Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

7

What is the role of observational drawing in


contemporary art & design curricula?

Michelle Fava

Introduction
Abstract

How can contemporary theoretical insights


This paper makes a case for the valuing
about the nature of drawing inform a
of cognitive and practical function of
discussion on curriculum design and
observational drawing in art and design
assessment?
curricula. Drawing skill is often regarded
as tacit in nature, and therefore can be
A decline in the prevalence of drawing in art
problematic in regard to contemporary 4
& design curricula has been noted , but the
assessment practices. The paper
reasons for this are unclear. Certainly,
identifies a range of cognitive skills
drawing for conventional purposes is no
inherent in the activity of drawing, arguing
longer indispensable, but maybe this shift
that a thorough consideration of drawing
can allow us to examine more subtle
and cognition makes possible a more
aspects of drawing skill. Such an
rigorous approach to assessing drawing
examination would be valuable in an
within existing assessment frameworks. It
educational climate that required being
also calls for a review of assessment
explicit about learning outcomes. Recent
practices and frameworks, highlighting
empirical findings and new theoretical
the need for a more holistic approach to
assessment to be based on 4
Concerns about a general decline in drawing
contemporary theoretical insights about ability have been expressed; Alsop (2002); Rose,
the nature of learning and cognition. Jolley & Burkitt (2006); Jolley (2009).
Observational drawing has been reported as not
really part of many/most curricula across the UK
anymore (Leo Duff 2010, personal
communication). A more general trend has also
been noted, whereby practical, situated and
embodied knowledge, once so central to the
study of art and design in Higher Education, has
since the late 1960s become more peripheral
(McGuirck 2008).

129
Fava

insights in cognitive psychology are Here we will consider how a contemporary


allowing an enhanced understanding of understanding of cognition might help
drawing, which might be useful in address drawing more directly in
informing and making explicit the way we assessment practices, but the limitations of
conceive of drawing. such practices will also be considered.

This paper invites opinion and discussion Practitioners views on the purpose of
about the place of observational drawing observational drawing
skills in contemporary art & design
curricula in further and higher education. Jane Tormey (1997) observed, in all the
In what ways is it still valuable to recent debate there seemed little articulation
students, how does it fit with of how drawing methods may be relevant, or
contemporary notions of graphicacy, and to the nature of activity and why it was
how should this be reflected in curriculum important. This paper asks what is it about
design and assessment practices? observational drawing that can be
considered of continuing value to art &
Personal communications with drawing design students?
practitioners will be used to illustrate
commonly held beliefs about Debates exist around the place of manual
observational drawing and the wider design drawing after the advent of drawing
5 6
abilities it enables . Cognitive and software , and around the relevance of
second generation embodied-cognitive representational drawing in contemporary
perspectives on learning will then be fine art practice. Aside from these ongoing,
considered and I will make some changeable debates lays a broader
tentative proposals about an expanded assumption that observational drawing
definition of drawing skill. enables a range of transferable skills.

One reason (among many) for the In a review conducted by Tormey,


perceived decline of observational Foundation Art and Design tutors most
drawing might be a clash between a frequently valued drawing as, underpinning
practice which is largely tacit, embodied, and essential, the means of analytical
non-verbal and growing institutional learning, the means to improve
imperatives to make learning explicit in perceptions, visual awareness and manual
the form of outcomes, level descriptors skills (1997).
and assessment criteria. It is important
that such requirements do not serve to It was suggested that drawing is the
marginalise tacit skills like drawing, which ultimate transferable skill and
risks relegation to a supporting role in encourages the ability to adapt. It
other learning activities, if it cannot provides the progression from research,
compete with more explicitly measurable through analysis and speculation to
skills. solution. It progresses visual thinking
(1997).

The debate:
5
The author would like to thank Leo Duff,
Rachel Pearcey, Eduardo Corte Real, Lisa
6
Moriarty, Doris Rohr and Richard Hare for For a detailed analysis of the role of digital and
their contributions and insights in response to manual drawing in design see Garner (1999),
questions sent through the DRN mailing list. Johnson (2005), Prats et. al.(2009).
Observational drawing

illustrated [] the need to balance Existing assessment practices


activity which is designed to provide
real learning through the process of The Quality Assurance Agency benchmark
drawing, with the kind of drawing statement for Art & Design (2008) does not
activity that is likely to generate more mention specific outcomes. These date
obvious and visible skill acquisition quickly and are left to the discretion of the
because the methods are institution. However, a wide range of generic
recognisable . and transferable skills are cited, including a
(1997, emphasis in original) requirement for particular cognitive
attributes (2008: 3). Drawing skill is
However, It is unclear what exactly is regarded as comprising the full spectrum of
meant by real learning through the cognitive activity: a prerequisite skill for
process, or what exactly is (or is observation, recording, analysis,
assumed to be) the nature of the visual speculation, development, visualisation,
thinking enabled by observational evaluation and communication (2008:5).
drawing. There is no indication of where
observational drawing is assumed to fit with
Drawing practitioners describe the this description, but these guidelines
purpose of observational drawing in many indicate general descriptors that might be
ways: to understand the engineering used to assess drawing. QAA (2006) codes
structure and texture of objects, human of practice require explicit verbalisation of
7
beings, environments (Rohr 2010, learning outcomes . This is regarded as
personal correspondence). It sharpens restrictive by some as there are limits to
observational skills generally (Hare verbalising creative learning (Dentith 2002,
2010, personal correspondence). Orr 2010, Gordon 2004), but guidelines are
[S]tudents need procedures of inquiry open ended enough that institutions can
that help them to understand/criticise the devise learning outcomes as they see fit.
existent(Corte-Real, 2010, personal
correspondence). The situation in FE is somewhat different as
unit outlines are written by external
8
Echoing Ruskins (1991[1887]) dictum awarding bodies , Edexcel being one of the
that to learn to draw is to learn to see,
Leo Duff describes the purpose of
7
observational drawing as to look, to see, These stipulate that Institutions have
transparent and fair mechanisms for marking and
to focus, to concentrate, to sustain your for moderating marks (QAA 2006: 31) and that
concentration. In her opinion, these be written and made available to the
observational skill enables decision student. Modules are expected to have clear
learning outcomes, assessment criteria based on
making, clarity of vision (2010, personal these outcomes, and descriptors which indicate
communication). how well the assessment criteria have been met
(QAA 2006: 39). Furthermore, institutions must
evaluate the extent to which assessment tasks
A consideration of learning and cognition and associated criteria are effective in measuring
can demystify these beliefs, showing that student achievement of the intended learning
real learning can be visible in observed outcomes of modules and programmes (QAA
2006: 8).
drawings. Furthermore, a consideration of 8
Another factor here is that FE funding
the relationship between perception and imperatives push achievement, and therefore
assessment criteria, to the forefront of
cognition can inform approaches to
institutional strategies and teaching methods,
teaching and assessment. perhaps sometimes unhealthily so.

131
Fava

9
largest for vocational courses . It can be descriptors are expressed for drawing.
said that their unit specifications do not There is also room to review the role and
reflect the wide range of purposes nature of observational drawing, both within
drawing is ascribed by the practitioners art and design practice and more generally
above. Of the 137 BTEC National as part of a set of transferable skills.
Diploma units Edexcel currently accredit,
none mention drawing in their Observational drawing and assessing
assessment criteria. Until 2006 there was cognitive skills
a unit named Drawing Development,
since renamed Visual Recording. Many point to purposes of drawing beyond
Standard drawing texts are suggested in making a representation, but what wider
this units reading list, but drawing is not abilities can we reliably attach to
necessary to pass it. To gain a distinction observational skill, and how should this
in this unit a student must be able to: affect the way we conceive and assess
students drawings? Which of the cognitive
demonstrate independence, skills described by QAA can be developed
innovation and individuality in through observational practice?
evaluating and using sources,
integrating visual recording skills In personal communications practitioners
and in-depth understanding in responded to the questions: What end does
10
communicating information. this practice serve? What abilities does it
(Edexcel 2009: 4) enable? Observational drawing practice
was said to enable: decision making, clarity
While this criterion refers to valuable of vision (Duff, 2010), [Ability] to analyse
skills, it does not refer directly to drawing. visual information through visual rather than
The situation in First, Foundation and textual means, Visual analytical thinking in
Higher National Diplomas is similar. The short, being able to synthesise data visually
act of recording visually (be it drawing, (Rohr, 2010) and The ability to
photography or otherwise) is relegated to communicate ideas visually, directly through
only one of four pass criteria, implying drawing and indirectly through transference
that implicit in this wording is the of skills learned through drawing to other
assumption that to draw is simply to forms of visual expression (Hare, 2010).
describe and that as such, it accounts for
only lower order skills of recognising and Many have likewise described observational
describing. drawing as a means of visual thinking and
analytical learning. David Haley maintains
It is clear there is more scope to consider that drawing is integral to perception and
the way that learning outcomes and level cognitive understanding (2010). Hudson
renamed the activity of observation as
Construction, describing:
9
A Levels are awarded by AQA, WJEC (in
Wales), CCEA, OCR (run by University of
Cambridge), or ISESB (for Independent a piece by piece assembling of
Schools). Edexcel are the only awarding body awareness, [which] was precisely the
to be run by a profit making company, after
way in which children learned by trial,
having been taken over in 2003 by Pearson
PLC. error, and the storing of resulting
10
It is unclear what exactly is meant by experience. He saw it therefore as the
descriptors like independence, innovation,
catalyst of creative education pre-
individuality or in-depth, at this level of
learning. existing, assisting other matters
"""""""""""""""""""""""""*Vjkuvngyqqf"3;:3<"46+"

132
Observational drawing

What is common from these opinions is symmetrical trace lines to balance a figure
that the range of cognitive skills drawing, or the use of anatomical or
associated with observational drawing structural knowledge to proportion or
include not only lower order cognitive construct a figure, building or machine.
skills, but the full range of educational Analysis can be seen by the activity of
objectives in the cognitive domain (Bloom questioning what is seen marks which
1956): observation, communication, refer to not only the visible but to structural
transference of skills, speculation and or relational properties can provide evidence
visual analysis, decision making, of analytical thinking. Likewise, there might
editorial skills, synthesis and solution. be evidence that a student has considered
the distinction between what they know and
A drawing might be assessed, therefore, what they see in their correction of
in terms of the cognitive sophistication mistakes or acknowledgement of
with which it was made, rather than its subjective distortions. Evaluation will be
representational accuracy, conformity to evident in traces of re-working, changes in
convention or even its contribution to a composition or measurement. The synthesis
further purpose, such as gathering visual of visual knowledge would allow
information for a design project (this sophisticated decision making, which might
could be a learning outcome better suited be evidenced by a novel choice of line to
to a design brief). Furthermore, I propose describe a form or texture, or by an
that traces of this activity can be visible in informed, perhaps unconventional,
13
the drawing itself and assessment need compositional decision . It is perhaps the
not rely on extensive annotation to as creativity invested in the drawing which
11 14
evidence . Table 1 suggests ways in gives rise to the wow factor .
which cognitive skills might be recognised
in an observed drawing. In this way, the range of cognitive objectives
can be evident in a single observed
15
We might find evidence of close drawing . A students engagement with this
observation and understanding in a drawing process can be made more explicit,
drawings correctness, alongside and sufficiently sophisticated objectives and
empathy with the subject, and depiction descriptors can be devised without the
12
of it in a personally engaged manner necessity for additional tasks to satisfy a full
16
(Duff 2010, personal correspondence). range of level descriptors .
Evidence of application might be seen in
the employment of analytical visual
knowledge, for example, the use of
13
These are intended only as examples. There
are surely as many ways of identifying cognitive
skills in a drawing as there are in a piece of
writing.
14
See Gordon (2004) for a discussion of the
11
Annotation can be useful for reflection when wow factor in art & design assessment.
15
appropriate, but it evidences a students ability Affective and psychomotor domains of skill can
to verbalise and post-rationalise their learning, also be regarded as important for observational
rather than the learning itself, and therefore is drawing. Space does not permit a full discussion
not a reliable form of evidence for of this here, but comments about how these
assessment. considerations might be included in curriculum
12
Leo Duff describes this as the main criteria design and assessment are also welcome.
16
with which she assesses the quality of Howard Rileys matrix of systems of choices
students observed drawings, communicating (2008) is also useful in defining and
this both in writing and in one-to-one and communicating learning objectives, it plots levels
group discussions (personal communication). of engagement against functions of drawing.

133
Fava

Table 1. Drawing activities corresponding to Blooms (1956) taxonomy of educational objectives.

Transferable skills between perception and cognition in their


18
recent research into artists perceptual
If cognitive skills are employed in advantages. They provide evidence that
observational drawing, can we assume perceptual skills confer an advantage in
that observational skill acquisition leads visual analysis enabling attentional
to any collateral, transferable benefits? strategies that enhance the perceptual
Any causal relation would be hard to encoding of stimulus features diagnostic for
measure as any enhancement of overall the identity of objects and inhibit the
graphicacy or visual cognitive ability will perception of potential distractors (2008:
most likely be the result of combined 153). This is, perhaps, another way
activities. observational skills can be considered in
relation to graphicacy.
Seely & Kozbelt use psychometric
17
testing to demonstrate a relationship
an image, they are also better judges, more
sensitive to the skill employed in the construction
17
Although their most recent work (Kozbelt et. of an image, suggesting necessarily tacit
al. 2010) relies on artists to assess the elements to assessing visual skills.
18
success of image making tasks, and shows They refer specifically to artists who have
that artists are not only better at constructing representational drawing or painting skill.

134
Observational drawing

QAA (2008) list transferable skills that sensory understanding and cognitive
associated with art and design, but what thought are inseparable, in that they
is implied above is that the abilities develop in tandem and make use of the
enabled by drawing are even more same neural pathways. For example, Usha
fundamental and have to do with ones Goswami describes how our knowledge
ability to concentrate, to attend, to representation is rooted in attention to the
consider, to observe, to comprehend. If perceptual structure of objects and events
practiced regularly, visual cognition can (2008: 51). He also describes how the
become embedded in ones thinking, in foundation of our cognitive framework for
much the same way that a habitual exploring and representing the behaviour of
practice of writing would establish the objects is shared with that of the behaviour
propensity to analyse the world of people:
19
linguistically .
the attribution of belief and desires
Two further senses in which seems to develop from the same
observational drawing skills might be sources as the attribution of causal
considered broadly transferable are mechanisms such as collision and
suggested below. The first considers support namely from the perceptual
embodied cognitive perspectives on the (mental) representations of the dynamic
relationship between perception and spatial and temporal behaviour of
cognition. The second considers notions objects and agents (Goswami 2008:
of analogical transfer and suggests ways 46).
of considering transferable skill in
teaching practice. He goes on to relate sensitivity to space,
objects and causal relations to more
Embodied cognition and drawing abstract cognition such as conceptual
representation, memory, logic, and a sense
Drawing is widely thought to sharpen of agency. Empirical evidence supporting
sensori-motor awareness and expand these claims lies in the shared neural loci of
visual memory by virtue of time spent experience for concepts and percepts.
paying close attention to the visual world.
A more intimate knowledge of the way Cognitive linguist Mark Johnson builds on
the world looks, and is physically this paradigm, explaining that embodied
structured, is valuable in itself, particularly understanding is the foundation for thought:
to artists and designers. However, it can meaning and thought emerge from our
be considered more widely valuable capacities for perception, object
when contemporary embodied-cognitive manipulation, and bodily movement (2007:
perspectives are taken into account, as 113 see also Johnson 1990). This is the
these perspectives illustrate the basis of metaphor. The neural body in the
relationship between perception and mind reconstructs tactile, spatial and visual
cognition. experiences to make sense of more abstract
ideas. Hence it makes perfect sense for an
Developmental cognitive neuro- idea to be thought of as having tactile or
psychologists now widely acknowledge spatial qualities; being slippery, fragile,
liquid, heavy, difficult to grasp, thrown or
19
Other examples of this view include bounced around. They can be held lightly,
Petherbridge (2008), Riley (2008), Corte-Real
(2009), IDEAL Project (2009), Haley (2010),
Kantrowitz (in press).
Fava

built up and then shattered, attacked and towards a teaching that facilitates cognitive
20
defended . understanding. She notes that in the past,
technical skill for representational purposes
The proposition this logic allows is that may have been separated from cognitive
time taken to conduct observed drawing understanding, but less so now (1997).
has the ability to enrich cognition in a Similarly, Bruce Archer identifies a need for
holistic way. This is, perhaps, another drawing as a training in thinking to be
way observational drawing might be taught in addition to drawing for the
included in contemporary notions of purposes of representation (1997). What
graphicacy. might drawing as a training in thinking
involve? Here I will suggest that drawing
The implication here is that time spent can be also used as a basis for analogical
drawing is valuable by virtue of the reasoning and learning.
intensity of experience the drawer has,
which will contribute to their cognitive Laura Novick (1988) describes surface and
capacities and to any design or visual deep levels of analogical reasoning. She
artistic practices. This is by no means a explains how a novice is able to apply
new idea. Nicolaides insisted that the surface features of an experience in
effort you make is not for one particular analogical reasoning, while an expert can
drawing, but for the experience you are relate superficially unrelated tasks by
having (1990 [1941]: 2). structural comparison. In this way learned
problem solving strategies can be
This holistic aspect of learning is primarily transferred across disciplinary and thematic
concerned not with the quality of the boundaries.
drawing, but of the drawers experience.
This is, again, something hard or even Daugherty & Mentzer (2008) review
impossible to measure. Yet the quality of research into analogical reasoning and its
the experience of drawing process is significance for engineering design
something most drawing tutors trust in, students. Emphasising its importance for
despite the fact that it cannot be creativity, they describe the way that
expressed in writing as a measurable analogies aid in the development of
outcome. knowledge. They state that this takes place
by the application of structural logic from a
Observational drawing & analogical base domain to a target domain. This can
transfer be enhanced by [r]epresenting either or
both the base or target domain to improve
Tormey suggests a need to move away the analogy [to] further establish conceptual
from teaching drawing simply as a means change.
to creating conventional images,
Gentner & Jeziorski (1993) note that while
20 tutors often use analogy to explain concepts
Despite the ubiquity of metaphor, it is not so
acceptable for it to be used in learning to students, analogical reasoning is rarely
outcomes and assessment criteria. Richard formally taught. Drawing can be thought of
Hamilton believed the learning process to be as useful in this way. It can serve as a base
analogous to that of organic growth
(Thistlewood 1981), but this metaphor would domain from which to source creative and
seem out of place in a module specification, learning experiences. As established above,
and might only be used after the fact to
observational drawing activity encompasses
explain the meaning of more obtusely worded
descriptors or objectives to puzzled students. cognitive activity in a relatively

136
Observational drawing

straightforward and visible way. Drawing complex creative or cognitive problems in


process can also be considered other domains. A tutor might refer to
analogous to creative process in a strategies for constructing a drawing, for
number of ways: example, when discussing approaches to
constructing a written argument, to the
Both begin from a single point, be it design process, or to perseverance in
given or chosen. learning more generally. Students might
also be encouraged to use drawing as a
Both involve cycles of construction base domain in analogical problem solving
and destruction. for design.

Focus must shift between smaller Again, this is a form of learning that cannot
elements and the whole to maintain easily be measured. Transferable skills are,
coherence (stepping back and by nature, flexible, rhizomatic,
leaning in during making). unpredictable. To apply the logic of one
discipline to an analogous novel situation
Both involve aesthetic decisions requires both flexibility of thinking and a
and compositional planning. repertoire of problem solving experiences.
What analogous experience a student may
A range of strategies can be have used in solving a design problem, or
chosen, e.g. beginning from a approaching an essay, might never be
specific point and working known. It could be that by encouraging a
outwards, or working from the student to consider analogical reasoning
general into the specific. based on an understanding of drawing, they
are inclined to a better understanding of
It might be necessary at some point analogical reasoning itself, and as a result
to erase large portions and re-draft. solve a novel problem by applying existing
For example, if an error is found, if knowledge. This would be highly individual
a preferable compositional choice is for each student, relating to their own
found (highlighting the need for unique experiences and challenges. Impact
ongoing evaluation), or if the of this kind would be neither entirely
purpose (or subject) of the drawing predictable nor measurable. It might even
changes. go wholly unnoticed, but it would be of
lasting importance to the student.
Strategies for constructing a coherent
whole can be tried and tested, and Both of these examples relate to holistic
progress over time is clearly visible, both approaches to teaching and assessment
within and between drawings. These that account for non-verbal learning and
qualities render drawing a valuable individual differences. I suggest that it is
resource for reflection on learning and for possible to consider observational drawing
approaching analogous processes. It can as part of an expanded definition of
be seen as a broad base for analogical graphicacy, which accounts for the capacity
reasoning. Acknowledgement of, and drawing experiences have to enable visual
reflection on, this structural similarity can cognitive abilities and creative problem
serve as a valuable opportunity to solving, and to facilitate abstract conceptual
consider analogical reasoning. Relatively thought through an enriched visual
simple drawing experiences can be used knowledge of the world.
as opportunities for considering more
Fava

Conclusion creative process is also analogous to the


practice. These facets render it a potentially
Tacit, non-verbal and difficult to measure broadly valuable source for analogical
knowledge is recognised as important but transfer and reasoning. Furthermore,
often cannot be made explicit. To embodied cognitive perspectives have the
acknowledge this is not fall short of our ability to shift concepts of the value and
obligation to the QAA, but to hold the purpose of observational drawing towards a
same rigour of accountability to the more holistic view of transferable skills.
mechanisms that audit our degrees. To
accept tacit and holistic learning I am not suggesting that we privilege
outcomes, we would need to expand the transferable skills over more immediate
rationalist box of practices that are applications of drawing, or that tacit skill be
legitimated by the QAA, Edexcel and valued over propositional knowledge. Only
other professional bodies, to be more that it is conceivable that we can openly
flexible and to include subtler, more acknowledge this type of learning within
holistic and tacit aspects of observational rational educational frameworks, both in
drawing and similar skills. teaching and assessment practices. In
doing so we can enable an expanded
At the same time I propose that it is definition of drawing with potentially
possible to work within these frameworks extensive benefits. It is possible to consider
to consider how learning outcomes for more explicitly the way learning outcomes
drawing might be better expressed to for drawing are worded, but it is also
include notions of transferable skill. I necessary to acknowledge that sometimes
propose that the range of cognitive this is not possible and that should not
learning represented by Blooms marginalise less measurable learning or
taxonomy can be found within the act of holistic approaches to curriculum
observational drawing. Likewise, the development.
Observational drawing

References
re/publications/newsletters/newsissue3/de
Alsop, W. (2002) Society's Inability to ntith.htm
Draw is Destroying the Art of Looking. The
Architects Journal. v. 216 no. 13 (October Edexcel (2009) Unit 1: Visual Recording in
10 2002) :24. Available online: Art and Design. In Edexcel BTEC Level 3
http://www.architectsjournal.co.uk/home/so Nationals Specification in Art and Design
cietys-inability-to-draw-is-destroying-the- Issue 1 January 2010, Edexcel Limited.
art-of-looking/177281.article
Garner, S. W. (1999) Digital Product
Archer, L. B. (1997) Drawing as a Tool for Design. Tracey, Drawing and Technology
Designers, (Conference paper), in T. Issue. Available online:
Moscovitch (ed), The Future of Drawing in http://www.lboro.ac.uk/departments/ac/trac
Design. UK: University of Huddersfield, 39- ey/dat/garner.html
42.
Gentner, D. & Jeziorski, M. (1993) The
Bloom, B. S. Krathwohl, D. R. (1956) shift from metaphor to analogy in western
Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: The science. In Ortony (Ed) Metaphor and
Classification of Educational Goals. Thought (2nd ed. 447-480) Cambridge
Handbook I: Cognitive Domain. New York: MA: Cambridge University Press
Longmans
Gordon, J. (2004) The 'wow' factors: the
Bowden, J. (2000) Art & Design: The assessment of practical media and
Rhetoric and the Practice. Journal of Art & creative arts subjects. Art, Design and
Design Education, vol. 19 - 20-36 Communication in Higher Education, vol.
3(1), 61-72
Corte-Real, E. (2009) A Smooth Guide to
Travel Drawing. Conference presentation, Goswami, U. (2008) Cognitive
Drawing Research Network conference: Development: The Learning Brain. New
th
Thinking Through Drawing. 8 Oct 2009. York: Psychology Press
Cochrane Theatre, London
Haley, D. (2010) How do artists teach
Daugherty, J.& Mentzer, N. (2008) drawing? Thread, UK Drawing Research
Analogical reasoning in the Engineering Network mailing list. 27 March 2010.
Design Process and Technology Available e-mail: drawing-
Education Applications. Journal of research@jiscmail.ac.uk
Technology Education, Spring 2008, vol19,
No2. Available online: IDEAL project (2009) Intercultural Drawing
http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/ejournals/JTE/v19 for European Adult Learning. Life Long
m2/daugherty.html Learning Programme, (on Drawing Spaces
gallery website) GRUNTVIG Available
Dentith, S. (2002) English and the Audit online:
Culture: An Introduction. http://drawingspacesen.weebly.com/ideal-
Higher Education Academy, English partnership---grundtvig.html [accessed
Subject Centre Online Newsletter. Issue 3, 24/10/09]
Jan 2002. Available online:
http://www.english.heacademy.ac.uk/explo

139
Fava

Johnson, B. (2005) Design ideation: the Artificial Intelligence, Cognitive Science


conceptual sketch in the digital age. and Philosophy. Helman, D. H. (ed)(1988)
Design Studies, vol. 26 (6) 613- 624 Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers

Johnson, M. (1990) The Body in the Mind: Orr, S. (2010) Making Marks: Assessment
the bodily basis of meaning, imagination, practices in art & design. Networks, Issue
and reason. Chicago: University of 10, Summer 2010, HEA Art, Design &
Chicago Press Media Subject Centre, 9-13

Johnson M. (2007) The Meaning of the Petherbridge, D. (2008) Nailing the


Body: aesthetics of human understanding. Liminal: The Difficulties of Defining
Chicago: University of Chicago Press Drawing. In Steve Garner (ed.) Writing on
Drawing, Essays on Drawing Practice and
Kozbelt, A., Seidel, A. El Bassiouny, A., Research. Bristol and Chicago: Intellect,
Mark, Y. & Owen, D. R. (2010) Visual 27-42
Selection Contributes to Artists
Advantages in Realistic Drawing. Prats, M., Lim, S., Jowers, I., Garner, S.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and W. & Chase, S. (2009) Transforming
the Arts, vol. 4 (2) 93-102 shape in design: observations from studies
of sketching. Design Studies, vol. 30 (5)
Kantrowitz, A. (in press) The Man behind 503 520
the Curtain: What Cognitive Science
Reveals about Drawing. Journal of Quality Assurance Agency (2006) Code of
Aesthetic Education practice for the assurance of academic
quality and standards in higher education:
McGuirk T. (2008) Knowing by Hand: Section 6: Assessment of students.
Embodied Knowledge in Higher Education Mansfield:
Fava Linney Direct / QAA
in Art and Design. In Bott G. &
Hrmnmaa M. (eds.) (2009) Language Quality Assurance Agency (2008) Subject
and the Scientific Imagination Benchmark Standards: Art & Design.
Proceedings of the 11th Conference of the Mansfield: Linney Direct / QAA
International Society for the Study of
European Ideas ISSEI, 28 July2 Aug. Riley, H. (2008) Drawing: Towards an
2008, University of Helsinki Intelligence of Seeing. In, S. (ed.) Writing
on Drawing. Bristol: Intellect/University of
McKillop, C. (2007) Drawing on Chicago Press, 153-167
assessment: using visual representations
to understand students' experiences of Rose, S. E., Jolley, R. P. & Burkitt, E.
assessment in art and design. Art, Design (2006) A Review of Childrens, Teachers
& Communication in Higher Education, vol. and Parents Influences on Childrens
5, 2. 131-144 Drawing Experience. Journal of Art and
Design Education vol. 25 no.3 341-349.
Nicolaides, K. (2008) [1941] The Natural London: NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing
Way to Draw. London: Souvenir Press
Ruskin, J. (1991) [1857] The Elements of
Novick, L. R. (1988) Analogical Transfer: Drawing. (illus. ed.) London: The Herbert
processes and individual differences. In Press
Analogical Reasoning: Perspectives of
Observational drawing

Seeley, W. P. & Kozbelt, A. (2008) Art, Thistlewood, D. (1981) A Continuing


artists, and perception: A model for Process. London: ICA
premotor contributions to perceptual
analysis and form recognition. Tormey, J. (1997) Foundation course
Philosophical Psychology, vol. 21, 149 drawing TRACEY (Education issue, Sept
171 1997) Available:
http://www.lboro.ac.uk/departments/ac/trac
ey/edu/tormey.html [accessed 03/10/09]

141

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen