation to individual change. Personal Relationships, 4,
front of a computer says to his canine companion, “on the Internet,
1–14.
nobody knows you’re a dog” (p. 61). On the basis of the current Craighead, W. E., Curry, J. F., & Ilardi, S. S. (1995). Relationship of
findings, however, it is perhaps more accurate to say “on the Children’s Depression Inventory factors to major depression among
internet, you behave like the dog that you are.” adolescents. Psychological Assessment, 7, 171–176.
Dishion, T. J., & Owen, L. D. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of friend-
ships and substance use: Bidirectional influence from adolescence to
References adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 38, 480 – 491.
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Cumberland, A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard,
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist and
S. A., Zhou, Q., et al. (2002). Prosocial development in early adulthood:
Revised Child Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont,
A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82,
Department of Psychiatry.
993–1006.
Achenbach, T. M. (1997). Manual for the Young Adult Self-Report and
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Face-
Young Adult Behavior Checklist. Burlington, VT: University of Ver-
book “friends”: Social capital and college students’ use of online social
mont, Department of Psychiatry.
network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 12(4),
Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., & McFarland, F. C. (2006). Leaders and
Article 1. Retrieved from http://jcmc.indiana.edu/vol12/issue4/
followers in adolescent close friendships: Susceptibility to peer influ-
ellison.html
ence as a predictor of risky behavior, friendship instability, and depres-
sion. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 155–172. Finder, A. (2006, June 11). For some, online persona undermines a resume.
Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., McFarland, F. C., Marsh, P., & McElhaney, The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2006/06/
K. B. (2005). The two faces of adolescent’ success with peers: Adoles- 11/us/11recruit.html?_r⫽1&pagewanted⫽all&oref⫽slogin
cent popularity, social adaptation, and deviant behavior. Child Develop- Franzoi, S. L., Davis, M. H., & Vasquez-Suson, K. A. (1994). Two social
ment, 76, 747–760. worlds: Social correlates and stability of adolescent status groups. Jour-
Allen, J. P., Porter, M. R., McFarland, F. C., McElhaney, K. B., & Marsh, nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 462– 473.
P. (2007). The relation of attachment security to adolescents’ paternal Furman, W. (1996). The measurement of friendship perceptions: Concep-
and peer relationships, depression, and externalizing behavior. Child tual and methodological issues. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, &
Development, 78, 1222–1239. W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship in childhood
Asarnow, J. R. (1988). Peer status and social competence in child psychi- and adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press.
atric inpatients: A comparison of children with depressive, externalizing, Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the
and concurrent depressive and externalizing disorders. Journal of Ab- personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental Psychol-
normal Child Psychology, 16, 151–162. ogy, 21, 1016 –1024.
Bagwell, C. L., Newcomb, A. F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1998). Preadoles- Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in per-
cent friendship and peer rejection as predictors of adult adjustment. ceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Development, 63,
Child Development, 69, 140 –153. 103–115.
Bargh, J. A., & McKenna, K. Y. A. (2004). The internet and social life. Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2003). Childhood peer relation-
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 573–590. ships: Social acceptance, friendships, and peer networks. Journal of
Baron, N. S. (2004). See you online: Gender issues in college student use School Psychology, 41, 235–284.
of instant messaging. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23, Gross, E. F. (2004). Adolescent internet use: What we expect, what teens
397– 423. report. Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 633– 649.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Gross, E. F., Juvonen, J., & Gable, S. L. (2002). Internet use and well-being
Depression Inventory–II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. in adolescence. Journal of Social Issues, 58, 75–90.
Berndt, T. J. (1999). Friends’ influence on students’ adjustment to school. Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group socializa-
Educational Psychologist, 34, 15–28. tion theory of development. Psychological Review, 102, 458 – 489.
Berscheid, E. (2003). The human’s greatest strength: Other humans. In Hartsell, T. (2005). Who’s talking online: A descriptive analysis of gender
L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of human and online communication. International Journal of Information and
strengths: Fundamental questions and future directions for a positive Communication Technology Education, 1, 42–54.
psychology (pp. 37– 47). Washington, DC: American Psychological As- Hartup, W. W. (1995). The three faces of friendship. Journal of Social and
sociation. Personal Relationships, 12, 569 –574.
56 MIKAMI, SZWEDO, ALLEN, EVANS, AND HARE

Heckman, J. J. (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error. Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2008). Demographics of internet
Econometrica, 47, 151–161. users. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/trends/User_
Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., Gardner, P. D., & Schmitt, N. (2001a). Gender Demo_2.15.08.htm
and the internet: Women communicating and men searching. Sex Roles, Pew Internet and American Life Project. (2009). Generations online in 2009.
44, 363–379. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/pdfs/PIP_Generations_
Jackson, L. A., Ervin, K. S., Gardner, P. D., & Schmitt, N. (2001b). The Memo.pdf
racial digital divide: Motivational, affective, and cognitive correlates of Sanders, C., Field, T., Diego, M., & Kaplan, M. (2000). The relationship of
internet use. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 2019 –2046. internet use to depression and social isolation among adolescents. Ado-
Joiner, R., Gavin, J., Duffield, J., Brosnan, M., Crook, C., Durndell, A., et lescence, 35, 237–242.
al. (2005). Gender, internet identification, and internet anxiety: Corre- Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The
lates of internet use. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 8, 371–378. development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation
Keisler, S., Seigel, J., & McGuire, T. W. (1984). Social and psychological from birth to adulthood.
aspects of Computer Mediated Communication. American Psychologist, Steer, R. A., Ball, R., Ranieri, W. F., & Beck, A. T. (1999). Dimensions of
39, 1123–1134. the Beck Depression Inventory–II in clinically depressed outpatients.
Kovacs, M., & Beck, A. T. (1977). An empirical clinical approach toward Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 117–128.
a definition of childhood depression. New York: Raven Press. Steiner, P. (1993, July 5). On the Internet, nobody knows you’re a dog
Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J., Helgeson, V., & Craw- [cartoon]. The New Yorker, 69, 61.
ford, A. (2002). Internet paradox revisited. Journal of Social Issues, 58, Stocker, C. M., & Richmond, M. K. (2007). Longitudinal associations
49 –74. between hostility in adolescents’ family relationships and friendships
Kraut, R., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukopadhyay, T., & and hostility in romantic relationships. Journal of Family Psychology,
Scherlis, W. (1998). Internet paradox: A social technology that reduces 21, 490 – 497.
social involvement and psychological well-being? American Psycholo- Subrahmanyam, K., Greenfield, P., Kraut, R., & Gross, E. F. (2001). The
gist, 53, 1017–1031. impact of computer use on children’s and adolescents’ development.
Lizotte, A. J., Chard-Wierschem, D. J., Loeber, R., & Stern, S. B. (1992). Applied Developmental Psychology, 22, 7–30.
A shortened Child Behavior Checklist for delinquency studies. Journal TechCrunch. (2008). Facebook no longer the second largest social net-
of Quantitative Criminology, 8, 233–245. work. Retrieved from http://www.techcrunch.com/2008/06/12/
Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Lahey, B. B., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1991). facebook-no-longer-the-second-largest-social-network/
Difference and similarities between children, mothers, and teachers as Timbremont, B., & Braet, C. (2004). Assessing depression in youth:
informants on disruptive child behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Relation between the Children’s Depression Inventory and a structured
Psychology, 19, 75–95. interview. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33,
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. 149 –157.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Twenge, J. M., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2002). Age, gender, race, socio-
Madden, M. (2006). Internet penetration and impact. Retrieved from economic status, and birth cohort differences on the Children’s Depres-
http://pewinternet.org sion Inventory: A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111,
Mesch, G. S., & Talmud, I. (2007). Similarity and the quality of online and 578 –588.
offline social relationships among adolescents in Israel. Journal of Tyler, T. R. (2002). Is the internet changing social life? It seems the more
Research on Adolescence, 17, 455– 466. things change, the more they stay the same. Journal of Social Issues, 58,
Mitchell, K. J., Finkelhor, D., & Becker-Blease, K. A. (2007). Classifica- 195–205.
tion of adults with problematic internet experiences: Linking internet Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Preadolescents’ and adolescents’
and conventional problems from a clinical perspective. CyberPsychol- online communication and their closeness to friends. Developmental
ogy & Behavior, 10, 381–392. Psychology, 43, 267–277.
Modayil, M. V., Thompson, A. H., & Varnhagen, S. (2003). Internet users’ Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J., & Schouten, A. P. (2006). Friend networking
prior psychological and social difficulties. CyberPsychology & Behav- sites and their relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social self-
ior, 6, 585–590. esteem. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9, 584 –590.
Morahan-Martin, J., & Schumacher, P. (2003). Loneliness and social uses Walther, J. B., Van Der Heide, B., Kim, S. Y., Westerman, D., & Tong,
of the internet. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 659 – 671. S. T. (2008). The role of friends’ appearance and behavior on evalua-
Papacharissi, Z., & Rubin, A. M. (2000). Predictors of internet use. Journal tions of individuals on Facebook: Are we known by the company we
of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 44, 175–196. keep? Human Communication Research, 34, 28 – 49.
Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1993). Friendship and friendship quality in Willoughby, T. (2008). A short-term longitudinal study of internet and
middle childhood: Links with peer group acceptance and feelings of computer game use by adolescent boys and girls: Prevalence, frequency
loneliness and social dissatisfaction. Developmental Psychology, 29, of use, and psychosocial predictors. Developmental Psychology, 44,
611– 621. 195–204.
Parks, M. R., & Roberts, L. D. (1998). Making MOOsic: The development Wolak, J., Mitchell, K., & Finkelhor, D. (2003). Escaping or connecting?
of personal relationships on line and a comparison to their off-line Characteristics of youth who form close online relationships. Journal of
counterparts. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15, 517– Adolescence, 26, 105–119.
537. Ybarra, M. L., Alexander, C., & Mitchell, K. J. (2005). Depressive symp-
Pedersen, S., Vitaro, F., Barker, E. D., & Borge, A. I. H. (2007). The timing tomatology, youth internet use, and online interactions: A national
of middle-childhood peer rejection and friendship: Linking early behav- survey. Journal of Adolescent Health, 36, 9 –18.
ior to early-adolescent adjustment. Child Development, 78, 1037–1051.
Peter, J., Valkenburg, P. M., & Schouten, A. P. (2005). Developing a Received May 1, 2008
model of adolescent friendship formation on the internet. CyberPsychol- Revision received June 26, 2009
ogy & Behavior, 8(5), 423– 430. Accepted July 21, 2009 䡲

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen