Sie sind auf Seite 1von 16

Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Early Childhood Research Quarterly

Review

Inuence of language nutrition on childrens language and cognitive


development: An integrated review
Lauren Head Zauche a, , Taylor A. Thul a , Ashley E. Darcy Mahoney a ,
Jennifer L. Stapel-Wax b,c
a
Emory University, Nell Hodgson Woodruff School of Nursing, 1520 Clifton Road NE, Atlanta, GA 30322, United States
b
Marcus Autism Center, Childrens Healthcare of Atlanta, 1920 Briarcliff Rd, Atlanta, GA 30329, United States
c
Emory University, School of Medicine, Department of Pediatrics, 1648 Pierce Drive NE, Atlanta, GA 30307, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Early childhood is a critical period for language and cognitive development. Evidence suggests that chil-
Received 18 July 2015 dren need language nutrition, or language-rich interactions with caregivers, for optimal language and
Received in revised form 22 January 2016 cognitive development. This integrated review was conducted to evaluate the inuence of language
Accepted 25 January 2016
nutrition, through talking, interacting, or reading, in early childhood and language or cognitive develop-
ment. Articles published from 19902014 were identied through PubMed, CINAHL, and Web of Science
Keywords:
databases and through reference lists of identied articles. Of the 1273 articles identied, 103 articles
Early childhood
met the search criteria. Aspects of speech, including the quantity of words, lexical diversity, linguistic
Language nutrition
Cognitive development
and syntactical complexity, intonation, and prosody, all contribute to the comprehension and produc-
Language development tion of language through enhancing speech processing, phonemic awareness, word segmentation, and
Literacy knowledge of grammatical rules. In addition to features of language, the delivery of language contributes
Parentchild interactions to variance in developmental outcomes. Language delivered in the context of an adultchild interaction
characterized by responsiveness and positive regard helps to scaffold a childs learning and encourages
verbal behaviors. Additionally, shared reading increases language and literacy skills by introducing new
vocabulary and facilitating dialogue between children and adults. In conclusion, studies consistently
demonstrate that quantity and quality of talking, interacting, and reading with a child in the rst three
years of life are strongly associated with language and cognitive development as well as school readi-
ness and academic performance. As a result, interventions aimed at increasing the quality and quantity of
language nutrition have the potential to leverage dramatic results for childrens developmental outcomes.
2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 319
2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 320
3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 320
3.1. Description of samples . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 320
3.2. Description of study designs and methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 321
3.3. Description of study purposes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 322
3.4. Findings related to parent or caregiver talk and child development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 322
3.4.1. Quantity of words . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 322
3.4.2. Lexical diversity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 323
3.4.3. Linguistic productivity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 324
3.4.4. Syntax . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 324
3.4.5. Intonation and prosody . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 324
3.4.6. Gestures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 325

Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: lmhead@emory.edu (L.H. Zauche), taylor.ann.thul@emory.edu (T.A. Thul), ashley.darcy@emory.edu (A.E.D. Mahoney),
jennifer.stapel-wax@emory.edu (J.L. Stapel-Wax).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.015
0885-2006/ 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 319

3.5. Findings related to parent/caregiver conversations and child development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 325


3.5.1. Responsiveness . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 325
3.5.2. Positive affect and sensitivity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 326
3.5.3. Cognitive-stimulating interactions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 327
3.6. Findings related to parent/caregiver book reading and child development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 327
3.6.1. Frequency of book reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 327
3.6.2. Quality of book reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 328
3.6.3. Other literacy-promoting activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 329
4. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 329
5. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 330
Funding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 331
Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 331
Appendix A. Supplementary data . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 331
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 331

1. Introduction

Early childhood is a critical period for developing language et al., 2009; Hoff, 2013). In a renowned study by Betty Hart and
skills, including learning to understand and speak language Todd Risley, children in low-income families heard, on average,
(National Research Council, 2000; National Scientic Council thirty-million fewer words than children in more afuent families
on the Developing Child, 2007). Research suggests that neural from birth to the age of three (Hart & Risley, 1995). This inequality
networks for language acquisition are present before birth and in language nutrition, referred to as the word gap, has been
that children begin to learn language in utero, suggesting that shown to result in disparities in language and cognitive outcomes
infants brains are primed to learn language (Kisilevsky et al., 2009; for children as young as 18 months old and recently has become
Perani et al., 2011). Throughout the rst three years of life, 85% a key target for improving educational opportunities on a national
of all neuronal connections, including those involved in language level (Fernald, Marchman, & Weisleder, 2013; Halle et al., 2009).
learning, are formed in response to environmental experiences The critical role of language nutrition for language and cognitive
and the majority of children begin to talk by the age of three development can be understood through the usage-based theory
(National Research Council, 2000). of language acquisition (Tomasello, 2009). The usage-based theory
Language learning is crucial for a childs developmental trajec- of language acquisition posits that language learning occurs on
tory. Language skills enable a child to communicate with others the basis of linguistic input and through generalizations made
in his or her environment, which encourages the development of by understanding how others use language (Tomasello, 2009). In
cognitive skills and promotes socio-emotional regulation through order to learn language, children must discern the intentions of
social interactions. Evidence indicates that a childs vocabulary speakers through social cognitive processes and must nd patterns
at the age of three is a key predictor of a childs ability to read at in language to create abstract linguistic constructions (Tomasello,
third grade, which is a powerful predictor of subsequent academic 2009). In other words, children learn language by extracting it
success; children who cannot read at grade level by the end of from a larger utterance and connecting it to the relevant aspects
third grade are four times more likely to drop out of high school of the experience shared with another person. This can be done
than those who can read (Dickinson & Porche, 2011; Fiester & through identifying words of a particular type (categorization),
Smith, 2010; Rowe, Raudenbush, & Goldin-Meadow, 2012). In fact, forming sequential units (chunking), memory, mapping a known
a childs language skills more strongly predict third grade reading pattern onto a new form (analogy), and linking form to meaning
comprehension than parent income, ethnicity, and level of parent (cross-modal association) (Tomasello, 2009). As such, this theory
education (Dickinson & Porche, 2011). In the 2013 Nations Report suggests that language learning occurs through language input by
Card, the National Assessment of Educational Progress reported means of social interactions and cognitive processes that serve to
that only 35% of fourth graders in the United States could read at make sense of social communication.
grade level (National Center for Education Statistics, 2013). This Vygotskys sociocultural theory and Bronfenbrenners ecologi-
low literacy rate among school children has enormous educational, cal systems theory further describes the role of social interactions
health, and economic implications, not only for the individuals on the language and cognitive development of young children
but also for the nation (Fiester & Smith, 2010; National Center for (Brofenbrenner, 2005; Vygotsky, 2005). Vygotskys sociocultural
Health Statistics, 2012; Robert Wood Johnson Foundation, 2009; theory illustrates this relationship through the concept known as
Sum, Khatiwada, McLaughlin, & Palma, 2009). Given that a childs the zone of proximal development (Vygotsky, 2005). The zone of
language skills predict third grade reading comprehension, strate- proximal development focuses on the area between what children
gies to promote language learning in early childhood are needed are able to do independently and what they are capable of doing
in order to improve educational outcomes in our nations children. with guidance from another, more capable person (Vygotsky,
One strategy to promote language learning may be through 2005). Language nutrition may help a childs learning by providing
improving the childs early language environment by increasing a children with the opportunity to learn new words and concepts
childs access to language nutrition, or early language exposure and to participate in conversations. Likewise, Bronfenbrenners
that is rich in both quantity and quality and may occur in a variety ecological systems theory proposes that a childs development is
of different wayssimply through talking, interacting, or reading inuenced by the environment and the people with whom the
with a child. Language nutrition refers to the idea that an envi- child interact (Brofenbrenner, 2005). Thus, these theories provide
ronment with sufcient language exposure is critical to facilitate, additional support that learning and development occurs through
or nourish, a childs brain (The Campaign for Grade Level Reading, social relationships and interactions between a child and the
2014). Indeed, research has demonstrated that language nutrition people in his or her surrounding environment (Brofenbrenner,
from parents and caregivers is critical for a childs neurodevelop- 2005; Vygotsky, 2005).
ment, including both language and cognitive skills (Forget-Dubois
320 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

As interventions to bridge the word gap develop around the ing the inclusion or exclusion of certain articles were discussed
country, it is important to understand the relationship between among the authors. The most common reasons for article exclu-
language nutrition during the rst three years of life and subse- sion based on title, abstract, or full-text review were related to the
quent language and cognitive outcomes in children. The purpose of age of study participants, purpose of article, exposure of interest
this integrated review was to investigate the inuence of language and outcome measures, and type of study. Fig. 1 details the process
nutrition in the rst three years of life on language and cognitive of article identication.
outcomes by examining available literature. Information about each article was coded and entered into a
table. Information extracted from each article included: the pur-
pose and design of each study; study size, participant ages, and
2. Methods characteristics of the sample; instruments used to assess parent
talk, reading, or engagement; outcome measures; results; and limi-
An integrated review of the literature was conducted on the tations. Two authors entered information about each article into the
inuence of language nutrition, which includes caregiver lan- tables under the supervision of another author. A summary table
guage input, social interactions, and shared reading experiences, can be found in Supplementary material of this article.
during early childhood, on subsequent language and cognitive out-
comes. An integrative review is a research review method that
allows for the simultaneous inclusion of diverse methodologies, 3. Results
variables, issues, and populations; as such, the integrative review
methodology enables a variety of perspectives to be synthesized 3.1. Description of samples
systematically (Whittemore & Kna, 2005). Talking was dened
as any word input, including both quantity and quality of speech. The samples included in this review varied greatly in terms of
Engagement was dened as parent interactions with the child. the childrens current developmental status, socioeconomic back-
Vocabulary included both receptive and expressive vocabulary. ground, and languages spoken in the home. Eight studies (7.8%)
Language outcomes included language acquisition and conversa- included samples of very preterm infants, four study samples (3.9%)
tional skills and cognitive outcomes included measures of executive consisted of children with permanent hearing loss, and nine studies
function and academic achievement. A search strategy was devel- (8.8%) included samples of children with language delays. Informa-
oped by three of the authors with terms related to outcomes, tion about socioeconomic background was not always reported but
exposures, and population of interest. The following search terms was primarily reported using maternal education or income as a
were used: (infant OR baby OR newborn OR toddler) AND (infant- proxy based on actual household income, eligibility for Early Head
directed speech OR child-directed speech OR talk OR read OR Start programs, eligibility for Medicaid, or geographic area. Other
engagement OR interact) AND (parent OR caregiver) AND (literacy socioeconomic factors that were considered in studies included
OR language acquisition OR vocabulary OR cognition OR language neighborhood safety, stressful life events, and unemployment.
development OR neurodevelopmental outcomes). To maximize the Nearly a quarter of the studies (N = 23) specically mentioned that
sensitivity of the search strategy, no lter for type of study was used. their sample consisted of children from low-income families. The
The search was limited to articles published in the English language entire sample of four studies were enrolled in an Early Head Start
between January 1990 and August 2014. A systematic search proto- program. There were twelve studies (11.8%) in which the childrens
col was followed using the dened search terms and was conducted rst language was not English; of these studies, 75% (N = 8) spoke
through the following electronic databases: PubMed, Web of Sci- Spanish. Other languages included German, Swiss, Italian, and Man-
ence, and Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature darin. Mothers represented the majority of parents or caregivers
(CINAHL). Reference lists of identied relevant articles were man- for which language nutrition was assessed. Only 8.8% of the studies
ually searched for additional eligible articles not detected by the (N = 9) considered the contributions of language nutrition provided
electronic search. by fathers.
All observational studies, randomized control trials, meta- Sample size ranged from three participants to 6,270. Excluding
analyses, and systematic reviews assessing the relationship reviews, the sample size of 39% of studies (N = 37) was less than
between language nutrition (talking, interacting, or reading during 50, with 25% of studies (N = 24) less than 30 participants. Twenty-
early childhood) and language, vocabulary, or cognitive outcomes seven percent (N = 26) of studies had between 51100 participants.
were included. Inclusion criteria were dened prior to beginning Studies with a sample size over 500 represented 12.6% (N = 12) of
the search. For inclusion, the articles had to meet the following the studies.
criteria: (1) examine talking, interacting, or reading by caregiver; At the time at which the input of the caregiver was assessed,
(2) children had to be 036 months old at the rst or all assess- participants ranged in age from 32 weeks corrected gestational
ments; (3) evaluate language or cognitive outcomes; (4) be a age to thirty-six months. Less than 10% (N = 9) of studies assessed
primary-research article, secondary data analysis, meta-analysis, caregiver input to children less than six months old. Twenty-
or systematic review; and (5) be in a peer-reviewed journal. Stud- nine percent of studies (N = 28) assessed infants between 611
ies were included even if the primary objective of the study was months. Nearly half of studies (47%) assessed toddlers between
not to evaluate the relationship between language nutrition (talk- 1217 months old; similarly, toddlers between 2429 months old
ing, reading, or interacting) and language or cognitive outcomes as and 3036 months old were assessed in 48% and 43% of the stud-
long as they included data on these exposures and outcomes. Titles, ies, respectively. Thus, the majority of the ndings in this review
abstracts, and full-text articles were reviewed for eligibility. are reective of language nutrition provided between the ages of
The combined search strategy yielded a total of total of 1480 1230 months and a minority of ndings relate to language nutri-
articles. After 207 duplicates were removed, 1273 articles remained tion received by children less than one year old.
for the initial review of titles and abstracts were marked for inclu- At the time at which language or cognitive outcomes were
sion or exclusion by the rst author using the criteria dened by all assessed, participants ranged in age from six months to eight years
authors. Following title and abstract review, 392 articles were iden- old. Six studies (6.2%) assessed children who were six to eleven
tied as potentially relevant and were included for full text review. months old; 20 studies (20.6%) assessed children between 1217
Following full text review, a total of 103 articles were identied as months; 16 studies (16.5%) assessed children who were 1823
meeting all of the specied criteria of the search. Questions regard- months old; 34 studies (35.0%) assessed 2429 month olds; 17
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 321

Fig. 1. Selection of articles.

studies (17.5%) assessed children who were 30-35 months old; 23 Excluding reviews, 69 (71.2%) of the studies assessed caregiver
studies (23.7%) assessed children between the ages of three and four input through observations, primarily through free play or semi-
years old; ten studies (9.7%) assessed four to ve year old children; structured play sessions that were video- or audio-recorded and
and ve studies (4.9%) assessed children over the age of ve. then either transcribed or coded for features of interest to the
study. Additionally, 17 studies (17.8%) assessed language nutri-
3.2. Description of study designs and methods tion through parent self-report and thirteen studies (13.4%) used
either the StimQ or Home Observation of Measurement of the
The studies employed a variety of research designs. Seventy- Environment, which assess parental verbal responsivity, parental
four (72%) of the articles were prospective cohort studies, in involvement in developmental activities, availability of learning
which the outcome was assessed at a different time point than materials, and reading activities in the home (Caldwell & Bradley,
the initial assessment of the childs language environment. The 2003; Dreyer, Mendelsohn, & Tamis-LeMonda, 2014). Other mea-
majority of these articles assessed caregiver speech or parentchild sures were also used within individual studies but were not
interactions at more than one time point. Eleven studies were consistently used across studies in the review.
experimental (10.7%) in which the primary aim was to evaluate Several studies used multiple instruments to assess childrens
an intervention aimed to improve shared book reading, increase developmental outcomes. The most commonly used standard-
caregiver talk, or promote caregiverchild interactions. Of these ized assessment to measure language or cognitive development
experimental studies, eight were randomized control trials. Ten was the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Invento-
studies (9.7%) were cross-sectional and six were reviews (5.8%). ries (MCDI). The MCDI are parent-completed instruments that
322 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

capture information about the development of vocabulary com- overall quantity whereas eleven studies focused on word frequen-
prehension and production, gesture use, and grammar in children cies. Signicant variability in the quantity of caregiver speech
ages 830 months (Fenson et al., 2007). Thirty-six (37.1%) of stud- directed towards infants or young children was observed in these
ies in the review assessed receptive and expressive language skills studies, with differences in linguistic input as large as eighteen-
using the MCDI. Other commonly used language tests included the fold (Hurtado, Marchman, & Fernald, 2008; Weisleder & Fernald,
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) (N = 16), which assesses 2013). Although variation in caregiver speech was observed across
receptive language skills in children over 30 months, the Preschool socioeconomic groups, large differences were also noted within
Language Scale, (N = 9), which assess developmental language skills samples that consisted of all middle-socioeconomic or all low-
of children up to eight years old and the Sequenced Inventory socioeconomic families. This variation highlights the importance
of Communication Development (N = 9), which assess receptive of considering a methodological approach that unpacks the de-
and expressive language skills in four month old children through nition of SES and evaluates the contributions of parent education,
the age of four years (Dunn & Dunn, 2007; Hendrick, Prather, & stress, living conditions, access to resources, neighborhood safety,
Tobin, 1984; Zimmerman & Castilleja, 2005). The majority of stud- and household income as individual factors.
ies that assessed cognitive development used the Bayley Scales A variety of measures were used to quantify caregiver speech,
of Infant Development Mental Development Index (BSID), which including recordings of motherchild interactions in which the
tests for childrens expressive and receptive language development, words were counted by study personnel or through digital language
sensory-perception knowledge, memory, attentional control, and processors that were developed by the LENA (Language Environ-
problem solving skills up to the age of 42 months (Bayley, 2006). mental Analysis) Research Foundation to capture all words spoken
Twenty-two studies in the review (22.6%) used the BSID. The in the infants or childs environment. Studies that utilized the
Reynell Developmental Language Scales and the Woodcock John- LENA digital language processors were able to assess child-directed
son Tests of Achievement were most commonly used for children speech over a much longer period of time in the home environment,
whose language or school readiness was assessed over the age of or up to 16 h each day, compared to studies that video or audio
three. The Reynell Developmental Language Scales is used to assess recorded a parentchild interaction for ve to ten minutes in either
vocabulary comprehension and expression in children ages three the home environment or a laboratory setting. As a result, studies
to seven years old and the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achieve- that utilized the LENA digital language processors may have greater
ment assess various aspects of academic achievement including validity as this technology may minimize the potential artifacts
letter/word identication, vocabulary comprehension and pro- introduced by observers.
duction, reading, writing, and mathematics (Edwards, Garman, Studies assessed the quantity of speech directed at children
Hughes, Letts, & Sinka, 1999; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2007). ranging in age from 32 weeks corrected gestational age to 19
months old and assessed child-directed speech between 18 months
3.3. Description of study purposes to 3.5 years. Outcome measures included word production during
recorded parent-child interactions as well as various standardized
Studies included in this review examined the inuence of the tests of language development and cognitive development.
quantity of words, lexical diversity, grammatical complexity, syn- One study evaluated the association between the quantity of
tactic diversity, and intonation and prosody in caregiver speech child-directed speech and cognitive outcomes. In a sample of very
and caregivers use of gestures on childrens receptive and expres- preterm infants, language input was assessed through LENA digital
sive language skills, cognitive development, grammar, syntax, and language processors in the neonatal intensive care unit when the
school readiness. Many studies looked at multiple features of care- infant was 32 weeks corrected gestational age (Caskey, Stephens,
giver speech. Additionally, studies addressed the contributions of Tucker, & Vohr, 2014). The number of words spoken to an infant
contingent and responsive speech, joint attention, and interactions accounted for 20% of the variance on cognitive scores on the Bayley
built on positive affect. Twenty studies (19%) in this review speci- Scales of Infant Development at 18 months corrected gestational
cally evaluated the inuence of the frequency, duration, and quality age (Caskey et al., 2014). While the study lacked sufcient power
of shared book reading on childrens development. and only controlled for birthweight as a potential confounder, these
The majority of studies, or 88% (N = 91), evaluated the rela- results suggest that the quantity of child-directed speech in the
tionship between language nutrition and expressive vocabulary earliest stages of infancy are signicantly related to a childs long-
skills whereas slightly fewer (80%, N = 83) looked at receptive lan- term cognitive development.
guage skills. Most of the studies that evaluated language skills All studies suggested that the quantity of linguistic input pre-
assessed both receptive and expressive language together. In addi- dicts later vocabulary of a child. Quantity of child-directed speech
tion, two studies assessed the relationship between language resulted in signicant variability in both expressive and recep-
nutrition and syntactical complexity of childrens speech and two tive language (Hurtado et al., 2008; Rowe, 2008; Rowe, 2012;
studies assessed childrens linguistic productivity, as measured by Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, 2012; Weisleder & Fernald, 2013;
mean length of utterance. Four studies evaluated the relationship Zimmerman et al., 2009). These results remained the same even
between caregiver language input and speech processing abilities. when controlling for earlier child vocabulary, ruling out the
Far fewer studies (31%, N = 33) examined the contributions of explanation that more talkative children elicit more speech from
caregiver language, interactions, and reading on childrens cogni- caregivers. In one study that assessed the quantity of child-directed
tive development, or executive function. Three studies specically speech using LENA digital language processors one day a month for
assessed literacy skills, three studies assessed school readiness, and 618 months in children 236 months old (13.8 10.0), each addi-
two studies assessed mathematical ability. tional 1000 words spoken to a child was associated with a 0.44 point
(95% CI: 0.090.79) increase on the Preschool Language Scale-4th
3.4. Findings related to parent or caregiver talk and child edition, which measures both receptive and expressive language
development (Zimmerman et al., 2009). Additionally, another study found that
each increase in standard deviation in the number of words from a
3.4.1. Quantity of words primary caregiver at 30 months old was positively associated with
Nineteen studies were categorized as evaluating the rela- a 0.54 standard deviation in the childs Peabody Picture Vocabulary
tionship between the quantity of words spoken to a child and Test (PPVT) scores at 42 months old (Shneidman, Arroyo, Levine, &
language outcomes. Eight of these studies specically focused on Goldin-Meadow, 2013). Interestingly, the relationships observed in
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 323

both studies were completely mediated by caregiver engagement to 36 months old. Sample size was very different as studies ranged
with the child through contingent comments or the back-and-forth in size from 15 to 1292 children. All studies that examined the
communication between a child and a caregiver (Shneidman et al., inuence of lexical diversity on language or cognitive outcomes
2013; Zimmerman et al., 2009). These results suggest that while reported signicant ndings.
the quantity of child-directed speech promotes the acquisition of Evidence suggests that the number of different words in adult
vocabulary, the social context in which these words are delivered speech is a predictor of language development apart from the
matters. quantity of words (Pan, Rowe, Singer, & Snow, 2005; Pancsofar,
More linguistic input also shapes childrens lexical processing Vernon-Feagans, & The Family Life Project Investigators, 2010;
efciency. Two studies evaluated the relationship between lan- Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow, 2012). In a study of 24-month-old
guage input and the lexical processing speed of children using Mayan children, each increase in standard deviation for the amount
the look-while-listening task (Hurtado et al., 2008; Weisleder & of different words in adult speech was associated with an increase
Fernald, 2013). This task involved presenting a child with pictures of 0.89 standard deviations in vocabulary scores on the PPVT and
of two familiar objects and with speech naming one of the objects. Early One Word Picture Vocabulary Test and accounted for 55% of
The gaze pattern of these children were videotaped in order to cap- variance in scores at 35 months (Shneidman & Goldin-Meadow,
ture the childs reaction time to look towards the target picture. 2012). In fact, lexical diversity may be a stronger predictor of lan-
Greater child-directed speech of caregivers was associated with guage development than the total amount of words directed at
faster processing speed and accounted for 1826% of the variance in an infant (Pan et al., 2005). In a prospective cohort study with
the mean reaction time to words, controlling for sociodemograph- a sample of 108 low-income mother-child dyads, the indepen-
ics (Hurtado et al., 2008). Additionally, processing speed mediated dent roles of quantity of words, lexical diversity, and gestures
the relationship between child-directed speech and vocabulary of maternal communication on childrens vocabulary production
knowledge, suggesting that faster processing speech reects a more were assessed for one to three year old children during free play
efcient uptake of lexical input provided by caregivers (Hurtado sessions (Pan et al., 2005). Diversity of maternal vocabulary pre-
et al., 2008; Weisleder & Fernald, 2013). Vocabulary knowledge and dicted growth in child vocabulary production and more strongly
processing efciency are interdependent and thus, it may also be predicted child vocabulary compared to the quantity of words (Pan
possible that as vocabulary develops, more rened processing skills et al., 2005). This relationship was strongest around 24 months
mature (Weisleder & Fernald, 2013). of life, suggesting that the developmental timing of lexical diver-
There are several mechanisms by which a greater quantity sity may be related to vocabulary outcomes (Pan et al., 2005).
of speech directed towards a child results in improved vocabu- This result also suggests that it may be possible that develop-
lary knowledge and development. First, more speech may provide mental timing may be important for other features of caregiver
children with more opportunities to interpret language and be speech.
exposed to words. This possible mechanism is supported by evi- Interestingly, two studies have suggested that the relationship
dence that suggests that the frequency of exposure to a certain between socioeconomic status and child language outcomes is at
word predicted knowledge of this specic word. This relation- least partially mediated by differences in caregiver lexical diversity
ship is seen both before and after a child begins talking with (Burchinal, Vernon-Feagans, & Cox, 2008; Huttenlocher, Waterfall,
words. Production of nouns, verbs, pronouns, numbers, and wh- Vasilyeva, Vevea, & Hedges, 2010). In one study with a sample of
question words (e.g., where, what, why, when) are all associated 1292 children from rural low-income communities, lexical diver-
with the frequency of concurrent or prior exposure to the word sity at six months was a mediator of the negative association
(Brent & Siskind, 2001; Gunderson & Levine, 2011; Hampson & between social risk factors, including maternal education, fam-
Nelson, 1993; Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer, & Lyons, 1991; ily income, single parent family, number of children in the home,
Lyytinen, Eklund, & Lyytinen, 2003; Majorano, Rainieri, & Corsano, number of stressful life events, unemployment, and neighborhood
2013; Naigles & Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998; OshimaTakane, Goodz, & safety, and infant cognitive outcomes at 15 months, as measured by
Derevensky, 1996; Rowland, Pine, Lieven, & Theakston, 2003; the Bayley Scales of Infant DevelopmentII (Burchinal et al., 2008).
Tardif, Shatz, & Naigles, 1997; Taumoepeau & Ruffman, 2006). In addition to lexical diversity, maternal sensitivity and warmth as
Second, it is possible that more exposure to language provides well as learning and literacy activities both mediated this associa-
more opportunities for children to develop and rene the skills tion as well (Burchinal et al., 2008). These results suggest that the
necessary for language learning, such as segmentation of words lack of access to learning opportunities and parental engagement
in speech and phonological awareness. Third, children may be may account for reasons why social risks negatively affect child
exposed to more semantics, syntactical compositions, and gram- outcomes (Burchinal et al., 2008).
matical combinations from which they can learn new words and Exposure to speech characterized by lexical diversity provides
develop their vocabulary as well as become more familiar with the children with more opportunities to expand their vocabulary by
structure and rules of the language spoken to them. Being able to learning new words. Hearing a different word not only enables a
better understand or predict the meaning of language input from child to learn the meaning of this word but also exposes a child to
caregiver may facilitate their ability to identify and interpret unfa- various phonological patterns. Additionally, exposure to a variety of
miliar words in sentences and contribute to greater vocabulary words allows children to hear words in different contexts or with
growth and language skills. Each of these possible explanations different descriptions, which may help children learn about that
are consistent with the usage-based theory of language acquisition word. For instance, the same word (ex. ball) may be used in two
(Tomasello, 2009). different sentences (ex. The ball is round and red and Im rolling
the ball to you!) to convey different information about that word.
3.4.2. Lexical diversity This diversity also helps a child learn more about that word. Simi-
Quantity of words is highly correlated with lexical diversity, larly, if a caregiver uses a variety of words in a sentence or phrase,
or the number of different words a parent speaks to his or her the child may know some but not all of the words the caregiver
child. Five prospective cohort studies examined the association says. Knowing other word meanings in a sentence constrains pos-
between lexical diversity and language or cognitive outcomes. All sible interpretations of a new word and thus, the lexical diversity
studies assessed lexical diversity through videotaping interactions of the sentence may contribute to children learning the meaning
between the parent and the child. Ages of the children in the sam- of a new word. These studies highlight the importance of lexical
ples at the assessment of lexical diversity ranged from one month diversity in caregiver speech.
324 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

3.4.3. Linguistic productivity understand the meaning of new words being presented, given their
Linguistic productivity refers to the ability to use language to knowledge of the words usually associated with such syntax.
construct new phrases or sentences. Mean length of utterance Similarly, the position of words in a sentence may affect the ease
(MLU), or the average number of words in an utterance, is com- of language acquisition. Positional salience, or the position where
monly used to assess linguistic productivity because generally the word is most noticeable, may help infants and young children
longer utterances tend to be more grammatically complex, with extract the word from the rest of the sentence. For instance, verbs
a greater diversity of grammatical categories, such as verbs, nouns, positioned in the nal position of an utterance were found to be
and adjectives. While linguistic productivity is typically examined predictors of verb acquisition (Naigles & Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998). In
as a measure of language prociency and grammatical competen- another study, children ages two to four years identied more tar-
cies in children, ve studies assessed the relationship between get words in predictive rhyme conditions, or when the target word
linguistic productivity of parent speech and the language outcomes was at the end of a rhyming stanza or when there was a pause
of young children. Four of these studies were prospective cohort before the word (Read, 2014). These studies show that the way in
studies whereas the other study was cross-sectional. Three of the which new words are set up affects how well children learn and
ve studies utilized the same study population consisting of 63 retain new words.
children ages 1829 months in middle to high-income families
(Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998; Hoff, 2003; Hoff & Naigles, 2002). All studies 3.4.5. Intonation and prosody
assessed linguistic productivity through determining the MLU dur- Parentese, sometimes also referred to as motherse or
ing audio-recordings of a parentchild interaction, and used either infant-directed speech, is a style of communicating with infants
a standardized measure to assess vocabulary outcomes or assessed and young children that is seen across most cultures and languages.
language production in the audio-recordings. This speech is characterized by varied intonation and prosody,
Evidence suggests that increased MLU is associated with expanded pitch range, elongated vowel and consonant sounds and
increased lexical diversity in parent speech and advances a childs repetition. Two studies, one of which was a systematic review of
language learning (Bornstein, Haynes, & Painter, 1998; Hoff, 2003; 144 studies, investigated whether the intonation and prosody of
Hoff & Naigles, 2002). Both increases in vocabulary as well as caregiver speech affected the acquisition of language in children
increased MLU in childrens speech have been observed as a result (Saint-Georges et al., 2013; Song, Demuth, & Morgan, 2010).
of greater MLU in parental speech directed at an infant or young To evaluate the role of specic aspects of parentese, one study
child (Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998; Szagun & Stumper, 2012). These results manipulated the characteristics of speech directed at 19 month
suggest that greater MLU is associated with advancing more com- old toddlers. In this study, toddlers were presented with pictures
plex linguistic development. of a target word and a distractor after listening to Wheres the
? in either typical parentese style or in a way that lacked one
3.4.4. Syntax acoustic property of parentese (Song et al., 2010). The toddlers
Few studies have examined the relationship between the syntax were slower to look at the target word when vowels were not
in parent language and childrens language outcomes. Three stud- hyper-articulated or when the speech was not slow, suggesting
ies have explored whether a possible association exists between that slow speech and vowel hyper-articulation help a young child
syntactic structures in parent language and a childs language pick out words from sentences (Song et al., 2010). A systematic
development (Huttenlocher et al., 2010; Naigles & Hoff-Ginsberg, review on parentese suggested that word segmentation may also
1998; Read, 2014). Of these studies, one was a randomized con- be facilitated by the prosodic quality of parentese as varying pitch
trol trial that manipulated the location of various words and ways or lengthening words at the end of sentences may provide clues
in which language was presented whereas the other two studies about grammatical units and saliently denote utterance boundaries
were prospective cohort studies. All three studies assessed parent (Saint-Georges et al., 2013). Faster word segmentation and recog-
syntax by videotaping or observing an interaction between a child nition allows infants to process speech faster, which affects the
at least 14 months old and his or her parent. growth of language acquisition. This review also suggested that dif-
Differences in syntactic structures in language input may affect ferences in characteristics of speech directed at infants have been
a childs language growth. In addition to promoting vocabulary noted between mothers of late talkers and mothers of early talkers,
and grammatical complexity of child speech, syntactical diversity as mothers of earlier talkers were more likely to use more prosodic
in parent speech may contribute to a childs understanding and features in their speech (Saint-Georges et al., 2013).
use of syntax. In a study of 146 infants ages 1446 months, clausal Another mechanism by which parentese may aid in language
diversity, or ways of combining clauses, in child speech was pre- acquisition is through the social aspects associated with parentese.
dicted by maternal clausal diversity and the frequency of clausal use Parentese usually is accompanied by positive affect of the speaker
(Huttenlocher et al., 2010). Similarly, a childs constituent diver- and used most commonly for praising, playing, teaching, and com-
sity, or the use of adverbs, adjectives, prepositions, and possessives forting a young child (Saint-Georges et al., 2013). This emotion
in a clause, was also predicted by maternal constituent diversity is conveyed through parentese and the actual intonation and
(Huttenlocher et al., 2010). In other words, the frequency of specic prosodic patterns of parentese provide cues about a speakers
syntactic structures by mothers predicted the timing of acquisition communicative intent, whether it be to give comfort, approval, or
of those structures in childrens speech. Syntactic structures that ask for a childs attentional focus (Saint-Georges et al., 2013). Par-
were more frequently used in maternal speech occurred earlier in entese may also help infants learn about objects from other by
childrens speech. These ndings suggest that more syntactically conveying their feelings about these objects.
diverse maternal speech leads to more advanced syntactic devel- Findings from a systematic review have suggested that par-
opment in a childs productive language. entese changes as children develop in response to the childs
Syntactic frames may contribute to the ability of the child to language abilities. The pitch of parentese increases from birth
learn a word by providing additional clues about the words mean- until about six months at which the pitch begins to decrease slowly
ing. For example, verbs that appear with a prepositional phrase (Saint-Georges et al., 2013). Additionally, exaggeration of vowel or
usually involve motion, mental state verbs usually are accompa- consonant sounds is more frequently heard in parents speaking
nied by sentence complements, and causative verbs usually appear with infants compared to parents speaking with toddlers (Saint-
with a direct object (Naigles & Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998). By intro- Georges et al., 2013). These changes may be related to the change
ducing words to children in predictable ways, children can better in pragmatic functions of speech directed at a child as caregiver
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 325

speech becomes more informative and less affective as the child samples less than 50. Interestingly, the samples of fourteen studies
develops. However, no studies have evaluated changes over time were composed of children who had or were at risk for a language
with parentese and how these changes affect childrens language or cognitive delay. Six studies included very low birthweight chil-
or cognitive development. dren in their samples, three studies included children with hearing
loss, and ve studies included children who had a conrmed neu-
3.4.6. Gestures rodevelopmental delay. While a range of standardized assessments
Four studies examined the relationship between gestures and for language, cognition, and school readiness were used, the MCDI
language development in children. All four studies suggested that was used in half of the studies. When studies followed children past
greater gesture use during speech is related to more advanced four years of age, the Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale and Wood-
receptive and expressive language skills of children ranging in cock Johnson Tests of Achievement were regularly utilized to assess
age from 11 months to four years (Goodwyn, Acredolo, & Brown, various domains of development and school readiness.
2000; Hahn, Zimmer, Brady, Swinburne Romine, & Fleming, 2014; Multiple studies suggest that joint attention and speech con-
Schmidt & Lawson, 2002; Topping, Dekhinet, & Zeedyk, 2013). This tingent on the vocalizations or attention of children promotes
relationship was also observed in children who were at risk for lan- their acquisition of language and improves both their recep-
guage delays (Hahn et al., 2014; Schmidt & Lawson, 2002). While tive and expressive vocabulary outcomes (Camp, Cunningham, &
overall gesture use resulted in improved language skills, two stud- Berman, 2010; Girolametto, Sussman, & Weitzman, 2007; Haebig,
ies examined specic types of gestures. Both studies found that McDufe, & Ellis Weismer, 2013; Hoff, 2006; Majorano et al., 2013;
gestures that focused attention, such as proximal pointing, resulted Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Roberts & Kaiser, 2011; Rollins, 2003;
in the greatest number of speech responses from children and bet- Trautman & Rollins, 2006; Warren, Brady, Sterling, Fleming, &
ter expressive language skills (Hahn et al., 2014; Schmidt & Lawson, Marquis, 2010; Yoder & Warren, 2002). In fact, 1018 month old
2002). In fact, gestures that did not focus attention or were not cou- children whose parents scored less than four on the Parental Ver-
pled with speech did not account for any variance in verbal skills bal Responsivity subscale of the StimQ, an assessment of the home
(Hahn et al., 2014; Schmidt & Lawson, 2002). It is possible that lan- learning environment, were 4.33 times more likely to score below
guage improvements were seen only with gestures that focused a the twenty-fth percentile on the MCDI between 1830 months
childs attention because these gestures may help a child attend to compared to children of parents who scored four or greater (Camp
his or her environment and thus, help scaffold the childs learning. et al., 2010). These ndings are consistent with other studies that
These results suggest that the way parents combine gestures with suggest that caregiver verbal responsivity and joint attention are
speech is important. predictive of the timing at which language developmental mile-
Another way that gestures may be associated with vocabulary stones, such as rst word, use of two-word combinations, and use
development is through enhancing the ability of children to use of past-tense, are achieved and may account for up to 64% of vari-
gestures themselves. One review discussed ndings that gesture ance in vocabulary outcomes in toddlers (Nicely, Tamis-LeMonda,
use by children was a signicant predictor of vocabulary and com- & Bornstein, 1999; Rollins, 2003; Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, &
prehension and that the relationship between parent gestures and Baumwell, 2001; Topping et al., 2013). Additionally, greater gram-
a childs vocabulary size was mediated by the childs use of gesture matical complexity and syntax development has been observed in
(Topping et al., 2013). Infants initially communicate through non- children of verbally responsive caregivers (Hoff, 2006; Trautman
linguistic means, such as through gazes, sounds, and gestures, and & Rollins, 2006). Remarkably, these results are observed from tod-
thus, some words are initially learned and then produced through dlerhood into early school age as evidenced by improved school
gesture rather than through language. As a result, early gesturing readiness, academic performance, and reading comprehension
may be the rst step in the comprehension and production of a new (Dodici, Draper, & Peterson, 2003; Landry, Smith, & Swank, 2003;
word and may lay a foundation for verbal language development. Martoccio, Brophy-Herb, & Onaga, 2014; Taylor, Anthony, Aghara,
Smith, & Landry, 2008). Thus, joint attention and contingency help
3.5. Findings related to parent/caregiver conversations and child children develop receptive and expressive vocabulary, phonemic
development awareness, and school readiness skills.
Improvements in language and cognition are not only observed
3.5.1. Responsiveness in normally developing children but also in children who have or
Nearly one-third of articles, or 34 studies, identied in this are at risk for language or cognitive delays, such as children affected
review addressed the role of responsiveness, most often through by autism spectrum disorder, preterm birth, sensorineural hear-
contingent speech or joint attention, on childrens language or cog- ing loss, or fragile X syndrome (Haebig et al., 2013; Janjua, Woll,
nitive development. Contingent speech refers to caregiver speech & Kyle, 2002; Landry et al., 2003; Landry, Smith, Swank, Assel, &
that is delivered as a response to a childs communicative attempts Vellet, 2001; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Schmidt & Lawson, 2002;
(Hoff, 2006). The content of such speech is dependent upon the Smith et al., 1996; Warren et al., 2010). Greater reciprocity dur-
focus of the childs communication (Hoff, 2006). Contingency is ing play sessions at six months predicted higher cognitive skills as
closely related to the concept of joint attention as well, which refers measured by the Mental Development Index of the Bayleys Scale
to the focus of both the caregiver and child on an object and is of Infant Development at 12 months, controlling for infant and
achieved when one individual cues another to focus their attention maternal risks (Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). Interestingly, the qual-
on an object through either a verbal or non-verbal communication ity of the interaction mediated the relationship between risk and
exchange (Hoff, 2006). The establishment of joint attention involves cognitive development for low birthweight infants (Poehlmann &
contingent communication, and thus, the results of studies that Fiese, 2001). Some evidence suggests that the relationship between
looked at either contingent speech or joint speech are presented the quality of interactions and cognitive development is stronger
together. in high-risk children compared to low-risk children as the shared
All but seven studies had a prospective cohort design and engagement may promote the development of complex attentional
assessed contingent speech and joint attention by observing a processes that often are compromised in children with devel-
parentchild interaction, either in a free play session or a semi- opmental delays (Landry et al., 2003, 2001). By being a childs
structured session designed to facilitate teaching opportunities for conversational partner, a caregiver encourages the child to shift his
caregivers or to elicit play behaviors from the child. Ten of the stud- or her attention between the caregiver, an object, and back to the
ies had sample sizes less than 30 and nearly half of the studies had caregiver and coordinate his or her gestures and own vocalizations.
326 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

Periods of joint attention and contingent comments may held scaf- greater signicance of contingency compared to the quantity of
fold the childs learning by providing children with opportunities to caregiver speech (Camp et al., 2010; Rollins, 2003; Zimmerman
establish references for words and receive feedback from parents et al., 2009). In fact, some studies found that the relationship
within a shared context. As a result, both the rate of language and between the quantity of words and language outcomes is mediated
cognitive growth increase. by caregiver contingent comments, joint attention, or the back-
Examples of verbal strategies parents or caregivers use to facil- and-forth conversation between children and adults (Martoccio
itate joint attention and ways in which parents or caregivers speak et al., 2014; Rollins, 2003; Zimmerman et al., 2009). This nding
contingently include using imitations, interpretations, questions, suggests that quantity of words alone do not predict language skills
repetitions, object descriptions, and expansions. A few studies have but rather, that the quality of caregiver speech, namely the conver-
closely examined the use of these strategies and their inuence on sational aspect of speech, accounts for varying courses of language
language outcomes and found that they were all correlated with development in children.
larger vocabularies, greater language production, and more gram- The importance of social engagement through back-and-forth
matically complex speech in children (Cruz, Quittner, Marker, & communication is highlighted further through evidence that sug-
DesJardin, 2013; Girolametto et al., 2002; Girolametto, Weitzman, gests that the words that a child hears need to be directed at the
Wiigs, & Pearce, 1999; Hampson & Nelson, 1993; Hoff, 2006; child in order for them to benet the child. Overheard speech and
Levickis, Reilly, Girolametto, Ukoumunne, & Wake, 2014; Majorano media exposure are not positively associated with childrens lan-
et al., 2013; Pancsofar & Vernon-Feagans, 2006; Szagun & Stumper, guage outcomes, suggesting that language heard but not delivered
2012). Interestingly, one study suggested that explanations and in the context of an engaged adultchild interaction is not sup-
descriptions in parent speech may be a stronger predictor of a portive of early lexical development (Weisleder & Fernald, 2013).
childs vocabulary near the start of pre-kindergarten compared to Even in a Mayan community, where most of the speech heard
when the child is younger. These results indicate that the relation- by children is overheard speech, overheard words failed to relate
ship between specic measures of input and language skills may to subsequent receptive and expressive vocabulary (Shneidman
be observed differently at various stages in development. & Goldin-Meadow, 2012). Furthermore, in three separate studies,
However, consistent verbal responsiveness throughout infancy adultchild conversations mediated the adverse effects of media
and the toddler years appear to both be necessary for improve- exposure on childrens language development (Ambrose, VanDam,
ments in developmental outcomes. The role of maternal respon- & Moeller, 2014; Mendelsohn et al., 2010; Zimmerman et al., 2009).
siveness in predicting cognitive development at various ages was These studies indicate the importance of the serve and return of
examined in a sample of 282 children at 6, 12, 24, 36, and 48 conversations for early childhood language development.
months (Landry et al., 2003, 2001). Mothers were classied as pro-
viding either high or low amounts of contingent responses during 3.5.2. Positive affect and sensitivity
both infancy (24 months) and toddlerhood (36 and 48 months) Parent or caregiver communication occurs within the con-
(Landry et al., 2003, 2001). Greatest cognitive growth was observed text of social interactions. As such, eleven studies, nine of which
throughout the toddler years up to second grade in children whose were prospective, considered the quality of the social interaction
mothers provided high amounts of contingency in infancy and tod- when investigating the relationship between parent or caregiver
dlerhood, and no developmental differences were seen between linguistic input and language or cognitive development. All stud-
children of mothers who provided only high amounts in either ies labeled parental behaviors and linguistic input during free
infancy or toddlerhood (Landry et al., 2003, 2001). These results or semi-structured play sessions as being characterized by posi-
suggest that developmental timing for the benets of contingent tive or negative affect, sensitivity or intrusiveness, and guiding or
speech begin in infancy and continue throughout toddlerhood. restrictive. The Bayley Scales of Infant Development was the most
Contingent communication and joint attention may be posi- commonly used measure to assess childrens language and/or cog-
tive predictors of a childs language outcomes by facilitating the nitive development as it was used in seven of the eleven studies.
development of language processing skills. Joint attention has been Positive affect, guidance, and sensitivity, dened as the degree
found to be signicantly correlated with faster habituation times to to which parents or caregivers responded to childs cues (ges-
looking-time displays, suggesting that shared attention and social tures, expressions, signals, and vocalizations) and recognized
engagement play a role in the learning and understanding of lan- childs needs or wants, were consistently associated with increased
guage (Dunphy-Lelii, LaBounty, Lane, & Wellman, 2014). language and cognitive outcomes throughout infancy and early
Contingent speech may also provide a mechanism by which childhood (Adi-Japha & Klein, 2009; Arevalo, Kolobe, Arnold, &
pre-verbal infants begin to learn phonological patterns and speech DeGrace, 2014; Dodici et al., 2003; Fagan, Lee, Palkovitz, & Cabrera,
morphology. In a randomized control trial, 64 mothers of nine 2011; Fish & Pinkerman, 2003; Hirsh-Pasek & Burchinal, 2006;
month old infants were instructed to provide speech to their infant Karrass & Braungart-Rieker, 2005, 2003; Masur, Flynn, & Eichorst,
that was either timed to be contingent or non-contingent on their 2005; Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, London, & Cabrera, 2002; Tamis-
infants babbling (Goldstein & Schwade, 2008). Infants of moth- LeMonda, Shannon, Cabrera, & Lamb, 2004). Interestingly, the
ers who provided a contingent response to their babbling modied benets of interactions characterized by positive affect, guidance,
their babbling to reect phonological patterns of their mothers and sensitivity extended into pre-kindergarten school readiness
speech; however, infants of mothers whose responses were not and performance in rst grade language, attention, and academic
contingent did not (Goldstein & Schwade, 2008). These results sug- outcomes and third grade mathematics ability (Arevalo et al.,
gest that infants may incorporate phonological patterns of their 2014; Dodici et al., 2003; Fish & Pinkerman, 2003; Hirsh-Pasek &
caregivers speech into their own communication, even before they Burchinal, 2006; Karrass & Braungart-Rieker, 2003). Remarkably,
are able to speak words (Goldstein & Schwade, 2008). Given the maternal interactions during infancy accounted for 811% of the
contingent timing of the caregivers speech, these results support variance in auditory and language comprehension and production
Vygotskys Sociocultural Theory, suggesting that language acquisi- at pre-kindergarten entry and mediated the relationship between
tion may occur through social guidance. having a resident father and cognitive outcomes (Fagan et al., 2011;
If language acquisition occurs through social guidance, it would Fish & Pinkerman, 2003). Conversely, interactions characterized by
be expected that language delivered in the context of social engage- negative affect, intrusiveness, restrictiveness, and limited acknowl-
ment would be more predictive of language acquisition than the edgement of the infants agency were consistently associated with
quantity of words alone. Indeed, several studies have reported poorer language and cognitive outcomes (Kelley, Smith, Green,
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 327

Berndt, & Rogers, 1998; Masur et al., 2005; Murray, Kempton, delay than children whose parents did report that they introduce
Woolgar, & Hooper, 1993; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2004). their child to new things (OR = 2.0; 95% CI: 1.382.78) (Glascoe
While most of the studies looked at maternal affect, guidance, & Leew, 2010). It is possible that this relationship was observed
and sensitivity, one study looked at the responsive behaviors and because introducing their children to new things may help facilitate
affect of fathers. This study demonstrated that two year old chil- word-object mapping and expose them to new words as well. Addi-
dren who scored in the normal range on the Bayley Scales of Infant tionally, in a study that looked at the level of father involvement
Development were ve times more likely to have a father who and child IQ, fathers were classied as either high-involvement or
demonstrated responsive behaviors and positive affect than chil- low-involvement based on maternal report of the amount of time
dren who scored below the mean (Shannon et al., 2002). Because the father-child dyad spent in play between 436 months (Yogman,
this study was cross-sectional, the directionality of this relation- Kindlon, & Earls, 1995). Mean IQ, as measured by the Stanford-
ship remains unclear. It is possible that children who exhibit Binet Intelligence Scale, at 36 months was six points higher in
more sophisticated language and cognitive activities elicit more children whose fathers were classied as high-involvement, after
responsive behaviors and positive emotions from fathers. However, controlling for maternal involvement (Yogman et al., 1995). These
given results of other studies that have examined the relationship ndings suggest that the amount of time spent in active engage-
between fathers and childrens development prospectively, it is ment with the father has cognitive benets beyond those conferred
likely that the behaviors of fathers during child engagement have from maternal engagement (Yogman et al., 1995).
an inuential role in their childrens learning and development.
3.6. Findings related to parent/caregiver book reading and child
3.5.3. Cognitive-stimulating interactions development
In addition to measuring positive affect, guidance, and sensi-
tivity, some studies looked at these measures along with parental 3.6.1. Frequency of book reading
involvement in cognitive growth and socio-emotional activities as Fourteen studies assessed the inuence of shared book read-
self-reported by parents or observed in free or structured play ses- ing on childrens language and cognitive outcomes. Eight studies
sions. All of these constructs were used to determine the level of were prospective cohort studies; two studies were randomized
cognitive stimulation present in the childs environment. As such, control trials; two studies were cross-sectional; and two studies
the role of these constructs in these studies cannot be parsed out. were reviews of primarily prospective cohort studies and ran-
All seven studies consistently showed that an environment that domized control trials. Three of the studies, two of which were
fosters cognitive stimulation is associated with improved language literature reviews, discussed the shared reading benets of the
and cognitive outcomes. Reach Out and Read program, an intervention within low-income
A commonly used measurement used to capture all these pediatric ofces at which pediatricians and pediatric nurse prac-
components of parentchild interactions was the Nursing Child titioners and nurses provide anticipatory guidance about shared
Assessment Teaching Scale (NCATS). Higher scores on the NCATS book reading as well as distribute childrens books (High, LaGasse,
indicate higher levels of parental engagement and were positively Becker, Ahlgren, & Gardner, 2000; Needlman & Silverstein, 2004;
associated with receptive and expressive language skills, cognitive Willis, Kabler-Babbitt, & Zuckerman, 2007). Children in most sam-
development, and literacy outcomes (Lugo-Gil & Tamis-LeMonda, ples were between 12 months and 24 months old. Samples ranged
2008; Magill-Evans & Harrison, 1999; Martoccio et al., 2014). in size from 43 participants to over 2000. The majority of these
Remarkably, NCATS scores at 14 months were correlated with lan- studies used parental reports of book reading activities and were
guage and literacy outcomes on the PPVT and Woodcock Johnson correlational in nature. All studies assessed receptive and expres-
Test of Achievement at ve years of age, which likely have impor- sive language skills, most commonly with the MCDI or the PPVT;
tant implications regarding school readiness and future academic additionally, three studies also assessed cognitive outcomes with
achievement (Martoccio et al., 2014). To add, Lugo-Gil and Tamis- the Bayley Scales Mental Development Index.
LeMonda reported that the NCATS explained up to 40% of variance Evidence suggests that children who are read to frequently have
on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at 36 months and larger vocabularies, greater language complexity, more advanced
that the association between family economic resources, includ- language comprehension skills, and better cognitive outcomes than
ing income per person, parent-living arrangement, and parent children who are not read to or are read to infrequently (High et al.,
education, and the childs cognitive development was completely 2000; Lyytinen, Laakso, & Poikkeus, 1998; Needlman & Silverstein,
mediated by scores on the NCATS (Lugo-Gil & Tamis-LeMonda, 2004; Raikes et al., 2006; Rodriguez et al., 2009; Schmitt, Simpson,
2008). & Friend, 2011; Willis et al., 2007). Studies have suggested that
Other commonly used measurements were the StimQ and reading frequency may account for between 922.5% of variance
the Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment in vocabulary outcomes (Deckner, Adamson, & Bakeman, 2006;
(HOME). The cognitive environment, as measured by the StimQ High et al., 2000; Karrass & Braungart-Rieker, 2005; Rodriguez
or HOME, positively predicted the childrens skills at kindergarten et al., 2009). As a result, high reading frequency may decrease a
on the Woodcock Johnson Test of Achievement, Letter Word Iden- childs risk for early intervention services or language delays. In fact,
tication Test, and the PPVT (Cates et al., 2012; Rodriguez & one study investigated the association between reading frequency
Tamis-LeMonda, 2011). In fact, the early cognitive environment when children were six months and 18 months old and their eli-
explained 32% of the variance on the PPVT (Rodriguez & Tamis- gibility for intervention services in New York at 21 months of age
LeMonda, 2011). Only 7% of children whose environment was low (Tomopoulos et al., 2006). Criteria for early intervention services in
in cognitive stimulation at 15 months scored above 100 on the New York includes scoring two standard deviations below the mean
PPVT compared to 70% of children whose environment was high in on either the Bayley Scales Mental Developmental Index or the
cognitive stimulation at 15 months (Rodriguez & Tamis-LeMonda, Preschool Language Scale, or scoring 1.5 standard deviations below
2011). the mean on both standardized assessments (Tomopoulos et al.,
Activities including teaching children about new things as well 2006). Being read to four or more days per week was signicantly
as engaging in play also are other ways in which parents or care- associated with a decreased risk of meeting early intervention cri-
givers enrich their childs learning environment. In one study, teria (adjusted OR = 0.16); 75% of children who were read to less
children whose parents reported that they do not introduce their than one day a week met eligibility criteria, whereas only 36.8%
child to new things were two times more likely to have a language of children who were read to more than four days a week met
328 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

eligibility criteria (Tomopoulos et al., 2006). Similarly, children who needs, provided encouragement, described experiences to child,
were read to more than ten minutes per day at nine months old and helped child give attention to things in the environment, was
were 2.5 times less likely to have poor vocabulary at school-entry, strongly associated with shared reading (Westerlund & Lagerberg,
or score in the bottom 20% on the PPVT, compared to children who 2008). However, after controlling for mothers communication
were read to less than ten minutes per day (Farrant & Zubrick, quality, reading at least six times per week still added more than
2013). Furthermore, shared book reading may be protective for a 0.4 standard deviation (medium effect size) in vocabulary scores
children who have language delays. Shared-book reading improved on the Swedish Communication Screening, the Swedish version of
the expressive language abilities of two year old children with the MCDI (Westerlund & Lagerberg, 2008). In another study, the
language delays (Buschmann et al., 2009). By the age of three, frequency of being read to accounted for 7% of the variance in
75% of the children whose parents were taught how to read to expressive vocabulary at 2127 months old, after controlling for
them had normal expressive language abilities, compared to 44% the childs overall exposure to language (Patterson, 2002). These
of the children whose parents were not taught how to read to results suggest that there may be a unique relationship between
their child (Buschmann et al., 2009). Together, these studies sug- reading and vocabulary development apart from language deliv-
gest that shared reading is positively associated with vocabulary ered outside of the context of shared book reading.
development of young children.
Several studies point to the possibility that shared reading 3.6.2. Quality of book reading
may not have the same inuence on young children at every age Eleven studies discussed the relationship between the quality
throughout early childhood. In a study of 1046 children from the of book reading and childrens language or cognitive outcomes.
Early Head Start program, the frequency of engagement in shared Interestingly, the primary aim of ve of these studies was to evalu-
book reading and storytelling accounted for 10%, 17%, and 20% vari- ate the efcacy of a parent-based intervention focused on teaching
ance on the MCDI at 14, 24, and 36 months respectively, controlling parents skills in dialogic book reading and improving the quality
for earlier shared reading and storytelling (Rodriguez et al., 2009). of shared book reading through a randomized control trial design.
Similarly in another study, greater frequency and duration of read- While the primary aim of these ve studies was not to describe
ing with children 1825 months old, but not 1317 months old, the role of the quality of book reading on childrens developmen-
signicantly improved vocabulary outcomes (High et al., 2000). tal outcomes, they discussed the inuence of the intervention on
These ndings are also supported by another study in which the the quality of book reading and linked these outcomes to chil-
inuence of book reading at both four months and eight months drens language and/or cognitive development. Like the studies that
old was examined and only the frequency and duration of book examined the frequency of book reading, most of these studies
reading at eight months old predicted expressive language skills obtained a parental report about the shared reading experience;
at 12 months and 16 months (Karrass & Braungart-Rieker, 2005). although, a few studies assessed the quality of an observed reading
These results suggest that shared reading may be more bene- session by the parentchild dyad. All studies assessed the vocabu-
cial as children progress throughout their early childhood years, lary of the children in the study sample, most commonly with the
which may be explained by the childrens developmental progress PPVT, MCDI, or Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test, and
throughout early childhood. As infants develop, their visual acuity ve studies also assessed cognitive development or school readi-
becomes increasingly rened, which may assist in the recognition ness.
of objects and colors that a caregiver may point to during book Quality of book reading, including level of joint attention and
reading, and their ne motor skills gradually increase and enable extra-textual talk, or talk outside of the words written in the book,
the child to hold and turn pages of the book, which may encourage may affect the relationship between book reading and language and
active engagement in the shared reading experience. Additionally, cognitive outcomes. Nine month old infants with low levels of joint
book reading often introduces new words to a child. Children with attention during shared book reading, as measured by the Commu-
larger receptive vocabularies may be able to take greater advantage nication and Symbolic Behavior Scales, had poorer vocabulary on
of the presentation of these new words because the meanings of the PPVT at 58 months than infants with high levels of joint atten-
the new words are constrained by their knowledge of other words. tion, suggesting that joint attention may affect how reading affects
Therefore, it is possible that shared book reading, while benecial at vocabulary development (Farrant & Zubrick, 2013). Likewise, mul-
earlier ages, becomes increasingly inuential on childrens vocab- tiple studies have found that inviting children into storytelling by
ulary production and comprehension as the child grows older. asking children questions, encouraging verbal responses, introduc-
While studies support the use of shared book reading for the pro- ing a diversity of words, and teaching new concepts during reading
motion of child development, these results should be interpreted improves their language comprehension, vocabulary skills, syntac-
in light of other research detailing the importance of the richness tical complexity, and literacy development (Cline & Edwards, 2013;
of language input and shared social experiences. Books provide Cronan, Brooks, Kilpatrick, Bigatti, & Tally, 1999; Huebner, 2000;
a tool by which parents and caregivers can engage in conversa- Huebner & Meltzoff, 2005; Pancsofar & Vernon-Feagans, 2006;
tion with their infant or toddler and introduce new words to their Valdezmenchaca & Whitehurst, 1992; Westerlund & Lagerberg,
child. However, is shared book reading really a unique predictor of 2008). Another study also found that using prompts, praise, and
a childs language or cognitive development, or rather, are other recasts during book reading improved the ability to learn target
measures of parental or caregiver linguistic input (i.e. joint atten- vocabulary words for 2241 month old children with expressive
tion, gestures, lexical diversity, number of words) highly correlated vocabulary delays (Tsybina & Eriks-Brophy, 2010). In this study,
with shared book reading and responsible for these associations? gains in overall vocabulary were observed but did not reach statis-
In other words, perhaps parents and caregivers who read with their tical signicance, most likely due to a small sample size and lack
children are more likely to provide an environment that is quanti- of statistical power (Tsybina & Eriks-Brophy, 2010). These studies
tatively and qualitatively rich in language and thus the variance in suggest that not only the frequency of book reading, but also the
language and cognitive outcomes observed in these studies could way in which books are shared with children, contribute to a childs
be explained by this potential correlation rather than book reading. language development.
Few studies have controlled for other factors that promote In fact, quality of book reading may mediate the relationship
vocabulary development and language acquisition. In a study between the frequency or duration of book reading and childrens
of 1719 month old toddlers (N = 1091), communication quality, language or cognitive outcomes. Although no studies investigated
which included the extent to which mothers were aware of child this potential mediating relationship, one study found that the
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 329

frequency of father book reading at 24 months was only predic- caregiver speech (Naigles & Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998; Read, 2014; Saint-
tive of PPVT scores at 36 months for children whose fathers had Georges et al., 2013; Song et al., 2010). All of these components are
at least a high school education (Duursma, Pan, & Raikes, 2008). It necessary for the acquisition of language. Additionally, caregiver
is possible that a high school education mediated the relationship contingent speech, gestures, and establishing joint attention dur-
between book reading and receptive vocabulary outcomes based ing interactions all have the potential to help children recognize
on the quality and complexity of the language provided during the and use words (Hoff, 2006; Martoccio et al., 2014; Rollins, 2003;
book reading. However, these measures were not assessed. Schmidt & Lawson, 2002). These methods of communicating with a
child respond to their childs focus of attention and verbal responses
and thus, help scaffold language learning, support the childs use of
3.6.3. Other literacy-promoting activities
their existing linguistic knowledge within an interactive context,
Literacy-promoting activities, other than book reading, may also
and help the child attend to their environment.
promote language acquisition and cognitive development. Engage-
While research strongly indicates the positive impact of lan-
ment in story-telling, singing nursery rhymes or the alphabet,
guage input by caregivers on childrens development, not all
and participating in activities to learn numbers and letters have
language appears to be benecial for a childs learning. The use
been shown to foster development of language and literacy skills
of imperatives and other types of directives that change the focus
(Bronte-Tinkew, Carrano, Horowitz, & Kinukawa, 2008; Rodriguez
of childs attention were found to be negatively associated with
et al., 2009; Song, Tamis-Lemonda, Yoshikawa, Kahana-Kalman,
childrens language development (Cruz et al., 2013; Hoff-Ginsberg,
& Wu, 2012; Topping et al., 2013). In a systematic review, stud-
1998; Rowe, 2008; Topping et al., 2013). This type of language often
ies suggested that literacy-promoting activities, including shared
has the primary goal of controlling or managing a childs behav-
book reading, enhance vocabulary, language, and literacy skills by
ior rather than facilitating child engagement. These directives shut
providing children with a greater diversity of vocabulary, more
down conversation and do not offer children the opportunity to
complex grammar, opportunities to learn phonemic skills and print
contribute verbally.
concept knowledge, and by facilitating interest in literacy (Topping
Social interaction is an essential ingredient to language nutri-
et al., 2013).
tion. The importance of social interaction paired with linguistic
input was a common thread between the studies in this review.
4. Discussion Social relationships drive the need for languageit is the basis
by which we communicate with others and so it makes sense
The purpose of this review was to examine available literature that children depend on social input for language learning. Lan-
investigating the inuence of language nutrition, or talking, read- guage learning does not occur passively. Interactions between
ing, and interacting with a child, in the rst three years of life parents/caregivers and the infant and toddler, built upon respon-
on language and cognitive outcomes. This review highlights the siveness, emotional tone, guidance and encouragement of joint
importance of language nutrition for a childs developmental and attention, are posited to have an positive inuence on the childs
educational trajectory. language development, controlling for parent education and house-
A childs brain grows rapidly in the rst three years of life hold income (Dodici et al., 2003; Dunphy-Lelii et al., 2014; Glascoe
and is dependent upon language nutrition in order to reach its & Leew, 2010; Lugo-Gil & Tamis-LeMonda, 2008; Masur et al.,
intellectual capacity (National Research Council, 2000). Decades 2005; Nicely et al., 1999; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001; Rodriguez &
of research consistently indicate the extreme importance of an Tamis-LeMonda, 2011; Rollins, 2003; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2001).
environment rich in language and social communication. Although Additionally, caregiver speech and actions that encourage verbal
evidence shows that the quantity of words spoken to a child responses all invite children to participate in conversation. Thus,
dramatically improves a childs language outcomes, possibly by caregivers who allow the child to contribute to the discourse in a
enhancing speech processing, the quantity of words is strongly developmentally expected way, rst in attention to the caregiver
correlated with measures of the quality of language, controlling through communication behaviors such as looks, facial expressions
for socioeconomic status (Hurtado, Marchman, & Fernald, 2008; and utterances and later in word approximations, comments and
Rowe, 2008; Weisleder & Fernald, 2013). In other words, the more conversation, provide children with the opportunity and modeling
words spoken to a child, the more varied, complex, and recipro- needed to promote the childs language development. In essence,
cal the linguistic input becomes. Speech that is varied in words, the impact of rich and varied language nutrition delivered con-
grammatical complexity and syntactical structure all aid in the sistently by caregivers in the context of an engaged and trusting
acquisition, comprehension, and production of language and pos- adultchild social relationship provides an essential component for
sibly is more strongly related to childrens language and cognitive the development of language competence that lays the foundation
learning than the actual number of words spoken to a child (Hoff- for academic success. Those who are academically prepared and
Ginsberg, 1998; Hoff & Naigles, 2002; Huttenlocher et al., 2010). successful are more likely to prot from education and are likely
Remarkably, the quality of the linguistic input mediates the adverse to lead lives characterized by better health (Robert Wood Johnson
effects of preterm birth, low levels of parent education, mater- Foundation, 2009).
nal depression, and poverty on language and cognitive outcomes Reading and other activities such as storytelling, singing, and
(Burchinal et al., 2008; Huttenlocher et al., 2010; Murray et al., teaching letters or numbers offer evidence-based and concrete
1993; Poehlmann & Fiese, 2001). This nding suggests that par- methods for which caregivers and children can engage in conver-
ents and caregivers have the potential and the power to mitigate sation. These activities introduce new and interesting vocabulary,
the inuences of various circumstances that threaten to limit their engage the child in word and phrase play, encourage caregiver-
childs success simply by making their child their conversational child engagement and turn taking, and lead to greater generative
partner early and often. language use (Topping et al., 2013). Such activities can have strong
The use and delivery of words in a way that children can easily and lasting impact on the childs cognitive processes and diversity
recognize is a powerful component in laying a secure foundation of language that result in later reading and language prociency
for development. Studies indicate that the varied intonation and (Buschmann et al., 2009; Farrant & Zubrick, 2013; Tsybina & Eriks-
prosody in caregiver speech and positional salience of target vocab- Brophy, 2010; Westerlund & Lagerberg, 2008). Interventions have
ulary words aid in phonemic awareness, word segmentation, and focused on counseling parents and other caregivers, such as day-
an understanding of syntactic structures and grammatical rules of care workers and teachers, on features of shared book reading
330 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

that facilitate childrens language learning and early literacy skills. the majority of these studies were prospective, these ndings are
These interventions represent a promising and potentially scal- not likely only a reection of variations in the childs linguistic
able framework for which to increase the quantity and quality of and cognitive abilities inuencing how much caregivers talk with
caregiverchild interactions. them. To add, many studies utilized maternal reports rather than
In all of the studies that were reviewed, less than 10% consid- standardized instruments in assessing parental behaviors, such as
ered the contributions of fathers to their childs early language and talking and reading. Although maternal reports have been shown
learning environment. While mothers were the most often studied to be related to childrens outcomes, there may be some report-
caregiver, fathers and other caregivers are often in the position of ing bias as a result of this method of assessing language nutrition.
providing a cognitively stimulating environment and are key par- Similarly, some of the results may be biased as mothers or other
ticipants in the childs learning experiences (Bronte-Tinkew et al., caregivers may have increased their linguistic input, positive affect,
2008; Yogman et al., 1995). While the ndings of studies that have or responsivity to their child as a result of being observed. Stud-
looked at fathers are congruent with those found in studies exam- ies that assessed language nutrition over a period of time or with
ining the role of maternal input, fathers may have a unique role objective measures, such as the LENA digital language processors,
in their childs language and cognitive development. Studies have likely are subjected to less bias from a caregivers change in behav-
generally assessed paternal input in the same way as maternal ior compared to studies that assessed parentchild dyads once for
input, but it is possible that this results in a biased assessment a 10 or 20 min play session. Additionally, these assessments only
of paternal input. In recognition of the diverse social networks offer a snapshot of the interactions between parent-child dyads.
and family constellations that comprise an infants environment Assessments in the home compared to in an observation lab may
and to be consistent with Brofenbrenners Ecological Systems The- provide a more accurate picture of these interactions.
ory, future studies should take a more comprehensive approach Despite these limitations, this is one of the rst integrated
to evaluating opportunities for children to develop linguistically reviews to examine the relationship between language nutrition
and cognitively from language nutrition by fathers and other care- and a childs language and cognitive development. Data were col-
givers, including daycare workers, teachers, grandparents, aunts lected systematically and over 100 articles, of which the majority
and uncles, and nannies. were quasi-experimental or observational cohort studies, were
There are several limitations of this review that should be con- included in this review. Study samples included in this review
sidered when interpreting these ndings. There is a possibility that represented children of diverse socioeconomic backgrounds with
some eligible articles were not captured by the literature search different risk proles for delayed development, which increases
and thus are not included in this review. Although three databases the generalizability of these ndings across various populations.
were searched in a comprehensive and systematic way, there are In addition, almost all studies controlled for known confounders,
more than three databases that could have been used to search including various aspects of socioeconomic status, known to affect
for articles. Because six reviews were included in this study, a few childrens developmental outcomes. Although methodological dif-
studies are represented more than once. However, reviews were ferences existed across studies, the vast majority of the articles
only cited when they contributed additional evidence that did not reported signicant benets of language nutrition, characterized by
involve an individual study already included in this review. Addi- a diversity of lexical input in the context of rich social engagement,
tionally, only one author identied and selected articles from the throughout early childhood on a childs language and cognitive
search. Although the protocol for selecting articles consistently fol- development.
lowed the guidelines set by three authors at the beginning of the
review, discrepancies about the inclusion of articles at any stage of
selection could have arisen if more than one author identied eli- 5. Conclusion
gible articles. Furthermore, signicant methodological differences
across studies makes it more difcult to assess the strength of these Given the dramatic impact that early language environment
ndings. Sample characteristics, primary study variables, and met- has on the developing child, those individuals who care for young
rics used to assess parent and child language were not consistent children can be powerful agents of change. Families and early child-
between studies. As such, meta-analysis was not possible. Another hood caregivers need to be a key target for information, education,
limitation of this review was that it looked specically at lan- and skill building. Family engagement is integral to the success of all
guage and cognitive development of children and did not consider young children. Teaching the power and the skill of language-rich
the inuences of language nutrition on childrens socioemotional interactions that can be built into the childs life from the beginning
development, self-regulation, and other developmental domains. lays the strongest possible foundation for a childs later language
While language and cognition represent distinct domains of devel- development and prociency. Changing the national conversation
opment, childrens development does not occur in siloes. Benets about early childhood development and bringing easily accessi-
of language nutrition on childrens socioemotional development or ble information and tools to families and caregivers is becoming
self-regulation may indirectly contribute to childrens language or the next public health challenge. Any parent and or early childcare
cognitive development. caregiver can be coached to embed rich and varied language into
Other limitations of this review include the individual limita- their daily activities with the infant and toddler. Methodologies
tions of each study. Many sample sizes were small. As a result, that not only illustrate the issues but coach parents and caregivers
several studies lacked sufcient statistical power to construct pre- to develop strong habits of embedding language nutrition into their
dictive statistical models. However, several studies did have very everyday lives through talking, reading, and singing are an area of
large sample sizes and had adequate power to make conclusions emerging research and study. Programs like the Kaiser Foundation
beyond correlations. While most studies made conclusions beyond and Next Generation/Clinton Foundations Too Small to Fail cam-
correlations, the directionality of the relationship between lan- paign, Thirty Million Word Gap, Providence Talks, and Talk With Me
guage nutrition and childrens language and cognitive development Baby are working to put information in the hands of parents, care-
could not be determined for every study. Children who contribute givers, and healthcare providers to encourage the rich and engaged
more frequent and sophisticated language responses may elicit language interactions that babies need to develop strong language
more language input and responsive behaviors from caregivers prociency.
or it is possible that the association between language nutrition Engaging those professionals who interact frequently with
and childrens development is bidirectional. However, given that families and caregivers provides a sound strategy for ensuring
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 331

access to information, tools, and skill building for families and expressive language delay: a randomised controlled trial. Archives of Disease in
caregivers. Elevating the importance of early brain development Childhood, 94(2), 110116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2008.141572
Camp, B. W., Cunningham, M., & Berman, S. (2010). Relationship between the
to include language nutrition is a necessary rst step but not cognitive environment and vocabulary development during the second year of
sufcient for change. While provision of information has merit, life. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 164(10), 950956. http://dx.
teaching the critical skills of language transactions to all families doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.169
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (2003). Home observation for measurement of the
is the currency for true and lasting habit building and change in environment: administration manual. Tempe, AZ: Family & Human Dynamics
caregiver behavior. Coaching our early childcare and healthcare Research Institute, Arizona State University.
providers to be the trusted providers of information related to Caskey, M., Stephens, B., Tucker, R., & Vohr, B. (2014). Adult talk in the NICU with
preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics, 133(3), e578e584.
early language and cognitive development puts the power and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-0104
inuence into the hands of workforces that are trusted and are Cates, C. B., Dreyer, B. P., Berkule, S. B., White, L. J., Arevalo, J. A., & Mendelsohn, A. L.
often in contact with young children and their caregivers. (2012). Infant communication and subsequent language development in
children from low-income families: the role of early cognitive stimulation.
Areas for continued research regarding early language develop-
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(7), 577585. http://dx.
ment are numerous. The importance of bridging the word gap in doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0b013e318264c10f
the United States has recently gotten national prominence through Cline, K. D., & Edwards, C. P. (2013). The instructional and emotional quality of
a series of meetings and summits and in the funding of the Bridge parentchild book reading and early Head Start childrens learning outcomes.
Early Education and Development, 24(8), 12141231. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
the Word Gap Research Network by the Health Resources and Ser- 10409289.2012.697431
vices Administration. We have much to discover about the variables Cronan, T. A., Brooks, L. B., Kilpatrick, K., Bigatti, S. M., & Tally, S. (1999). The effects
that are involved in early language development and how to best of a community-based literacy program: one-year follow-up ndings. Journal
of Community Psychology, 27(4), 431442.
translate these discoveries to positively inuence childrens devel- Cruz, I., Quittner, A. L., Marker, C., & DesJardin, J. L. (2013). Identication of
opmental and educational trajectory. effective strategies to promote language in deaf children with cochlear
implants. Child Development, 84(2), 543559. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
8624.2012.01863.x
Funding Deckner, D. F., Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (2006). Child and maternal
contributions to shared reading: effects on language and literacy development.
This work was supported by the Talk With Me BabyTM initiative Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(1), 3141. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1016/j.appdev.2005.12.001
through the Dare to Forget the Box Prize from the United Way of Dickinson, D. K., & Porche, M. V. (2011). Relation between language experiences in
Greater Atlanta. preschool classrooms and childrens kindergarten and fourth-grade language
and reading abilities. Child Development, 82(3), 870886. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01576.x
Acknowledgements Dodici, B. J., Draper, D. C., & Peterson, C. A. (2003). Early parentchild interactions
and early literacy development. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education,
We would like to thank the members of the Talk With Me BabyTM 23(3), 124136. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/02711214030230030301
Dreyer, B. P., Mendelsohn, A. L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2014). StimQ congitive home
initiative for their guidance and support in the conceptualization of
environment. http://pediatrics.med.nyu.edu/developmental/research/the-
this manuscript and acknowledge Arianne B. Weldon, Director of belle-project/stimq-cognitive-home-environment.
the Get Georgia Reading Campaign, who coined the term language Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4: peabody picture vocabulary test (fourth
ed.). Mineapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments.
nutrition.
Dunphy-Lelii, S., LaBounty, J., Lane, J. D., & Wellman, H. M. (2014). The social
context of infant intention understanding. Journal of Cognition and
Appendix A. Supplementary data Development, 15(1), 6077. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.710863
Duursma, E., Pan, B. A., & Raikes, H. (2008). Predictors and outcomes of low-income
fathers reading with their toddlers. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 23(3),
Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, 351365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.06.001
in the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016. Edwards, S., Garman, M., Hughes, A., Letts, C., & Sinka, I. (1999). Assessing the
01.015. comprehension and production of language in young children: an account of
the Reynell Developmental Language Scales III. International Journal of
Language and Communication Disorders, 34(2), 151171. http://dx.doi.org/10.
References 1080/136828299247487
Fagan, J., Lee, Y., Palkovitz, R., & Cabrera, N. (2011). Mediators of the relationship
Adi-Japha, E., & Klein, P. S. (2009). Relations between parenting quality and between stable nonresident households and toddler outcomes. Journal of
cognitive performance of children experiencing varying amounts of childcare. Family Issues, 32(11), 15431568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Child Development, 80(3), 893906. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624. 0192513x11400172
2009.01304.x Farrant, B. M., & Zubrick, S. R. (2013). Parentchild book reading across early
Ambrose, S. E., VanDam, M., & Moeller, M. P. (2014). Linguistic input, electronic childhood and child vocabulary in the early school years: ndings from the
media, and communication outcomes of toddlers with hearing loss. Ear and longitudinal study of Australian children. First Language, 33(3), 280293.
Hearing, 35(2), 139147. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182a76768 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723713487617
Arevalo, A., Kolobe, T. H., Arnold, S., & DeGrace, B. (2014). Early childrearing Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007).
practices and their relationship to academic performance in Mexican American MacArthurBates communicative development inventories (second ed.).
children. Pediatrics Physical Therapy, 26(2), 214222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/ Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
pep.0000000000000033 Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A. (2013). SES differences in language
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (third ed.). San processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental
Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Science, 16(2), 234248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019
0734282906297199 Fiester, L., & Smith, R. (2010). Early warning: why reading by the end of third grade
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary matters KidsCount. Baltimore, MD: Annie E. Casey Foundation.
competence: a multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25(2), 367393. Fish, M., & Pinkerman, B. (2003). Language skills in low-SES rural Appalachian
Brent, M. R., & Siskind, J. M. (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early children: normative development and individual differences, infancy to
vocabulary development. Cognition, 81(2), B33B44. preschool. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 539565. http://
Brofenbrenner, U. (2005). Ecological models of human development. In M. dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0193-3973(02)00141-7
Gauvain, & M. Cole (Eds.), Readings on the development of children (fourth ed., Forget-Dubois, N., Dionne, G., Lemelin, J. P., Perusse, D., Tremblay, R. E., & Boivin, M.
pp. 37). New York, NY: Worth Publishers. (2009). Early child language mediates the relation between home environment
Bronte-Tinkew, J., Carrano, J., Horowitz, A., & Kinukawa, A. (2008). Involvement and school readiness. Child Development, 80(3), 736749. http://dx.doi.org/10.
among resident fathers and links to infant cognitive outcomes. Journal of 1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01294.x
Family Issues, 29(9), 12111244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0192513x08318145 Girolametto, L., Bonifacio, S., Visini, C., Weitzman, E., Zocconi, E., & Pearce, P. S.
Burchinal, M., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Cox, M. (2008). Cumulative social risk, (2002). Motherchild interactions in Canada and Italy: linguistic
parenting, and infant development in rural low-income communities. responsiveness to late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language &
Parenting: Science and Practice, 8(1), 4169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ Communication Disorders, 37(2), 153171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
15295190701830672 13682820110116794
Buschmann, A., Jooss, B., Rupp, A., Feldhusen, F., Pietz, J., & Philippi, H. (2009). Girolametto, L., Sussman, F., & Weitzman, E. (2007). Using case study methods to
Parent based language intervention for 2-year-old children with specic investigate the effects of interactive intervention for children with autism
332 L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333

spectrum disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 40(6), 470492. http:// cognitive development. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(4), 733744. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2006.11.001 dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(98)90041-8
Girolametto, L., Weitzman, E., Wiigs, M., & Pearce, P. S. (1999). The relationship Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M., Brown, C. A., Lee, C. T., Cowperthwaite, B., Stutzman, S.
between maternal language measures and language development in toddlers S., . . . & Wang, Z. (2009). Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and
with expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-Language language. Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 5971. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Pathology, 8(4), 364374. 1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002
Glascoe, F. P., & Leew, S. (2010). Parenting behaviors, perceptions, and psychosocial Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2003). The importance of parenting during
risk: impacts on young childrens development. Pediatrics, 125(2), 313319. early childhood for school-age development. Developmental Neuropsychology,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3129 24(23), 559591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2003.9651911
Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008). Social feedback to infants babbling Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early
facilitates rapid phonological learning. Psycholical Science, 19(5), 515523. responsive parenting have a special importance for childrens development or
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02117.x is consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology,
Goodwyn, S. W., Acredolo, L. P., & Brown, C. A. (2000). Impact of symbolic gesturing 37(3), 387403.
on early language development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(2), 81103. Levickis, P., Reilly, S., Girolametto, L., Ukoumunne, O. C., & Wake, M. (2014).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1006653828895 Maternal behaviors promoting language acquisition in slow-to-talk toddlers:
Gunderson, E. A., & Levine, S. C. (2011). Some types of parent number talk count prospective community-based study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
more than others: relations between parents input and childrens Pediatrics, 35(4), 274281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/dbp.0000000000000056
cardinal-number knowledge. Developmental Science, 14(5), 10211032. http:// Lugo-Gil, J., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Family resources and parenting
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01050.x quality: links to childrens cognitive development across the rst 3 years. Child
Haebig, E., McDufe, A., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2013). The contribution of two Development, 79(4), 10651085. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.
categories of parent verbal responsiveness to later language for toddlers and 01176.x
preschoolers on the autism spectrum. American Journal of Speech-Language Lyytinen, P., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2003). The play and language behavior of
Pathology, 22(1), 5770. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0004) mothers with and without dyslexia and its association to their toddlers
Hahn, L. J., Zimmer, B. J., Brady, N. C., Swinburne Romine, R. E., & Fleming, K. K. language development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(1), 7486.
(2014). Role of maternal gesture use in speech use by children with fragile X Lyytinen, P., Laakso, M. L., & Poikkeus, A. M. (1998). Parental contribution to childs
syndrome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(2), 146159. early language and interest in books. European Journal of Psychology of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2013 ajslp-13-0046 Education, 13(3), 297308.
Halle, T., Forry, N., Hair, E., Perper, K., Wander, L., Wessel, J., et al. (2009). Disparities Magill-Evans, J., & Harrison, M. J. (1999). Parentchild interactions and
in Early Learning and Development: Lessons from the Early Childhood Longitudinal development of toddlers born preterm. Western Journal of Nursing Research,
Study-birth Cohort. Washington DC: Child Trends. 21(3), 292307. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/01939459922043893
Hampson, J., & Nelson, K. (1993). The relation of maternal language to variation in Majorano, M., Rainieri, C., & Corsano, P. (2013). Parents child-directed
rate and style of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 20(2), 313342. communication and child language development: a longitudinal study with
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of Italian toddlers. Journal of Child Language, 40(4), 836859. http://dx.doi.org/10.
young American children. Baltimore: Brookes. 1017/s0305000912000323
Hendrick, D. L., Prather, E., & Tobin, A. R. (1984). Sequenced inventory of Martoccio, T. L., Brophy-Herb, H. E., & Onaga, E. E. (2014). Road to readiness
communication development. Seattle: University of Washington Press. pathways from low-income childrens early interactions to school readiness
High, P. C., LaGasse, L., Becker, S., Ahlgren, I., & Gardner, A. (2000). Literacy skills. Infants & Young Children, 27(3), 193206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/iyc.
promotion in primary care pediatrics: can we make a difference? Pediatrics, 0000000000000014
105(4 Pt. 2), 927934. Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D. L. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive
Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Burchinal, M. (2006). Mother and caregiver sensitivity over time: behaviours and utterances as predictors of childrens lexical development.
predicting language and academic outcomes with variable- and Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 6391.
person-centered approaches. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly-Journal of Developmental Mendelsohn, A. L., Brockmeyer, C. A., Dreyer, B. P., Fierman, A. H.,
Psychology, 52(3), 449485. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2006.0027 Berkule-Silberman, S. B., & Tomopoulos, S. (2010). Do verbal interactions with
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeconomic status to infants during electronic media exposure mitigate adverse impacts on their
childrens language experience and language development. Applied language development as toddlers? Infant and Child Development, 19(6),
Psycholinguistics, 19(4), 603629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ 577593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.711
s0142716400010389 Murray, L., Kempton, C., Woolgar, M., & Hooper, R. (1993). Depressed mothers
Hoff, E. (2003). The specicity of environmental inuence: socioeconomic status speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive
affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(7), 10831101.
74(5), 13681378. Naigles, L. R., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on childrens early verb use.
Developmental Review, 26(1), 5588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 Journal of Child Language, 25(1), 95120.
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from National Center for Education Statistics (2013). The nations report card: a rst look:
low-SES and language minority homes: implications for closing achievement 2013 mathematic and reading. (NCES 2014-451).(NCES 2014-451). Washington,
gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 414. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ D.C.
a0027238 National Center for Health Statistics. (2012). Health, United States 2011: with special
Hoff, E., & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child features on socioeconomic status and health. (2012-1232). Washington, D.C: U.S.
Development, 73(2), 418433. Government Printing Ofce.
Huebner, C. E. (2000). Promoting toddlers language development through National Research Council. (2000). In J. P. Shonkoff, & D. A. Phillips (Eds.), From
community-based intervention. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, neurons to neighborhoods: the science of early childhood development.
21(5), 513535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(00)00052-6 Washington, D.C: National Academies Press.
Huebner, C. E., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Intervention to change parentchild reading National Scientic Council on the Developing Child. (2007). A science-based
style: a comparison of instructional methods. Journal of Applied Developmental framework for early childhood policy: using evidence to improve outcomes in
Psychology, 26(3), 296313. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.02.006 learning, behavior and health for vulnerable children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input inuence uptake? University. Retrieved from: www.developingchild.net/pubs/persp/pdf/Policy
Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Framework.pdf
Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31F39. http://dx. Needlman, R., & Silverstein, M. (2004). Pediatric interventions to support reading
doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00768.x aloud: How good is the evidence? Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early Pediatrics, 25(5), 352363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200410000-
vocabulary growthrelation to language input and gender. Developmental 00007
Psychology, 27(2), 236248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1999). Mothers attuned
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of
Sources of variability in childrens language growth. Cognitive Psychology, language milestones. Infant Behavior & Development, 22(4), 557568. http://dx.
61(4), 343365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.08.002 doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(00)00023-0
Janjua, F., Woll, B., & Kyle, J. (2002). Effects of parental style of interaction on OshimaTakane, Y., Goodz, E., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth order effects on early
language development in very young severe and profound deaf children. language development: do secondborn children learn from overheard speech?
International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 64(3), 193205. Child Development, 67(2), 621634.
Karrass, J., & Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (2005). Effects of shared parent infant book Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of
reading on early language acquisition. Journal of Applied Developmental growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child
Psychology, 26(2), 133148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2004.12.003 Development, 76(4), 763782. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.
Karrass, J., & Braungart-Rieker, J. M. (2003). Parenting and temperament as 00876.x
interacting agents in early language development. Parenting Science and Pancsofar, N., & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to
Practice, 3(3), 235259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327922PAR0303 03 young children: contributions to later language development. Journal of
Kelley, M. L., Smith, T. S., Green, A. P., Berndt, A. E., & Rogers, M. C. (1998). Applied Developmental Psychology, 27(6), 571587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Importance of fathers parenting to African-American toddlers social and appdev.2006.08.003
L.H. Zauche et al. / Early Childhood Research Quarterly 36 (2016) 318333 333

Pancsofar, N., Vernon-Feagans, L., & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2010). the high cost for taxpayers. Northeastern University: Center for Labor Market
Fathers early contributions to childrens language development in families Studies.
from low-income rural communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), Szagun, G., & Stumper, B. (2012). Age or experience? The inuence of age at
450463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.001 implantation and social and linguistic environment on language development
Patterson, J. L. (2002). Relationships of expressive vocabulary to frequency of in children with cochlear implants. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing
reading and television experience among bilingual toddlers. Applied Research, 55(6), 16401654. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-
Psycholinguistics, 23(4), 493508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ 0119)
s0142716402004010 Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal
Perani, D., Saccuman, M. C., Scifo, P., Anwander, A., Spada, D., Baldoli, C., . . . & responsiveness and childrens achievement of language milestones. Child
Friederici, A. D. (2011). Neural language networks at birth. Proceedingws of the Development, 72(3), 748767.
National Academy of Sciences USA, 108(38), 1605616061. http://dx.doi.org/10. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shannon, J. D., Cabrera, N. J., & Lamb, M. E. (2004). Fathers
1073/pnas.1102991108 and mothers at play with their 2-and 3-year-olds: contributions to language
Poehlmann, J., & Fiese, B. H. (2001). Parentinfant interaction as a mediator of the and cognitive development. Child Development, 75(6), 18061820. http://dx.
relation between neonatal risk status and 12-month cognitive development. doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00818.x
Infant Behavior & Development, 24(2), 171188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ Tardif, T., Shatz, M., & Naigles, L. (1997). Caregiver speech and childrens use of
s0163-6383(01)00073-x nouns versus verbs: a comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of
Raikes, H., Pan, B. A., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, Child Language, 24(3), 535565.
J., . . . & Rodriguez, E. T. (2006). Motherchild bookreading in low-income Taumoepeau, M., & Ruffman, T. (2006). Mother and infant talk about mental states
families: correlates and outcomes during the rst three years of life. Child relates to desire language and emotion understanding. Child Development,
Development, 77(4), 924953. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006. 77(2), 465481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00882.x
00911.x Taylor, H. B., Anthony, J. L., Aghara, R., Smith, K. E., & Landry, S. H. (2008). The
Read, K. (2014). Clues cue the smooze: rhyme, pausing, and prediction help interaction of early maternal responsiveness and childrens cognitive abilities
children learn new words from storybooks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 http://dx. on later decoding and reading comprehension skills. Early Education and
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00149 Development, 19(1), 188207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409280701839304
Roberts, M. Y., & Kaiser, A. P. (2011). The effectiveness of parent-implemented The Campaign for Grade Level Reading (2014). Third grade reading succes matters.
language interventions: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Retrieved from: http://gradelevelreading.net (March, 2014).
Pathology, 20(3), 180199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10- Tomopoulos, S., Dreyer, B. P., Tamis-LeMonda, C., Flynn, V., Rovira, I., Tineo, W., &
0055) Mendelsohn, A. L. (2006). Books, toys, parent-child interaction, and
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. (2009). . pp. 115. Education matters for health development in young Latino children. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 6(2), 7278.
(Vol. 6) Robert Wood Johnson Foundation. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ambp.2005.10.001
Rodriguez, E. T., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the home learning Tomasello, M. (2009). The usage-based theory of language aquisition. In E. L. Bavin
environment across the rst 5 years: associations with childrens vocabulary (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 6988). Cambridge:
and literacy skills at prekindergarten. Child Development, 82(4), 10581075. Cambridge University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01614.x Topping, K., Dekhinet, R., & Zeedyk, S. (2013). Parentinfant interaction and
Rodriguez, E. T., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Spellmann, M. E., Pan, B. A., Raikes, H., childrens language development. Educational Psychology, 33(4), 391426.
Lugo-Gil, J., & Luze, G. (2009). The formative role of home literacy experiences http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2012.744159
across the rst three years of life in children from low-income families. Journal Trautman, C. H., & Rollins, P. R. (2006). Child-centered behaviors of caregivers with
of Applied Developmental Psychology, 30(6), 677694. http://dx.doi.org/10. 12-month-old infants: associations with passive joint engagement and later
1016/j.appdev.2009.01.003 language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(3), 447463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
Rollins, P. R. (2003). Caregivers contingent comments to 9-month-old infants: s0142716406060358
relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 221234. Tsybina, I., & Eriks-Brophy, A. (2010). Bilingual dialogic book-reading intervention
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0142716403000110 for preschoolers with slow expressive vocabulary development. Journal of
Rowe, M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, Communication Disorders, 43(6), 538556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.
knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child 2010.05.006
Language, 35(1), 185205. Valdezmenchaca, M. C., & Whitehurst, G. J. (1992). Accelerating language
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality development through picture book reading: a systematic extension to Mexican
of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), daycare. Developmental Psychology, 28(6), 11061114. http://dx.doi.org/10.
17621774. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x 1037//0012-1649.28.6.1106
Rowe, M. L., Raudenbush, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). The pace of Vygotsky, L. S. (2005). Interaction between learning and development. In M.
vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Development, Gauvain, & M. Cole (Eds.), Readings on the development of children (fourth ed.,
83(2), 508525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01710.x Vol. 6, pp. 3440). New York, NY: Worth Publishers.
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V., & Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants of Warren, S. F., Brady, N., Sterling, A., Fleming, K., & Marquis, J. (2010). Maternal
acquisition order in wh-questions: re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech. responsivity predicts language development in young children with fragile X
Journal of Child Language, 30(3), 609635. syndrome. American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities,
Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., 115(1), 5475. http://dx.doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-115.1.54
. . . & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: at the cross-road of emotion Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: early language
and cognition? (a systematic review). PLoS One, 8(10), e78103. http://dx.doi. experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078103 Science, 24(11), 21432152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145
Schmidt, C. L., & Lawson, K. R. (2002). Caregiver attention-focusing and childrens Westerlund, M., & Lagerberg, D. (2008). Expressive vocabulary in 18-month-old
attention-sharing behaviours as predictors of later verbal IQ in very low children in relation to demographic factors, mother and child characteristics,
birthweight children. Journal of Child Language, 29(1), 322. communication style and shared readin. Child: Care, Health and Development,
Schmitt, S. A., Simpson, A. M., & Friend, M. (2011). A longitudinal assessment of the 34(2), 257266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2214.2007.00801.x
home literacy environment and early language. Infant and Child Development, Whittemore, R., & Kna, K. (2005). The integrative review: updated methodology.
20(6), 409431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/icd.733 Journal of Advanced Nursing, 52(5), 546553.
Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., London, K., & Cabrera, N. (2002). Beyond Willis, E., Kabler-Babbitt, C., & Zuckerman, B. (2007). Early literacy interventions:
rough and tumble: low-income fathers interactions and childrens cognitive reach out and read. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 54(3), 625642. http://dx.
development at 24 months. Parenting: Science & Practice, 2(2), 77. doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2007.02.012
Shneidman, L. A., Arroyo, M. E., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2013). What Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2007). Woodcock Johnson III tests of
counts as effective input for word learning? Journal of Child Language, 40(3), achievement. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside.
672686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000912000141 Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (2002). Effects of prelinguistic milieu teaching and
Shneidman, L. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Language input and acquisition in a parent responsivity education on dyads involving children with intellectual
Mayan village: how important is directed speech? Developmental Science, disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(6),
15(5), 659673. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01168.x 11581174.
Smith, K. E., Landry, S. H., Swank, P. R., Baldwin, C. D., Denson, S. E., & Wildin, S. Yogman, M. W., Kindlon, D., & Earls, F. (1995). Father involvement and
(1996). The relation of medical risk and maternal stimulation with preterm cognitive/behavioral outcomes of preterm infants. Journal of the American
infants development of cognitive, language and daily living skills. Journal of Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(1), 5866. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(7), 855864. 1097/00004583-199501000-00015
Song, J. Y., Demuth, K., & Morgan, J. (2010). Effects of the acoustic properties of Zimmerman, F. J., Gilkerson, J., Richards, J. A., Christakis, D. A., Xu, D., Gray, S., &
infant-directed speech on infant word recognition. Journal of the Acoustical Yapanel, U. (2009). Teaching by listening: the importance of adult-child
Society of America, 128(1), 389400. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.3419786 conversations to language development. Pediatrics, 124(1), 342349. http://dx.
Song, L., Tamis-Lemonda, C. S., Yoshikawa, H., Kahana-Kalman, R., & Wu, I. (2012). doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-2267
Language experiences and vocabulary development in Dominican and Zimmerman, I. L., & Castilleja, N. F. (2005). The role of a language scale for infant
Mexican infants across the rst 2 years. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), and preschool assessment. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities
11061123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0026401 Research Reviews, 11(3), 238246.
Sum, A., Khatiwada, I., McLaughlin, J., & Palma, S. (2009). The consequences of
dropping out of high school: joblessness and jailing for high school dropouts and

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen