Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Applied Ergonomics
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/apergo

Effects of vibration on occupant driving performance under simulated


driving conditions
Amzar Azizan a, b, *, M. Fard a, Michael F. Azari c, Reza Jazar a
a
School of Engineering, RMIT University, Melbourne, Australia
b
University of Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia
c
School of Health and Biomedical Sciences, RMIT University, Melbourne, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Although much research has been devoted to the characterization of the effects of whole-body vibration
Received 14 January 2016 on seated occupants' comfort, drowsiness induced by vibration has received less attention to date. There
Received in revised form are also little validated measurement methods available to quantify whole body vibration-induced
30 September 2016
drowsiness. Here, the effects of vibration on drowsiness were investigated. Twenty male volunteers
Accepted 27 December 2016
were recruited for this experiment. Drowsiness was measured in a driving simulator, before and after 30-
min exposure to vibration. Gaussian random vibration, with 1e15 Hz frequency bandwidth was used for
excitation. During the driving session, volunteers were required to obey the speed limit of 100 kph and
Keywords:
Human vibration
maintain a steady position on the left-hand lane. A deviation in lane position, steering angle variability,
Drowsiness and speed deviation were recorded and analysed. Alternatively, volunteers rated their subjective
Lane deviation drowsiness by Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) scores every 5-min. Following 30-min of exposure to
Speed deviation vibration, a signicant increase of lane deviation, steering angle variability, and KSS scores were observed
Steering angle in all volunteers suggesting the adverse effects of vibration on human alertness level.
2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction transmitted vibration to the seated human body has a signicant


inuence on human perception and ride comfort (Grifn, 1990;
Drowsiness is one of the leading causes of accidents on motor- Baik, 2004; Factors et al., 1997). Exposure to vibration also has
ways and major roadways, accounting for approximately 20% of been found to correlate with a range of physiological reactions of
road accidents worldwide (Horne and Reyner, 1995). Drowsiness, the human body such as lower back pain and reduction in heart rate
which refers to sleepiness, is a multifactorial state that may lead to variability (Vicente et al., 2011; Callaghan and McGill, 2001).
inappropriate driving behaviour such as lack of awareness, poor Although many studies have contributed much to the under-
judgement and slowed reaction times (Anderson and Horne, 2013). standing and prediction of the subjective human body response to
In addition, drowsiness as a result of alcohol intake or monotonous vibration (Manseld and Maeda, 2011; Maeda et al., 2008; Tewari
driving conditions or night driving is known to signicantly inu- and Prasad, 2000; Kjellberg and Wikstro m, 1985), few studies
ence driving performance, compromising transportation safety have considered the effect of vibration specically on drowsiness
(Larue, 2010; Kamdar et al., 2004; Akerstedt et al., 2005; Fairclough levels for seated occupants in the vehicle.
and Graham, 1999). Although the performance of vehicle drivers Therefore, there is considerable scope for dening the exact
has been well investigated under various conditions, vibration- effects of vehicle and particularly seat vibration on driver drowsi-
induced drowsiness is not well-characterised. Relationships be- ness levels. According to several published reports on drowsiness
tween amplitude and frequency of vibration and drowsiness levels and vehicle control, there is a close relationship between drowsi-
have been assumed without sufcient quantitative data. According ness and vehicle lateral control (standard deviation of lateral
to ISO 2631-1 (1997) International Standards (1997), the position-SDLP, steering angle variability) as well as longitudinal
control (speed deviation) (Wierwille and Kirn, 1994; Thiffault and
Bergeron, 2003). SDLP is calculated as a standard deviation of the
* Corresponding author. School of Engineering, RMIT University, Melbourne, average lateral position and corresponds to the amount of weaving
Australia. in the car and increases in SDLP may ultimately result in the lane
E-mail address: s3356158@student.rmit.edu.au (A. Azizan).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2016.12.020
0003-6870/ 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355 349

crossing into the road shoulder and adjacent trafc lane. Steering vibration. This vibration setup also was built to be somewhat
angle variability is calculated as a deviation from the center of similar to the vibration that is transferred from the vehicle oor to
steering angle. Zero deviation means the center of the vehicle co- the seat. The vibration table below the seat was designed to be
incides or is parallel to the center of lane position. Speed adjust- dynamically rigid in frequencies below 100 Hz. This is to ensure
ment from the posted speed limit will result in speed deviation. that there is no interaction with vehicle seat structural dynamics.
Therefore, the primary dependent variables for this investigation Prior to drowsiness measurement, measurement of total trans-
were volunteers' SDLP measured from simulated driving vehicle, mitted vibration for each volunteer has been done in accordance
steering angle variability and speed deviation. with ISO 2631-1 (1997) International Standards (1997). The total
Although many studies have attempted to demonstrate the links transmitted vibration was measured both from the vehicle's seat-
between driving performance and drowsiness, drowsiness caused back and seat pan. The measurement was carried out to adjust the
by vehicle vibration has not been experimentally assessed by required hydraulic input force for every volunteer to become
simulated driving. Therefore, it was also important to investigate 0.2 ms2 r.m.s.
the feasibility and utility of simulated driving in the detection of Two tri-axial accelerometer pads (SVANTEK SV-38V model)
drowsiness caused by vibration. Hence, it was the primary aim of were used to measure the total transmitted vibration to the human
this study to investigate the effects of vibration on human drows- body located at the seat cushion and the seatback (Fard et al., 2014).
iness level using both objective (Simulated Driving Test) and sub- The SV 106 Human Vibration Exposure (HVE) meter (analyser),
jective (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale) measurement methods. which was connected to the accelerometer pads, was used to obtain
the total frequency weighted transmitted vibration to the seated
2. Methods human body. The HVE analyser uses the weighting factors (Wk, Wd,
Wc) and multiplication factors (Table 1) to calculate the total
2.1. Recruitment and screening frequency-weighted transmitted vibration. The weighting curves
(Wk, Wd, Wc) given in Table 1 were from ISO 2631-1 (1997)
Twenty young male (n 20) participated in this investigation International Standards (1997). The frequency weighting curves
with a mean age (SD) 23.0 1.3. They were randomly selected dene the values by which the vibration magnitude at each specic
from university students. They had no history of low back pain frequency is to be multiplied in order to weight the measured vi-
(LBP) and normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Their de- bration in accordance with the human body (Grifn, 1990). The
mographic were recorded at enrolment. They were (mean SD) multiplication factors (Table 1) were used to weight the effects of
168.2 4.0 cm and weighed (mean SD) 64.2 12.2 kg. The seatback and seat cushion vibrations (Grifn, 1990; M. Amzar and
average BMI of participants was (mean SD) 22.6 2.54 kg/m2. Fard, 2013; Fard et al., 2014).
One week prior to a laboratory experiment, volunteers were
required to maintain their normal amount of sleep (between 7 and 2.4. Drowsiness measurement
9 h) and keep a normal sleep schedule. Therefore, they were asked
to keep a sleep diary of when they go to sleep at night and wake up 2.4.1. Objective measure (Simulated Driving Test)
each day. All volunteers were screened before the day of experi- Volunteers were tested on the York driving simulator software
ment using Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) to measure the (York Computer Technologies, Kingston, Ontario, Canada) as shown
sleep quality (Buysse et al., 1989). Volunteers who showed poor in Fig. 2. The simulator has been determined to be an ecologically
sleep quality index (PSQI > 5) were excluded from the valid research tool to measure psychomotor performance related to
investigation. driving (Arnedt et al., 2005; Chung et al., 2005). The simulator as-
sembly consists of a personal computer, a 40-inch monitor and
2.2. Ethical considerations peripheral steering wheel, accelerator and brake accessories. A
customized driving scenario was developed in which volunteers
Before the investigation, volunteers were provided with verbal were presented with a forward view from the driver's seat. The
and written explanations of the purpose and contents of the driving simulation showed a cross-country highway, with two
experiment. They were also informed that they have right to refuse lanes in each direction.
participation in the experiment, and the results of the experiment The two primary instruction for volunteers are:
would remain condential. Following this, the informed written
consent form was obtained from all the volunteers after the pro- 1. Maintain a steady position within the left trafc lane during the
cedure of the experiment was explained, and the laboratory facil- entire test.
ities were introduced to them. The experimental protocol was 2. Maintain a constant speed (100 kph).
reviewed and approved by the RMIT University Human Research
Ethics Committee (Approval Number: EC 00237). Outcome variables measured by the simulator included (a)
standard deviation of lateral position (SDLP) (b) speed deviation
2.3. Experiment setup and (c) steering angle variability. The variation in these three
outcome measures shows how well the volunteers able to conduct
The experiment setup for drowsiness assessment is illustrated the test according to this instruction.
in Fig. 1. The vehicle seat with adjustable headrest was mounted on
a vibration table. The vibration table was mounted on four air 2.4.2. Subjective measure (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale-KSS)
cushions. The vehicle seat's inclination angle was set at 15  to the Subjective drowsiness level was assessed using Karolinska
vertical direction. Experiment set-up has been developed with a Sleepiness Scale (KSS). KSS is a self-reported and subjective
single vertical hydraulic actuator to replicate the vibration assessment of sleepiness that measures changes in sleepiness level
perceived by seated occupant in a moving vehicle. Although, the at the time (Gillberg et al., 1994). It is a 9-point Likert scale varied
input vibration was not independent on each axis, however, the from 1 extremely alert, 2 very alert, 3 alert, 4 rather alert,
input vibration generated from the hydraulic vertical actuator is 5 neither alert nor sleepy, 6 some sign of sleepiness, 7 sleepy,
located below the table away from the center of the table. The off- but no effort to stay awake, 8 sleepy, some effort to stay awake,
center excitation provides the multi-axial (x,y, z-axis) input 9 very sleepy, great effort to stay awake. KSS has been widely
350 A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355

Fig. 1. (a) Schematic drawing of the experiment setup. An actual vehicle's seat was mounted on a vibration table. A hydraulic actuator located at the corner of the table will provide
multi-axial input to the volunteer. A simulator also consists of a personal computer, a 40-inch monitor and peripheral steering wheel, accelerator, and brake accessories. (b) Forward
view of simulated driving software. Volunteers were required to obey the speed limit of 100 kph and maintain a steady position on the left-hand lane.

Table 1 2.5. Experimental procedures


The frequency weighting coefcients and the multiplication factors, from ISO 2631-1
(1997) International Standards corresponding to X (fore-aft), Y (lateral), and Z
(vertical) accelerations in the seat cushion and the seatback. Wk, Wd, Wc are shown
The experiment was carried out in a temperature and light
here. controlled (21  Ce23  C, <70 lux) laboratory and the noise level
was below 60 dB. Volunteers arrived at the laboratory at 8.00 h. The
Seat accelerometer pad
experiment began at 8.30 for all volunteers. Prior to the experi-
Seat cushion Seatback ment, all volunteers were screened using Epworth Sleepiness Scale
Axis X Y Z X Y Z (ESS) to detect any abnormalities in sleep (John, 1991). Those with
Weighting Wd Wd Wk Wc Wd Wd scores above ten, which indicated excessive daytime sleepiness,
Multiplication factor k 1 1 1 0.8 0.5 0.4 were excluded from the experiment.
Volunteers performed two separate test conditions [baseline
(no-vibration condition) and with-vibration condition] in a ran-
domized cross-over design, one week apart. The orders of two
conditions were randomly ordered to avoid order-related in-
uences. To standardise the learning effect, volunteers underwent a
10-min practice session before baseline and with-vibration condi-
tions to familiarize themselves with the simulator interface. During
with-vibration condition, volunteers were asked to drive for 10-
min with no vibration followed by 30-min sitting with exposure
to vibration with their eyes open. Volunteers were exposed to
Gaussian random vibration, with 1e15 Hz frequency bandwidth.
Total transmitted acceleration to the human body measured from
seat pan and backrest was kept constant at 0.2 ms2 r.m.s. Volun-
teers rated their subjective sleepiness scale using KSS before vi-
bration exposure, every 5-min of vibration and after vibration
exposure (Akerstedt and Gillberg, 1990). The rating was initiated by
the test leader saying KSS. Immediately after 30-min sitting,
Fig. 2. SDLP and steering angle variability were calculated as a deviation from the volunteers were required to drive for another 10-min. Similar
center of lane position. Zero deviation occurs when the center of the vehicle coincides procedures and sitting arrangement as in with-vibration condition
with the center of lane position. Speed deviation is measured as the deviation from the with the only difference being no vibration exposure were applied
posted speed limit (100 kph). for 30-min sitting. See Fig. 3 for an overview of the experiment
setup. The total duration of each condition (no-vibration and with-
vibration) is 50-min.
used as a prediction tool particularly to measure the changes over
time of sleepiness/drowsiness level. Lower KSS scores (<5) may
indicate a difference state of alertness level. Higher KSS scores (>7) 3. Results
may refer to the state of drowsiness where subjects may experience
involuntary dozing behaviour and frequent lapse episodes. At every 3.1. Objective measurement
5-min interval during the simulated driving task, volunteers were
prompted by the word KSS by the investigator to provide sub- Statistical analysis was performed using GraphPad software
jective rating score according to scales visible at all times next to (GraphPad Prism 6). All data were checked for normality prior
the monitor screen. analysis. Two-tailed (p < 0.05) was considered statistical signi-
cance. Two-way repeated measures - Analysis of Variance (ANOVA)
A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355 351

Fig. 3. The volunteers were required to complete a 10-min driving session both before and repeated after 30-min sitting with no-vibration and with vibration. 10-min practice
session was provided for all the volunteers at the beginning of every condition.

was used to assess the changes in objective measurement over the the middle of the left-hand lane when alertness was lowest
course of vibration exposure. All post hoc testing was performed via following exposure to vibration. No signicant difference was
the Tukey post-hoc test. There was no signicant differences in observed in the baseline (no-vibration condition) where SDLP was
alertness level measured by Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) at reduced from (mean SD: 23.5 0.05 to 22.8 0.04; t(19) 0.91,
baseline measurement between no-vibration condition and with- p > 0.05). The repeated-measures analysis of both conditions (no-
vibration conditions. Comparison of driving performance indexes vibration and with-vibration) revealed a signicant difference be-
(SDLP, speed deviation, and steering angle variability) and subjec- tween group variations (p < 0.01) that suggest a considerable in-
tive sleepiness scale (KSS) between no-vibration and with- uence of vibration on drowsiness level measured by SDLP.
vibration condition are presented here. The secondary outcome index measured in this study was speed
Results of Standard Deviation of Lateral Position (SDLP) are deviation. Speed deviation is calculated as the average sum of the
shown in Fig. 4. Each panel of the gure shows the average and differences of the vehicle speed from the posted speed limit. The
standard deviation (SD) of SDLP before and after 30-min of expo- results obtained from the speed deviation are presented in Fig. 5.
sure to vibration and sitting (no-vibration). From Fig. 4, it can be This gure shows the comparison of speed deviation measured for
seen that in the rst 10-min driving, there was no signicant dif- all volunteers between the no-vibration condition and with-
ference in SDLP was observed between before no-vibration con- vibration condition. Even though there seemed to be a slight in-
dition and before with-vibration condition (p > 0.05). As compared crease of speed deviation following exposure to 30-min vibration
to the baseline (no-vibration condition) SDLP measure between from (mean SD: 5.30 5.96 to 6.28 5.97; t(19) 1.26, p > 0.05)
before exposure and after exposure to vibration showed that 30- and 30-min sitting from (mean SD: 4.68 3.28 to 5.59 3.70;
min exposure to vibration had signicant inuences on volunteer t(19) 1.45, p > 0.05), this did not reach statistical signicance
lateral keeping performance. We found that following 30-min (p > 0.05). Speed deviation did not show signicant variation as
exposure to vibration, the deviation from a lateral position or compared to SDLP. These results suggest that speed deviation was
lateral variability was signicantly increased from (mean SD: not sensitive to levels of alertness caused by vibration as used
23.6 0.07 to 26.2 0.06; t(19) 2.65, p < 0.01). The analysis of under these experimental conditions.
lateral variability showed difculties in maintaining the vehicle in The third outcome index measured in this study was steering

Fig. 4. This bar graph represents the mean (SD) of SDLP for twenty volunteers in no-vibration condition and with-vibration condition. The measurement was taken at 10-min
driving before and 10-min driving after 30-min sitting in no-vibration and 30 min sitting with-vibration The signicant increase of lateral position variability (SDLP) (p < 0.01)
was observed in with-vibration condition. These differences, corresponding to common legal limits for driving were 2.4 cm (BAC 0.05%) (Verster and Roth, 2011). The changes
indicate the inability of the volunteer to maintain straight position once subjected to 30-min vibration. **p < 0.01.
352 A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355

Fig. 5. This bar graph represents the mean (SD) of speed deviation for twenty volunteers in no-vibration condition and with-vibration condition. The measurement was taken at
10-min driving before and 10-min driving after 30-min of sitting in no-vibration and 30-min sitting with-vibration. No signicant changes (p > 0.05) in speed deviation were
observed in both conditions (no-vibration and with vibration) following 30-min exposure to vibration and sitting.

angle variability. Steering angle variability is calculated as the de- 3.2. Subjective measurement (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale)
viation from the center of steering angle. Fig. 6 shows the mean of
steering angle variability measured for all twenty volunteers be- Comparison between the means of subjective sleepiness scale
tween the no-vibration condition and with-vibration condition. As (KSS) for all volunteers in both conditions (no-vibration and with-
Fig. 6 shows, there is a signicant increase of steering angle vari- vibration) are plotted against time and presented in Fig. 7. To
ability (mean SD: 0.67 0.20 to 0.82 0.20; t(19) 3.34, p < 0.01) investigate statistical signicance, two-way repeated measures-
between before 30-min vibration and after 30 min exposure to ANOVA corrected for Turkey's multiple comparisons was carried
vibration. Data for this table can be compared with the data in no- out. As illustrated here, initial KSS values did not differ between
vibration condition. No signicant differences were detected be- both conditions (p > 0.05). The average KSS value was (mean SD:
tween before 30-min sitting and after 30-min sitting (mean s.d: 2.11 0.57 in with-vibration condition and 2.21 0.85 in no-
0.69 0.22 to 0.73 0.28; t(19) 0.58, p > 0.05). vibration condition) at the beginning of the experiment. During
the rst 10-min of driving, no signicant difference was observed in
both conditions with KSS score of (mean SD: 3.58 1.26 in with-
vibration condition and 3.16 0.83 in no-vibration condition). It is

Fig. 6. This bar graph represents the mean (SD) of steering angle variability for twenty volunteers in no-vibration condition and with-vibration condition. The measurement was
taken at 10-min driving before and 10-min driving after 30-min of sitting in no-vibration and 30-min sitting with-vibration. As can be seen in this gure, with-vibration condition
reported signicantly increase in steering angle variability than in no-vibration condition (p < 0.01). **p < 0.01.
A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355 353

Fig. 7. The average score (mean SD) of subjective sleepiness scale (KSS) plotted against time for twenty volunteers in no-vibration condition and with-vibration condition. At the
beginning of the experiment, no signicant differences were observed between the two conditions (p > 0.05). The onset of drowsiness can be observed following 10-min of vi-
bration exposure in with-vibration condition, and the difference was signicant compared to no-vibration condition (p < 0.001).

apparent from Fig. 7 that there is a denite decline in alertness can be experienced in a vehicle. We demonstrated that the increase
levels indicated by a progressive increase in subjective sleepiness of human drowsiness level, measured by simulated driving soft-
score throughout the course of exposure to vibration. The onset of ware and Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) signicantly correlated
drowsiness was more rapid following 15-min exposure to vibration with exposure to vibration. These data support the hypothesis that
with KSS value of (mean SD: 6.00 1.41) whereas KSS value of exposure to vibration (random vibration with 1e15 Hz frequency
only (mean SD: 4.05 1.13) was observed in no-vibration con- band), even for as little as 30-min under these experimental con-
dition. After 30-min, KSS value increased signicantly with expo- ditions, causes drowsiness and adversely affects psychomotor
sure to vibration than without vibration (mean SD: 7.89 0.81 performance as measured by lane keeping performance and
and 4.53 0.70 respectively; p < 0.0001). The second driving test steering angle variability, found in all twenty volunteers. The def-
was conducted after the vibration stopped. The mean sleepiness icits in performance observed in this study are comparable to
level reduced indicating a slight decrease in overall sleepiness level performance on the York Driving Simulator (YDS) under alcohol
(mean SD: 7.26 1.00 in with-vibration condition and 4.42 0.61 intoxication (blood alcohol levels above 0.05% which corresponds
in no-vibration condition). to severe performance decits) (Arnedt et al., 2001; Verster et al.,
2014) (Table 2).
3.3. Correlation SDLP is related to the levels of drowsiness in the driver. SDLP
values become larger as the driver becomes drowsier. As drowsi-
Furthermore, we interrogated the correlation between subjec- ness increases, situational awareness decreases and the drivers'
tive measure (KSS) and objective measure (SDLP, speed deviation ability to predict an upcoming event is lowered. This lead to over-
and steering angle variability of this study by the use of Pearson's compensation by the driver and the wheel tends to be moved
(r) correlation). It is apparent from Fig. 8(a) that, there was a sig- frequently. The continuous variations of the standard deviation of
nicant positive correlation between SDLP and KSS (r 0.972; lateral position (SDLP) suggest low levels of alertness when vol-
p < 0.05) and between steering angle variability and KSS (r 0.983; unteers were exposed to vibration and hence cause difculties in
p < 0.05). A positive correlation indicated that impairments in SDLP maintaining the vehicle in the middle of the left-hand lane. The
and steering angle variability were related to subjective self- ndings observed in this study mirror previous studies that have
reported sleepiness, and the relationship was statistically signi- reported SDLP changes as the most common and persistent nding
cant (p < 0.05). A positive correlation between speed deviation and in sleep deprivation and drowsiness (Verster et al., 2014;
KSS did not reach statistical signicance (r 0.619; p > 0.05). Ramaekers et al., 2000). Our results demonstrate, for the rst
Further analysis between objective measures showed that there time that exposure to whole-body vibration may cause psycho-
were no signicant correlation between speed deviation and SDLP motor decits under simulated driving conditions. The current
(r 0.418; p > 0.05) and between speed deviation and steering study also found that there is an increase of steering angle vari-
angle variability (r 0.678; p > 0.05). As expected, the correlation ability following exposure to vibration. The observed increase in
between steering angle variability and SDLP was high, and the re- steering angle variability could be attributed to drowsiness, and the
sults were signicant (r 0.928; p < 0.05). ndings are consistent with previous studies (He et al., 2011;
Forsman et al., 2013; Nakayama et al., 1999). Another important
nding to emerge from this study is that speed deviation failed to
4. Discussion
exhibit any statistically signicant variation between no-vibration
condition and with-vibration condition. The present ndings
Driver drowsiness has been one of the primary causes of road
seem to be consistent with other research that found that a lon-
accidents (Hosking, 2006). However, drowsiness that is caused by
gitudinal measure such as speed deviation is not capable of
vehicle vibration is not well understood or investigated. The pre-
detecting drowsiness (Arnedt et al., 2001). As expected, results
sent study was designed to determine the relationship between
from the subjective measurement (KSS) also showed a signicant
human drowsiness levels and exposure to whole-body vibration as
354 A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355

Fig. 8. (a) The correlation between objective measures (SDLP, steering angle variability, speed deviation) and subjective sleepiness scale (KSS) in with-vibration condition. A high
correlation (r > 0.9) between objective measures (SDLP and steering angle variability) and KSS were observed, and the results were signicant (p < 0.05). (b) Correlation between
objective measures (SDLP, steering angle variability, speed deviation). A highly signicant correlation was observed between SDLP and steering angle variability (r 0.928; p < 0.05).
However, no signicant correlation was observed between Speed deviation and SDLP (r 0.418; p > 0.05) and between speed deviation and steering angle variability (r 0.678;
p > 0.05).
*p < 0.05.

Table 2 replicate actual driving conditions. Therefore, the vibration


Comparison of the SDLP (cm) in this study with those reported by Verster (Verster perceived by volunteers in this study is similar to the actual vi-
et al., 2014).
bration felt in a typical vehicle. An actual vehicle seat was selected
This study J. C Verster to ensure proper vibration transmission to the human body. The
Baseline With-Vibration Baseline BAC 0.05% assessment and guidelines of human body ride comfort caused by
23.6 26.2 24.2 26.1 vibration are were founded in ISO 2631-1 (1997) International
*BAC Blood Alcohol Concentration. Standards (1997). Since then, the relevant weighting factor has
been established to represent the human perception of vibration.
Although the guidelines for health effect due to exposure to vi-
decline in alertness level for all the volunteers following 30-min bration are well documented, there is little quantitative research
exposure to vibration. The increase in subjective sleepiness scores data available on the inuences of vibration on seated human
provides important corroborating evidence that exposure to 30- drowsiness. This study demonstrates a link between exposure to
min of vibration level can steadily reduce human alertness levels vibration and drowsiness, at least under these experimental con-
that are linked to drowsiness (Hallvig et al., 2013). We also found ditions. Therefore, it is imperative to further characterize this as-
that sleepiness levels immediately decreased after the vibration sociation under combined noise and vibration conditions that more
stopped, showing an acute improvement, albeit not down to alert closely resemble driving and to identify the component of noise
levels. and vibration that are most responsible for the decline in driver
A high correlation was observed (r > 0.9) between the objective alertness.
measures (SDLP and steering angle variability) and the subjective
measure (KSS) adding weight to the results. This study also con- 5. Conclusion
rms the association between SDLP and steering angle variability
(r 0.928; p < 0.05). The results demonstrated that, as drowsiness The novel contribution of this study is the characterization the
increases, there is a reduction in vehicle lateral control measured by role of vibration on occupant drowsiness. Our ndings have iden-
SDLP and steering angle variability. However, longitudinal control tied vibration as an important source of driver drowsiness. The
measured by speed deviation is found to be much less affected by evidence from this study clearly demonstrates that exposure to
drowsiness. low-frequency random vibration between 1 and 15 Hz has
Our experiment design and procedures were developed to considerable inuence on subjective sleepiness levels, and more
A. Azizan et al. / Applied Ergonomics 60 (2017) 348e355 355

importantly, human psychomotor performance and lapse of Fard, M., Lo, L., Subic, A., Jazar, R., 2014. Effects of seat structural dynamics on
current ride comfort criteria. Ergonomics 57 (10), 1549e1561. Available at:
attention. Exposure to vibration signicantly impacts lane keeping
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25017144 [Accessed December 11,
performance of the driver (SDLP). Our ndings show a low trans- 2014].
mitted vibration at 0.2 ms2 r.m.s. Increased SDLP by 11% which is Forsman, P.M., Vila, B.J., Short, R.A., Mott, C.G., Dongen, H.P.A., 2013. Efcient driver
comparable to SDLP under the inuence of alcohol (BAC 0.05%). drowsiness detection at moderate levels of drowsiness. Accid. Anal. Prev. 50,
341e350. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22647383
These data suggest that SDLP and steering angle variability are [Accessed December 8, 2014].
stable measures of driving performance with high reliability. KSS Gillberg, M., Kecklund, G., Akerstedt, T., 1994. Relations between performance and
also showed utility as a sensitive measure of vibration-induced subjective ratings of sleepiness during a night awake. Sleep 17 (3), 236e241.
Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7939123.
drowsiness. These data provide tantalising evidence that should Grifn, M.J., 1990. Handbook of Human Vibration. Academic Press Limited, London.
prompt further research in this area. Such research would ulti- Hallvig, D., Anund, A., Fors, C., Kecklund, G., Karlson, J.G., Wahde, M., Akerstedt, T.,
mately lead to the development of relevant and practical guidelines 2013. Sleepy driving on the real road and in the simulator - a comparison. Accid.
Anal. Prev. 50, 44e50.
to limit vibration exposure in the automotive industry, in an effort He, Q., Li, W., Fan, X., 2011. Estimation of driver's fatigue based on steering wheel.
to reduce the burden of disease of road accidents. These guidelines Eng. Psychol. Cognitive Ergon. 145e155.
will complement the existing ISO 2631-1 (effects of vibration on Horne, J.A., Reyner, L.A., 1995. Sleep related vehicle accidents. BMJ Clin. Res. Ed. 310
(6979), 565e567.
comfort) to extend these guidelines in assessment and establish- Hosking, S., 2006. The Relationship between Driving Performance and the Johns
ment of thresholds and safe limits for drowsiness-inducing Drowsiness Scale as Measured by the Optalert System. Monash University Ac-
vibration. cident Research Centre (MUARC).
International Standards, 1997. ISO 2631-1 Mechanical Vibration and Shock - Eval-
uation of Human Exposure to Whole-body Vibration.
References John, M.W., 1991. A new method for measure daytime sleepiness the Epworth
Sleepiness Scale. Sleep 14 (6), 540e545.
Akerstedt, T., Gillberg, M., 1990. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active Kamdar, B.B., Kaplan, K.A., Kezirian, E.J., Dement, W.C., 2004. The impact of
individual. Int. J. Neurosci. 52, 29e37. extended sleep on daytime alertness, vigilance, and mood. Sleep. Med. 5 (5),
Akerstedt, T., Peters, B., Anuand, A., Kecklund, G., 2005. Impaired alertness and 441e448. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15341888
performance driving home from the night shift: a driving simulator study. [Accessed September 28, 2014].
J. Sleep Res. 14 (1), 17e20. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/ Kjellberg, A., Wikstrom, B.-O., 1985. Subjective reactions to whole-body vibration of
15743329. short duration. J. Sound Vib. 99, 415e424.
Amzar, A., Fard, M., 2013. Effects of Vehicle Seat Dynamics on Ride Comfort Larue, G.S., 2010. Predicting Effects of Monotony on Driver's Vigilance. Thesis-
Assessment. International Congress on Noise and Vibration. Queensland University of Technology, p. 309.
Anderson, C., Horne, J.A., 2013. Driving drowsy also worsens driver distraction. Maeda, S., Manseld, N.J., Shibata, N., 2008. Evaluation of subjective responses to
Sleep Med. 14 (5), 466e468. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/ whole-body vibration exposure: effect of frequency content. Int. J. Ind. Ergon.
pubmed/23523431 [Accessed October 7, 2014]. 38 (5e6), 509e515. Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/
Arnedt, J.T., Wilde, G.J., Munt, P.W., Maclean, A.W., 2001. How do prolonged S0169814107001667 [Accessed December 9, 2014].
wakefulness and alcohol compare in the decrements they produce on a simu- Manseld, N.J., Maeda, S., 2011. Subjective ratings of whole-body vibration for
lated driving task? Accid. Anal. Prev. 33 (3), 337e344. Available at: http://www. single- and multi-axis motion. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 130 (6), 3723e3728. Available
ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11235795. at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22225028 [Accessed May 7, 2013].
Arnedt, J.T., Owens, J., Crouch, J., Stahl, Carskadon, M.A., 2005. Neurobehavioral Nakayama, O., Futami, T., Nakamura, T., Boer, E.R., 1999. SAE Technical Paper.
performance of residents after heavy night call vs after alcohol ingestion. JAMA: Development of a Steering Entropy Method for Evaluating Driver Workload, vol.
J. Am. Med. Assoc. 294 (9), 1025e1033. 724, pp. 5e8. Available at: http://www.sae.org/technical/papers/1999-01-0892.
Baik, S., 2004. A study on the characteristics of vibration in seat system. KSCE J. Civ. Ramaekers, J.G., Robbe, H.W.J., O'Hanlon, J.F., 2000. Marijuana, alcohol and actual
Eng. 8 (1), 135e139. driving performance. Hum. Psychopharmacol. 15 (7), 551e558.
Buysse, D.J., Reynolds, C.F., Monk, T.H., Berman, S.R., Kupfer, D.J., 1989. The Pitts- Tewari, V.K., Prasad, N., 2000. Optimum seat pan and back-rest parameters for a
burgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and comfortable tractor seat. Ergonomics 43 (2), 167e186. Available at: http://www.
research. Psychiatry Res. 28 (2), 193e213. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm. ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10675057 [Accessed May 15, 2014].
nih.gov/pubmed/2748771. Thiffault, P., Bergeron, J., 2003. Monotony of road environment and driver fatigue: a
Callaghan, J.P., McGill, S.M., 2001. Low back joint loading and kinematics during simulator study. Accid. Anal. Prev. 35 (3), 381e391. Available at: http://www.
standing and unsupported sitting. Ergonomics 44 (3), 280e294. Available at: ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12643955.
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11219760 [Accessed May 15, 2014]. Verster, J.C., Roth, T., 2011. Standard operation procedures for conducting the on-
Chung, F., Kayumov, L., Sinclair, D.R., Edward, R., 2005. What is the driving per- the-road driving test, and measurement of the standard deviation of lateral
formance of ambulatory surgical patients after general anesthesia? Anasthesi- position (SDLP). Int. J. Gen. Med. 4, 359e371.
ology 103 (5), 951e956. Verster, J.C., Bervoets, A.C., De Klerk, S., Vreman, R.A., Oliver, B., Roth, T.,
Factors, H., Kitazaki, S., Grifn, M.J., 1997. A modal analysis of whole-body vertical Brookhuis, K.A., 2014. Effects of alcohol hangover on simulated highway driving
vibration, using a nite element model of the human body. J. Sound Vib. 200, performance. Psychopharmacology 231 (15), 2999e3008.
83e103. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/ Vicente, J., Laguna, P., Bartra, A., Bailon, R., 2011. Detection of driver s drowsiness by
S0022460X96906744. means of HRV analysis. Comput. Cardiol. 38, 89e92.
Fairclough, S.H., Graham, R., 1999. Impairment of driving performance caused by Wierwille, W., Kirn, C.L., 1994. Research on Vehicle-based Driver Status/perfor-
sleep deprivation or alcohol: a comparative study. Hum. Factors 41 (1), mance Monitoring; Development, Validation, and Renement of Algorithms for
118e128. Available at: http://hfs.sagepub.com/cgi/doi/10.1518/ Detection of Driver Drowsiness. Report no. DOT HS 808 247. NTIS, Springeld,
001872099779577336 [Accessed October 8, 2014]. VA, p. 22161.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen