Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/47788561

Effects of a Kinesthetic Cursive Handwriting


Intervention for Grade 4-6 Students

Article in The American journal of occupational therapy.: official publication of the American Occupational
Therapy Association September 2010
DOI: 10.5014/ajot.2010.08128 Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

10 207

3 authors, including:

Gwenyth I Roberts Jodi E Siever


Alberta Health Services University of British Columbia - Vancouver
7 PUBLICATIONS 31 CITATIONS 36 PUBLICATIONS 305 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Jodi E Siever on 28 August 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
Effects of a Kinesthetic Cursive Handwriting Intervention
for Grade 46 Students

Gwenyth I. Roberts, Jodi E. Siever, Judith A. Mair

KEY WORDS OBJECTIVE. We studied whether Grade 46 students who participated in a kinesthetic writing intervention
 handwriting improved in legibility, speed, and personal satisfaction with cursive handwriting.
 kinesiology, applied METHOD. Small groups of students with handwriting difficulties were seen weekly for 7 wk using a kin-
esthetic writing system. A repeated measures design was used to evaluate change in global legibility, in-
 kinesthesis
dividual letter formation, specific features of handwriting, and personal satisfaction.
 task performance and analysis
RESULTS. Analysis revealed (1) a significant increase in ratings of global legibility (p < .01; clinically
 treatment outcome
significant improvements in 39% of students); (2) significant improvements in letter formation and legibility
features of baseline, closure, and line quality (all p < .05); (3) increased handwriting speed (p < .05; not
clinically significant); and (4) significant increase in measures with personal satisfaction of handwriting
(p < .01).
CONCLUSION. A kinesthetic handwriting intervention may be effective in improving the skills of students
with handwriting challenges.

Roberts, G. I., Siever, J. E., & Mair, J. A. (2010). Effects of a kinesthetic cursive handwriting intervention for grade 46
students. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 64, 745755. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2010.08128

Gwenyth I. Roberts, MSc, BOT, is Occupational


Therapist/Clinical Leader, Regional School Health
Program, Alberta Health Services, and Occupational
L earning to write legibly and efficiently is an important occupation for school-
age children. The quality of handwriting may influence academic outcomes,
with higher marks assigned for neatly written papers (Sweedler-Brown, 1992).
Therapy Research Affiliate: Decision Support Research
Team, Alberta Childrens Hospital, Alberta Health Services, The nature and extent of instruction affects a students ability to perform skilled
Acadia Community Health Centre, 132-151 86th Avenue handwriting, with explicit instruction increasing the legibility and efficiency of
SE, Calgary, Alberta T2H 3A5 Canada; gwen.roberts@
albertahealthservices.ca
written work (Goldberg & Simner, 1999), as well as improving composition
skills (Berninger et al., 1997; Graham, Harris, & Fink, 2000). Cursive script, in
Jodi E. Siever, MSc, is Senior Analyst/Biostatistician, which the letters within words are connected by joined strokes, has been tra-
Public Health Innovation and Decision Support,
ditionally reported to be the form of writing preferred for efficiency in legibility
Population and Public Health, Alberta Childrens Hospital,
Alberta Health Services, Calgary, Alberta, Canada. and speed, although controversy does exist (Graham, Weintraub, & Berninger,
1998).
Judith A. Mair is Occupational Therapist, Neurosciences Legibility of cursive writing has been evaluated using global rating scales that
Program, Alberta Childrens Hospital, Alberta Health
compare the individuals performance with a series of model specimens (Feder,
Services, Calgary, Alberta, Canada.
Majnemer, & Synnes, 2000). Legibility has also been judged in terms of
components: slant, alignment, spacing, size, and letter formation (Feder &
Majnemer, 2007). Speed has been evaluated using letters per minute, but the
use of varied instructions and task demands confounds the ability to determine
typical writing speeds across ages (Graham, Berninger, Weintraub, & Schafer,
1998).
Some students struggle in their written work, and teachers typically estimate
that 12% of children in their class have difficulties with handwriting (Barnett,
2005). Boys have primarily been identified with poor handwriting skills
(Berninger et al., 1997; Graham et al., 2000), as well as children with motor
coordination or learning disabilities (Jongmans, Linthorst-Bakker, Westenberg,

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 745


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
& Smits-Engelsman, 2003; Missiuna et al., 2008). Graham handwriting. Secondary objectives were to determine
and Harris (2005) indicated that inadequate instruction whether children who participate in a kinesthetic writing
results in writing challenges. Handwriting difficulties may intervention would (1) improve in speed of handwriting
lead children to avoid writing (Berninger et al., 1997; at 4 mo postintervention and (2) improve in their personal
Graham et al., 2000) and may affect a students motiva- satisfaction with handwriting.
tion, self-image, and academics (Piek, Baynam, & Barrett,
2006). Method
Handwriting difficulties are cited as one of the most
frequently mentioned reasons for student referral to school- Research Design
based occupational therapists (Feder et al., 2000; Missiuna
A repeated measures design, with four time points, was
et al., 2008). Studies have found that handwriting can be
used to evaluate change in legibility, speed, and personal
improved after 910 hr of individualized occupational
satisfaction with handwriting over time. The study re-
therapy intervention (Case-Smith, 2002; Peterson &
ceived ethics approval from the health region and two
Nelson, 2003). Feder et al. (2000) found that 56% of
school district ethics review boards.
occupational therapists surveyed provided handwriting
remediation sessions on a weekly basis. Participants and Recruitment
Several studies have described features of remediation
programs to help develop handwriting skills. Laszlo and A convenience sample was obtained by contacting 72
Broderick (1991) reported that kinesthetic instruction schools in Calgary and outlining the study objectives and
had a positive effect on the handwriting of students who protocol. Participants were identified by their teachers as
had difficulties, whereas Sudsawad, Trombly, Henderson, having handwriting problems, determined on the basis of
and Tickle-Degnen (2002) found that handwriting diffi- the students handwriting performance within the class-
culties could not be improved by means of kinesthetic room setting. We screened the students through parent and
training. Studies have supported reintroduction of the caregiver contact. Students who had moderate to severe
letter forms explaining each form visually and verbally physical limitation, identified cognitive impairment, or
(Karlsdottir, 1996), self-instruction ( Jongmans et al., persistent behavioral concerns; were receiving occupational
2003), random practice (Ste.-Marie, Clark, Findlay, & therapy services for handwriting difficulties; or whose pa-
Latimer, 2004), and model/review of stroke direction be- rents did not give informed consent were excluded.
fore practice (Berninger et al., 1997). Little consensus has
been reached on the most effective strategy for remediation, Instruments
and further research on current remediation practices has Handwriting Skill. Three handwriting samples were
been recommended (Asher, 2006; Bonney, 1992). collected from each participant to measure skill:
Many occupational therapists recommend the pro- Copying: the phrase The quick brown fox jumped
gram Loops and Other Groups: A Kinesthetic Writing Sys- over the lazy dogs (Sovik, 1975);
tem (Benbow, 1990). This program focuses on the motor Composition: the Handwriting Subtest from the Test
aspects of cursive handwriting, with letters taught in of Written Language (Hammill & Larsen, 1983); and
groups that share movement patterns. It combines sen- Alphabet samples: both connected and unconnected.
sorimotor techniques, along with letter formation prac- Handwriting Quality and Speed. We used the following
tice, and includes modeling and verbal analysis of letters, methods of rating the quality and speed of the handwriting
motor learning through tracing, revisualization, verbal samples:
self-guidance, handwriting from memory, and self- The Test of Written Language (TOWL) Handwriting
assessment of letters most accurately produced. We could Subtest Rating Scale rates global legibility of a students
not find any peer-reviewed studies evaluating the effec- composition by comparing it with a series of graded
tiveness of this program; therefore, the need to evaluate specimens with a value ranging from 0 to 10, which is
its effectiveness is evident. then standardized on a scale ranging from 0 to 20.
Testretest reliability is reported at .89, interscorer
reliability at .76, and criterion validity with teacher
Objectives ratings at .46.
Our primary objective was to determine whether children The Handwriting Evaluation Scale (HES; Malloy-
who participate in a kinesthetic writing intervention Miller, 1985) evaluates legibility by direct analysis of
would improve significantly in legibility of cursive handwriting errors, on the basis of definitions for

746 September/October 2010, Volume 64, Number 5


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
seven components of spacing within words, spacing faction and took place 4 times during this study. Parents
between words, size of letters within words, size be- and teachers completed the questionnaires concurrently.
tween words, baseline orientation, closure, and line Intervention Procedure. The remediation program was
quality. A percentage correct score (0%100%) is ob- based on the Loops and Other Groups program and was
tained for each of the components. This instrument is provided by an occupational therapist and a therapy as-
reported to have face validity by using handwriting sistant. The program was extended from a 6-wk in-
errors directly related to in-school handwriting tasks tervention to a 7-wk intervention by dividing one session,
(Malloy-Miller, Polatajko, & Ansett, 1995). Interrater in which several new letters were introduced, into two
reliability of the HES has been reported at r 5 .94 sessions. Small groups of students were seen once a week
(Roberts & Samuels, 1993). for an hour after school in a quiet room in the therapy area
Speed was evaluated on the copying sample, with the of a local childrens hospital. The students were evaluated
student writing the phrase repeatedly until 2 min had to ensure correct seating at a table as well as functional
elapsed (Sovik, 1975). Letters per minute were calcu- finger and wrist posture. Pencil grips were provided as
lated. required. Hand and arm activities occurred for 10 min at
Personal Satisfaction With Handwriting. The Attitude the beginning of each session in preparation for handwrit-
Scale was developed specifically for this study to measure ing. The letters of the lowercase alphabet were taught in
personal satisfaction on the basis of the semantic differ- four groupings according to shared movement patterns.
ential, as outlined by Mueller (1986). It consists of seven These included clock climbers (a, d, g, q, c), kite strings
evaluative adjective pairs specific to handwriting. State- (i, u, w, t, j, p, r, s, o), loop group (h, k, b, f, l, e), and
ments are provided, and the student places an X on one hills and valleys (n, m, v, y, x, z). During the remediation
of seven spaces on a visual analog scale that has an ad- program, the student received homework sheets to be
jective at each end. An example statement is, I feel my completed nightly with their parents for 1520 min.
handwriting is . . . with the evaluative pair ugly and Data Collection. After Test 4, the composition samples
beautiful at either end of the scale. The total score on this from each of the test sessions (with the date of testing
measure ranges from 7 to 49 points. We piloted the scale excluded) were provided to the caregiver, the teacher, and
with 122 Grade 5 and Grade 6 students, and a descriptive an occupational therapist to rate global legibility using the
analysis of the data, using histograms, successfully showed TOWL. Scoring instructions were provided. The occu-
a range in responses for each question. The Student In- pational therapist involved in the data collection was not
ventory (Alberta Childrens Hospital, 2001) is a non- the same occupational therapist who administered the
standardized attitude scale that includes eight questions, intervention.
each with five pictures that range from a picture of a very Interrater reliability of the HES was completed by two
happy dog (rated as 1) to one of a very unhappy dog occupational therapists who independently scored the
(rated as 5). An example of a question is, How do you same three handwriting samples, after agreeing on scoring
feel about how neatly you write? The total score ranges criteria. Pearsons productmoment correlation co-
from 8 to 40 points. efficients ranged from .78 to .96 for the seven compo-
Parent and Teacher Reports. Questionnaires for parents nents; the overall r 5 .86. The two therapists, blind to
and teachers were designed for this study. Open-ended the time of testing, scored half of the copying, compo-
questions determined the type and amount of instruction sition, and alphabet samples independently, calculating
and handwriting practice in which students participated at the percentage of correct responses for each of the seven
home and school. Nine questions regarding attitude, legibility components.
speed, and legibility were rated on a 10-point visual analog Sample Size. The sample size was estimated using the
scale; 0 indicated a poor response, and 10 indicated a good paired t test of mean differences to allow for analysis of
response (e.g., How would you rate the legibility of your within-participant changes preintervention and post-
childs [this students] written work?). The teacher intervention. Using an a of .05 and power of 80%,
questionnaire included a 10th question asking whether it a sample size of 30 was chosen because it would detect
was expected that written work be completed in cursive a minimum change of 3.0 standard scores in the TOWL
handwriting. (standard deviation [SD] 5 2.0), detect a minimum
change of 5.0 points in the HES (SD 5 10.0), and allow
Procedure and Data Collection for stratification by grade and gender.
Testing Procedure. Testing consisted of three hand- Data Analysis. Data were entered into Microsoft Excel
writing samples and the two measures of personal satis- 2003 spreadsheets, scored with a higher score relating to

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 747


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
a more positive outcome, and then transferred into Stata All statistical tests for both primary and secondary
S/E, Version 9.2 (StataCorp, College Station, TX) for objectives were two-tailed, with p values of .05 considered
analysis. Data were checked and corrected for entry errors to be statistically significant. Childrens samples were
by producing the range, frequency, and histogram for each excluded from all analyses if students did not attend
variable; no data required elimination as a result. De- a minimum of four treatment sessions. Analyses were
scriptive analyses were performed to examine the char- based on available data from all instruments completed in
acteristics of the children who participated in the study a valid manner. Several students printed, scribbled, or left
and all outcome measures. Categorical variables were blanks during the testing sessions; therefore, the de-
expressed as frequencies and percentages, and continuous nominator varied for each measure at each time point.
variables were reported as means with standard deviations.
For the primary objective, graphical displays of the Results
median value at each time point were constructed for the
Recruitment, Participation, and Baseline
TOWL and the HES for composition, copying, and al-
Characteristics
phabet samples. The occupational therapists TOWL
scores were used because the return rates from the Forty-two students from 28 schools were recruited to the
caregiver and teacher ratings were too small to be in- study, and 32 students attended four or more of the
cluded. The distribution of the TOWL and all compo- treatment sessions. Six group interventions occurred, with
nents of the HES scores were each assessed individually a range of 37 students per group. There were more boys
for normality using the WilkShapiro test (Shapiro & (84%) than girls (16%), with an average age of 10.5 yr
Wilk, 1965). The normality assumption was violated; (range 5 8 yr, 7 mo, to 11 yr, 11 mo). One-third of the
therefore, the nonparametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test students were in each of Grades 4 (n 5 11), 5 (n 5 11),
(Wilcoxon, 1945) was used to examine the paired data and 6 (n 5 10). Although 25 students completed the
for all outcome measures (i.e., TOWL and HES). postintervention testing session, only 18 of those students
Four separate statistical comparisons were carried out produced valid handwriting samples.
to test the impact of the handwriting intervention: (1) Test
1 results were compared with Test 2 results to establish any Primary Outcome: Legibility of Handwriting
natural improvement in handwriting technique over time Global Legibility. Table 1 and Figure 1 show the sta-
without the intervention, (2) Test 2 results were compared tistical comparisons and median scores, respectively, on
with Test 3 results to determine whether the intervention the TOWL. Scores increased during the intervention
was effective, (3) Test 3 results were compared with Test 4 period, and gains were maintained. There were im-
results to assess long-term improvement after the hand- provements in handwriting from Test 1 to Test 4.
writing intervention, and (4) Test 4 results were compared TOWL scores showed that 39% of students (7 of 18)
with Test 1 results to determine improvement from the improved by a score of 3 or more from Test 1 to Test 4.
start of the study to 4 mo postintervention. Seventy-five percent of Grade 6 students (6 of 8), 20% of
To define a clinical indicator of success in handwriting Grade 5 students (1 out of 5), and 40% of Grade 4
improvement, the five graded specimens provided in the students (2 of 5) improved in their ratings on the
TOWL manual were examined a priori, and an increase of TOWL. However, statistically significant differences were
3.0 points on the TOWL standard scale (811 yr) was set not found in grade-level proportions (p 5 .18), gender
as a clinically important change from a less to a more (boys 5 53%, girls 5 33%, p 5 1.00), attendance at
legible specimen. Differences in demographic varia- intervention (5 sessions 5 100%, 6 sessions 5 57%, 7
bles (e.g., gender, grade), attendance at intervention, and sessions 5 40%, p 5 .65), or amount of homework
reported homework practice were examined using Fish- practice reported (<30 min per week 5 60%, 30 min
ers exact test (Agresti, 1992; Fisher, 1922) for a differ- per week 5 45%, p 5 1.00).
ence in proportions from Test 1 to Test 4 on the TOWL. Legibility ComponentsAlphabet Samples. Table 1 and
For the secondary objectives, speed and satisfaction Figure 2 show the statistical comparisons and median
with handwriting were both examined using the non- scores, respectively, for letter formation and baseline
parametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test at the various time orientation, closure, line quality, size, and space (only for
points, and graphical displays of the median change over connected alphabet) components using the HES. There
time were constructed. Speed by grade level, as well as parent were no changes in the median scores from Test 1 to Test
and teacher reports, were examined descriptively with the 2, except in closure for the connected alphabet. For the
mean and standard deviation reported for each time point. intervention period, there were positive changes for all

748 September/October 2010, Volume 64, Number 5


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Table 1. Statistical Comparisons Over Time for the Test of Written Language (TOWL), the Handwriting Evaluation Scale (HES), and
Personal Satisfaction With Handwriting
Test 1 to Test 2 Test 2 to Test 3 Test 3 to Test 4
(Preintervention (Intervention (Postintervention Test 1 to Test 4
Period) Period) Period) (Overall)
Measure z p z p z p z p
TOWL: Composition task n 5 24 n 5 25 n 5 18 n 5 18
2.01 .04 3.87 <.01 20.48 .64 2.70 <.01
HES: Connected alphabet n 5 24 n 5 28 n 5 19 n 5 18
Letter formation 1.87 .06 4.60 <.01 22.30 .02 3.22 <.01
Size of letters 20.33 .74 4.44 <.01 23.27 <.01 1.61 .11
Letter space 1.80 .17 4.60 <.01 21.87 .06 3.30 <.01
Baseline 1.46 .14 4.32 <.01 21.36 .17 3.44 <.01
Line quality 1.43 .15 4.62 <.01 22.57 .01 3.54 <.01
Closure 2.18 .03 4.53 <.01 21.82 .07 3.62 <.01
HES: Unconnected alphabet n 5 29 n 5 29 n 5 19 n 5 19
Letter formation 1.41 .16 4.68 <.01 22.76 <.01 3.55 <.01
Size of letters 0.32 .75 4.68 <.01 23.20 <.01 2.42 .02
Letter space 0.99 .32 4.34 <.01 21.50 .13 3.63 <.01
Baseline 0.32 .75 4.71 <.01 23.21 <.01 3.49 <.01
Line quality 1.49 .14 4.70 <.01 23.08 <.01 3.69 <.01
HES: Copying task n 5 25 n 5 25 n 5 18 n 5 17
Size within words 0.89 .37 3.61 <.01 22.57 .01 2.13 .03
Size between words 20.50 .62 2.59 .01 21.63 .10 0.62 .54
Baseline 20.11 .91 2.25 .03 1.33 .18 2.11 .04
Letter space 20.82 .41 3.11 <.01 1.11 .27 0.39 .70
Word space 20.28 .78 0.63 .53 20.76 .45 0.66 .51
Line quality 21.12 .26 3.14 <.01 2.24 .03 3.15 <.01
Closure 20.63 .53 2.11 .04 0.20 .85 1.56 .12
HES: Composition task n 5 24 n 5 24 n 5 18 n 5 18
Size within words 0.36 .72 3.00 <.01 22.16 .03 20.28 .78
Size between words 21.90 .06 2.33 .02 20.76 .45 20.68 .50
Baseline 21.70 .09 2.57 .01 21.15 .25 1.63 .10
Letter space 20.92 .36 1.87 .06 20.41 .68 0.54 .59
Word space 21.67 .95 1.67 .10 0.28 .78 20.20 .85
Line quality 0.59 .56 2.60 .01 20.81 .42 1.48 .14
Closure 21.53 .13 2.31 .02 0.37 .71 1.98 .05
Personal satisfaction n 5 31 n 5 25 n 5 25 n 5 25
Attitude Scale 2.28 .02 2.73 .01 20.77 .44 2.52 .01
Student Inventory 0.08 .94 3.54 <.01 20.27 .79 3.29 <.01
Note. The nonparametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used for all paired comparisons.

components in both alphabet samples. There was a de- noted in the size of letters within words, whereas an in-
crease in several median scores of measures, including crease was noted in line quality. There were increases from
letter formation, size of letters, and line quality in both Test 1 to Test 4 in the size of letters within words, base-
alphabet samples and in the closure variable for the un- line, and line quality components. In the composition
connected alphabet from Test 3 to Test 4. From Test 1 to sample, there were increases in all HES components from
Test 4, there were significant increases in all components Test 2 to Test 3 except in letter space and word space. The
in the unconnected alphabet and in all but size of letters size of letters within words demonstrated a decrease from
in the connected alphabet. Test 3 to Test 4. There was an increase from Test 1 to Test
Legibility ComponentsCopying and Composition. Ta- 4 for the closure component. Visual inspection of Figure 3
ble 1 and Figure 3 show the statistical comparisons and shows that scores for the size-within-words component
median HES scores, respectively, for the copying and were higher for the composition task, whereas scores for
composition samples. In the copying samples, there were line quality were higher for the copying task.
increases from Test 2 to Test 3 in all legibility components Secondary Outcome: Speed of Handwriting. Copying
except word space. From Test 3 to Test 4, a decrease was speed increased from Test 1 to Test 2 (t 5 2.78, p < .01,

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 749


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Figure 1. Global ratings of handwriting legibility on the Test of Written Language Handwriting Subtest Rating Scale.

N 5 25) and from Test 2 to Test 3 (t 5 1.98, p < .05, satisfaction over the preintervention phase. For both
N 5 25). From Test 3 to Test 4, gains were maintained, measures of personal satisfaction, there was an increase
but there were no further increases in speed post- during the intervention period (Test 2 to Test 3) and overall
intervention (t 5 0.87, p 5 .38, N 5 18). An increase (Test 1 to Test 4; Table 1 and Figure 4).
was noted between Test 1 and Test 4 (t 5 2.11, p 5 .04, Parent and Teacher Reports. Classroom instruction and
N 5 17). Mean letters per minute changed from 18.7 practice was reported by 9 of 24 teachers (38%), ranging
(SD 5 16.0) at Test 1 to 23.8 (SD 5 15.8) at Test 4. from 1 to 5 times per week for 430 min per session.
An increase in average speed of writing between Test 2 Handwriting practice at home was reported by 4 of 32
and Test 3 was observed for Grades 4 and 5 but not for parents (13%), from 1 to 5 times per week for 525 min
Grade 6. Increased writing speed was noted for all grades in length. Table 3 shows increased ratings in the areas of
from Test 1 to Test 4, and the greatest gains were observed attitude, speed, and legibility on the parent and teacher
in Grade 4 students (Table 2). questions, with the parents reporting greater preinter-
Secondary Outcome: Personal Satisfaction With Handwriting. vention to postintervention changes. Both parents and
Student Inventory scores showed an increase in personal teachers reported the largest increase on the question that

Figure 2. Component scores for the unconnected and connected alphabet.

750 September/October 2010, Volume 64, Number 5


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Figure 3. Component scores for the copying and composition tasks.

rated the childs feelings toward handwriting. The reflected the primary strategy of the intervention pro-
teachers average score for student expectation to com- gram and is of great importance in legibility (Feder &
plete written work using cursive writing was 3.7 (N 5 Majnemer, 2007). Significant improvements were identified
24) preintervention and 3.5 (N 5 11) postintervention. in the legibility components of baseline, closure, and line
quality, which are reported to relate to kinesthetic feedback
(Malloy-Miller, 1985). Parents and teachers reported im-
Discussion provements in aspects of legibility, speed, and attitude.
The greater proportion of boys than girls recruited was We found that students improved more in the
representative of the gender difference found in hand- copying task than in the composition task. In the copying
writing problems (Berninger et al., 1997). The Loops and samples, there were significant increases in six handwriting
Other Groups program, presented in a small-group for- components during the intervention period and in three of
mat, was shown to be effective in improving the partici- the components from preintervention until 4 mo post-
pants skills. More than one-third of the students intervention. In the composition sample, significant
demonstrated improvement in global legibility of cursive increases occurred in five handwriting components during
handwriting in composition samples. A significant im- intervention and in one component from preintervention
provement in the cursive formations of individual letters to 4 mo postintervention. Composition is reported to

Table 2. Handwriting Copying Speed, by Grade


Test 1 Test 2 Test 3 Test 4
Grade Mean Letters/Min (SD) Mean Letters/Min (SD) Mean Letters/Min (SD) Mean Letters/Min (SD)
4 6.4 (4.8) 9.9 (5.3) 15.9 (5.6) 13.3 (6.4)
n58 n58 n57 n55
5 19.0 (17.6) 18.4 (20.0) 24.0 (10.4) 20.9 (8.6)
n59 n58 n 5 10 n55
6 28.4 (14.3) 33.5 (16.2) 31.0 (13.2) 32.2 (19.4)
n 5 10 n 5 10 n 5 10 n58
Note. The number of students at each test time varies because of absences or incomplete data. SD = standard deviation.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 751


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
Figure 4. Attitude Scale and Student Inventory for personal satisfaction with handwriting.

involve an integration and synthesis of cognitive skills within-words component, and we question whether it
more complex than those needed for a simpler writing should be evaluated when examining handwriting legi-
task, such as copying (Amundson, 1992, p. 67). When bility (Roberts & Samuels, 1993). Immediately after
children focus on the mechanical aspect of handwriting, treatment, the students wrote larger in the provided line
they may not be able to fully attend to the content of space for the copy task (1.4 cm high), but initially and at
their work and vice versa (Berninger et al., 1997; Graham 4 mo postintervention, they demonstrated a preference to
& Harris, 2005). The copying phrase was used several write smaller, within half of this space, eliciting size er-
times in this study, and the students may have developed rors. Fewer size errors occurred in the composition task
skill in the formations and joins in this task. The com- in which the line spacing provided for handwriting was
plexity of composition imposes challenges to optimal smaller (0.8 cm high). Studies of space size with
performance of the legibility components, and consider- younger students reported more correct letter strokes
ation should be given to students who have difficulties with wide-spaced paper than with normal-spaced paper
in this area. More time and instructional probes would (Hill, Gladden, Porter, & Cooper, 1982; Waggoner,
be beneficial so that improved legibility could be in- LaNunziata, Hill, & Cooper, 1981). Benbow (1990),
corporated gradually into compositional work. however, found that Grade 4, 5, and 6 students scored
Spacing between words did not improve significantly significantly better on the TOWL writing scale when
on either the copying or the composition tasks, a com- using narrow-ruled paper.
ponent that the Loops and Other Groups program does The speed of writing improved initially with the
not address. Few errors were observed in the spacing- students but did not continue to improve with time, as

Table 3. Parent and Teacher Report of Childs Attitude, Speed, and Legibility of Written Work
Parent Report Mean Score (SD) Teacher Report Mean Score (SD)
Test 1 Test 2 Test 3 Test 4 Test 1 Test 2 Test 3
Characteristic n 5 32 n 5 29 n 5 27 n 5 24 n 5 24 n 5 27 n 5 11
Childs attitude
1. Attitude toward completing written work 4.3 (2.6) 4.4 (2.5) 6.2 (2.1) 6.1 (2.4) 5.0 (2.2) 5.6 (2.5) 6.0 (1.8)
2. Willingness to complete written work 4.5 (2.6) 4.4 (2.6) 6.3 (2.0) 6.4 (2.4) 5.1 (2.4) 5.8 (1.3) 6.0 (1.6)
3. Feelings about handwriting 3.6 (2.3) 6.7 (2.4) 6.4 (2.6) 3.0 (2.2) 6.0 (1.0)
Childs speed
4. Completes written work in a given timeframe 4.3 (2.3) 4.7 (1.8) 6.4 (2.2) 6.1 (2.2) 4.4 (3.0) 4.9 (2.6) 5.8 (3.0)
Childs legibility
5. Writes legibly 4.3 (1.9) 4.4 (2.3) 6.2 (2.0) 6.2 (1.8) 4.2 (2.0) 5.4 (2.2) 5.1 (2.0)
6. Does not erase work 3.4 (2.0) 3.5 (2.2) 4.4 (1.7) 4.3 (1.8) 5.2 (2.3) 4.9 (1.8) 5.2 (1.4)
7. Writes normal-sized letters 4.2 (2.4) 4.6 (2.2) 6.2 (2.0) 6.2 (2.2) 4.4 (2.2) 5.0 (2.1) 5.0 (2.4)
8. Writes on a line without staying above or below the line 4.7 (2.4) 4.8 (2.6) 6.5 (2.2) 6.4 (2.3) 5.0 (2.5) 5.4 (2.0) 4.7 (2.0)
9. Makes spaces between words 6.0 (2.6) 6.1 (2.4) 7.0 (2.1) 7.1 (1.7) 5.5 (2.7) 6.3 (1.7) 5.8 (1.7)
Teacher expectation
10. Student is expected to use cursive handwriting in assignments 3.7 (3.1) 3.5 (2.8)
Notes. Scores range from 0 5 poor response to 10 5 good response; Test 4 teacher report was omitted because of new school year with a new teacher. 5 data
missing because of administrative error.

752 September/October 2010, Volume 64, Number 5


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
would be expected with practice of the skill. Even with the expectations regarding style and use of written output
significant gains made from preintervention to post- needs to be explored more fully. Examination of this
intervention, the students continued to be well below the programs effectiveness when delivered by others, such as
norm for their grade on handwriting speed. The students teachers, would be valuable. Explicit instruction and
had average speeds of 1824 letters per minute, compared practice in the motor aspect of handwriting in the regular
with norms for Grade 4 students of 34 letters per minute, classroom has been recommended (Asher, 2006; Berninger
Grade 5 of 39 letters per minute, and Grade 6 of 56 et al., 1997; Ste.-Marie et al., 2004), including monitoring
letters per minute (Coulter, Pollock, & Lockhart, 1992). of students who present with handwriting challenges
This finding is in keeping with other researchers (Jongmans (Graham et al., 2000). It may be that some handwriting
et al., 2003; Volman, van Schendel, & Jongmans, 2006), challenges would be reduced or eliminated with im-
who found that children with handwriting problems plementation of a program such as Loops and Other
had slower speeds of writing. On the basis of the premise Groups in the classroom. Use of a structured program in
that greater handwriting speed develops with practice the classroom may eliminate the need for referral and in-
(Graham, Berninger, et al., 1998), a period of >4 mo may tervention by occupational therapy for some students.
be required to produce ongoing increases in the speed of Limited resources could then focus on the students who
handwriting. A reason for the lack of progress during the would benefit most from this service (Asher, 2006).
postintervention phase may be that these students were
not expected to practice cursive writing skills in the Limitations
context of classroom work (Asher, 2006). In this study, Several limitations, some inherent in intervention studies
low teacher expectation was reported for written work to with children, may have affected the results of this study.
be completed in cursive handwriting, and the students The study used a small, convenience sample, which may
may not have chosen to practice their newly learned skills. have been biased by teacher and parent interest in the
A higher percentage of Grade 6 students improved in study. Some teachers may have identified their students as
legibility than Grade 4 or 5 students, although this finding having handwriting difficulties because of problems ob-
was not statistically significant, perhaps because of the served in print style. The sample was not geographically
relatively small sample size. Graham, Berninger, et al. representative of all children in Calgary. It is not known
(1998) found that childrens handwriting became more what influence teaching style and instruction in the
legible with advancing elementary grades. The Grade 6 classroom had on the performance of the participants.
students had slower speed of handwriting after the in- Data were incomplete, because not all of the children
tervention. They may have taken more time to demon- attended all of the intervention sessions, some children
strate improvement in the quality of handwriting. were lost to follow-up, and some test forms were not
Weintraub and Graham (1998) found that when children completed adequately for analysis. Hence, given the small
were asked to write quickly, there was a corresponding sample size based on available data at each time point and
decline in legibility; when asked to write neatly, their multiple testing required, caution needs to be taken when
speed of handwriting decreased. interpreting the results.
Personal satisfaction improved in the students after the
intervention program. Moreover, parents and teachers
perceived a noteworthy improvement in the students Conclusions
feelings toward handwriting. The cooperative, small- The objective of this study was to determine whether
group format and the improvements in cursive handwrit- Grade 46 students who participated in a kinesthetic
ing skills may have contributed to this improved attitude writing intervention would improve in legibility, speed,
(Townsend & Hicks, 1997). It is possible that the students and their personal satisfaction with handwriting. The
reported improved attitudes toward handwriting because group of students recruited was primarily boys with
they knew that they were not alone in facing their hand- handwriting challenges. Global legibility and components
writing challenges (Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997). of legibility improved, as well as speed of handwriting
and personal satisfaction with handwriting. The Loops
Future Research and Other Groups program appears to have had an im-
Further research is needed to determine the intervention pact on developing handwriting skills with these students,
programor combination of intervention components particularly in components that have been reported to be
and timing of the intervention that is most effective related to kinesthetic feedback, with better performance
(Asher, 2006; Berninger et al., 1997). Teacher and student on copying tasks and with narrow-ruled paper. This

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 753


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
program can be used and recommended by occupational Feder, K. P., & Majnemer, A. (2007). Handwriting develop-
therapists with greater confidence, with the provision of ment, competency, and intervention. Developmental Med-
supplementary instruction in word spacing. Further study icine and Child Neurology, 49, 312317. doi:10.1111/
j.1469-8749.2007.00312.x
is required to confirm these findings. In addition, further
Feder, K. P., Majnemer, A., & Synnes, A. (2000). Handwrit-
evaluation of teacher expectations for cursive handwriting, ing: Current trends in occupational therapy practice. Ca-
the ongoing integration of skills into daily compositional nadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 76, 197204.
tasks, and the use of this program in the classroom would Fisher, R. A. (1922). On the interpretation of x2 from contin-
be beneficial. s gency tables, and the calculation of P. Journal of the Royal
Statistical Society, 85(1), 8794.
Goldberg, E., & Simner, M. (1999). A comparison of child-
Acknowledgments rens handwriting under traditional versus whole language
The study team acknowledges the children, parents, and instruction. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 14,
teachers who participated in the study. We thank the 1130. doi:10.1177/082957359901400203
Decision Support Research Team, particularly Suzanne Graham, S., Berninger, V., Weintraub, N., & Schafer, W.
(1998). Development of handwriting speed and legibility
Tough, Brenda Wilson, and Karen Tofflemire; intervention
in Grades 19. Journal of Educational Research, 92, 4252.
teams (Aynsley Wennberg, Chandra Kipfer); research staff doi:10.1080/00220679809597574
(Brigitte Roy, Gina Blumes); and leadership support (Lori Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (2005). Improving the writing
Craig, Darlene Winder, Jane Pollack). Financial support of performance of young struggling writers: Theoretical and
the Alberta Childrens Hospital Foundation and the Oc- programmatic research from the Center on Accelerating
cupational Therapy Service and Regional School Health Student Learning. Journal of Special Education, 39, 1933.
doi:10.1177/00224669050390010301
Program of Alberta Health Services is acknowledged.
Graham, S., Harris, K. R., & Fink, B. (2000). Is handwriting
causally related to learning to write? Treatment of hand-
References writing problems in beginning writers. Journal of Educa-
Agresti, A. (1992). A survey of exact inference for contingency tional Psychology, 92, 620633. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.
tables. Statistical Science, 7, 131153. 92.4.620
Alberta Childrens Hospital. (2001). Student Inventory, Calgary, Graham, S., Weintraub, N., & Berninger, V. W. (1998). The
Alberta: Author, Occupational Therapy Department. relationship between handwriting style and speed and legi-
Amundson, S. (1992). Handwriting: Evaluation and intervention bility. Journal of Educational Research, 91, 290296. doi:
in school settings. In J. Case-Smith & C. Pehoski (Eds.), 10.1080/00220679809597556
Development of hand skills in the child (pp. 6378). Rockville, Hammill, D. D., & Larsen, S. C. (1983). Test of Written
MD: American Occupational Therapy Association. Language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Asher, A. V. (2006). Handwriting instruction in elementary Hill, D. S., Gladden, M. A., Porter, J. T., & Cooper, J. O.
schools. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 60, (1982). Variables affecting transition from wide-spaced to
461471. normal-spaced paper for manuscript handwriting. Journal
Barnett, A. (2005). Handwriting: Its assessment and role in the of Educational Research, 76, 5053.
diagnosis of developmental coordination disorder (LEEDS Jongmans, M. J., Linthorst-Bakker, E., Westenberg, Y., &
Consensus Statement [2006]: ESRC Seminar Series). Re- Smits-Engelsman, B. (2003). Use of a task-oriented self-
trieved March 24, 2010, from www.dcd-uk.org/seminar2. instruction method to support children in primary school
html with poor handwriting quality and speed. Human Movement
Benbow, M. (1990). Loops and other groups: A kinesthetic writ- Science, 22, 549566. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2003.09.009
ing system. Tuscon, AZ: Therapy Skill Builders. Karlsdottir, R. (1996). Development of cursive handwriting.
Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbot, R., Abbot, S., Rogan, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 82, 659673.
L., Brooks, A., et al. (1997). Treatment of handwriting Laszlo, J. I., & Broderick, P. (1991). Drawing and handwriting
problems in beginning writers: Transfer from handwriting difficulties: Reasons for and remediation of dysfunction.
to composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, In J. Wann, A. M. Wing, & N. Sovik (Eds.), Development
652666. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.89.4.652 of graphic skills: Research, perspectives and educational im-
Bonney, M. (1992). Understanding and assessing handwriting plications (pp. 259280). London: Academic Press.
difficulty: Perspectives from the literature. Australian Oc- Malloy-Miller, T. (1985). An examination of handwriting prob-
cupational Therapy Journal, 39, 715. lem subtypes. Unpublished masters thesis, University of
Case-Smith, J. (2002). Effectiveness of school-based occupa- Calgary, Alberta, Canada.
tional therapy intervention on handwriting. American Malloy-Miller, T., Polatajko, H., & Ansett, B. (1995). Hand-
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 56, 1725. writing error patterns of children with mild motor diffi-
Coulter, J., Pollock, N., & Lockhart, J. (1992). Resource book culties. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 62,
on handwriting assessment in primary and junior school-aged 258267.
children. Hamilton, Ontario: Chedoke-McMaster Hos- Missiuna, C., Pollack, N., Egan, M., DeLaat, D., Gaines, R.,
pitals Occupational Therapy & McMaster University. & Soucie, H. (2008). Enabling occupation through

754 September/October 2010, Volume 64, Number 5


Downloaded From: http://ajot.aota.org/ on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms
facilitating the diagnosis of developmental coordination handwriting performance in first-grade students. American
disorder. Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 75, Journal of Occupational Therapy, 56, 2633.
2634. Sweedler-Brown, C. O. (1992). The effect of training on the
Mueller, D. J. (1986). Measuring social attitudes. New York: appearance bias of holistic essay graders. Journal of Re-
Teachers College Press. search and Development in Education, 26, 2429.
Peterson, C. Q., & Nelson, D. L. (2003). Effect of an occu- Townsend, M. A. R., & Hicks, L. (1997). Classroom goal
pational intervention on printing in children with eco- structures, social satisfaction, and the perceived value of
nomic disadvantages. American Journal of Occupational academic tasks. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
Therapy, 57, 152160. 67, 112.
Piek, J. P., Baynam, G. B., & Barrett, N. C. (2006). The Volman, M. J. M., van Schendel, B. M., & Jongmans, M. J.
relationship between fine and gross motor ability, self- (2006). Handwriting difficulties in primary school chil-
perceptions, and self-worth in children and adolescents. dren: A search for underlying mechanisms. American Jour-
Human Movement Science, 25, 6575. doi:10.1016/j. nal of Occupational Therapy, 60, 451460.
humov.2005.10.011 Waggoner, J., LaNunziata, L. J., Hill, D. S., & Cooper, J. O.
Roberts, G., & Samuels, M. (1993). Handwriting remediation: (1981). Space, size, and accuracy of kindergarten and first
A comparison of computer-based and traditional ap- grade students manuscript handwriting. Journal of Educa-
proaches. Journal of Educational Research, 87, 118125. tional Research, 74, 182184.
Shapiro, S. S., & Wilk, M. B. (1965). An analysis of variance Weintraub, N., & Graham, S. (1998). Writing legibly and
test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika, 52(3 quickly: A study of childrens ability to adjust their hand-
4), 591611. writing to meet common classroom demands. Learning
Sovik, N. (1975). Developmental cybernetics of handwriting and Disabilities Research and Practice, 13, 146152.
graphic behavior. Oslo, Norway: Universite-forlaget. Wentzel, K. R., & Caldwell, K. (1997). Friendships, peer ac-
Ste.-Marie, D., Clark, S., Findlay, L., & Latimer, A. (2004). High ceptance and group membership: Relations to academic
levels of contextual interference enhance handwriting skills achievement in middle school. Child Development, 68,
acquisition. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 36(1), 115126. 11981209.
Sudsawad, P., Trombly, C., Henderson, A., & Tickle-Degnen, Wilcoxon, F. (1945). Individual comparisons by ranking meth-
L. (2002). Testing the effect of kinesthetic training on ods. Biometrics, 1, 8083.

The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 755


DownloadedViewFrom:
publicationhttp://ajot.aota.org/
stats on 08/28/2015 Terms of Use: http://AOTA.org/terms

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen