Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

The Clock Drawing Test: Portuguese Norms, by Age and Education,


for Three Different Scoring Systems
Isabel Santana1,*, Diana Duro1, Sandra Freitas 2, Lara Alves 3, Mario R. Simoes3,4
1
Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
Centre for Neuroscience and Cell Biology, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
3
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
4
Centro de Investigacao do Nucleo de Estudos e Intervencao Cognitivo Comportamental (CINEICC), University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal
2

Accepted 25 February 2013

Abstract
The Clock Drawing Test has been systematically used to assess visuospatial deficits related to the parietal lobes, but we now acknowledge
its much more complex relation with other cognitive abilities. Despite its common use in clinical and investigational practices, no study has
developed normative data for the Portuguese population. We present the distribution of clock drawing scores using three scoring systems in a
representative community sample of cognitively healthy subjects. We found that the systems were well correlated with each other and with
cognitive screening tests widely used and had good psychometric properties. Normative data for the three scoring systems were developed
considering age and education. These results allow a more rigorous interpretation of the test performance in clinical context and are especially
relevant for epidemiological research.
Keywords: Clock Drawing Test; Normative data; Scoring systems; Neuropsychological tests; Cognitive screening

Introduction
The Clock Drawing Test (CDT) was created as a simple and ecological way to assess visuoperceptual functions related to
the parietal lobes. It has been incorporated in several neuropsychological assessment instruments and test batteries as an indication of heminegligence and visuospatial processes both related to several medical conditions and different age groups
(Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). Later studies suggested its relation with other cognitive measures, namely symbolic
and graphomotor representation, auditory linguistic abilities, hemiattention, semantic memory, conceptual abilities, and executive function (organization, planning, and parallel processing) (Cosentino, Jefferson, Chute, Kaplan, & Libon, 2004; Freedman
et al., 1994; Libon, Malamut, Swenson, Prouty Sands, & Cloud, 1996; Mendez, Ala, & Underwood, 1992; Rouleau, Salmon,
Butters, Kennedy, & McGuire, 1992; Shulman, 2000; Strauss et al., 2006). These functions can also be associated with a large
spectrum of pathologies and aging strata, including numerous neurological disorders that can affect younger people. In fact,
several studies have shown the potential of the CDT in these populations (e.g., Huntingtons disease, schizophrenia, cortical
lesions, depression; Strauss et al., 2006). However, in the last 20 years, the CDT has been mostly used with the elderly as a brief
cognitive test to differentiate cognitively normal subjects from subjects with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and dementia
(Cahn-Weiner et al., 2003; Lowery et al., 2003; Parsey & Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2011). In this context, some of CDTs characteristics should be stated: (a) the CDT capacity to evaluate multidomain impairment, with a special emphasis in frontal and
temporoparietal involvement typical of Alzheimers disease (AD; Freedman et al., 1994; Strauss et al., 2006) and that may be
undetected by other cognitive screening instruments such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Brodaty & Moore,
1997; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975); (b) the CDT relative independence of verbal abilities (Strauss et al., 2006;
# The Author 2013. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
doi:10.1093/arclin/act016 Advance Access publication on 21 March 2013

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

*Corresponding author at: Neurology Department, Coimbra University Hospitals, Praceta Mota Pinto, 3000-075 Coimbra, Portugal. Tel.: +351-911514364;
fax: +351-239822637.
E-mail address: isabeljsantana@gmail.com (I. Santana).

376

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

Methods
Participants and Procedures
We intended to obtain a sample of subjects living across all Portugal continental areas, whose distribution should be representative of the Portuguese population according to geographical variables, gender and education. For age stratification purposes, we used pre-established groups with an approximate fixed number of 215 subjects in each age category.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

Sunderland et al., 1989) that makes it especially useful in patients who present expressive verbal impairment or aphasia; (c)
quick and economic instrument, therefore, easy to administer to the elderly (Shulman, 2000); (d) its good test retest reliability
confirmed by many studies (Mendez et al., 1992; Strauss et al., 2006), as well as high intra- and inter-rater reliabilities that
justifies the inclusion of CDT in several neuropsychological cognitive screening batteries (Mendez et al., 1992; Rouleau
et al., 1992; Strauss et al., 2006; Sunderland et al., 1989).
Most studies with the CDT have been focusing on the development and standardization of simple and easy to interpret
scoring methods (see Garca-Caballero et al., 2006; Hubbard et al., 2008), resulting in several quantitative and qualitative
scoring criteria for the different investigational universes (Fisher & Loring, 2004; Shulman, 2000). The Rouleau and colleagues
(1992) scoring system was the first to compare the qualitative aspects of CDT performance (size, graphic quality, pull to
stimulus, conceptual deficit, spatial organization, and perseveration) among different types of dementia. It is a 10-point quantitative system that encompasses the three major clock components: clock face, numbers, and hands. The Cahn and colleagues
(1996) scoring system is a more complex system as it combines the Rouleau quantitative score and a qualitative analysis of the
eight types of errors most commonly found in clock drawing as described by Freedman and colleagues (1994). Hubbard and
colleagues (2008) compared the Mendez and colleagues (1992), Freund and colleagues (2005), and Cahn and colleagues
(1996) scoring systems and concluded that they are all easy and quick to score (less than a minute), produce well-correlated
results, and so the choice for a certain system should be based on the assessment purposes (Hubbard et al., 2008). Yamamoto
and colleagues (2004) compared the Sunderland and colleagues (1989), Rouleau and colleagues (1992), and Cahn and colleagues (1996) scoring systems and concluded that the Cahn system is more likely to detect MCI (Yamamoto et al., 2004). This
conclusion was also supported by the American Academy of Neurology, which recommended the use of the CDT Cahn scoring
system as a complement of short cognitive instruments such as the MMSE (Petersen et al., 2001).
Several studies have developed normative data for the CDT in different countries. The main results have shown the negative
effect of age in the CDT performance (Bozikas, Giazkoulidou, Hatzigeorgiadou, Karavatos, & Kosmidis, 2008; Caffarra et al.,
2011; Hubbard et al., 2008; Menon, Hall, Hobson, Johnson, & OBryant, 2011). Regarding the effect of education, some
studies seem to confirm its positive effect: higher education correlates with higher CDT scores (Bozikas et al., 2008;
Hubbard et al., 2008; Lourenco, Ribeiro-Filho, Moreira, Paradela, & Miranda, 2008; Menon et al., 2011). The Bozikas and
colleagues (2008) study explored the effect of gender in CDT performance and found significant differences in one of the
forms of presentation (pre-drawn circle and 6:05 as pre-determined hour). Nonetheless, this effect was not considered for
normative data calculation (Bozikas et al., 2008). The Caffara and colleagues (2011) Italian study also explored the effects
of age, education, and gender in the normative population and found only a significant effect of age (Caffarra et al., 2011).
The multiethnic study conducted by Menon and colleagues (2011) with bilingual rural elderly subjects found a significant
negative effect of age and a positive effect of education and female gender in the non-Hispanic sample. The clock drawing
performance of the Hispanic sample was only negatively affected by age (Menon et al., 2011).
There are no CDT normative data for the Portuguese population, and the potential effect of demographic and geographical
variables has never been explored in our country. The Portuguese population is characterized by a low mean educational level
in older ages and a big disparity regarding the geographical distribution of the population. Rural areas, mostly located on the
inland, have older and less educated people, whereas coastal areas present a predominance of urban areas with younger, highly
educated people.
In this study, we explored the effect of several relevant sociodemographic and geographical variables on the performance of
the CDT, using a representative sample of Portuguese population. Although the CDT is more commonly used with the elderly,
in this study, we investigated a broad range of ages in adults in order to explore the potentialities of the CDT in a large spectrum
of neurological pathologies, including those affecting younger strata. This information was further used to perform the normative study of the CDT for the Portuguese population, according to three scoring systemsRouleau and colleagues (1992), Cahn
and colleagues (1996), and Babins, Slater, Whitehead, and Chertkow (2008). Even though it is suggested that the Babins
scoring system is more sensitive to MCI and AD and there is correlation between all the systems, the Rouleau and Cahn
scoring systems excel in the literature in different pathologies and studies. Moreover, we know that they are commonly
used in Portugal hence adequate Portuguese normative data should be developed.

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

377

Materials
Several instruments were used in order to account for as many relevant areas of functioning as possible:
(a) A demographic questionnaire and a medical history and habits inventory.
(b) Subjective Memory Complaints (SMC; Gino et al., 2008; Schmand, Jonker, Hooijer, & Lindeboom, 1996) was used
to exclude relevant SMC.
(c) The Irregular Word Reading Test (Teste de Leitura de Palavras Irregulares, TeLPI; Alves, Simoes, & Martins, 2009)
as a premorbid intelligence estimate instrumentwe analyzed the effect of TeLPI raw score of the total number of
errors in a subsample of 363 subjects; these analyses of TeLPI results assume an exploratory nature as there are still
no normative data available.
(d) The MMSE (Folstein et al., 1975; Guerreiro, 1998)Portuguese normative values were used for sample inclusion
(possible cognitive decline if MMSE 22 for subjects from 1 to 11 years of education, and 27 for those with .11
years of education).
(e) The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA; Freitas, Simoes, Alves, & Santana, 2011; Nasreddine et al., 2005) was
used applying the cutoff scores proposed for the Portuguese population according to age and education. Subjects
were excluded if their score was below 1 SD. For subjects with age ranging from 25 to 49 years, cutoff points of

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

Initially, we assessed a community-based sample of 936 subjects. The subjects were recruited at the local primary healthcare
services and at daycare centers by indication of their general physician or the institutional technical directors, respectively. A
smaller percentage of subjects volunteered themselves but the information given was corroborated with a family member or the
general practitioner. Several inclusion criteria were considered: age of 25 years or older; Portuguese as mother language and
having attended school in Portugal; evidence of no motor, visual, or auditory deficits that could act as confounding variables
throughout the neuropsychological assessment; cognitively healthy adultsall subjects should have their capacity to perform
activities of daily living intact; no history of alcohol or substance abuse; no recent history of psychiatric or neurologic disorders; as well as instable chronic systemic diseases, significant depressive symptoms or medication intake with a known impact
on cognition. An initial interview was conducted by a clinical psychologist to insure that all inclusion criteria were fulfilled.
The interview was based on a common script that included a complete demographic questionnaire, medical history, drinking
habits, and present health status. For the older subjects, all gathered information was corroborated with the general physician,
community center directors, and/or an informant, preferably a close family member or currently cohabitating with the participant. Of the 936 total subjects, 194 (20.73%) were excluded following the initial interview due mostly to the personal history of
neurological or psychiatric disorders and the history of alcohol abuse.
The second inclusion phase was based on the subjects performance on the several tests that constituted the neuropsychological battery specially built for this study, as well as functional and depression scales. Ninety-two (9.83%) subjects were
excluded due to their performance on the assessment batterytheir performances suggested the presence of cognitive impairment or depressive symptoms according to Portuguese cutoff scores. Additionally, 20 (2.14%) subjects were excluded due to
inability to complete the neuropsychological tests battery.
The sample selection and stratification process culminated on the final sample of 630 subjects. This sample was stratified
according to six sociodemographic variables: age, gender, education, geographic region, geographical localization, and residential area. Age considered subdivided into three groups: 25 49 years; 50 64 years; and 65 years or older. Regarding education, we established four levels according to the total number of years of education that the subject had successfully
completed: 1 4 years; 5 9 years; 10 12 years; and more than 12 years. The general variable geographic localization encompassed both the distribution of the Portuguese population across the countrys different regions and proximity to sea. As for
geographic localization by regions, we used the level II territorial units (NUTS II; INE, 2010) that consider the following continental Portuguese regions: North, Center, Lisbon, Alentejo, and Algarve. We also considered a second subdivision into
coastal and inland areas. As for the residential area classification, we considered the typology that integrates three levels of
classification: predominantly urban areas (PUAs); moderately urban areas (MUAs); and predominantly rural areas (PRAs)
(INE, 2010). The global recruitment process was guided by the previous stratification by sociodemographic variables and we
stopped the recruitment when the sample (630 subjects) could be considered representative of the Portuguese population according to all the selected variables (age, education, gender, geographic region, geographical localization, and inland/coastal areas).
This study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Medicine of the University of Coimbra. All
study purposes and procedures were thoroughly explained to each subject and/or informant by one of the team members, and
their informed consent was obtained. All study subjects were evaluated by two psychologists with experience in neuropsychological assessment.

378

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

As for the three selected scoring systems, we followed the scoring instructions proposed by the authors. The Rouleau and colleagues (1992) is a 10-point quantitative system that encompasses the three major clock components: clock face (2 points), numbers
(4 points), and hands (4 points). The Cahn and colleagues (1996) scoring system combines the Rouleau quantitative score and a
qualitative analysis of the eight most common types of errors: to the quantitative score (maximum 10 points), one should subtract
the number of qualitative errors found in the subjects performance (maximum 8 points) to achieve the final score10 (best)
to 28 (worst). The Babins and colleagues (2008) is an 18-point quantitative scoring system with three main components: (a)
assessment of circle integrity (2 points); (b) number placement and sequencing (6 points); and (c) placement and size of the
hands (6 points). Additionally, there are two points for representation of the clocks center and two points for general gestalt.
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), version 19.0. The study sample
was characterized through descriptive statistics. Psychometric properties of the three scoring systems were analyzed with the
Cronbachs a for internal consistency and Pearsons correlation coefficient for concurrent and discriminant validity. Students
t-test, analysis of variance (ANOVA), and Tukey post hoc test were used to analyze differences between stratified groups. The influence of gender and geographical variables in the CDT performance was analyzed with the analysis of covariance (ANCOVA)
controlling the effects of education and age. The partial eta squared index (h2p ) was used as a measure of effect size (Cohen, 1988).
Linear multiple regression, through the enter method, was performed to assess whether the influence of age (in years) and education
(in total number of years completed successfully) on CDT performance. Multicolinearity was explored through tolerance and variance inflation factor (Meyers, Gamst, & Guarino, 2006). The determination coefficient (R 2) was considered in the analysis of the
regressions effect size (Cohen, 1988). Finally, CDT normative data for the three scoring systems were stratified and determined
according to the demographic variables significantly associated with CDT performance. The normative data are represented as
the mean + standard deviation (SD), and the distribution is represented as mean minus 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD.
Results
The final sample had 630 healthy control subjects, 63.7% women, mean age of 55.96 years (+15.30 years; range 25 91
years), and a mean education of 8.08 (+4.58 years; range 1 17). Data related to the demographic and geographical variables
are presented in Table 1. One of the essential challenges of this study was to obtain the best representativeness of the real

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

21, 24, 26, and 28 were used according to educational levels 1 4 (primary), 5 9 (middle), 10 12 (high), and .12
(superior), respectively. Subjects with ages ranging from 50 to 64 were excluded if MoCA scores were under 19
(primary), 23 (middle), 24 (high), and 25 (superior). In the older group of subjects aged 65 years, the following
MoCA cutoff values were adopted: 18 (primary), 22 (middle), 23 (high), and 25 (superior).
(f) The autonomy in daily life activities was assessed through an interview with the subject and with information
obtained from the general practitioners, community center directors, and/or an informantin subjects over 49
years, the information was supplemented with the Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) Scale (Garrett et al., 2008;
Hughes, Berg, Danziger, Coben, & Martin, 1982).
(g) The depressive complaints were measured through the clinical interview and the Geriatric Depression Scale, 30
items (GDS-30; Barreto, Leuschner, Santos, & Sobral, 2008; Yesavage et al., 1983); subjects with a score of 20
or more points were excluded.
(h) The CDT was applied to all subjects in the spontaneous drawing modality. The verbal instruction was as follows: I
would like you to draw a round clock. Place all the numbers on the clock and, once youre finished, set the time for
11 hours and 10. Let me know once youre done. The selected time setting was 11:10 as recommended by several
authors (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983; Kaplan, 1988). It has the advantage of requiring one hand on each hemispace,
on the upper quadrants, considered the temporal area. This time setting involves the executive functions, allowing
the assessment of abstract processing abilities and the patients tendency to process information on a more perceptive
level (and not semantic). It can also result in a stimulus-bound response in which the subjects place the hands for
10:50 (the minute hand is placed toward the number 10 instead of 2). This is one of the errors described by
Freedman and colleagues (1994) that has shown discriminative power in AD cases. The verbal instruction condition
was selected as it involves memory capacity to remember the visual model of the clock and the instructions regarding the time as well as executive functions. This condition is, therefore, more sensitive to temporal lobe and frontal
dysfunction. The instruction deliberately withheld the word hand as it could be an auxiliary for patients with abstraction difficulties.

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

379

distribution of the Portuguese population throughout the selected variables. According to the results presented in Table 1, we
considered that this goal was achieved.
The Rouleau and Cahn systems showed inadequate values of internal consistency (a 0.492 and 0.487), whereas the
Babins system had good internal consistency (a 0.878). The three systems had good concurrent validity, showing a moderate
correlation with the MMSE and a high correlation with the MoCA. Depressive symptoms presented significant negative correlations with CDT scores. The total number of errors in the TeLPI presented moderate significant correlations with the three
scoring systems, as well as with the MMSE, MoCA, and age, and a high correlation with education (Table 2).
The analysis of the relation between the CDT and the demographic and geographical variables was initially explored
through univariate ANOVA. Different significant effects were found according to the scoring system selected: (a) men
scored higher than women according to the Rouleau system, t(628) 2.393, p .05, the Cahn system, t(628) 2.663, p
.01, and the Babins system, t(628) 2.517, p .05; (b) age had a significant effect in the Rouleau, F(2, 627) 42.582,
p .001, Cahn, F(2, 627) 40.369, p .001, and Babins scoring systems, F(2, 627) 51.458, p .001, with younger subjects scoring higher than older subjects (Table 3); (c) a significant effect was also found for education in the Rouleau, F(3,
626) 45.838, p .001, Cahn, F(3, 626) 50.224, p .001, and Babins scoring systems, F(3, 626) 64.846, p .001

Variable

Levels

Sample (n [%])

Portuguese population (n [%])

Age (years)

2549
5064
65
Women
Men
1 4
5 9
1012
.12
North
Center
Lisbon
Alentejo
Algarve
Coast
Inland
PUA
MUA
PRA

203 (32.2)
211 (33.5)
216 (34.3)
401 (63.7)
229 (36.3)
251 (39.8)
165 (26.2)
106 (16.8)
108 (17.1)
243 (38.6)
172 (27.3)
156 (24.8)
44 (7.0)
15 (2.4)
526 (83.5)
104 (16.5)
428 (67.9)
112 (17.8)
90 (14.3)

3,946 (52.6)
3,559 (47.4)
2,426 (36.6)
2,280 (34.4)
960 (14.5)
956 (14.5)
2,722 (36)
1,794 (24)
2,091 (28)
577 (8)
321 (4)
6,379 (85)
1,126 (15)
5,103 (68)
1,200 (16)
1,200 (16)

Gender
Education (years)

NUTS II

Geographical localization
Residential area

Notes: PUA predominantly urban area; MUA moderately urban area; PRA predominantly rural area. Reference values for the Portuguese population
are represented in thousands and correspond to the Portuguese population of 25 years and older living in Portugal continental areas (INE, 2010).

Table 2. Correlation coefficients of the CDT scoring systems

Education
MMSE
MoCA
GDS
TeLPI
Rouleau and colleagues
(1992)
Cahn and colleagues
(1996)
Babins and colleagues
(2008)

Age

MMSE

MoCA

GDS

TeLPI

Rouleau and colleagues


(1992)

Cahn and colleagues


(1996)

Babins and colleagues


(2008)

2.451**
2.382**
2.516**
.072, ns
.233**
2.377**

.508**

.649**
2.172**
2.434**
.453**

.661**
.649**

2.222**
2.617**
.604**

2.194**
2.172**
2.222**

.136**
2.132**

2.688**
2.434**
2.617**
.136**

2.425**

.405**
.453**
.604**
2.132**
2.425**

.421**
.453**
.606**
2.129**
2.447**
.974**

.463**
.499**
.637**
2.134**
2.442**
.951**

2.377** .453**

.606**

2.129** 2.447** .974**

.930**

2.416** .499**

.637**

2.134** 2.442** .951**

.930**

Notes: CDT Clock Drawing Test; MMSE Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA Montreal Cognitive Assessment; GDS Geriatric Depression
Scale; TeLPI Teste de Leitura de Palavras Irregulares [Irregular Word Reading Test]; ns non significant.
**p .001.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

Table 1. Demographic characterization of the study sample (n 630)

380

Table 3. Effect of demographic variables on CDT scores (post hoc analyses)


Variables

Rouleau
(M + SD)

F(3, 626) 32.959, p .001

F(3, 626) 46.605, p .001

Post hoc

Cahn
(M + SD)

A = B, C, D

8.73 + 1.940

B = C, D

7.93 + 2.316

A = B, C, D
B=D

t/F

F(3, 626) 31.434, p .001

Post hoc

Babins
(M + SD)

A = B, C, D

16.44 + 2.329

B = C, D

14.99 + 3.101

6.99 + 2.885

13.62 + 4.232

5.76 + 3.267

12.08 + 4.540

6.36 + 2.996
8.04 + 2.308
8.87 + 1.598

F(3, 626) 51.144, p .001

A = B, C, D
B = C, D

9.24 + 1.261

12.71 + 4.238
15.36 + 2.967
16.59 + 1.766

t/F

Post hoc

A = B, C, D
F(3, 626) 38.181, p .001

B = C, D

F(3, 626) 65.625, p .001

A = B, C, D
B = C, D

17.03 + 1.330

t(628) 2.330, p .05

7.51 + 2.739
8.08 + 2.454

t(628) 2.585, p .01

14.53 + 3.748
15.28 + 3.477

t(628) 2.466, p .05

F(4, 625) 2.106, p .079

7.69 + 2.712
7.73 + 2.452

F(4, 625) 1.130, p .341

14.82 + 3.568
14.49 + 3.767

F(4, 625) 2.961, p .05

C=E

14.81 + 3.721
14.77 + 3.397

t(628) 20.108, p .914

15.03 + 3.526
14.35 + 3.876
14.31 + 3.985

F(2, 627) 2.489, p .084

t(628) 20. 388, p .698

F(2, 627) 2.599, p .075

7.94 + 2.670

15.42 + 3.395

7.45 + 2.857

14.55 + 3.915

6.53 + 2.973

12.53 + 4.984

7.78 + 2.613
7.39 + 2.826

t(628) 21.369, p .172

7.90 + 2.558
7.06 + 3.014
7.66 + 2.496

F(2, 627) 4.557, p .05

A=B

Notes: CDT Clock Drawing Test; M mean; SD standard deviation; PUA predominantly urban area; MUA moderately urban area; PRA predominantly rural area.

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

Age
A: 25
9.22 + 1.373
49
B: 50
8.65 + 1.690
64
C: 65
7.89 + 2.284
74
D: 75
6.96 + 2.457
Education
A: 1 4
7.47 + 2.329
B: 5 9
8.72 + 1.709
C: 10
9.29 + 1.112
12
D: .12
9.54 + 0.877
Gender
Women
8.32 + 2.054
Men
8.71 + 1.861
Geographic region
A: North 8.48 + 1.955
B:
8.44 + 1.932
Center
C:
8.65 + 1.954
Lisbon
D:
8.30 + 2.195
Alentejo
E:
7.13 + 2.722
Algarve
Geographic location
Coast
8.48 + 2.004
Inland
8.39 + 1.943
Residence area
A: PUA
8.58 + 1.912
B: MUA 8.12 + 2.155
C: PRA
8.34 + 2.126

t/F

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

381

Table 4. Analysis of group differences in the CDT scores (with covariates controlled)
Scoring system

Variables

Covariates

ANCOVA

Effect size

Rouleau

Age

Education

F(2, 626) 17.108, p .001

Education

Age

F(3, 625) 21.714, p .001

Gender

F(1, 626) 7.668, p .01

Age

Age
Education
Education

Education

Age

F(3, 625) 25.005, p .001

Gender

F(1, 626) 9.556, p .01

Age

Age
Education
Education

Education

Age

F(3, 625) 32.505, p .001

Gender

Age
Education

F(1, 626) 9.411, p .01

Small
h2p 0.052
Small
h2p 0.094
Small
h2p 0.012
Small
h2p 0.045
Small
h2p 0.107
Small
h2p 0.015
Small
h2p 0.055
Small
h2p 0.135
Small
h2p 0.015

Cahn

Babins

F(2, 626) 14.580, p .001

F(2, 626) 18.327, p .001

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

(Table 3), with higher education resulting in higher CDT scores; (d) as for geographic variables, the geographic region had a
significant effect on the Babins scoring system, F(4, 625) 2.961, p , .05, and the residential area influenced the results in the
Cahn system, F(2, 627) 4.557, p , .05. No significant effects were found for the remaining variables (Table 3).
We performed an ANCOVA to assess if the differences found in the CDT scores due to gender and geographic variables
persisted once we had controlled the effects of age and education. The results showed that gender maintained its significant
effect across all scoring systems but with a low effect size: Rouleau, F(1, 626) 7.431, p , .01, h2p .012, Cahn, F(1,
626) 9.268, p , .01, h2p .015, and Babins, F(1, 626) 9.069, p , .01, h2p .014 (Cohen, 1988). A similar effect was
found for the residential area in the Cahn system, F(2, 622) 3.497, p .031, h2p 0.011. As for the Babins scoring
system, the results showed that there was no significant effect of geographic region when we use age and education as covariates, F(4, 623) 0.677, p .608 (Table 4).
Multiple linear regression analysis, enter method, was used to compare the effects of age and education in the CDT score
and to examine the additional contribution of significant variables such as depressive symptoms and their interaction. Gender
was excluded from this level of analysis due to its low effect sizes in the three scoring systems. We performed preliminary
analyses to check the linearity, multicolinearity and homoscedasticity assumptions. Results confirmed that education and
age had a significant contribution for the prediction of CDT scores according to the Rouleau scoring system, F(2, 627)
82.598, p .001. The same result was found for the Cahn system, F(2, 627) 87.223, p .001, and for the Babins
system, F(2, 627)112.199, p .001. Depressive symptoms did not have a significant effect on the prediction of CDT
scores on the Rouleau, b 20.034, t(614) 20.908, p .364, Cahn, b 20.024, t(614) 20.628, p .530, and
Babins, b 20.022, t(614) 20.602, p .547, scoring systems. The adjusted R 2 value was .209 for the Rouleau scoring
system, .218 for the Cahn system, and .261 for the Babins system, indicating that approximately 21%, 22%, and 26% of
the scores variance in the selected scoring systems was explained by this model (Table 5). TeLPI results were not included
in these models due to its high correlation with education, thus increasing the risk of multicolinearity. An exploratory second
model included TeLPI raw score and age as predictors. The results showed that both age and TeLPI scores have a significant
effect on the prediction of CDT scores on the Rouleau, F(2, 360) 62.202, p .001, Cahn, F(2, 360) 68.672, p .001, and
Babins, F(2, 360) 72.673, p .001, scoring systems. The adjusted R 2 value was .253 for the Rouleau scoring system, .272
for the Cahn system, and .284 for the Babins system, indicating that approximately 25%, 27%, and 28% of the scores variance
in the selected scoring systems was explained by this model (Table 6).
The multiple regression results confirmed the need to consider both age and education in the development of normative data
for the Portuguese population. The values were determined and stratified according to the distribution of each variablewe
considered the subjects education, divided into four levels (1 4 years, 5 9 years, 10 12 years, and more than 12 years)
and three age groups: 25 49, 50 64, and over 65 years; additionally, subjects with the lowest educational level (1 4
years) were divided into two age groups, 65 74 and over 75 years. This subdivision was made for the lower educational category because 63% of subjects over 65 years had achieved 1 4 years of education when compared with 42% of subjects with
50 64 years and 12% of subjects with 25 49 years. The results are expressed as the mean + SD; values below 1 SD, 1.5 SD,
and 2 SD (Tables 7 9).

382

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

Table 5. Results of multiple linear regression: influence of education and age in predicting CDT score
Scoring system

Variable

Standard error

Adjusted R 2

Rouleau and colleagues (1992)

Education
Age
Education
Age
Education
Age
Education
Age
Education
Age
Education
Age

0.121
20.032
0.176
20.049
0.171
20.042
0.243
20.065
0.260
20.063
0.371
20.100

0.017
0.005
0.016
0.005
0.022
0.007
0.021
0.006
0.030
0.009
0.028
0.009

0.288**
20.248**
0.405**
20.377**
0.307**
20.240**
0.421**
20.372**
0.337**
20.265**
0.463**
20.416**

0.209

Cahn and colleagues (1996)

Babins and colleagues (2008)

0.163
0.141
0.218
0.176
0.141
0.261
0.213
0.172

Table 6. Results of multiple linear regression: influence of age and TeLPI total number of errors in predicting CDT score
Scoring system

Variable

Standard error

Adjusted R 2

Rouleau and colleagues (1992)

TeLPI
Age
TeLPI
TeLPI
Age
TeLPI
TeLPI
Age
TeLPI

20.083
20.035
20.098
20.120
20.047
20.141
20.152
20.068
20.182

0.011
0.006
0.011
0.015
0.008
0.015
0.019
0.010
0.019

20.359**
20.284**
20.425**
20.381**
20.284**
20.447**
20.369**
20.313**
20.442**

.253

Cahn and colleagues (1996)

Babins and colleagues (2008)

.178
.272
.198
.284
.193

Notes: CDT Clock Drawing Test; TeLPI Teste de Leitura de Palavras Irregulares [Irregular Word Reading Test].
**p .001.
Table 7. Normative data for Rouleau and colleagues (1992) CDT scoring system according to age and education
Education (years)

N
1 4
SD a
N
5 9
SD a
N
1012
SD a
N
.12
SD
N
All education
SD a

Age

All ages

2549

5064

65

65 74

75

25
8.44 + 1.938
7, 6, 5
63
9.02 + 1.591
7, 7, 6
55
9.35 + 1.142
8, 8, 7
60
9.65 + 0.755
9, 9, 8
203
9.22 + 1.373
8, 7, 7

89
8.06 + 1.927
6, 5, 4
58
8.78 + 1.545
7, 7, 6
33
9.30 + 1.015
8, 8, 7
31
9.45 + 1.121
8, 8, 7
211
8.65 + 1.690
7, 6, 5

87
7.15 + 2.518
5, 3, 2

49
6.49 + 2.408
4, 3, 2

44
8.23 + 1.987
6, 5, 4
18
9.11 + 1.231
8, 7, 7
18
9.33 + 0.767
9, 8, 8
216
7.56 + 2.381
5, 4, 3

250
7.47 + 2.329
5, 4, 3
165
8.72 + 1.709
7, 6, 5
106
9.29 + 1.112
8, 8, 7
109
9.54 + 0.877
9, 8, 8
630
8.46 + 1.993
7, 6, 5

Notes: CDT Clock Drawing Test; SD standard deviation.


CDT values below 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD.

Discussion
This study intended to explore the potentialities of the CDT in a broad spectrum of adulthood aging strata and to analyze the
effect of sociodemographic variables in the CDT performance of Portuguese cognitively healthy subjects, recruited in the

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

Note: CDT Clock Drawing Test.


**p .001.

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

383

Table 8. Normative data for Cahn and colleagues (1996) CDT scoring system according to age and education
Education (years)

All ages

2549

50 64

65

65 74

75

25
7.48 + 2.568
5, 4, 2
63
8.40 + 2.211
6, 5, 4
55
8.96 + 1.633
7, 7, 6
60
9.38 + 1.166
8, 8, 7
203
8.73 + 1.940
7, 6, 5

89
7.04 + 2.615
4, 3, 2
58
8.10 + 2.023
6, 5, 4
33
8.82 + 1.570
7, 7, 6
31
9.19 + 1.470
, 7, 6
211
7.93 + 2.316
6, 5, 3

87
6.06 + 3.074
3, 2, 21

49
5.08 + 3.233
2, 0, 21

44
7.43 + 2.697
5, 3, 2
18
8.67 + 1.609
7, 6, 6
18
8.83 + 1.150
8, 7, 7
216
6.56 + 3.072
4, 2, 0

250
6.36 + 2.996
3, 2, 0
165
8.04 + 2.308
7, 6, 5
106
8.87 + 1.598
7, 7, 6
109
9.24 + 1.261
8, 7, 7
630
7.72 + 2.651
5, 4, 2

Notes: CDT Clock Drawing Test; SD standard deviation.


a
CDT values below 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD.

community. Our decision to select the 25 years as the lower age limit was based in the literature, as it is considered by several
authors the beginning of adulthood and the age at which we supposedly reach the full development of cognitive abilities (as
measured by Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales). This study population was community-based because it was developed with
Portuguese habitants who resided in the community, had their lives completely integrated in the community, and were autonomous in their activities of daily living. As we referred, we started by recruiting the potential test subjects in national healthcare
services and, in order to accomplish a rigorous selection, we previously established an agreement with the directors of the
healthcare centers involved and all the physicians were instructed to select patients who fulfilled the inclusion/exclusion criteria. All the criteria were again revised by the neuropsychologists and, when it was possible, all information, including
medical history, was revised with a caregiver (spouse, son, or other relative in cohabitation) in order to assure that the
person was indeed completely autonomous, especially for the older subjects. Portugal has a high rate of older population
and a high percentage of that population lives alone. Therefore, there are many older people who consciously choose to
benefit from daycare centers facilities, despite being autonomous. A special attention was given to subjects recruited in
such centers as we only selected autonomous subjects who spent only part of the day in the institution developing social activities. Once again, all inclusion/exclusion criteria were respected and confirmed by the daycare centers directors to assure the
inclusion of only cognitively healthy subjects. We also used a rigorous methodology in order to exclude participants with comorbidities that could influence cognitive performance as well subjects with objective cognitive impairment on the assessment
battery. This explains the high rate of inclusion failures: of the 936 total subjects, 194 (20.73%) were excluded following the
initial interview due mostly to the personal history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and 112 participants (+12%) were
further excluded due to impairment or inability to complete the neuropsychological tests. Despite this high rate of initial exclusions, we consider that the process of recruitment did not influence the representativeness of the final sample or the interpretation of normative data. First, because the global recruitment was always guided by previous stratification, and second, the
process was maintained until we obtained a representative sample of the Portuguese population according to all the selected
variables (age, education, gender, geographic region, geographic localization, and inland/coastal areas).
Regardless of the absence of normative data for the CDT in our country, the test is very popular in Portugal and the three
scoring systems studied are commonly used, justifying an adequate exploration of their psychometric properties and the development of Portuguese normative data for all of them. These scoring systems presented different values of internal consistency:
Rouleau and Cahn systems showed inadequate internal consistency, whereas the Babins system showed good internal consistency. Although it is not the original purpose of the CDT, the scoring systems presented high correlations with global measures
of cognition, which supported its potential as a cognitive screening instrument (Strauss et al., 2006) and moderate correlations
with the Irregular Word Reading Test, which has been found in several studies with the CDT (e.g., Crowe, Allman, Triebel,
Sawyer, & Martin, 2010; Hubbard et al., 2008).
The Babins and colleagues (2008) 18-point scoring system was the one that revealed better psychometric properties in all
parameters evaluated among the selected systems. There are no other available studies in which the Babins scoring system was

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

N
1 4
SD a
N
5 9
SD a
N
1012
SD a
N
.12
SD
N
All
education
SD a

Age

384

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

Table 9. Normative data for Babins and colleagues (2008) CDT scoring system according to age and education
Education (years)

N
1 4 SD a
N 5 9 SD a

All ages

2549

50 64

65

65 74

75

25
14.88 + 3.370
12, 10, 8
63
15.92 + 2.842 13,
12, 10
55
16.91 + 1.444
16, 15, 14
60
17.22 + 1.195
16, 15, 15
203
16.44 + 2.329
14, 13, 12

89
13.57 + 3.570
10, 8, 6
58
15.41 + 2.471
13, 12, 11
33
16.33 + 1.848
15, 14, 13
31
16.84 + 1.695
15, 14, 14
211
14.99 + 3.101
12, 10, 9

87
12.10 + 4.588
8, 5, 3

49
11.12 + 4.395
7, 5, 2

44
14.48 + 3.547
11, 9, 7
18
16.11 + 2.349
14, 13, 11
18
16.72 + 0.958
16, 15, 15
216
13.08 + 4.393
9, 7, 4

250
12.71 + 4.238
9, 6, 4
165
15.36 + 2.967
12, 11, 9
106
16.59 + 1.766
15, 14, 13
109
17.03 + 1.330
16, 15, 14
630
14.80 + 3.667
11, 10, 8

Notes: CDT Clock Drawing Test; SD standard deviation.


CDT values below 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD.

used with cognitively healthy subjects, a fact that limits the corroboration of our data. Nonetheless, our results are a new argument in favor of the use of this scoring system, which revealed a high specificity and sensitivity in differentiating AD patients
from cognitively normal subjects as well as in the identification of MCI subjects who developed dementia. As referred, this
system takes into account more details than the Rouleau system, including the assessment of circle integrity, number placement
and sequencing, size and placement of the hands. According to the authors, the time representation task, which encompasses
three main actionscorrect placement of both hands, correct representation of their asymmetry, and correct orientation of the
hours hand, proved to be the most discriminative task among the four analyzed groups. In fact, it was able to differentiate MCI
subgroups (progressors vs. non-progressors) which could be a good indicator of future cognitive decline. The discriminative
capacity of the CDT in MCI patients remains controversial (Ehreke, Luppa, Konig, & Riedel-Heller, 2010); nonetheless, comparatively with the Cahn and colleagues (1996) scoring system, the Babins system appeared to be more informative and
revealed higher discriminative capacity between MCI patients who progressed to dementia and those who remained stable
(Babins et al., 2008; Duro, Freitas, Alves, Simoes, & Santana, 2011; Freitas & Simoes, 2010).
The use of a representative sample, stratified according to several levels of each demographic and geographic variable and
with a very close distribution of the Portuguese population, allowed us to take more robust conclusions from the obtained data.
Education and age were the variables that contributed more significantly to predict CDT scores according to the Babins and
colleagues (2008) scoring system, explaining 26% of the total variance. According to Cohen (1988), this is considered a
medium effect. The other scoring systems revealed proximate results, with age and education explaining 21% of the total variance for the Rouleau and colleagues (1992) system and 22% for the Cahn and colleagues (1996) system.
There was a linear positive relation between the CDT total score and the total number of years of education completed successfully. On the other hand, there was a linear negative relation with age. The influence of age has been systematically found
in several studies published with the CDT (Bozikas et al., 2008; Caffarra et al., 2011; Hubbard et al., 2008), confirming a negative effect on the subjects performance. The influence of education has been less explored but appears to be equally relevant,
presenting an inverse pattern to a highest education corresponds a better performance (Bozikas et al., 2008; Hubbard et al.,
2008; Lourenco et al., 2008). In the present study, the results of multiple linear regression supported the need to considerate
both age and education for the establishment of normative data. These results can be related to the existence of an important
segment of the study sample with a low educational level, typical of the Portuguese population. The effect of gender on the
CDT performance, although described in the literature (Strauss et al., 2006), has not been explored in recent studies
(Bozikas et al., 2008; Caffarra et al., 2011). In the present study, gender had a significant effect, with men scoring higher
than women. Nonetheless, this was considered a minor effect (Cohen, 1988) and it justified its non-inclusion as a criterion
for the establishment of normative data for the Portuguese population. The analysis of TeLPI results showed that, together
with age, it significantly explained CDT scores in the Portuguese population: the model explained approximately 25%,
27%, and 28% of the total variance for the Rouleau, Cahn, and Babins systems, respectively. It will be interesting to
compare these same results in future studies, especially with well-defined clinical samples. There are very few Portuguese
studies that explored the influence of geographic variables in cognitive screening tests. Recently, Freitas and colleagues

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

N
1012
SD a
N
.12
SD
N
All education
SD a

Age

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

385

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

(2011) concluded that geographic region (NUTS-II classification: North, Center, Lisbon, Alentejo, and Algarve), geographical
localization (coastal and inland areas), and residential area (according to the Types of Urban Areas: PUA, MUA, and PRA)
have a non-significant or reduced effect on MoCA scores after controlling for the effect of age and education (Freitas
et al., 2011). In the present study, the variables considered, namely geographic region, geographical localization, and residential area, did not show a significant influence in the CDT score according to the three scoring systems selected once we controlled the effects of age and education. Considering the clinical variables, depressive symptoms presented significant and
negative correlations with CDT scores; nonetheless, according to the results of multiple linear regression analysis, there
was no significant effect of depressive symptoms in our sample. However, these data should be interpreted with caution as
they result from the analysis of performance of cognitively healthy and non-depressed to mildly depressed individuals.
Therefore, these findings should not be generalized to individuals with clinical conditions.
To establish normative data, we considered the subjects education, divided into four levels, and three age groups: 25 49,
50 64, and over 65 years. Additionally, subjects with the lowest educational level (1 4 years) were subdivided into two age
groups, 65 74 and over 75 years. We presented the values for scores less than 1 SD, 1.5 SD, and 2 SD. These values found
cannot be corroborated, since there are no other Portuguese studies that established normative data for the three CDT scoring
systems used. Moreover, there are no publications of international studies comparing all the three systems. Regarding the Cahn
and colleagues (1996) scoring system, other authors have demonstrated it to be a valid and reliable measure in several normative studies, where normative data were also established based on age and education (Hubbard et al., 2008). The Hubbard and
colleagues (2008) study developed normative data for the Cahn and colleagues (1996) scoring system in a sample of 207 cognitively normal elderly aged 55 98. The results found on our study are relatively close to the ones found in the white sample of
subjects when they accounted for both age and education in the development of norms. The Babins and colleagues (2008) study
presented results for the Rouleau and colleagues (1992) 10-point system and their 18-point system. Our results for the older,
highly educated subjects are slightly lower than the ones presented for the normal group; nevertheless, differences in sample
size, age ranges, and mean education limit the comparison of such data. The Yamamoto and colleagues (2004) study also compared the Rouleau and Cahn scoring systems and determined seven points as the optimal cutoff score for detecting MCI
patients with the Cahn system. Our findings suggest that there is a great variability in CDT scores across different populations
as many of our normal subjects would be misclassified as cognitively impaired using previously published cutoffs.
Several studies have shown a strong association between reading ability and cognitive test performance that has led other
authors to develop normative data for the CDT based on the reading ability scores (Crowe et al., 2010; Hubbard et al., 2008).
Although we found a strong association between TeLPI raw scores and CDT scores, in this study, normative data were based on
age and education because the normative study of TeLPI for the Portuguese population is not yet available.
This study has some limitations. Although every effort was made to select community-based cognitively healthy subjects,
the test battery selected for this study enabled us to rule out cases of moderate to severe dementia but it is possible that very
mild dementia cases as well as MCI cases were included in the normative sample due to lack of sensitivity of the instruments.
The use of MoCAs Portuguese cutoff scores allows us to state with a certain degree of confidence that the sample did not
include subjects with cognitive impairment; nonetheless, that possibility cannot be completely discarded.
The inclusion of illiterate subjects was not considered, which limits the application of the CDT with this segment of the
population. The decision was supported by the literature, given the strong possibility of floor effects. The low performance
of illiterate subjects appears to be related to a low exposure to bi-dimensional representations and abstract graphic representation (Howieson, Loring, & Hannay, 2004); also, illiterate subjects tend to have poorer performances in several cognitive
domains, particularly in tests that require pen or pencil to make drawings. The Portuguese population was characterized by
a high prevalence of older people with low educational level or illiterates, whether young people had a mean level of education.
As an illustrative example, 30 years ago, according to the 1981 Census (Instituto Nacional de Estatstica, 2010), the percentage
of illiteracy in Portugal was close to 26.4% and only 1.6% of the population held a university degree. In the last decades, the
educational scenery has rapidly changed as a result of the reorganization of the school system and the imposition of higher
obligatory educational plateaus. These changes are already reflected in the younger strata of the population studied;
however, the older group continues to be characterized by a very low education level. The sample distribution could not completely eliminate this discrepancy; nonetheless, the distribution achieved is relatively close to the real one. We were also not
able to subdivide the over 65 years group in the four educational levels as there were not enough subjects in each group. Such
division was performed only for the lowest educational category (1 4 years) but our results showed that there were no significant differences between the two subgroups (65 74 and 75 years).
We intend to further explore the data regarding the Irregular Word Reading Test as several papers have shown a significant
effect of reading ability in clock drawing performance in different countries (e.g., Crowe et al., 2010; Hubbard et al., 2008).
Additionally, the findings about depressive symptoms would benefit from better operationalization of such variables. The inclusion of more specific and descriptive instruments may shed greater light on the influence of the variables on CDT

386

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

performance. Furthermore, these findings must be complemented with studies that consider patients with cognitive impairment
and those with depression.
These normative results will be useful in population or epidemiological studies to be developed in Portugal, allowing the use
of the CDT as a brief cognitive screening test. Furthermore, the confirmed influence of education and age will be relevant for
the future application of this scoring system to clinical groups with cognitive deterioration.
Funding
This work was supported by the Lundbeck Foundation.
Conflict of Interest
None declared.

We thank Ana Pedroso for the English revision of our manuscript.


References
Alves, L., Simoes, M. R., & Martins, C. (2009). Teste de Leitura de Palavras Irregulares (TeLPI) [Irregular Word Reading Test]. Coimbra, Portugal: Servico
de Avaliacao Psicologica da Faculdade de Psicologia e de Ciencias da Educacao da Universidade de Coimbra [Psychological Assessment Department,
Faculty of Psychological and Educational Sciences].
Babins, L., Slater, M-E., Whitehead, V., & Chertkow, H. (2008). Can an 18-point clock-drawing scoring system predict dementia in elderly individuals with
mild cognitive impairment? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30 (2), 173186.
Barreto, J., Leuschner, A., Santos, F., & Sobral, M. (2008). Escala de Depressao Geriatrica [Geriatric Depression Scale]. In Grupo de Estudos de
Envelhecimento Cerebral e Demencias [Study Group on Brain Aging and Dementia] (Eds.), Escalas e testes na demencia [Scales and tests in dementia]
(pp. 6972). Lisboa, Portugal: GEECD.
Bozikas, V. P., Giazkoulidou, A., Hatzigeorgiadou, M., Karavatos, A., & Kosmidis, M. H. (2008). Do age and education contribute to performance on the clock
drawing test? Normative data for the Greek population. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 30 (2), 199 203.
Brodaty, H., & Moore, C. M. (1997). The Clock Drawing Test for dementia of the Alzheimers type: A comparison of three scoring methods in a memory
disorders clinic. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12, 619627.
Caffarra, P., Gardini, S., Zonato, F., Concari, L., Dieci, F., Copelli, S., et al. (2011). Italian norms for the Freedman version of the Clock Drawing Test. Journal
of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33 (9), 982988.
Cahn, D. A., Salmon, D. P., Monsch, A. U., Butters, N., Wiederholt, W. C., & Corey-Bloom, J. (1996). Screening for dementia of Alzheimer type in the
community: The utility of the Clock Drawing Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 2, 529 539.
Cahn-Weiner, D. A., Williams, K., Grace, J., Tremont, G., Westervelt, H., & Stern, R. A. (2003). Discrimination of dementia with Lewy bodies from Alzheimer
disease and Parkinson disease using the clock drawing test. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 16 (2), 85 92.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Cosentino, S., Jefferson, A., Chute, D., Kaplan, E., & Libon, D. J. (2004). Clock drawing errors in dementia: Neuropsychological and neuroanatomical
considerations. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 17 (2), 74 84.
Crowe, M., Allman, R. M., Triebel, K., Sawyer, P., & Martin, R. C. (2010). Normative performance on an executive clock drawing task (CLOX) in a
community-dwelling sample of older adults. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 25, 610617.
Duro, D., Freitas, S., Alves, L., Simoes, M. R., & Santana, I. (2011). O Teste do Desenho do Relogio na populacao portuguesa: Dados preliminares [The Clock
Drawing Test in the Portuguese population: Preliminary data]. Sinapse, 11 (2), 67.
Ehreke, L., Luppa, M., Konig, H-H., & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (2010). Is the Clock Drawing Test a screening tool for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment?
A systematic review. International Psychogeriatrics, 22 (1), 56 63.
Fisher, J. S., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Construction. In M. D. Lezak, D. Howieson, & D. W. Loring (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed., pp. 531
568). New York: Oxford University Press.
Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of
Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.
Freedman, M., Leach, L., Kaplan, E., Winocur, G., Shulman, K. I., & Delis, D. C. (1994). Clock drawing: A neuropsychological analysis (Vol. 6). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Freitas, S., & Simoes, M. R. (2010). Teste do Desenho do Relogio: Utilidade e validade como instrumento de rastreio cognitivo [Clock Drawing Test: Utility
and validity as a cognitive screening instrument]. Psicologia, Educacao e Cultura [Psychology, Education and Culture], XIV (2), 319 338.
Freitas, S., Simoes, M. R., Alves, L., & Santana, I. (2011). Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Normative study for the Portuguese population. Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33 (9), 989 996. doi:10.1080/13803395.2011.589374.
Garcia-Caballero, A., Recimil, M. J., Darca-Lado, I., Gayoso, P., Cardaso-Suarez, C., Gonzalez-Hermida, J., et al. (2006). ACE clock scoring: A comparison
with eight standard correction methods in a population of low educational level. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 19, 216219.

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

Acknowledgements

I. Santana et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 28 (2013) 375387

387

Downloaded from http://acn.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Cambridge on October 9, 2014

Garrett, C., Santos, F., Tracana, I., Barreto, J., Sobral, M., & Fonseca, R. (2008). Avaliacao Clnica da Demencia [Clinical Dementia Rating]. In Grupo de
Estudos de Envelhecimento Cerebral e Demencias [Study Group on Brain Aging and Dementia] (Eds.), Escalas e Testes na Demencia [Scales and
tests in dementia] (pp. 17 32). Lisboa, Portugal: GEECD.
Gino, S., Mendes, T., Ribeiro, F., Mendonca, A., Guerreiro, M., & Garcia, C. (2008). Escala de Queixas de Memoria. In Grupo de Estudos de Envelhecimento
Cerebral e Demencias [Study Group on Brain Aging and Dementia] (Eds.), Escalas e testes na demencia [Scales and tests in dementia] (pp. 117 120).
Lisboa, Portugal: GEECD.
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.
Guerreiro, M. (1998). Contributo da neuropsicologia para o estudo das demencias [Contribution of Neuropsychology for dementia study]. Unpublished
Doctorship Thesis, Lisbon, Portugal Lisbon University.
Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., & Hannay, J. (2004). Neurobehavioral variables and diagnostic issues. In M. D. Lezak, D. B. Howieson, & D. W. Loring
(Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed., pp. 286 334). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hubbard, E. J., Santini, V., Blankevoort, C. G., Volkers, K. M., Barruo, M. S., Byerly, L., et al. (2008). Clock drawing performance in cognitively normal
elderly. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 295327.
Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., & Martin, R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. British Journal of Psychiatry,
140, 566572.
Instituto Nacional de Estatstica (2010). Portal de Estatsticas Oficiais [Official Statistics Portal]. Retrieved June 22, 2010, from http://www.ine.pt.
Kaplan, E. (1988). A process approach to neuropsychological assessment. In T. Bull, & B. K. Bryant (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology and brain function:
Research, measurement, and practice (pp. 129167). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Libon, D. J., Malamut, B. L., Swenson, R., Prouty Sands, L., & Cloud, B. S. (1996). Further analyses of clock drawings among demented and nondemented
older subjects. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11 (3), 193 205.
Lourenco, R., Ribeiro-Filho, S., Moreira, I., Paradela, E., & Miranda, A. (2008). The Clock Drawing Test: performance among elderly with low educational
level. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 30 (4), 309 315.
Lowery, N., Giovanni, L., Mozley, L. H., Arnold, S. E., Bilker, W. B., Gur, R. E., et al. (2003). Relationship between Clock-Drawing and neuropsychological
and functional status in elderly institutionalized patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11 (6), 621 628.
Mendez, M., Ala, T., & Underwood, K. (1992). Development of scoring criteria for the clock drawing task in Alzheimers disease. Journal of the American
Geriatrics Society, 40, 10951099.
Menon, C., Hall, J., Hobson, V., Johnson, L., & OBryant, S. E. (2011). Normative performance on the executive clock drawing task in a multi-ethnic bilingual
cohort: A project FRONTIER study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. doi:10.1002/gps.2810.
Meyers, L. S., Gamst, G., & Guarino, A. J. (2006). Applied multivariate research: Design and interpretation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Nasreddine, Z., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief
screening tool for Mild Cognitive Impairment. American Geriatrics Society, 53 (4), 695 699.
Parsey, C. M., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2011). Quantitative and qualitative analyses of the Clock Drawing Test in Mild Cognitive Impairment and
Alzheimer Disease: Evaluation of a modified scoring system. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 24 (2), 108 118.
Petersen, R. C., Stevens, J. C., Ganguli, M., Tangalos, E. G., Cummings, J. L., & DeKosky, S. T. (2001). Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild
cognitive impairment (an evidence-based review). Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology, 56
(9), 1133 1142.
Rouleau, I., Salmon, D. P., Butters, N., Kennedy, C., & McGuire, K. (1992). Quantitative and qualitative analyses of clock drawings task. Journal of Neurology,
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 64, 588 594.
Schmand, B., Jonker, C., Hooijer, C., & Lindeboom, J. (1996). Subjective memory complaints may announce dementia. Neurology, 46 (1), 121 125.
Shulman, K. (2000). Clock-drawing: Is it the ideal cognitive screening test? International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15, 548 561.
Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Sunderland, T., Hill, J. L., Mellow, A. M., Lawlor, B. A., Gundersheimer, J., Newhouse, P. A., et al. (1989). Clock drawing in Alzheimers disease: A novel
measure of dementia severity. Journal of the American Geriatric Society, 37, 725 729.
Yamamoto, S., Mogi, N., Umegaki, H., Suzuki, Y., Ando, F., Shimokata, H., et al. (2004). The Clock Drawing Test as a valid screening method for mild
cognitive impairment. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 18, 172 179.
Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A
preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17 (1), 37 49.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen