Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Social Neuroscience

ISSN: 1747-0919 (Print) 1747-0927 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/psns20

Left hemisphere EEG coherence in infancy


predicts infant declarative pointing and preschool
epistemic language

N. Khn-Popp, S. Kristen, M. Paulus, J. Meinhardt & B. Sodian

To cite this article: N. Khn-Popp, S. Kristen, M. Paulus, J. Meinhardt & B. Sodian (2016) Left
hemisphere EEG coherence in infancy predicts infant declarative pointing and preschool
epistemic language, Social Neuroscience, 11:1, 49-59, DOI: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1024887

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1024887

Published online: 02 Apr 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 57

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=psns20

Download by: [LMU Muenchen] Date: 12 February 2016, At: 03:06


SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2016
Vol. 11, No. 1, 4959, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1024887

Left hemisphere EEG coherence in infancy predicts


infant declarative pointing and preschool epistemic
language

N. Khn-Popp, S. Kristen, M. Paulus, J. Meinhardt, and B. Sodian

Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Munich, Germany


Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

Pointing plays a central role in preverbal communication. While imperative pointing aims at inuencing another
persons behavior, declarative gestures serve to convey epistemic information and to share interest in an object.
Further, the latter are hypothesized to be a precursor ability of epistemic language. So far, little is known about
their underlying brain maturation processes. Therefore, the present study investigated the relation between brain
maturation processes and the production of imperative and declarative motives as well as epistemic language in
N = 32 infants. EEG coherence scores were measured at 14 months, imperative and declarative point production at
15 months and epistemic language at 48 months. Results of correlational analyses suggest distinct behavioral and
neural patterns for imperative and declarative pointing, with declarative pointing being associated with the
maturation of the left hemisphere. Further, EEG coherence measures of the left hemisphere at 14 months and
declarative pointing at 15 months are related to individual differences in epistemic language skills at 48 months,
independently of child IQ. In regression analyses, coherence measures of the left hemisphere prove to be the most
important predictor of epistemic language skills. Thus, neural processes of the left hemisphere seem particularly
relevant to social communication.

Keywords: Declarative and imperative point production; Epistemic language; EEG; Infancy.

Pointing is an important form of preverbal commu- to share the interesting event and thus point to it,
nication in infancy (Aureli, Perucchini, & Genco, hence, rstly, in order to direct the mothers attention
2009; Moore & DEntremont, 2001; Tomasello, to the tree and secondly, in order to share the joy
Carpenter, & Liszkowski, 2007). Two main underly- about the lights the child points at the tree. It has
ing motives have been distinguished: an imperative been argued that in declarative pointing, infants impli-
and a declarative one. While imperative pointing aims citly represent and manipulate others intentional and
at inuencing another persons behavior (typically to epistemic states, and therefore declarative, but not
make an adult hand the infant an object), declarative imperative pointing, is predicted to be a developmen-
pointing serves the transmission of information from tal precursor of a Theory of Mind (i.e. the ability to
the sender to the recipient when sharing interesting comprehend others internal states such as intentions)
events. For instance, there is something interesting (Camaioni, Perucchini, Bellagamba, & Colonnesi,
going on behind the mothers back (e.g., Christmas 2004). This view is supported by longitudinal ndings
lights on a tree in town square) and the toddler wants indicating that the production of declarative pointing

Correspondence should be addressed to: Nina Khn-Popp, Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilian University, Leopoldstrasse 13,
Munich 80802, Germany. E-mail: kuehn-popp@psy.lmu.de
Special thanks go to Sabrina Krimmel, Hannah Perst, the Babylab and the EEG-lab team for support in data acquisition and coding as well
as all families who participated in this study.
No potential conict of interest was reported by the authors.
The work was supported by a DFG grant So 213/27-1 to Beate Sodian.

2015 Taylor & Francis


50 KHN-POPP ET AL.

gestures was linked to understanding another persons distinct electrical signatures and activation patterns of
intention in an imitation task (Camaioni et al., 2004) the developing brain (De Haan, 2007). Therefore,
and that declarative joint attention at 20 months was developmental science has shown great interest in
predictive of Theory of Mind at 44 months (Charman the neural underpinnings of early non-verbal commu-
et al., 2000). Moreover, 12-month-olds who pointed nicative abilities such as attention or affect sharing,
more frequently to share information with an unfami- pointing comprehension or point following
liar tester and who followed pointing gestures more (Gredebck, Melinder, & Daum, 2010; Grossman &
consistently had higher social competence scores at Johnson, 2010; Henderson, Yoder, Yale, & McDufe,
30 months than their peers (Vaughan Van Hecke et al., 2002; Hoehl, Palumbo, Heinisch, & Striano, 2008;
2007). Mundy, Card, & Fox, 2000; Striano, Reid, & Hoehl,
According to socio-cognitive accounts, pointing ges- 2006). However, the neural underpinnings of the dis-
tures serve as precursor abilities to language develop- tinction between declarative and imperative pointing
ment (Bates, Camaioni, & Volterra, 1975; Brooks & have not been specically addressed yet. Nor has the
Meltzoff, 2008; Colonnesi, Stams, Koster, & Noom, neural relationship between the preverbal expression
2010). In autistic children, who are selectively impaired of declarative motives and more explicit, epistemic
in declarative but not in imperative skills (Camaioni, language development been investigated.
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

Perucchini, Muratori, Parrini, & Cesari, 2003), these In a classical neural study on early communicative
decits in declarative gestural communication were behavior, Mundy et al. (2000) investigated the neural
related to language development (Mundy, Sigman, & substrates of initiating joint attention versus initiating
Kasari, 1994). More specically, there is evidence for a behavioral requests. They assessed initiative declara-
link between declarative pointing and epistemic state tive behavior through all, gestures, gaze and vocaliza-
terms (e.g., knowing, thinking, and guessing). tions, thus encompassing affect sharing as well as
Epistemic (or cognitive state) language refers to thought joint engagement. They used electroencephalogram
processes within a person (cf. Bretherton & Beeghly, (EEG) power to indicate brain electrical activity and
1982). While related to general language development EEG coherence to measure the degree of brain inte-
(Kristen, Sodian, Licata, Thoermer, & PoulinDubois, gration processes. Results showed that left frontal
2012), epistemic state language has precursor abilities cortical activity at 14 months was associated with
and correlates that are independent of general language. the tendency to initiate action as to share an experi-
For instance, infants non-verbal ability to respond to ence of a toy with another person. Further, relatively
intentional gestures, as well as to manipulate others greater left fronto-central EEG coherence at 14 months
attentional states and knowledge states via declarative was predictive of initiating behavior at 18 months as
pointing gestures, seems to be related to their later ability well as with requestive behavior (i.e. pointing, reach-
to verbally express the same epistemic concepts (e.g., to ing, or using eye contact to elicit aid in obtaining an
know). The comprehension of referential pointing at object) at 14 months. Using the same behavioral joint
9 months, which turned out to be the single most impor- attention tasks and EEG procedure, Mundy, Fox, and
tant predictor in a subsequent regression analysis, and Card (2003) showed also that left-hemispheric pro-
declarative point production skills at 12 months, but not cesses at 14 months, related to initiating behavior at
imperative point production skills, were found to be 14 months, predict growth in language development at
correlates of cognitive state language at 36 months, 24 months.
independently of general language skills (Kristen, Henderson et al. (2002) employed the same tasks
Sodian, Thoermer, & Perst, 2011). Since epistemic pro- as Mundy et al. (2000), but they specied the beha-
cesses are intangible and do not always have behavioral vioral form by dening pointing as an extended index
correlates, talking about them reects childrens ability nger toward a referent. They reported cortical activ-
to take another persons mental perspective. ity in bilateral frontal regions at 14 months to be
Consistently, research shows that 30-month-olds visual correlated with initiative joint attention at 18 months,
perspective taking-ability, which can be seen as an but not with initiative behavior request. These appar-
important precursor skill of later mental state compre- ently divergent neural ndings might be related to
hension, is concurrently related to childrens references rather loose or different denitions of the communi-
to inner, intangible states (Chiarella, Kristen, Poulin- cative gestures. Homogeneous denitions and con-
Dubois, & Sodian, 2013). Further, infants mental state trols, though, seem critical to clearly separating
references predict their later Theory of Mind (Olineck & imperative and declarative motives (Cochet &
Poulin-Dubois, 2007). Vauclair, 2010).
The distinction between different types of early Initiating joint attentionbehaviors may be dri-
communicative motives is assumed to be reected in ven by a variety of motives, such as sharing a positive
EPISTEMIC COMMUNICATION IN INFANCY 51

affect, a novel experience or conveying information Increased coherence values suggest that cortical
about an object or event. Since the focal objects are regions are intricately connected and working
typically in the visual eld shared by the experimenter together. When applied in the developmental context,
and the child in the tasks used by Mundy et al., it is coherence measures are assumed to be markers of
hard to isolate the epistemic aspect of joint attention neural maturation and organization processes that cor-
from other dimensions such as affect sharing. The respond to the emergence of skills such as early com-
epistemic understanding underlying declarative point- municative abilities.
ing was specically addressed by Camaioni et al.
(2004), who required the infant to use the pointing
gesture to draw the experimenters attention to a target MATERIALS AND METHODS
that was behind her back. Twelve- and 15-month-old
infants passed this task (Camaioni et al., 2004; Kristen Participants
et al., 2011), thus indicating a sensitivity to the experi-
menters ignorance of an event taking place behind her The nal sample consisted of 32 infants (17 female).
back. Since research on the neural correlates of Theory Childrens mean age at the EEG measurement
of Mind reasoning indicates a relationship between left- appointment was 14 months (M = 13.99 months;
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

side activity and epistemic reasoning in adults (Liu, range = 13.4014.43 months), at the pointing mea-
Meltzoff, & Wellman, 2009; Meinhardt, Khn-Popp, surement appointment 15 months (M = 15.04 months;
Sommer, & Sodian, 2012; Sabbagh & Taylor, 2000) as range = 14.2315.50 months), and at the language
well as in school-aged children (Khn-Popp, Sodian, measurement appointment 48 months
Sommer, Dhnel, & Meinhardt, 2013; Liu, Sabbagh, (M = 48.28 months; range = 48.0349.33 months).
Gehring, & Wellman, 2009; Meinhardt, Sodian, Eleven additional infants were tested in the EEG study
Thoermer, Dhnel, & Sommer, 2011), we hypothesize but not included in the nal sample due to fussiness
that declarative point production in infancy may speci- (i.e. the infant was too distressed as to start the experi-
cally be associated with brain maturation processes in ment, N = 6; or the experiment had to be interrupted,
the left hemisphere. Further, we also expect epistemic N = 5). The children were a subsample part of a larger
language development to be subserved by left- sample who participated in a larger study (Licata
hemispheric processes. et al., 2014; Paulus, Khn-Popp, Licata, Sodian, &
To test these hypotheses, we used the point produc- Meinhardt, 2013).
tion tasks by Camaioni et al. (2004) to clearly separate
imperative and declarative motives. We also measured
epistemic language competence at 48 months of age, Behavioral assessment: Pointing
an age at which children refer to epistemic states quite measures (based on Camaioni et al.,
frequently when narrating stories and at which mental 2004)
state utterances should be evidential of their develop-
ing mental state comprehension (Colonnesi et al., The order of the tasks was kept constant across con-
2010). ditions. First, children received imperative point pro-
Given that the production of pointing gestures duction and then, declarative point production. The
causes active motor movements, neural activity may table where infants were seated at was rectangular and
be masked, thus making it difcult to clearly distin- measured 78 cm in height, 120 cm in width, and
guish the communicative intent. Therefore, neural 78 cm in depth. E made eye contact with children to
activity has been recorded during a passive, but get their attention when the trial began.
awake, state, which is then correlated with the point-
ing behavior and language performance respectively Declarative point production task
assessed in an extra sessions. Coherence measures,
based on resting state EEG measures, are assumed to In this task, the child had to infer from E not seeing
be markers of the link between brain maturation pro- the object that he or she, in contrast to himself or
cesses and socio-cognitive abilities at this age (Bell, herself, also cannot know about it and thus, to share
2012; Cuevas, Bell, Marcovitch, & Calkins, 2012; his or her interest on the object the child needs to
Mundy et al., 2000). EEG coherence is the fre- point at the object. In two trials, a second assistant
quency-dependent squared cross-correlation between (hidden from the child) activated a mobile (pig) hang-
two scalp electrode sites and is a measure for the ing from the wall opposite the child and thus, behind
degree of phase synchrony or shared activity between Es back. The mobile moved for about 15 seconds
spatially separated EEG generators (Nunez, 1981). during which the experimenter looked silently at the
52 KHN-POPP ET AL.

child. When the mobile stopped, E uttered: What gestures (as indicated by a whole-hand point and
happened? without turning back to look at the repeated opening of the hand) at the stimulus either
mobile. E then looked silently at the child for 15 sec- before or after the verbal trigger in addition to the
onds (waiting for the childs reaction). Finally, he following target behaviors: leaned forward or reached
looked at the mobile and named it. for the stimulus, produced a request vocalization/
proto-word, or whined. Again, if children merely
Imperative production task reached for the stimulus or simply looked at it, they
were rated as non-competent.
In two trials, E activated a windup car (toy bear), For each score and joint attention task, 100% of the
moved it in front of the child for about 10 seconds and children were scored by an independent observer and
then stopped it and looked silently at the child for Cohens k was 1.00 for all scores for both declarative
15 seconds (waiting for the childs reaction). E then and imperative point production.
said, Isnt it nice? Do you like it? and looked
silently at the child for another 15 seconds (waiting Epistemic language and IQ measures
for the childs reaction). After that, the toy was given
to the child. In this task, the child had to indicate that When children were 48 months of age, to test their
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

he or she wants to obtain the object with a pointing narrative skills children were asked to tell a story
gesture. about a bear family in 4 phases (Bornstein, Hahn, &
Haynes, 2004). First the child could freely play with
Coding the bear toy gurines. Then, after 5 minutes the
assessor prompted the child to tell a story and again,
Childrens declarative communicative competency after 5 minutes the child was prompted with the
(yes [score = 1, competent] versus no [score = 0, non- question What is happening next in the story?.
competent]) was coded as follows: First, it was impor- After another 5 minutes, the assessors were instructed
tant that children did point to inform the experimenter to ask How does the story end?. After another
about the object and not point for themselves or point 15 minutes, the narrative was ended. In this study,
to simply obtain the object. Hence, pointing gestures childrens use of epistemic state terms frame in regard
had to be related to both, E and the object behind Es to the story characters, within a xed time frame, was
back. Thus, children were only rated as competent coded using a coding-scheme for mental state terms
when looks at Es face occurred within 2 seconds (Bretherton & Beeghly, 1982). Thereby, terms such as
before or after the pointing gesture or when looks at think, know, guess, believe were coded as episte-
E and pointing gestures occurred simultaneously. mic. Note that in order to control effects of general
Further, to be rated as communicative competent chil- verbosity, only the propositions containing different
dren had to produce a pointing gesture before or after types or usages of epistemic state terms were counted,
the verbal trigger in addition to the following target but not the mere quantity of propositions containing
behaviors: smiled and/or vocalized toward the stimu- epistemic state terms. Further, childrens overall verb-
lus, produced a proto-word or a word relative to the osity was controlled for in partial correlations.
stimulus, or re-enacted what the stimulus did. One hundred percent of transcripts were coded by a
Pointing was dened as an arm and index-nger second observer. Cohens k was .98 for epistemic state
extension toward the stimulus with the remaining terms. The verbal and non-verbal IQ at 48 months
ngers curled tightly or lightly under the hand were assessed with the respective scales of the
(Franco & Butterworth, 1996). If children pointed German version of the WPPSI (Petermann, 2009).
for themselves (without attending to the experimen-
ter), reached for the stimulus or merely looked at the
stimulus, they were rated as non-competent. Electrophysiological assessment
Childrens imperative pointing competency (yes
[score = 1, competent] versus no [score = 0, non- EEG recording
competent]) was coded as follows: Children were
rated as competent when looks at Es face occurred During the recording of brain electrical activity
within 2 seconds before or after the pointing gesture (EEG), infants sat quietly on their mothers lap and
or when looks at E and pointing gestures occurred were presented with brightly colored bubbles on a
simultaneously. Further, children were coded as com- computer screen. This kind of stimuli resembles to
municative competent when, in both trials, they others used in EEG resting state investigations
pointed at the stimulus while producing request-like (Mundy et al., 2000; Paulus, Khn-Popp, et al., 2013)
EPISTEMIC COMMUNICATION IN INFANCY 53

FPZ
FP1 FP2

AF7 AF8
AF3 GN AF4
F9 F10
F7 F8
F5 F3 F4 F6
F1 FZ F2

FT7 FT8
FC5 FC3 FC1 FC2 FC4 FC6
FCZ

T7 C5 C3 C1 CZ C2 C4 C6 T8
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

CP3 CP1 CPZ CP2 CP4 CP6


CP5
TP7 TP8
TP9 TP1
P3 P1 PZ P2 P4
P5 P6
P7 P8
PO3 POZ PO4
PO7 PO8
O1 O2
OZ

Figure 1. Electrode layout of the EEG baseline measurement. Gray colored and striped electrodes identify the electrodes inserted in the
analyses of the coherence scores.

and was used to keep the infants visual attention with The electrical activity from each lead was ampli-
reduced movement during the recording time. The ed using BrainAmp amplier (Brain Products,
recording lasted for at least 3 minutes until the child Gilching, Germany) and bandpassed from 0.016 to
lost interest in the stimulus as evidenced by yawning, 100 Hz. Activity for each electrode was displayed
crying, or strong motor activity. on the monitor of an acquisition computer. The EEG
Recordings were made from 17 sites (Fp1, Fp2, F3, signal was digitized online at a continuous sampling
F4, F7, F8, F9, F10, C3, Cz, C4, T7, T8, P3, P4, O1, rate of 500 Hz, i.e. the data were not affected by
O2) using an infant-size cap with Ag/AgCl active aliasing.
electrodes (ActiCap, Brain Products, Germany)
with a layout following the 10/20 system (see EEG analysis
Figure 1 for an overview). F3, F4, F7, F8, C3, Cz,
C4, T7, T8, P3, P4, O1, and O2 are electrode pairs EEG data were examined and analyzed using
commonly employed in EEG resting state research Vision Analyzer Software (Brain Products,
(Cuevas et al., 2012; Mundy et al., 2000; Paulus, Germany). Ofine, all electrodes were re-referenced
Khn-Popp, et al., 2013). Fp1 and Fp2 were inserted to an average reference, and a digital band pass lter
to allow for the detection of blinks and vertical eye- of 0.1 to 20 Hz was applied. The EEG data were
movements; F9 and F10 were included to detect hor- segmented into equal sized epochs of 1024 ms with
izontal eye-movements. All electrode sites were refer- 50% overlap. To assure that EEG was only analyzed
enced to Cz during recording. Prior to the EEG from epochs in which the infants were attending to the
acquisition, electrode impedances were measured baseline stimulus, epochs with artifacts were detected
and accepted if they were below 10 k. by means of visual inspection and a semiautomatic
54 KHN-POPP ET AL.

artifact rejection function (amplitude exceeding RESULTS


120 V, minimal-maximal difference 100 V) and
were eliminated from further analyses if they con- Preliminary results
tained eye movements, blinks, or (motor) artifacts.
On average 44.7% of all epochs were excluded from For descriptives see Table 1. Note that, in order to
subsequent analyses, providing on average 294 include and exclude control variables systematically,
(SD = 111.8) epochs per infant. throughout the whole paper Baron and Kennys
The data then were analyzed using fast Fourier (1986) and Holmbecks (1997) mediation models
transformation (Hanning window: 10%, frequency were applied (see also Kochanska, 2002). Thus, con-
resolution: 0.977 Hz). Several coherence pairings trol variables were considered as possible mediators
were possible. However, the present study focused when they were correlated with the outcome related to
on coherence measures that had previously been asso- the independent variable and when they themselves
ciated with joint attention skills. Thus, in line with were related to the independent variable (see reported
Mundy et al. (2000), we calculated summary coher- correlations below).
ence scores for each hemisphere reecting the ratio of Both gender and age proved to be unrelated to the
short-distance coherence divided by long-distance study variables analyses (gender: all ps > .12; age: all
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

coherence (frontal-central coherence/frontal-occipital ps > .11). Both childrens scores for the verbal IQ
coherence, i.e. F7-T7/F7-01, F3-C3/F3-P3, F8-T8/ (N = 22, M = 107.14, SD = 12.71, range = 86137)
F8-02, F4-C4/F4-P4). These coherence scores and their non-verbal IQ (N = 22, M = 98.64,
included electrode combinations along the vertical SD = 10.37, range = 75123) lay in the normal
lines, i.e. for the left hemisphere F7-T7-O1 and F3- range. While marginally signicantly related to each
C3-P3, and for the right hemisphere F4-C4-P4 and other (N = 22, r = .36, p = .05), only childrens non-
F8-T8-O2, respectively (see Figure 1). Coherence was verbal IQ was related to the number of utterances
computed for the 69 Hz frequency band using coher- referring to others epistemic states when narrating a
ence correlation as implemented in the Vision story (N = 20, r = .55, p = .01). Further, childrens
Analyzer. This alpha-range band is useful from the non-verbal IQ was related to childrens left-coherence
end of the rst year of life across all scalp regions scores (F3-C3/F3-P3: N = 23, r = .43, p < .05; F3-C3/
(Saby & Marshall, 2012). Previous studies have F3-01: N = 23, r = .52, p < .05), as well as to right-
shown that EEG values in this spectrum are related coherence scores (F4-C4/F4-O2: N = 23, r = .47,
to socio-cognitive behavior and cognitive develop- p < .05). Further, childrens verbal IQ was negatively
ment (Cuevas et al., 2012; Henderson et al., 2002; related to children pointing imperatively (N = 22, r =
Paulus, Hunnius, & Bekkering, 2013). -.40, p < .05). No other signicant relations occurred.

TABLE 1
Descriptives of study variables
Measures M SD Range/scores

Imperative pointing (N = 31) 0^ = 39% 0/1


1* = 61%
Epistemic declarative pointing (N = 31) 0^ = 55% 0/1
1* = 45%
Variety of epistemic terms (sum) (N = 21) 2.3 0.58 03
Verbosity (number of words) (N = 21) 198.14 161.48 10568
EEG right-coherence measures (N = 32)
F4-C4/F4-P4 3.66 6.87 .0426.58
F4-C4/F4-O2 1.64 2.60 .0412.30
F8-T8/F8-O2 2.43 5.86 .0925.93
EEG left-coherence measures (N = 32)
F3-C3/F3-P3 8.69 23.57 .0692.24
F3-C3/F3-O1 3.79 11.80 .0958.59
F7-T7/F7-O1 2.57 8.41 .0847.58
Verbal IQ (N = 22) 107.15 12.71 86137
Non-verbal IQ (N = 23) 98.61 10.13 75123

Notes: ^0 = no competence, *1 = competence.


EPISTEMIC COMMUNICATION IN INFANCY 55

Behavioral results TABLE 3


Correlations of the EEG coherence scores at 14 months,
imperative (IP) and epistemic declarative (DP) pointing scores
On a descriptive level, the dichotomous competence at 15 months and epistemic language at 48 months, control-
scores of imperative and epistemic declarative point- ling for verbosity
ing (see Table 1) proved to be unrelated (Phi = .05, Epistemic language
p = .76) and did not differ signicantly from each
other (2(1) = .10, p = .76; n = 31). Right-coherence scores
F4-C4/F4-P4 rpartial = .35^ p < .07
F4-C4/F4-O2 rpartial = .36^ p < .06
F8-T8/F8-O2 rpartial = .04
Relations between EEG patterns and Left-coherence scores
behavioral tasks F3-C3/F3-P3 rpartial = .47* p < .02
F3-C3/F3-O1 rpartial = .59** p < .003
F7-T7/F7-O1 rpartial = .17
The present studys aim was to investigate the relation Pointing scores
between hemispheric coherence patterns, infants IP competence rpartial = .16
communicative pointing behavior and epistemic lan- DP competence rpartial = .44* p < .02
guage. Given that we expected epistemic declarative
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

pointing and epistemic language to be associated with Notes: **p .01, *p .05, ^p .10 (one-tailed); N = 18.
left-lateralized brain maturation processes, we con-
ducted one-tailed tests.
To this end, point-biserial Pearson product correla- TABLE 4
Linear regression analysis predicting epistemic language at
tions were calculated between left and right ratio EEG
48 months (predictors based on mediation models by Baron
coherence scores and competency pointing scores (see and Kenny (1986) and Holmbeck (1997))
Table 2). Most importantly, epistemic declarative
Predictor variables t p
pointing, but not imperative pointing, was signi-
cantly correlated with left hemisphere coherence DP competence .30 1.63 .13
scores (see Table 2). Right-coherence scores
F4-C4/F4-P4 .08 .43 .67
We also computed partial point-biserial Pearson
F4-C4/F4-O2 .18 .64 .54
product correlations for left and right ratio EEG coher- Left-coherence scores
ence scores and epistemic language scores, controlling F3-C3/F3-P3 .31 .92 .37
for verbosity (see Table 3). As expected, the correla- F3-C3/F3-O1 .92* p < .05 2.18 .05
tions of the EEG data with language revealed that the Non-verbal IQ .23 1.15 .27
14-month ratio measure of left hemisphere coherence
Notes: *p < .05. DP = epistemic declarative pointing. df = 19.
was signicantly correlated with epistemic language
at 48 months, while right hemisphere coherence
scores were marginally signicantly related to episte-
mic language. Finally, epistemic declarative pointing are the most important predictor of epistemic state
was related to epistemic language (see Table 3). As talk at 48 months (see Table 4). While the overall
the regression analyses show, left-coherence scores model, F(6, 13) = 4.29, p = .01, was found to explain
66% of variance, left hemisphere coherence scores
proved to be the only independent predictor. Further,
TABLE 2 imperative pointing scores do not mediate the relation
Correlations of the EEG coherence scores with imperative (IP) between declarative pointing and epistemic language
and epistemic declarative (DP) pointing scores
(see Table 5).
IP competence DP competence

Right-coherence scores
F4-C4/F4-P4 rpbi = .144 rpbi = .07
F4-C4/F4-O2 rpartial = .08 rpbi = .11 DISCUSSION
(N = 20)
F8-T8/F8-O2 rpbi = .08 rpbi = .13 The goals of this study were to reveal the brain
Left-coherence scores maturation processes underlying the production of
F3-C3/F3-P3 rpbi = .10 rpbi = .34* p < .03
imperative and epistemic declarative pointing gestures
F3-C3/F3-O1 rpbi = .18 rpbi = .30^ p < .05
F7-T7/F7-O1 rpbi = .12 rpbi = .26^ p < .08 in infants as young as 15 months and their relation to
epistemic language at 48 months of age. To this end,
Notes: *p .05 (one-tailed), ^p .10 (one-tailed); N = 31. we measured baseline EEG at 14 months and
56 KHN-POPP ET AL.

TABLE 5 hemispheres are equally recruited (Henderson et al.,


Linear regression analysis predicting epistemic state language 2002). This inconsistency may be due to the fact that
at 48 months
single aspects of pointing behavior have not been
Predictor variables t p controlled for, such as the type of gesture and the
Epistemic declarative pointing (15 months) .40 2.21 .04 communicative setting. This control, though, is criti-
Imperative pointing (15 months) .07 .39 .67 cal to isolating the epistemic dimension of the declara-
F4-C4/F4-P4 .37 1.96 .07 tive motive from other communicative motives such
Non-verbal IQ .49 2.73 .02 as affect sharing (Camaioni et al., 2004; Henderson
et al., 2002). In the present study, we chose a stricter
Notes: F(4, 16) = 4.09, p = .018; overall variance explained by
model: 50.6%. criterion for the presence of a declarative motive (in
the sense of sharing information) (cf. Liszkowski,
Carpenter, Striano, & Tomasello, 2006).
independently assessed point production competen- Accordingly, to be considered as competent, the infant
cies as well as epistemic language skills. Results had to aim at directing the experimenters attention to
suggest distinct behavioral and neural patterns for the mobile that was behind her back, requiring some
declarative and imperative pointing, with epistemic sensitivity to the experimenters ignorance of this
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

declarative pointing being particularly associated object. Therefore, the present results suggest that
with the maturation of the left hemisphere. Further, brain integration processes of the left hemisphere
epistemic declarative point production at 15 months may be specically relevant for the understanding of
and EEG coherence measures of the left hemisphere at the epistemic dimension of declarative pointing.
14 months predict individual differences in epistemic Intriguingly, previous neural investigations have
language at 48 months, independently of child IQ. reported a strong relationship between Theory of
Previous behavioral research has indicated distinct Mind reasoning and left-lateralized processes. For
developmental patterns for imperative and declarative instance, pretense reasoning, a precursor of a Theory
point production with imperative point production of Mind, is linked to left posterior and left frontal
arising earlier in development (Behne, Liszkowski, activity in adults (Meinhardt et al., 2012; Sabbagh &
Carpenter, & Tomasello, 2012; Kristen et al., 2011). Taylor, 2000) as well as in 6- to 8-year-old children
The present study supports these ndings by showing (Khn-Popp et al., 2013). Pretense requires the ability
a higher competence level for imperative point pro- to understand that another person has the same knowl-
duction than for epistemic declarative pointing at edge of reality as oneself (e.g., both see the rabbit
15 months of age. This dcalage may be related to jump out of the cage, thus the cage is empty), but the
differences in socio-cognitive demands: whereas other consciously behaves counterfactually on it (e.g.,
imperative pointing simply aims at inuencing he behaves as if the rabbit was still inside the cage and
another persons behavior (i.e. cause the experimenter pretends to feed it). That is, to make sense of his
to hand over the toy), declarative pointing, as mea- behavior, the child has to affect the others epistemic
sured in this study, implies the capacity to share state, i.e. his knowledge. This form of knowledge
interest toward novel sights and thus to affect another acquisition has been associated with left-sided neural
persons epistemic state (e.g., cause someone to attend activity. Another study reported left hemispheric pro-
to the object behind his or her back). In other words, cesses to be related to responding to joint attention in
by using the declarative gesture children intentionally infants as young as 5 months (Grossman & Johnson,
direct the others attention toward the object 2010). Accordingly, parts of the left prefrontal cortex
(Camaioni, 1993; Camaioni et al., 2004). are selectively activated when the experimenter draws
This difference between imperative and epistemic the infants attention toward an object. Despite differ-
declarative pointing can also be seen on a neural level: ences in development and task complexity (Aureli
while epistemic declarative pointing was signicantly et al., 2009; Camaioni et al., 2004), both declarative
correlated with left hemispheric coherence scores, and responsive communicative behaviors involve the
imperative pointing signicantly correlated with the capacity to cope with differences in knowledge about
right hemisphere. This lateralization effect suggests an object or a situation, thus the transmission of
that imperative and epistemic declarative pointing information from the sender to the recipient. Given
rely on distinct neural maturation processes. the focus on the epistemic quality of joint attention in
Previous neural studies on early communicative beha- the present study, we might conclude that left-
vior showed that the left hemisphere is critically lateralized maturation processes are critical to the
involved in declarative joint attention abilities development of informational-representational compe-
(Mundy et al., 2000), whereas others report that both tencies in infancy.
EPISTEMIC COMMUNICATION IN INFANCY 57

In contrast to declarative gestures, imperative an object from someone, declarative point produc-
pointing aims at inuencing another persons behavior tion can also be interpreted as some form of proso-
by requesting some object. Neural studies with adults cial behavior given that the infant helps the other
have related the requestive aspect of communication person by directing his or her attention toward a
to right hemispheric processes. For instance, Weylman relevant and interesting object. Later in develop-
and colleagues revealed that people with right hemi- ment, this preverbal gesture might be replenished
sphere damage were specically impaired in reques- by verbal utterances, which, importantly, serve the
tive comprehension (Weylman, Brownell, Roman, & same goal: inform someone about something that he
Gardner, 1989). Further, De Langavant et al. (2011) cannot see or know. Note that declarative pointing
showed that communicative pointing toward a was related to epistemic language, independently of
requested object is associated with activity in the childrens IQ. Given the present ndings, this com-
right hemisphere. The present study contributes to petence seems to be subserved by left-lateralized
these ndings by suggesting that even in infants brain maturation processes.
requestive behavior is subserved by right hemispheric Taken together, the present neural insight provides
brain maturation processes. However, given the rather further support for socio-cognitive accounts proclaim-
weak correlations, further investigations are required ing that communicative (declarative) pointing has a
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

to specify this relationship. key function in the development of social understand-


Besides, epistemic language acquisition requires ing linked to the understanding of others epistemic
similar information-representational skills as declara- states, which again is also required for successful
tive pointing. That is, both involve the ability to language development (Bates et al., 1975; Camaioni
represent epistemic states and symbolic meaning to et al., 2004; Colonnesi et al., 2010). This ability is
transmit information, which, though, is not required suggested to be subserved by left hemispheric brain
for imperative behavior. Intriguingly, the present nd- maturation processes, which proved to be the promi-
ings reect on a behavioral level a specic relation as nent independent predictor of references to others
declarative, but not imperative, pointing competence epistemic states at 48 months.
predicts subsequent epistemic language skills and on a To conclude, the present study aimed at providing
neural level as both declarative pointing and language a systematic insight into the neural correlates of
development rely on brain maturation processes of the imperative and declarative gestures. Results revealed
left hemisphere. It seems likely that children who are signicant differences between pointing behavior with
competent at declarative pointing, while they display instrumental (imperative pointing) and declarative
an understanding of others attentional states, also motives on the behavioral as well as neural levels.
show higher motivation to share information with The network properties of shared information and
others. By eliciting social interactions more coupling as reected by EEG coherence seem to differ
frequently, children might receive more learning for imperative versus declarative motives, too.
opportunities fostering their epistemic language devel- Whereas the maturation of the left hemisphere is
opment. Thus, declarative pointing competency might associated with epistemic declarative pointing,
provide a fruitful learning context. imperative pointing seems to be related to right hemi-
Hence, it seems possible that childrens motiva- spheric integration processes. These results provide
tion to point for others and thus to inform someone neural evidence that imperative and declarative point-
about an interesting event reects some kind of ing serve different socio-cognitive functions.
underlying prosocial comprehension. Intriguingly, Furthermore, the present study enlarges previous nd-
early forms of prosocial understanding have also ings by elucidating the relation between the epistemic
been linked to neurophysiological processes of the aspect of non-verbal (epistemic declarative pointing)
left hemisphere. For instance, there is ample evi- and verbal communication (epistemic language) both
dence that relatively higher left than right frontal relying on brain maturation processes of the left
activity is related to social approach behavior (see hemisphere.
for review Harmon-Jones, Gable, Peterson, 2010); One goal of future social neuroscience should be to
more specically, prosocial action such as under- specify the functional role of the right hemisphere
standing someones distress and responding empha- related to imperative pointing motives. For instance,
tically toward him is associated with relatively it could be claried whether the comprehension of
stronger left frontal cortical activation in 2-year- requestive pointing behavior, such as when the child
old infants (Paulus, Khn-Popp, et al., 2013). In needs to respond to another persons behavioral
contrast to imperative point production, which request, is similarly related to right hemispheric
might be driven by the egocentric goal to obtain maturation processes, or, since empirical evidence
58 KHN-POPP ET AL.

suggests that it predicts early theory of mind (desire- childhood: Stability and gender considerations. First
psychology) (cf. Kristen et al., 2011), it might share Language, 24(3), 267304. doi:10.1177/
neuronal correlates with declarative point production 0142723704045681
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal
than imperative production does. states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of mind.
Note that the variety of childrens epistemic state Developmental Psychology, 18, 906921. doi:10.1037/
talk seems to be more closely related to childrens 0012-1649.18.6.906
non-verbal IQ than to their verbal IQ. Further, we Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following
controlled for verbosity to differentiate between the and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth
through two years of age: A longitudinal, growth curve
verbal versus cognitive aspect of mental state talk. modelling study. Journals of Child Language, 35,
However, childrens epistemic state talk is still to be 207220. doi:10.1017/S030500090700829X
considered as a linguistic measure of Theory of Mind Camaioni, L. (1993). The development of intentional com-
and thus could be related to EEG left-coherence mea- munication: A re-analysis. In J. Nadel & L. Camaioni
sures because of a strong linguistic component. Note, (Eds.), New perceptions in early communicational devel-
opment. London: Routledge.
though, that while left-coherence measure were more Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., & Colonnesi,
important, right-coherence measures were also related C. (2004). The role of declarative pointing in developing
to the variety of childrens epistemic talk. Thus, future a theory of mind. Infancy, 5, 291308. doi:10.1207/
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

research should include additional measures, possibly s15327078in0503_3


even completely non-verbal measures of mental state Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., Parrini, B., &
Cesari, A. (2003). The communicative use of pointing
comprehension, assessing a more socio-cognitive ver- in autism: Developmental prole and factors related to
sus linguistic component of Theory of Mind. change. European Psychiatry, 18, 612. doi:10.1016/
Moreover, further neuroscientic research is S0924-9338(02)00013-5
needed to explore whether and how these distinct Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird, G.,
neural maturation processes become integrated in the Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint attention,
imitation, and play as infancy precursors to language
course of socio-cognitive development to provide a and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15,
more coherent insight into the neural underpinnings of 481498. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00037-5
early social communicative abilities. Chiarella, S. S., Kristen, S., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Sodian, B.
(2013). Concurrent relations between perspective-taking
Original manuscript received 12 July 2014 skills, desire understanding, and internal-state vocabu-
Revised manuscript accepted 24 February 2015 lary. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14(3),
First published online 1 April 2015 480498.
Cochet, H., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Pointing gestures pro-
duced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: Different
functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant
Behavior and Development, 33, 431441. doi:10.1016/
REFERENCES j.infbeh.2010.04.009
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J.
Aureli, T., Perucchini, P., & Genco, M. (2009). Childrens (2010). The relation between pointing and language
understanding of communicative intentions in the middle development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review,
of the second year of life. Cognitive Development, 24, 30, 352366. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2010.10.001
112. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.07.003 Cuevas, K., Bell, M. A., Marcovitch, S., & Calkins, S. D.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator (2012). Electroencephalogram and heart rate measures of
mediator variable distinction in social psychological working memory at 5 and 10 months of age.
research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considera- Developmental Psychology, 48, 907917. doi:10.1037/
tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, a0026448
11731182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 De Haan, M. (2007). Infant EEG and event-related poten-
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisi- tials. New York, NY: Wiley.
tion of performatives prior to speech. Merril-Palmer De Langavant, L. C., Remy, P., Trinkler, I., McIntyre, J.,
Quarterly, 21, 205226. Retrieved from http://www. Dupoux, E., Berthoz, A., & Bachoud-Lvi, A.-C. (2011).
jstor.org/stable/23084619 Behavioral and neural correlates of communication via
Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. pointing. PLoS One, 6, e17719. doi:10.1371/journal.
(2012). Twelve-month-olds comprehension and production pone.0017719
of pointing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, Franco, F., & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social
30, 359375. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02043.x awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year.
Bell, M. A. (2012). A psychobiological perspective on Journal of Child Language, 23, 307336. doi:10.1017/
working memory performance at 8 months of age. S0305000900008813
Child Development, 83, 251265. doi:10.1111/ Gredebck, G., Melinder, A., & Daum, M. (2010). The
j.1467-8624.2011.01684.x development and neural basis of pointing comprehen-
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (2004). sion. Social Neuroscience, 5, 441450. doi:10.1080/
Specic and general language performance across early 17470910903523327
EPISTEMIC COMMUNICATION IN INFANCY 59

Grossman, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Selective prefrontal theory of mind. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,
cortex responses to joint attention in early infancy. 1, 6776. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2010.08.001
Biology Letters, 6, 540543. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.1069 Moore, C., & DEntremont, B. (2001). Developmental
Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus.
The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emo- Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 109129.
tion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological doi:10.1207/S15327647JCD0202_1
Psychology, 84, 451462. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho. Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. A. (2000). EEG correlates of the
2009.08.010 development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental
Henderson, L. M., Yoder, P. J., Yale, M. E., & McDufe, A. Psychobiology, 36, 325338. doi:10.1002/(SICI)
(2002). Getting the point: Electrophysiological correlates 1098-2302(200005)36:4<325::AID-DEV7>3.0.CO;2-F
of protodeclarative pointing. International Journal of Mundy, P., Fox, N. A., & Card, J. (2003). EEG coherence,
Developmental Neuroscience, 20, 449458. joint attention and language development in the second
doi:10.1016/S0736-5748(02)00038-2 year. Developmental Science, 6, 4854. doi:10.1111/
Hoehl, S., Palumbo, L., Heinisch, C., & Striano, T. (2008). 1467-7687.00253
Infants attention is biased by emotional expressions and Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint attention,
eye gaze direction. Neuroreport, 19, 579582. developmental level, and symptom presentation in aut-
doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f97897 ism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 389402.
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, concep- doi:10.1017/S0954579400006003
tual, and statistical clarity in the study of mediators and Nunez, P. (1981). Electrical elds of the brain. New York, NY:
Downloaded by [LMU Muenchen] at 03:06 12 February 2016

moderators: Examples from the child-clinical and pedia- Oxford.


tric psychology literatures. Journal of Consulting and Olineck, K. M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2007). Imitation of
Clinical Psychology, 65, 599610. doi:10.1037/0022- intentional actions and internal state language in infancy
006X.65.4.599 predict preschool theory of mind skills. European
Kochanska, G. (2002). Committed compliance, moral self, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 1430.
and internalization: A mediational model. Developmental doi:10.1080/17405620601046931
Psychology, 38, 339351. doi:10.1037/0012-1649. Paulus, M., Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2013).
38.3.339 Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying social learning
Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Licata, M., Thoermer, C., & Poulin in infancy: Infants neural processing of the effects of
Dubois, D. (2012). The development of internal state others actions. Social Cognitive and Affective
language during the third year of life: A longitudinal Neuroscience, 8, 774779. doi:10.1093/scan/nss065
parent report study. Infant and Child Development, 21 Paulus, M., Khn-Popp, N., Licata, M., Sodian, B., &
(6), 634645. doi:10.1002/icd.1767 Meinhardt, J. (2013). Neural correlates of prosocial behavior
Kristen, S., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., & Perst, H. (2011). in infancy: Different neurophysiological mechanisms sup-
Infants joint attention skills predict toddlers emerging port the emergence of helping and comforting. Neuroimage,
mental state language. Developmental Psychology, 47, 66, 522530. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.041
12071219. doi:10.1037/a0024808 Petermann, F. (2009). Wechsler Preschool and Primary
Khn-Popp, N., Sodian, B., Sommer, M., Dhnel, K., & Scale of Intelligence - Third Edition (WPPSI-III).
Meinhardt, J. (2013). Same or different? ERP correlates of Deutsche Version. Frankfurt/M.: Pearson Assessment.
pretense and false belief reasoning in children. Sabbagh, M. A., & Taylor, M. (2000). Neural correlates of
Neuroscience, 248, 488498. doi:10.1016/j. theory-of-mind reasoning: An event-related potential
neuroscience.2013.06.017 study. Psychological Science, 11, 4650. doi:10.1111/
Licata, M., Paulus, M., Thoermer, C., Kristen, S., 1467-9280.00213
Woodward, A., & Sodian, B. (2014). Mother-infant inter- Saby, J. N., & Marshall, P. J. (2012). The utility of EEG band
action quality and infants ability to encode actions as power analysis in the study of infancy and early childhood.
goal-directed. Social Development, 23, 340356. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37, 253273.
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. doi:10.1080/87565641.2011.614663
(2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide informa- Striano, T., Reid, V. M., & Hoehl, S. (2006). Neural mechanisms
tion for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, of joint attention in infancy. European Journal of
7, 173187. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2 Neuroscience, 23, 28192823. doi:10.1111/j.1460-
Liu, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Wellman, H. M. (2009). Neural 9568.2006.04822.x
correlates of belief- and desire-reasoning. Child Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A
Development, 80, 11631171. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624. new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78,
2009.01323.x 705722. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.x
Liu, D., Sabbagh, M. A., Gehring, W. J., & Wellman, H. M. Vaughan Van Hecke, A., Mundy, P., Acra, C. F., Block, J.,
(2009). Neural correlates of childrens theory of mind Delgado, C., Parlade, M., & Pomares, Y. B. (2007).
development. Child Development, 80, 318326. Infant joint attention, temperament, and social compe-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01262.x tence in preschool children. Child Development, 78,
Meinhardt, J., Khn-Popp, N., Sommer, M., & Sodian, B. 5369. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00985.x
(2012). Distinct neural correlates underlying pretense and Weylman, S. T., Brownell, H. H., Roman, M., & Gardner,
false belief reasoning: Evidence from ERPs. Neuroimage, H. (1989). Appreciation of indirect requests by left- and
63, 623631. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.019 right-brain-damaged patients: The effects of verbal con-
Meinhardt, J., Sodian, B., Thoermer, C., Dhnel, K., & text and conventionality of wording. Brain and
Sommer, M. (2011). True- and false belief reasoning in Language, 36, 580591. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(89)
children and adults: An event-related potential study of 90087-4

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen