Sie sind auf Seite 1von 22

595

The
British
Psychological
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology (2009). 82, 595-615 n d U H c h,
((' 2009 The British Psychokigicat Society * - ' ^ ^ bociety

www. bpsjournals.co.uk

'Flow* at work: An experience sampling approach

Clive J. Fullagar'* and E. Kevin Kelloway^


'Kansas State University. Manhattan, Kansas, USA
^Saint Mary's University, Halifax, Canada

One of the core constructs of the positive psychology movement is that of 'f
oroptimalexperience. The current study investigated the relationship between 'flow', the
core job dimensions, and subjective well-being (SWB). as well as distinguishing between
the state and trait components of flow. Experience sampling methodology (ESM) was
used to track 40 architectural students over a 15 week semester while they engaged in
studio work. Hierarchical linear modelling (HLM) indicated that 74% of the variance
in flow was attributable to situational characteristics compared to dispositional factors.
Results also indicated that academic work that was high in skill variety and autonomy was
associated with flow. Flow was found to be correlated with positive mood. Cross-lagged
regression analysis showed that momentary flow was predictive of momentary mood and
not vice versa. The strengths and limitations of using ESM to study subjective work
experiences and well-being are discussed, as well as the implications of the study of flow
or optimal experience for industrial/organizational psychology.
,.

A repercussion of Martin Seligman's (1999) call for a 'positive psychology' can be


witnessed in the upswing in organizational research that emphasizes optima!
experience, transcendent pertbrmance, excellence, and positive deviance ((Cameron,
Dutton, & Quinn, 2003; Luthans, 2001). One concept that would seem to be particularly
appropriate to positive or^ianizationa! scholarship isthat of "flow'. Flow' was a term first
coined by Csikszentmihal) i (1975) wbo defined it as the holistic sensation tbat people
feel when they act witb total involvement (p. 4)'. Flow bas been defined as the
experience of working at full capacity, with intense engagement and effortless action,
where personal skills match required challenges (Nakamura & Csikszentniihalyi, 2002).
It is regarded as an 'optimal experience' to such an extent tbat the two terms are often
used interchangeably.
Originally Csikszentmihalyi studied flow among painters, rock climbers, dancers,
and musicians. Most research has focused on the experience of flow in voluntary
and pleasurable leisure and sporting activities. However, the experience of flow
has also been frequently reported wbile engaging in work related tasks as opposed to
leisure activities (Csikszentmibalyi & LeFevre, 1989; Fave & Massimiiii, 1988). This is not

* Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Clive j. Fullagor, Department of Psychology, Kansas State University, Manhattan,
KS 66506, USA (e-mail: fullagar@ksu.edu).
I
DOI: 10.1348/096317908X357903
596 Give }. Fullagar and L Kevin Keihway

siir|:)rising given tiiat many of the precursors of flow (such as immediate feedback,
comiiiensurate challenges and skills, and clear goals) are more likely to be found in work
activities. Despite the fact that flow experiences have been reported in work, relatively
little research has studied flow in the work context. The current research is a
longitudinal investigation of some of the antecedents and outcomes of flow while
engaging in work activities.

Operationalizations of flow
A remarkable consistency has been found in the description of flow across diverse
settings (C^sikszentmihalyi, 1975). The dominant characteristic of early models of flow
was the emphasis on the balance between challenges and skills (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975).
However, qualitative and quantitative research has identified several key elements
inherent in the experience offlow(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990,1993;Jackson, 1996:Jackson
& Marsh, 1996). Specifically flow has been characterized as consisting of nine
component states (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1993). The first is a challenge-skill halunce
and it refers to the match that must be present between the skills required to peribrni
the task and challenges of the situation. If the challenges in the activity exceed the
individual's skill level then faistration rather than flow occurs. If the activity does not
challenge the individual's skills then the result is boredom. If both challenges and skills
are balanced but do not exceed a specific level of difficulty and complexity then apathy is
likely to occur. Flow only occurs when the balance between challenges and skills
exceeds a level that is topical for daily experiences. Second, there is a merging of action
and atvareness. One s involvement in the task is such that behaviour becomes automatic
and spontaneous and there is little awareness of the self other than what one is doing.
Third, there is a strong sense of what has to be done and clarity of goals. Fourth, the
activity itself provides clear, immediate and unambiguous feedback concerning
progress towards one's goals. Fifth, there is an extremely high degree of involvement
with, focus on, and concentration on the task at hand. Attention and energies are
exquisitely focused on the task and there is an absence of distraction. Sixth, there is a
sense of exercising control without having to try and be in control thai is termed the
paradox of control. People in flow report they feel in control but as soon as their
attention shifts to tr> ing to maintain control they lose the sense offlow.Seventh, there is
a loss of self-consciousness as all concern for self disappears and the person becomes
one with the activity. Eighth, there is a transformation of time that seems to occur in
which there is loss of time awareness. Finally, the ninth dimension offlowis the autotelic
experience. Csikszentmihalyi (1990) coined the term autotelic experience from the two
Greek words auto, meaning self, and telos meaning goal. Such optimal experiences are an
end in themselves and are so enjoyable that they become intrinsically motivating. Flow is
described as such an enjoyable experience that one is motivated to return to this state.
Considered together these nine dimensions of flow provide a comprehensive
measure of optimal experience (Jackson & Ecklund, 2004). In the most recent research
on flow in the workplace flow has been measured as either a global construct
(e.g. Demerouti, 2006) or the challenge/skill dimension of flow has been focused on
(e.g. Eisenberger. Jones, Stinglhaniber, Slianock, & Randall, 2005). Such unidimensional
approaches have been found to be psychometrically acceptable ackson & Ecklund,
2002; Jackson & Marsh, 1996; Marsh & Jackson, 1999), Indeed, the nine subscaies of
flow have been found to be highly correlated and there appears to be a substantial
proportion of shared variance (Jackson & Ecklund, 2004).
Flow at work 597

Ffow: State or trait


Theodore Mischel (1969) has stated that 'clarification of key concepts must be part of
the busiticss of theory construction in psychology' (p. 264). An important piece
of this clarification process is distinguishing whether a construct is a trait or a state
(Alien & Potkay. I98I). A trait construct is a behaviour that individuals perform regularly
- it is a reliably permanent internal disposition, A state construct, on the other hand,
is more ephemeral and is a behaviour that is performed occasionally in response to
situational contingencies (Allport & Odbtrt, 193(>)-
There is some debate as to whether flow is a state or a trait. Flow as a state is defined
as a psychological state of mind that is transitory, existing at a given moment in time and
at a partictilar level of intensity, and experienced while performing a specific activity.
However, Csikszcntmihaiyi (1990) has also referred to the 'autotelic personality" in that
certain individuals may have psychological characteristics that make them more prone
to the experience of flow regardless of the situation. Allen and Potkay (1981) have
pointed out that the distinction between tniit and state constructs is frequently and
arbitrarily made by varying the instructions on scales that contain the same items. Wben
a state construct is being assessed participants are asked to rate their state of minti
'today' or 'now'. When traits are measured, subjects are required to rate their state of
mind 'in general". This arbitrary distinction can be found in operationalizations of flow
(Jackson & Ecklund, 2004). Dispositional assessments of tbe fiow experience (e.g. tbe
dispositional fiow scale, Jackson & Ecklund, 2004) measure the general tendency to
experience flow during an activity usually selected by the participant. Respondents are
required to recall and rate the frequency' with which he/she experience tbe various
characteristics of flow G^ckson & Ecklund, 2004), The premise bebind this approach is
that individuals who report higher frequencies of flow characteristics have a greater
predisposition to experience How, State measures offlow(e,g. the flow state scale (FSS),
Jackson & Ecklund, 2004) are usually administered immediately following an activity',
and assess the level of flow characteristics during the activity. By surveying participants
close to the conclusion of an activity it is assumed thai a more accurate assessment of
the flow experience is assessed, one that is less susceptible to recall bias. As Allen and
Potkay (19K1) note, changing the instructions presumes that there is a change in the
respondent's orientation from state to trait. Using such minor changes in the wording of
survey instructions to differentiate between state and trait constructs is an arbitrary and
tenuous distinction.
Tbere are other, more valid ways of distinguishing between state and trait constrticts,
Zuckerman (1983) has suggested that state and trait variables behave in ver)' different
ways. First, trait variables should be expected to exhibit low variation from situation to
situation. That is they should have high test-retest reliabilit)' and shotild n<H change
much with transient changes in situational conditions. State variables, on the other
hand, should vary considerably as situational contingencies and characteristics change
(i.e. they should have low te.st-retest reliability). Second, trait variables sbould correlate
more bighly with other trait measures and state measures sbould be more strongly
asstKiated witb situational. or moment-to-nioment variables. To date no research has
been undertaken that sifts out the trait versus state components of fiow. This is an
important distinction, not only from the perspective of conceptual clarification, but also
in terms of the practical and theoretical implications of the study of flow.
As we point out above, recent operationalizations offlow(Jackson &. Ecklund, 2002,
2004) have defined it as both a state and a trait construct. Flow is described as a
psychological state inflluenced by state-based factors with individuals varj'ing in their
598 Clive j. Fullagar and L Kevin Kellaway

propensity to regularly experience flow Oackson, Kitniecik, Ford, & Marsh, 1998).
Most research that has investigated both the trait and state aspects of flow has
proposed that the relationship between situational characteristics and state-based flow
is tnoderated by dispositional eletnents. For example, Kimiecik and Stein (1992) have
suggested that ibr athletes tbe relationship between experience of flow and engaging
in sporting activities is contingent upon whether tbe athletes have a task- or egt>
involved goal orientation. More recently, two studies that have investigated flow at
work found tbat tbe relationship between flow (challenge-skill balance) and mood, task
interest and performance was moderated by need ibr achievement ( Eisenberger et al.,
2005), and that tbe relationship between flow and in-role and extra-role performance
was dependent on conscientiousness (Demerouti, 2006).
Although personality characteristics stich as achievement motivation, aestheticism,
and inquisitiveness have been associated with flow (Csikszentmibalyi, Ratbunde, &
Wlialen, 1993: Jackson, 1984) no research bas been conducted tbat sifts out tbe trait
versus state components of optimal experience. The first aim of the current study was
to track flow over ati extended period of time in order to assess the extent to which
it varied from task to task and across individuals. If flow is a dispositional construct,
tben one would expect variation between tbe individual's predisposition to experience
flow, and little moment-to-moment variance. On the otber hand, if tbere is a large state-
related component in flow, tben one should expect to find considerable variation in
momentary flow.

Hypothesis /: Ratings of flow will exhibit both within-individual variance (state) and between-
individual (trait) variance with the fornner accounting for most of the variance in ratings.

Task characteristics and fiow


lhere has been a considerable amount of research that has investigated the links
between specific aspects of a person s job and his/her well-being (see Warr, 1999, for a
stimmary). One framework that has been particularly influential in tinderstanding this
relationship is the job characteristics model (Hackman & Oldham, 1980; Oldham, 1996).
Turner, Barling, and Zacharatos (2002) have argued the utility of tbis model in under-
standing the concept of "healthy' work and in promoting psychological and physical
well-being.
The JCM outlines five core job cbaracteristics that influence workers' attitudes and
behaviour (Hackman & Oldbam. 1975, 1980). Skill variety refers to tbe degree to wbich
tbe job requires different activities and skills to carry out the task. Task identity is the
extent to wbich the job requires completion of a whole and identifiable piece of work.
Task significance indicates the degree to which the job bas a meaningful impact on
otber people. Autonomy refers to the extent to which the worker has independent
discretion in determining the schedule and process of work. Finally, feedback is tbe
extent to which the worker is provided with information concerning bow well be/she Is
performing. Tbese live core job characteristics are proposed to impact on critical
psychological states tbat then influence affective and motivational outcomes.
Although the majority of research has supported tbe validity of tbe JCM (Fried &
Ferris. 1987; Taber SL Taylor, 1990), most studies have not tested tbe full, tbree-
stage model (Behson, Eddy, & Lorenzet, 2000). Specifically, tbe mediating effects of tbe
critical psycbological states have been neglected, witb the preponderance of research
assessing the direct impact of the core job characteristics on a varietj' of outcomes
Row at work 599

(Behson et al., 2000; Johns, Xie, & Fang, 1992). Recently there has been some meta-
analjtical evidence to suggest that the critical psychological states in the J("M do explain
some additional variance in the outcome measures (Behson et al.. 2000). One consistent
conclusion regarding research on the JCM is that more research attention needs to take
into account p,sychological .states in the development of an understanding ofthe impact
of work on employees Oohns et al., 1992; Renn & Vandenberg, 1995).
The aim of the present research was not to validate the JCM but to use it as a
nomological fratnework to establish the validit)' of flow as a critical psychological state
that mediates the relationsliip between certaiti core job characteristics atid well-being
among individuals who are engaged in work activities. Recently, Demerouti (2006)
found that motivating job characteristics are indeed positively related to How at work.
However, Demerouti did not ascertain the differential effects of each ofthe five core
job dimensions on flow. Following Fried and Ferris' (1987) guidelines she calculated a
single, unweighted additive index that assessed the motivating potential score of the
job. We believe that curtain core job characteristics have stronger conceptual links to
flow and that others will be weakly related or unrelated to optimal experience.
For example, the utilization of a variety of skills to meet the challenges of work has
consistently been found to be associated with job satisfaction and motivation (Ciavin &
Axelrod, 1977; O Brien. 1983). Csikszentmihalyi's flow theory also holds that an optimal
balance between challenges and skills inherent in a task is crucial for the intrinsically
motivating and satisfying subjective state that he termed flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990).
Those tasks that require the utilization of many skills antl abilities will be the most
intrinsically rewarding and conducive of flow (('sikszentmihaiyi & Rathimde, 1992).
C;onseqiiently, one would expect a strong association between the core job charac-
teristic of skill variety and the experience of flow.
Not only is it important that the task be challenging to facilitate flow, but there must
also be clear goals so that the performer can remain fully engaged in the task. Feedback
becomes the mechanism that enables individuals to clarifj- their progress towards
these goals (Jackson & Bcklund, 2004). Csikszentmihalyi (1997) states that immediate
feedback is a necessary component to experience flow. Feedback is also an important
core dimension outlined by the job characteristics model. However, unlike the flow
experience where feedback is intrinsic to the task itself. Hackman and Oldham (1975)
emphasize feedback from extrinsic sources such as supervisors, co-workers and clients.
Nonetheless, one would expect the two forms of feedback to be related and that
feedback from outside sources should be related to the overall experience of flow.
Autonomy and a sense of control also have been argued to be central elements of
both flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1997) and the core job dimensions (Hackman & Oldham,
1975). There is also some empirical evidence indicating that autonomy is an antecedent
of flow experiences (Bakker. 2005, 2008). More recently a tneta-analysis of locus of
control at work (Ng, Sorensen. & Eby. 2006) found internal locus of control to be
significantly associated with positive task characteristics and motivation. Consequently
on would expect high levels of autonomy to be positively correlated witb flow.
However, task significance and task identity refer to broader perspectives of work
concerning whether a job has a substantial impact on the lives of other people and
the extent to which the job entails an entire product. Flow has been associated with
individual engagement in specific, isolated, activities that do not necessarily have an
impact on any one else beyond the individual performing the task or consist of a
meaningful whole. Consequently one would not expect these job characteristics to be
associated with optimal experience.
600 Clive j. Fullagar and E. Kevin Kelloway

Hypothesis 2: The experience of flow will be positively related to (a) skill variety, (b) feedback,
and (c) autonomy.

F/ow and well-being


One of the repercussions of positive psychology' has been an upswing in interest in
well-being. A distinction has been made between hedonic and eudaimonic well-heing
(Kyan & Deci, 2001; Waterman, 1993). Hedonic approaches are pleasure-based and
define well-being in terms of tbe presence of pleasure and tbe absence of pain. Well-being
frequently is assessed by measuring momentary positive aJfecl or mood (e.g. Diener, Sub,
Lucas, & Smitb, 1999; Kabneman, Diener. & Schwarz, 1999). Eudaimonic approaches,
on tbe other hand, emphasize optimal functioning and personal expressiveness
(Waterman, 1993). For example, Ryff has defined psychological well-being from a
eudaimonic perspective and as consisting of six dimensions: autonomy; personal
growth; seli-acceptance; life purpose; mastery; and positive relatedness (Ryff. 1989,
1995; Ryff & Keyes, 1995). Flow could also be regarded as a momentary form of
eudaimonic well-being. It consists of many of the same aspects of psychological well-
being; a sense of control and mastery over the task, a clarity of purpose, a belief
that one possesses the skills to perform well, engagement in and relatedness to the task,
and a sense of enjoyment (Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988). There is evidence
to suggest that well-being is best conceptualized as a multidimensional construct that
includes botb hedonic and eudaimonic aspects (King & Napa, 1998; Mciregor & Little,
1998; Ryan & Deci, 2001). However, emotional positivity is regarded as being
independent of eudaimonic well-being. Indeed, positive affect has been suggested to be
an outcome of eudaimonic processes. Tbere is some longitudinal research tbat suggests
that positive affectivity is an outcome of both eudaimonic functioning (Reis, Sbeldon,
Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan, 2000; Ryff & Singer, 1998; Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996) and flow
(Han, 1988; Hull, 1991; Massimini & Carli, 1988) further strengthening the convergent
validity argument tbat eudaimonic well-being and flow are conceptually similar.

Hedonic well-being has been primarily operationalized as subjective well-being


(SWB) or bappiness (Diener & Lucas, 1999; Kabneman. 1999; Ryan & Deci. 2001).
An important component of SWB is the presence of positive mood. The otber
component is life satisfaction. In the current study we focused on assessing momentary
positive affectivity rather tban life satisfaction as the latter bas been found to be
relatively stable across time and to only slightly fluctuate around an individual set-point
(Fujita & Diener, 2005). Flow experiences have been ibund to be associated with such
positive moods and emotions as excitement, joy, ecstasy, happiness, and pride (Bloch,
2002) and SWB (Br>ce & Hawortb, 2002; Clarke & Haworth, 1994; Fave & Massimini,
1988; Seongyeul, 1988). LeFevre (1988) found tbat the more time people spent in (low
the more positive affect tbey experienced. Tbe little empirical researcb tbat has been
undertaken on flow in a work context suggests tbat people who enjoy tbeir jobs (jften
report experiences of flow while at work (Hryce & Haworth, 2002; c;sikszentmihalyi,
1990; Csikszentmihalyi & LeFevre, 1989; Haworth & MiU. 1992). More recently flow
(speciflcally bigh challenge skill balance) bas been ibund to be associated witb greater
positive mood and bigher performance, particularly among bigb need for achievers
(Eisenberger et al., 2005).
However, much (f the research tbat has investigated tbe relationsbip between
flow and mood is confounded by a methodology that makes for a tautt)logous rationale
for the relationsbip between flow and well-being. Csikszentmibalyi ( 1975) initially asked
Flow at work 601

subjects to describe highly enjoyable experiences or activities in order to define


the characteristics of flow. Also there has been some research to suggest that the
relationship between flow and well-being may not be as strong as originally proposed.
Clarke and Haworth (1994) found that college students perceived work where skills
exceeded challenges to be more enjoyable than flow activities where skills matched
challenges. Csikszentmihalyi (1997) has commented on the paradoxical nature of flow
in that although individuals report positive experiences with flow at worit, they may not
necessarily feel happy at work. The current research surveyed a wide variety of work
activities (not just enjoyable ones) in order to determine the relationship between flow
and hedonic well-being.
Finally, although flow has been associated with positive mood, the causal direction
of this relationship has yet to be established, AH of the research on flow at work
and mMid has been cross-sectional in nature. In contrast, in the current study we adopt
a longitudinal design in order to sift out the lagged effects of momentary flow on
momentarj- mood and vice versa. Consistent with our view of flow as a critical
psychological state, we, we hypothesize that;

Hypothesis 3: The experience offlowwill be positively related to positive mood.


[ .
The current study
Thus, the current study is an examination of the relationships between flow, task
characteristics and hedonic well-being. We propose that flow is primarily a state-like
variable (111 ), that flow is related to task characteristics (H2), and that the experience of
flow is associated with hedonic well-being or mood (H3). To examine these hypotheses
we use an experience sampling methodology (ESM) in a sample of architecture students
engaged in studio work.
Architectural students were chosen as the focus for our study for two reasons. First,
architectural students spend long hours working on creative projects in studio that
would tend to indicate that they are engaged in tasks that have a high motivating
potential and that are conducive to flow. Second. 5th year students engage in studio
projects that are representative of the work that they would do as professional archi-
tects, so the ecological validity of the work tasks is high. In our study, all participants
were enrolled in a Sth-year studio in architectural design.
Studio sessions focus on the synthesis of basic social, functional, teclinical, and
aesthetic factors in design and students are required to work independently on projects
that visually represent design ideas. Being the final studio in the architectural
programme, students engage in work higlily similar to the activities of their profession.
Architectural studio consists of two components; 3 weekly, 4 hour formal sessions,
where students collectively meet with their professor to discuss their projects; and more
frequent and inf(rmal independent sessions where students work on their own on their
projects. These latter sessions usually take place at night and continue into the early
hours of the morning. Our data collection focused on these sessions in which
individuals worked freely on their own architecture projects.

Methodology
Experience samp/ing methodology
The current study utilized an HSM. ESM Is a method of data collection where participants
are assessed at repeated moments over the course of time while functioning in their
602 Clive }. Fuilagar and Ksvin Kelioway

natural settings. Data were collected by handheld personal digital assistants (PDAs).
Participants were required to carry the PDAs for a specific sampling period and to
record their behaviours, attitudes, and experiences. The PDAs were configured using a
eustomized software called experience sampling program (ESP; Barrett & Feldman
Barrett, 2000).

Participants and procedure


Participants in the study consisted of 40, 5th-year architectural students from the school
of architecture at a large American university in the Midwest of the United States. At the
beginning of the semester tour studio classes were solicited ibr 40 volunteers.
Participation in the study was completely voluntary and individuals who fully partici-
pated received an honorarium ($50) in return for their participation. The research
protocol was reviewed and approved by the university Institutional Review Board.
The data were collected across 15 weeks of the Fall semester of 2004. During an initial
session participants were asked to provide the researcher with three days and time
periods during the coming week when they were most likely to be working
independently on their projects in studio.' Each student was then given a PDA that was
pre-programmed to signal participants at two random moments during the three
stipulated weekly studio periods. Students were instructed to complete the work task
that they were involved in before completing a short survey on the PDA. Once tbe initial
signal had beeped, the alarm repeated itself every 5 minutes for 15 minutes until the
survey bad been completed. The average time between the initial signai and the
participant s response was 4.7 minutes. Bu-ticipants were informed that they should
only respond when beeped during a work-related activity and that they should respond
only once during any single studio session. This process was repeated t)n a weekly basis
until 25 trials had been completed (giving 1,000 data points in total). Tbe average time
between measurement periods was 2.3 days.

Experienced-sampied measures
Eacb PDA was programmed with an experience sampling form (ESF) that was designed
to elicit a wide range of information on tbe participant s activities, mood, perceptions of
optimal performanee, and psychological state offlow.For the researcb process to be the
least disruptive, it was important that each trial should not take longer than two
minutes. Each PDA was programmed to beep participants at two random times during
every block of diarized studio time, with the restriction that no two signals would be less
tban 60 minutes apart.
Row was measured using nine items from tbe FSS-2 (Jackson & Ekiund, 2004;
Jackson & Marsh, 1996). Although this scale was designed to assess flow experiences
within a particular physical activity, it was deemed appropriate for tbe current research.
Like sports activities (the tiriginal focus for measurement), studio work occurred during
a clearly defined time period witb clearly defined activities comprising the 'event'. The
scale was theoretically grounded in Csikszentmihalyi's (1990) nine-dimensional
operationalization of flow. It would have been too intnisive and disruptive to include
all 36 items of FSS-2 scale for each of the 25 trials. Moreover, althougli designed to

' These times did not include the regular formal meeting times of studio which occurred on Mondays, Wednesdays, and Fridays
from 1:00 p.m. to 5:00 p.m.
flow at work 603

measure nine dimensions offlow,it is common to take an 'overall'flowscore as the sum


of the dimensional scores for use in fiirther analysis (see e.g. Jackson & Marsh, 1996).
Consequently, nine items were chosen on tbe basis of their loadings on each of the nine
dimensions derived in the original construct validation study (Jackson & Eklund, 2004).
The flow questions were preceded by tbe momentary statement, 'Please answer tbe
following questions in relation to your experience in the work activity' that you were
engaged in when you were beeped'. Ratings were obtained by tapping with the
stylus one of the following options; strongly disagree, disagree, neither agree nor
disagree, agree, strongly agree which were scored 1-5 accordingly. Each item had to be
responded to lor tbe trial to progress. The order in wbich theflowitems appeared in ihe
survey was randomized across situations in order to reduce response set. A total flow
score was computed tbat consisted of the sum of tbe scores on the nine flow items. The
internal consistency coefficient of the flow scale (computed on witliin-individual mean
item ratings) was satisfactory (a = .83).

job characteristics model


Fifteen items from tbe job diagtiostic survey were used to assess participants' perceptions
of tbe task characteristics associated witb studio work (Hackman & Oldbam, 1975).
Tlie job diagnostic survey is thet)reticaliy grounded in tbe job characteristics model
and assesses each of the five core task dimensions (skill variety, task identity, task
significance, autonomy, and feedback from the job) witb six items. Items were reworded
to be appropriate for studio work (e.g. How much were you left on your own to do your
own work?'; "How repetitious was the work in .studio?) The measure was administered at
the end of the semester. Internal consistency was satisfactory for allfivecore dimensions
(skill variety. .71; task identity, .85; task significance, .60; autonomy, .83; and feedback
from tlie job, .81).

Hedonic well-being
Mood was measured witb 10 positive emotions (alert, bappy, cheerfiil, strong, active,
sociable, involved, excited, clear, and relaxed) and 10 negative emotions (drowsy, sad,
irritable, weak, passive, lonely, detaehed, bored, confused, and tense) tbat were
arranged as bipolar items (e.g. alert-drowsy, bappy-sad). These emotions were selected
as they have been used to conceptually represent the major forms of pleasant and
unpleasant emotion (Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988; Diener, Smith, &
Fujita. 1995). A shorter list was selected as more appropriate fbr experience samplinf;
(Scollon, Diener. Oishi, &. Biswas-Diener. 2005). Tbe mood items were formatted as
'slider' questions, where respondents bad to move a slider towards the bipolar adjective
that best described their mood at the time that tbey were beeped. Tbe slider questions
were scored on a scale of 1-10, with higher scores being indicative of more positive
mood. Again the order of tbe mood items was randomly varied for eacb trial. The
internal reliability of the mood scale was found to be satisfactory (Cronbach'sa .96).

I
Analyses
To investigate tbe effect of job characteristics on flow and fiow on hedonic well-being,
HLM was used (Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992). The HLM modelling approach is an
extension of multiple regression that allows investigation of tbe relationships between
variables manifested at two levels of analysis. In longitudinal analyses there are a series
604 Clive ]. Fulhgar and Kevin Kelhway

Table I. Means (M), standard deviations (SD), and intercorrelations across individuals for ali study
variables (N = 40)

M SD 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Flow 32.31 3.06 073* 0.64** 0.26 0.58** 0.40* 0.46**


2. Mood 5.91 1.25 0.60** 0.41** 0.69^*^ 0.43** 0.53**
3. Si<ili variety 3.82 0.58 0.21 0.72** 0.17 0.30*
4, Tasi< identity 4. i 2 0.40 0.14 0.33* 0.57**
5. Tasl< significance 3.75 0.70 0.19 0.30*
6. Autonomy 3.25 0.71 0.45**
7. Feedback 3.16 0.71

*p < .05 (one-tailed); **f < .01 (one-taiied).

of repeated measures for each individual that constitute the lowest level of variables and
the individual level becomes the second level. Thus, in the current research, to examine
within-individual relationships between flow and mood, at the first-level, mood was
regressed on to flow for each of the 40 participants in the study. At the second level the
parameters estimated at level 1 (intercepts and slopes) were regressed on level 2
variables - in this case the job characteristics. HLM 5 was used (Bryk, Raudenbush, &
Congdon, 2()()()) to test the hierarchical models.
One of the assumptions of regression analysis is that residual effects are indepen-
dent. Obviously time-series data violate this assumption of residual independence at
level 1 (Hofmann, Griffin. & Gavin, 2000). Cinisequently level 1 models were developed
that accounted for the residual autocorrelation in the data. This was accomplished by
controlling for lagged flow and lagged mood in the level 1 equations. 1-igged flow and
mood were centred at the mean for each individual thus removing tbe between-
individual variance in this control variable (Bryk & Raudenbusch, 1992), In the current
study, we hypothesize and test a lagged hypothesis (i.e. flow predicts mood). To test this
we estimated a series of cross-lagged regressions.

Results !
Hypothesis t: Between- and within-individuaf variation '
Means, standard deviations, and inter-correlations across individuals for all study
variables are presented in Table 1. Before proceeding with the tests of the hypotheses,
systematic within- and between-individual variance in the ratings of flow and mood
were investigated. These statistical assumptions were tested using two null models for
both flow and mood (see Table 2). The results indicated that there was substantial
within- and between-individual variance for both flow and mood (Flow: a' 22.81,
Too = 8.19; Mood: CT^ 2.22, TIH) = 1.45). Altbough HLM does not provide a
significance test for within-individual variance (a*^), it does provide a chi-squared test
that indicated that the between-individual variance was significant for both flow and
mood (Flow: T,, ^ 8.19, x^(39) = 388.95,/i < .01; Mood: Too = 1-45, x^(39) = 673.83,
p < .01). The intra-class correlation for the flow measure was .26.^ indicating that

+ TOO)).
Flow at work 605

T a b l e 2. Parameter estimates and variance components of null models tested {N = 999)

Model equations -yoo "io ""^ Too

Null model 1^
Row^ = oi + U 32.30 - 22.81 8.19**
Mood,j = oj + r^ 5.90 - 2.22 1.45**
oj ~ "Yoo "t" "Oi

"oj is the average level of f l o w / m o o d f o r individual j; -yoo is the grand mean of f l o w / m o o d scores;
a^ var{r,j) the within-individual variance in flov^i/mood (computed as the average squared distance
f r o m individual momentary scores and the individual's mean score); and TOO v^riUoj) the betv/een-
individual variance in f l o w / m o o d (the variance of the 40 average flov//mood scores).
*p < .05; **p < .01.

between-individual variance accounts for 26% of the total variance in flow. This would
suggest that 74% of the variance is attributable to witbin-individual variation. For mt>od,
40% of the total variance was accounted for by between-individual variance, indicating
that 60% of the variance was due to witbin-individual variation in momentary' mood.
Tbese results suggest that How varies considerably from situation to situation :md tbat it
behaves predominantly like a state construct.
We also tested whether individuals' flow and mood nitings were randomly
distributed or serially dependent (Hies & Judge, 2002). Comparing the level I variance
components of the lagged regression model with that of tbe null models indicated that
tbe lagged measures explained fbur and less tban one percent of the within-individual
variance for How and mood respectively.^ Lagged flow was found to be a significant
predictor of momentarj- flow (Flow: -YK, =^ 0.20, p < .05), but mood was serially
independent of its lagged effect (Mood: ^n, = 0.08, p > .05). All subsequent models
where flow was the outcome variable were controlled for tbe lagged effect.**

Hypothesis 2: Job characteristics and flow


Hypotbesis 2 suggested that the individual variance in flow would be associated witb
tbe job characteristics outlined by Hackman and Oldbam's model (1975). Based on
tbeoretical arguments, the ve job characteristics were bierarchically entered into the
model as ibilows: skill variety; feedback; autonomy; task identity; and task significance.
In order to test tbis hypothesis, two models were calculated: a random-coefficient
regression model and an intercepts-as-outcomes model (see Table 3). Tests were
undertaken to ascertain the proportion of variimce in tbe intercept terms tbat was
explained by the five job characteristics at level 2 (see Table 3). Tbe t tests for the
fixed effects in tbe intercepts-as-outcomes model assessed the significance of
the relationships between tbe five job characteristics and momentary' flow, wbile
contniJling for the lagged effects of flow. Tbese tests indicated that botb skill variety
(7i, = 0.05, t(M) = 2.64, p < .05) and autonomy (7,, ^ 0.03, i(34) = 2.12, p < .05)
were significant predictors of between-group variimce in flow after controlling for

'Percent = in^(nullmodel) - (T^OaggedeffectsmodelWtr'inulimodel).


* Following Hies and judge (2002) these results arc not presented in the main models as they ore not of substantive
inleresi fiere.
606 Clive }. Fullagar and Kevin Kelloway

.0 rt

01
- ~ ^

L.
00
gi CQ.

Si y
o >
V
a.
m
a. "S
T)
0 0
Ti
L.
II
"S
n

resit
C V)
I 1
c i (SI
M "33
a C >
o
01 ^ c
1. '^0)
I I a. u

o c

01
T3 I I
o
E "O 0) _ ::?
S - ^
"= 0) t

t I

cf

^ a.

S O
w o c E
5 >-

0) 0)
y

s 0 ^ ^ g
V
0. . - - ^
C Q]
V

> re "= y i lA
Row at work 607

serial dependence effects. Neither feedback, task significance nor task identity were
significant predictors.
To determine tbe magnitude of the relationship between the two significant job
characteristics and flow, we calculated separate intercepts-as-outcomes models. Using
the residual variance in tbe intercepts, we found that skill variety' accounted for 1 S% of
tbe between-individual variance in flow (TQ ^ 23.91, X"C39) ^ 58.67, p < .05), and
autonomy explained 8% of the variance (TOO = 25.72, x^(39) = 59.45, p < .05).^

Hypothesis 3: Flow and mood


It was predicted that momentary flow would be related to momentary positive mood
both across and witbin individuals. The results of the hierarchical linear model that
was tested are presented in Table 4. Tlie pooled slope parameter was significantly
different from zero (7io ^ 0.19, i(39) ^ 16.04, p < .01). The direction of the pooled
slope of the regression of flow on to mood (710) indicates that higher levels of flow
are associated with more positive mood. The magnitude of this relationship was
calculated by comparing the within-individual variance in mood from tbe first null
model with the within-individual variance after controlling for flow.^' Flow accounted
for i6% of tbe within-individual variance in mood. Similarly, comparing the level 2
variance components of random regression model and the null model indicated that
flow explained 30% of the between-individual variance in mood. "'^ In sum, results
tended to support tbe bypothesis that momentary flow was assoeiated with
momentary positive mood.
We also tested the causal direction of the relationship between flow and mood using
lagged measures of mood andflow,respectively. Results suggested tbat lagged fitiw was
predictive of mood (7,0 = 0.11, i(39) = 2.28, p < .05), and that lagged mood was not
associated with later now (7,0 = 0.10, i(39) = 0.74, p > .05).

Discussion
The current study tried to understand tbe nature of flow at work by longitudinally
tracking the flow state in a sample of architecture students working on studio projects.
Specifically, we want to sift out the state and trait components of flow, to ascertain
wbich work-related characteristics were associated with flow, and to determine the
relationship between momentary flow and positive mood. Our results suggested that
botb within-individual and between-individual variance comprised flow with tbe
former accounting for 74% of the total variance. Thus, flow bas both state and trait
components with the former predominating. Second, our data suggest that skill variety
and autonomy are the two job characteristics that make unique independent
contributions to the predictions of flow. The remaining three job characterizitics

*R^ = (Toolrandomregression) - Too(Jntercepts-os-outcomes))/T(jo(ran</omregression).


R for level I model (tr (one-way ANOVA model) - (/(random regression model))I(r(one-way ANOVA model).
R for level 2 model (-(jofone-way ANOVA model) - Tgolrandom regression model))lToo(one-way ANOVA model).
^R^ = (^a^(control model) - o^i^random regression modelW(^{one-way ANOVA model) - (165.15 - 112.77)/
165.15 = 0.32.
' R ^ = (Too(random regression model) - TQo(controlmodel))lToo(one-way ANOVA model) = (71.79 - 62.36)1
128.77^0.07.
608 Clive J. Fullagar and E. Kevin Keltoway

Table 4. Parameter estimates and variance components testing relationship between flow and mood
{N = 999)

Model equations -yoo Tio f^^ Too TK

Ran do m-coefficient regression model t '


LI:Mood = o, + ,^(Flow,^) + r,j
L2: oj = 7oo + ^o,
L2:|^ = ^,o + U
-0.27 0.19* 1.41 2.95 0.00

"yoo is the mean of the intercepts across groups: 7io is the mean of the slopes across groups; (T^{ri,) is
the level I residual variance; TOO(UO,) is the variance in intercepts; and 7| i (U|,) is the variance in slopes.
*p< .01.

(feedback, task significance, and task identity) did not contribute to the prediction.
Finally, our results showed that the experience of flow preceded changes in mood.
Howard Ciardner (2002) has pointed out that one of the reasons that psychologists
have neglected to study the actual experience of work has been the lack of an
appropriate methodology to scientifically study internal feelings. In this context, it is
appropriate to note that our results emerge from the use of a rigorous methodolog)' that
allows the testing of longitudinal relationships and addresses the known problems of
cross-sectional survey data (e.g. Scolion, Kim-Prieto, & Diener. 2003; Smyth & Stone,
2003). We believe that the use of the ESM methodology substantially advances the study
of How and offers considerable advantage in other areas of organizational research. ESM
has been used to assess flow experiences in daily life among elementar)' and secondary
school sttidents (Csikszentmihalyi & Hunter, 2003), students (Carli. Delle Fave, &
Massimini, 1988; Csikszentmihalyi & Urson, 1984), family members (Larson & Richards,
1994), psychiatric patients (Massimitii, Csikszentmihalyi, & Carli, 1987), and nursing
home residents (Voelkl, 1990). The product of such a methodology- is a den.se and
systematic description of the external conditions and subjective experience of flow in
different situational contexts and at different moments in time. Despite the iaet that ESM
allows for the study of the interactions between person and situational variables without
tbe limitations of traditional research designs (Hormuth, 1986; Hies & Judge, 2002),
relatively little use has been made of ESM in organizational psycbological research.
Tlie current study had three mainfindings.First, we bave argued that it is important
to distinguish between the state and trait components of flow. Tbe results of the present
research suggest that flow, or optimal experience, is predominantly a situational state
of mind rather tban a trait or disposition. We found tbat approximately tbree quarters of
tbe variance in flow was attributable to situation-io-situation fluctuations. Dispositional
variables or traits tend to be stable across time and place (Zuckerman, 1983).
Consequently such variables are less amenable to manipulation. State variables on the
other hand are dynamic and cbange across time and situation. State variables are capable
of being manipulated and are likely the result of an interaction between personal
dispositions and tbe environment. Previous research on flow (e.g. Demerouti, 2006;
Eisenberger et ai, 2005) has tended to blur the state-trait distinction. Flow has been
treated as a state variable, in tbat situational variables have been correlated with it,
but measured as a tniit construct, in tbat participants' flow experiences have been
assessed across a broad range of activities. Furthermore, both of these studies were
Fiow at work 609

cross-sectional in nature making it impossible to sift out between-individual variation


from within-individual variance.
Our study allowed this separation :md resulted in the finding that most of the
variance in fiow was within-individual giving rise to the possibility that the experience
offlowis governed by characteristics ofthe task or situation. In the particular context of
the workplace, our results suggests that tasks can be designed to enhance the likelihood
of experiencingflow.Just what aspects of task design are important is informed by our
second finding.
Tiie second finding of the current research was that certain work or task
characteristics are associated with optimal experience. Earlier research by Demerouti
(2006) has found that motivating job characteristics are correlated with the experience
of flow at work. However, Demerouti did not specifically identify which job charact-
eristics were associated with flow. Our finditigs indicate that flow was associated
with studio work that was perceived as requiring different skills and that allowed
the student creative choice and discretion in their work. That these two job
characteristics should be correlated with flow may be due to the nature of the work
that we studied. Architectural work is cognitive in nature, with students having to use
logic and reasoning to identifv' the strengths and weaknesses of alternative solutions or
approaches to problems. Architecture studio requires students to engage in complex
problem solving where they have to independently identify problems and review
related information to develop and evaluate options and implement unique solutions.
The architectural student is more isolated, less monitored, and engaged in activities that
are more cognitively complex and creative in nature than many other kinds of work.
Even though a different state of mind was investigated in the current research, the
results lend support to Hackman and Oldham's (1975) contetition that core job
characteristics are important in generating critical psychological states (such as flow).
Thus, organizations wishing to enhance employees' experience of. or likelihood
of experiencing flow, are well-advised to design ta.sks that incorporate these elements
of skill variety and autonomy. Our findings suggest that engaging in tasks that require
complex skills, resolving challenging problems and expressing creativity leads to the
experience of flow.
Finally, the current findings also indicated a significant and reliable relationship
between flow and pt)sitive mood. Students who experienced higher levels of flow also
reported being more alert, happy, involved, excited, and so forth. This is consistent with
previous research that has found fiow to be associated with hedonic well-being, or
positive mood (Eisenberger fi rt/., 2005; Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi. 2002). However
the rationale for this relationsliip has been criticized for its circularity. Research on flow
has tended to focus on self-reports of single moments of well-being. The current study
attempted to overcome this methodological shortcoming by randomly sur\'eying a
broad range of work activities, not just optimal ones, over several weeks. I.eFevre ( 1988)
found that motivation, activation, and satisfaction were higher in flow experienced at
work, hut that affect was not. In fact mildly negative mood was reported by engineers
and managers who had experiencedflow.Positive affect was more likely to be related to
fiow in leisure activities. The difference between these and the current findings may
again be in the nature of work that was studied, ln architectural studio work many ofthe
tasks that are associated with high levels of flow are the same as those experienced in
leisure activities (e.g. drawing atid model making).
Thefindingthat flow is associated with positive mood has significant theoretical and
practical implicatiotis. Prederickson (1998, 2001) has argued tliat positive emotions
610 Clive J. Fullagar and Kevin Kelhway

broaden individuai resources and tbougbt-action repertories. There is growing evidence


that tbe psychological well-being of workers is predictive of employee performance, job
satisfaction, and turnover (Cropanzano &Wrigbt, 2001; Staw & Barsade, 1993; Wright &
Bonett, 2007; Wright & Staw, 1999). We have argued that flow is a form of eudaimonic
well-being. Our findings suggest tbat flow may be a critical psycbological state that is
associated with positive mood, a core component of psycbological well-being.
Furtbermore, we established that the majtirity of variance in flow is determined by
situational characteristics ratberthan individual dispositional factors. This has important
practical implications for the design of work. Providing work tbat is bigh in autonomy
and skill variety is more likeiy to induce flow whicb is likely to have beneficial
consequences for the organization. Flow has been found to be positively associated with
optimal ftmctioning. specifically with botb in-role and extra-role performance
(Demerouti, 2006; Eisenherger et al., 2005). Eisenberger et ai. (2005) bave shown
tbat tbe relationship between flow and employee performance is partially mediated by
positive mood. Earlier research has indicated that the relationship between positive
mood and performance is particularly pertinent to tasks ihat require spontaneity and
creativity (Eisenberger & Rhoades, 2001; George & Brief, 1992). Specifically, positive
mood has been .shown to promote creative thinking. The longitudinal nature of our
research contributes to the literature tbat bas established a relationship between
optimal experiences at work and positive mood by suggesting tbat this association is
consistently reliable across time and situations. Clearly flow is an important positive
psycbological variable tbat has been found to be associated withflourishingand one tbat
can be influenced by tbe design of work.

Potential limitations of the research


Although the ESM used in tbe current study offers considerable advantage to tbe study of
flow and other aspects of organizational behaviour, our application of the method
suggests several potential limitations to be addressed by future researcb. First,
participants in tbe research were architecture students engaged in work-like but not
work tasks suggesting the need to replicate our findings in the context of paid
employment. The experience of employment is more tban perfonning a set of tasks
{Kelloway, Gallajbcr, & Barling, 2005) and the extent to whicb ourfindingsgeneralize to
the context of paid employment remains an empirical question.
Second, it couid be argued that tbere was not much variation in the nature of the
work that we investigated, that is architectural studio work. Cxmsequently any within-
subject variance could be attributable to dispositional variations rather than a change in
the work situatiijn. However our sample was drawn from three different studio sessions.
Students were responsible for designing unique solutions to architectural problems, Tbe
nature of the work varied considerably and included a variety of tasks such as reading,
drawing, computer programming, model building, writing, computer rendering and
data analysis. Nonetheless, the variance in task characteristics was relatively small.
However, this would indicate tbat the strength of relationships between tbe job
cbaracteristics and flow were underestimated in our sample.
Third, in this study we focus on a single 'narrow-band:' indicator of well-being
(i.e. mood). In doing so, we parallel earlier studies which have also focused on tbe
relation between flow and mood (e.g. Eisenberger et ai, 2005) Broader conceptuali-
zations of well-being would suggest going beyond measures of affect to include
measures sucb as aspiration or self-efficacy (e.g. Warr, 1987) and future research would
Flow at work 611

be well advised to consider broadening the domain of inquiry to more fully understand
the relationship between flow and well-being.
Finally, we used a short composite measure of flow in our research. This was
primarily dictated by the ESM that we used, that necessitates the use of short scales that
are minimally disruptive of the experience being assessed. There seems to be confusion
as to an appropriate psychometric form with which to assess flow. There is a growing
body of theory to suggest that flow is a multidimensional construct (Jackson & Marsh,
1996; Marsh & Jackson. 1999), yet these dimensions are highly correlated. Furthermore,
composite operationalizations of flow fail to distinguish between the preconditions
or antecedents of flow (e.g. challenge-skill balance, clear goals) and the components
of optimal experience (e.g. enjoyment of. and concentration on the activity).
Future research needs to move away from uni-dimensional conceptualizations of flow,
and to distinguish between the antecedents and qualities of the construct.

Conclusion
Recently there has been an interest in the application of positive psychology to
understanding organizational behaviour (Cameron et al., 2003; Luthans, 2001, 2002;
Turner et al.. 2002). Flow is a construct that is at the centre o the ptjsitivc psychology
movement (Seligman & Csikszentmihalyi, 2000) and provides researchers with a
valuable conceptual framework with which to study the nature and process of w<rk
experiences. Luthans has termed the application of positive psychology to workplace
issues positive organizational behaviour (POB; Luthans, 2002). He defines POB as the
study and application of positively oriented human resource strengths and
psychological capacities that can be measured, developed, and effectively managed
for performance improvement' (Luthans, 2002, p. 58). The focus of POB is on strengths
and virtues that are measurable and contribute to better performance. Luthans (2002)
outlines several criteria associated with constructs if they are to make a contribution to
POB. Specifically they should be positive, measurable, capable of being developed, and
associated with optimal performance. Our research has indicated that flow cicarh' fulfils
these criteria. Furthermore, in order for a POB construct to be developmental it has to
be state-like in nature. We have sbown that fiow is predominantly a state construct. This
is concordant with C^sikszentmihalyi's (1990) definition of flow as engagement in a
specific activity. He defined flow as the state in which people are so intensely involved
in an activity that nothing else seems to matter; the experience itself is so enjoyable that
people will do it even at great cost, for the sheer sake of doing it' (Csikszcntmihalyi,
1990, pp. 3-4). Luthans (2002) has identified five variables that fulfil the criteria for
making a contribution to POB: confidence/self-efficacy; hope; optimism; SWB/happi-
ness; and emotional intelligence. The current research demonstrates that flow also has
the potential to be a meaningful positive psychological construct that mediates the
relationship between certain core characteristics of work and well-being.

References
Allen. B. P., & Potkay, C. R. (1981). On the arbitrary distinction between states and traits, foumal
of PersonafHy and Social Psychology. 41. 916-928.
Allport. G. W.. & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-name.s; A psycho-lexical study. Psychological
Monographs, 47(1, Whole No. 211). 171-220.
612 Clive J. Fultagar an E. Ksvin Ketloway

Bakker, A. B. (2005). Flow among music teachers and their students: The crossover of peak
experiences. Joitmal of Vocational Behcwior, 66. 26-44.
Bakkcr, A. II. (2008). The work related ilow inventor)': Construction and initial validation of the
\i/OLf. Joumtii of Vocational Behavior, 72, 400-414.
Barrett, D. J., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2000). The experience^ampUng program (ESP). Retrieved
from http://www2.bc.edu/ ~ barrelli/esp/
Behson, S. J., Eddy, E. R., & Lorenzet, S. J. (2000). The importance of the critical psychological
states in the job characteristics model: A meta-analytic and structural equations modeling
examination. Ciirrenl Research in Social Psychology, 5(12), 170-189.
Bloch, C. (2002). Moods and the quality of liie. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 101-128,
Bryce, j . , it Haworth, J. (2002). Well-being and flow in sample of male and female office workers.
Leisutv Studies, 21, 249-263.
Bryk, A. S., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1992). Hierarchical linear models: AppUcatiom and data
analysis methods. Ihousand Oaks. CA: Sage.
Bryk, A. S., Raudenbush, S. W., & Congdon. R. X, Jr. (2000). HLM S for windows. Chicago, IL:
Scientific Software International.
Cameron, K, S., Dutton, J. E., & Quinn. R. E. (Bds.), (2003)- Pf>sitire orficniizational scholarship:
Foundations of a new discipline. San Fransisco, CA: Berrett-Koeliler Publishers.
Carli, M., Delle Fave, A., & Massimini, F (1988). Tlie quality of experience in tbe flow channels:
Comparison of Italian and U.S. students. In M. Csikszcntniibaiyi & 1. S. ([isikszentmihaiyi (Eds.),
Optimalexpciienci': I'sychoiogicalstudiesoftnc in conscioiis/uvs {pp. 26(i-50(^). New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Clarke. S. G., & Haworth, J. T. (1994). Flow' experience in ibe daily lives of sixth-form college
students. British Journal of Psychology. 85, 511-523.
Cropanzano, R.. i Wrigbi. T. A. (2(M>1 ), When a happy' worker is a productive' woriver: A review
and further refinement of the happy-productive worker thesis. Consulting Psychology
Journal: Practice and Research, 53, 182-199.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Bey-oml boredom and anxiety: Experiencing flow in work and play.
San Fransisco, CA: Josscy-Bass.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flour The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper
and Row.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). The evolving self. New York: Harper and Row,
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding ow: The pr^chology of engagement with everyday life.
New York: Basic BcMiks.
(Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1988). Introduction to part IV. In
M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszenimihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological
studies ofjlow in consciousness (pp. 251-265). New 'Vork: Cambridge Univcrsit)' Press.
Csikszentmihalyi. M., & Hunter, J. (2003). Happiness in everyday life: The uses of experience
sampling. /o//77-/ of Happiness Studies, 4, 185-199.
Csikszentmihaiyi, M., & Lar.son, R. (1984). Being adolescent: Conflict and growth in the teenage
years. New York: Basic B(H)ks.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989). Optimal experience In work and leisure. yoMrn/o/
Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 815-822.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Rathunde, K. (1992). The measurement of flow in everyday life: Toward a
tlieor>' of emergent motivation. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Developmental perspectives on
niotiration: Voiume if) of the Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 5^-97). Lincoln,
Nebraska: University of Nebra.ska Press.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde. K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers: A longitudinal
study of their development. New York: c:ambridge University Press.
Demerouti, E. (2006). Job characteristics, flow, and performance: Tbe moderating role of
conscientiousness. yo/rf/ of Occupational Health Psychology, 11, 266-280.
Flow at work 613

Diener, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Personality and subjective well-being. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener,
& N. Schwarz (Eds.), Welt-being: The foundations of hedonic psychoiog)' (pp. 213-229).
New York; Russell Sage.
Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personalit>' structure oaect. Journal of Personatit)'
and Sociai Psycholog)', 69, 130-141.
Diener, E., Suh, E., Lucas, R, E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being; Three deeadcs of
progress. Ps)>chological Bulletin. 125. 276-302.
Eisenberger, R., Jones, J. R..Stinglhamber, F.Shanock, L.,&RandaU. A. T (2()0S). Flow experiences
at work: For high need achievers alone? Journal of Organizatiotial Behavior, 26, 755-775.
Eisenberger. R., & Rhoades, L. (2001). Incremental effects of reward on creativity-./owrwrt/ of
Personality and Sociai Psychology. St. 728-741.
Fave, A. D., & Massimini, F (1988). Modernization antl the changing contexts of flow in work and
leisure. In M. Csikszentmihahi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience:
Psychologicai studies of flow in consciousness (pp. 193-213). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Frederickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive cmotionsi* Review of Gnerai Psycholog)\ 2,
300-319. !
Frederickson. B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions n positive psychology': The Broaden-and-
Build rheor>' i)f positive emotions. American Psychologist. 56, 218-226.
Fried, Y, & Ferris, G. R. (1987). The validity of the job characteristics model: A review and meta-
anal)'sis. Personnel Psychotogy, 40, 287-322.
Fujita, E, & Diener, E. (2(M)'>). Ufc satisfaction set point: Stability and change. Jourtial of
Personality and Social P.sychotogy, 88. I 58-164.
Gardner, H. (2002). Good work, well done: A psych()l(gic;il study. Chronicle ofHigljer Education.
February 22, 7.
Gavin. J. F, & Axelrod, W. L. (1977). Managerial stress and strain in a mining oi^anization.
Journal of Vocationat Behavior. II. 6 6 - 7 4 .
G e o i ^ , J. M., 8t Brief, A. P (1992). Feeling good-doing good: A conceptual analysis of the mood
at work-organizational spontaneity relationship. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 310-329.
Hackman. J. R.. & Oklham, (i. R. (197S). Development of the joh diagmslic ?.\.iv\ey. founud of
Appiied Psychotogy, 60, 159-170.
Hackman. J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1980). Work redesign. Reading. MA: Addison-We.sley.
Han, S. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and fiow in eldtrl)' Korean immigrants.
ln M. Csikszentmihalyi i 1. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optttnat e.xperience: Psychoiogicai
studies ofjlow in counscisousness (pp. 138-149). New York: (Cambridge University Press.
Haworth, J. T., & Hill, S. (1992). Work, leisure, and psychological well-being in a sample of young
adults. Jourtial of Communit)- and Applied Social Psychoiog): 2(2). 147-160.
Hofmann, D. A., Griifin, M. A., & Gavin, M. B. (2000). The application of hierarchical linear
modeling to organizational research. In K. Klein & S. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multiievei theory.
researcij. and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions, and new directions
(pp. 467-511). San Francisco. CA: Jossey-Bass.
flormiith. S, H. (1986). Tlie SAmpMngoftxpt^cnccs in situ. Journat of Perstinatity. 5. 262-293.
Hull, H. B. (1991). Mood as a product of leisure: Causes and consequences. In B. L. Driver,
P J. Brown, & G. L. Peterson (Eds.), Benefits of teisure (pp. 250-262). State College, PA:
Venture Publishing.
Ilies. R., & Judge. T A. (2002). Understanding the dynamic relationships among personality, mood,
and joh satisfaction: Afieldexperience sampling study. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 89. 1119-1139-
Jackson, D. (1984). Personality research form manual. Port Huron, MI: Research Psycho
logists Press.
Jackson, S. A. (1996). Toward a conceptual understanding of the flow experience in elite athletes.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport. 67(1), 76-90.
614 Clive ]. Fullagar and E. Kevin Kelloway

Jackson, S. A., & Eklund. R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: The flow state scale-2 and
dispositional flow scaic-2. Jounud of Sport and E.xercise Psychotogy, 24. 133-l''O.
Jackson, S, A., ik Kklund, R. C. (2004). The Jlow scides manual. Morgantown, WV: Fitness
Information Technology.
Jackson. S. A., Kimiecik, J, C;.. Ford. S., & Marsh, H. W. (1998). Psychologicai correlates of How in
span. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychotogy, 20, 358-378.
Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a seale to measure optimal
experience: The flow state scale. Joumal of Sport and Exercise Psychoo>y. 18, 17-35.
Johns, G., Xie,J. L., &Fang, Y. (1992). Mediating and moderating effects in job desigj}. Joumal of
Management, 18, 657-676.
Kahneman, D. (1999). Objective happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Well-being: We fotmdations of hedonic psy^chology (pp. 3-25). New York: Russell Sage.
Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.), (1999). Well-being: The foundatitms of hedonic
psychology. New York: Russell Sa};e.
Kelloway, F. K.. (iallagher, D., & Barling, J. (2005). Work employment and the individual.
In B. Kaufman (Ed.), Theoretical perspectives on work and the employment relationship
(pp. 105-132). Champaign, IL: Industrial Relations Research Association.
Kimieeik, J. C , & Stein, (i. L. (1992). Examining flow experiences in sports contexts: Coneeptual
issues and methodological concerns. Journat of Applied Sports Psychoiog^\ 4, 144-160.
King, L. A., & Napa, C. K. (1998). What makes life good? Joumal of Personality and Social
Psychology. ^5. 156-165.
Larson, R., & Richards, M. H.(1994). Divergent realities: The emotional lives of mothers, fathers,
and idolescents. New York: Basic Books.
LeFevre. J. (1988). Flow and tht qualitj' of experience during work and leisure. In
M. Csikszentmihalyi & I, Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: Psychological studies
ofjtow in corLsciousness (pp. 307-518). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lutbans, F (2001). i'be case tor positive organizationai bebavior (.POB). Current Issues in
Management, /, 10-21.
Lutbans, F (2002). Positive organizational behavior: Developing and managing psycbological
strengths. Academy of Management Executive, /6(I), 57-72.
Marsh, H. W, & Jackson, S. A. (1999). Flow experiences in spon: Constnict validation of
multidimensional, hierarchical state and trait responses. .Structurai Equation Modelling, 6,
343-371.
Massimini, F, & Carii, M. (19H8). The systematic assessment of flow in daily lift-. In
M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csiks7x'ntniibalyi (Eds,), Optimai experience: Psychotogicat studies
of Jlow in counsdsousness (pp. 266-287). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Massimini, F., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & CarIi, M. (1987). Tbe monitoring of optimal experience: A tool
for psychiatric rehabilitation.yo//ri;rt/ oj'Nervous and Mental Disease. I ~5(9), 545-549.
McC>regor, I.. & Litlle, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: On doing well and
being yourself. Journai of Persomitity and Snciat Psychology, 74, 494-512.
Miscbel, T. (1969). Scientific and philosophical psychology: A Iiistorical introduction. In T. Mischel
(Ed.), Human action (pp. 1-40). New York: Academic Press.
Nakamuni, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). TIic concept of flow. In C. R. Snyder &J. S. Lopez
(Eds.), Handbook of posittvepsychology (pp. 89-105). New York: Oxford University Press.
Ng, T. W. H., Sorenseon, K, L., & Eby, L. T. (2006), Locus of control at work: A meta analysis.
Joumal of Organizational Behavior. 21, 1057-1087.
O Brien. (}. E. (1983). Skill-iitiliz;uion. skill-variety atid the job characteristics model. Australian
Joumal of Psychology, J 5 . 455-460.
Oklbam, G. R. (1996). Job design. In C. L. Cooprt I. T. Robertson (Eds.), Intemational review of
industrial and organizational psychology' CWoX. 11, pp. 33-60). Cbichester, UK: Wiley.
Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoc, J.. & Ryan. R. M. (2(KH)). Daily well being: Tbe
role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
26,419-435.
Flow at vifork 615

Renn. R. W., & Vandenberg, R. J. (1995). Tlie critical psychological states: An urulerrcpresented
component in job characteristics model research. Jotirtiiil of Manu}'eweut. 21, 279-303.
Ryan. R. M.. & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness und human potentials: A review of research on
hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psycb(tl(>}>}\ ^2. 1- I-U>6.
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? ExploratUms on the meaning of psychologicul
well-being. Joumal of Peisoncttity and Sociat Psychology, 57, 1069-1081.
Ryff. C^ D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life, entrent Directions in Psycbological
Science, 4,99-104.
Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of p.sychological well-being revisited.youma/
of Persnnaiit}' anil Social i\s;i'cholog}', 69, 719-727.
Ryff. C. D.. & Singer, B. ( 1998). The contours of positive human health. Psycbological Inquiry,
9, 1-28.
ScoUon, C. N., Diener, E., Oishi, S., & Biswas-Diencr. R. (2(H)5). An experience saniplinfi and cross-
cultural investigation of the relation between pleasant and unpleasant affect. Cognition ami
Emotion. /9, 27-52.
ScoUon. C N.. Kim-Prieto. C , & Diener, E. (2003). Experience sampling: Promises and pitfalls,
strengths and weaknesses, yo/zr/ of Happiness Studies, 4, 5-34.
Seligman. M. E. P (1999). The presidents address. American Psycbologist. 54, 559-562.
Seligman. M. E. P. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American
Psycboiogist. 55(1), 5-14.
Seongyeul. H. (1988). The relationship between life satisfaction and flow in elderly Korean
immigrants. In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszcntmiiialyi (Eds.). Optimal experience:
Psycbotogical studies of flow in conscious7iess (pp. 158-149). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Sheldon, K. M.. Ryan. R. M.. & Reis, H. T. ( 1996). Wliat makts fbr a good day? Competence and
autonomy in the day and in the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
1270-1279.
Smyth, J. M.. & Stone, A. A. (2003). Ecological momentary assessment research in behavioral
mciWcine. Journal of Happiness Studies. 4. 35-^2.
Staw, B. M.. & Barsade, S. G. (1993). Affect and majierial performance: A test of the sadder-but-
wiser vs. happier-and-smarter hypotheses. Administratife Scietue Quarterly, ,^8. 304-331.
Taber, T D., & Taylor, E. (1990). A review and evaluation of the psychometric properties of the job
diagnostic survey. Personnel Psycbology. 4,i. 467-')OO.
Turner, N.. Barling, J., & Zacharatos, A. (2002). Positive psycholog>' at work. In C. R. Snyder &
S. J. Lopcx (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp.71S-728). New York: Oxford
University Press.
Voelkl, J. E. (1990). The challenge skill ratio of daily experiences among older adults residing
in institutional environments. Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 24i2), 7-17.
Warr. P B. (1987). Work, unemployment und mental healtb. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Warr, P B. (1999). Well-being in the workplace. In D. Kahneman. E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.),
Well-being: The foundations of bedonic psycbology (pp. 392-412). New York: RusscI Sage.
Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: (x)ntra.sts of personal expressiveness
(eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64,
678-91.
Wright, T. A., & Bonett. D. G. (2007). Job satisfaction and psychological well-being as non-additive
predictors of workplace turnover./o/zr;// of Management, .., 141-160.
Wriglit, T A.. & Staw. B. M. (1999). Affect and favorable work outcomes: Two longitudinal tests of
the happy-productive worker thesis./o//ri// of Organizational fiehavior. 20, 1 -23
Zuckerman, M. (1983). The distinction between trait and state .scales is not arbitrary: Comment
on Allen and Potkay's 'On the arbitrary distinction between traits and States'. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1083-1086.

4 April 2008; revised version received 13 August 2008

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen