Sie sind auf Seite 1von 174

UNDERSTANDING THE CRIMINAL MIND:

PSYCHOANALYTIC PERSPECTIVES REGARDING THE

ETIOLOGY AND TREATMENT OF MALE VIOLENT PSYCHOPATHS

A Clinical Dissertation

Presented to the Faculty of

The California School of Professional Psychology

San Francisco Bay Campus

ALLIANT INTERNATIONAL UNIVERSITY

In Partial Fulfillment

Of the Requirements of the Degree

Doctor of Psychology

By

Louis D. Monaco, M.S.

January 2004

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
UMI N um ber: 3114116

Copyright 2004 by
Monaco, Louis D.

All rights reserved.

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy
submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and
photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized
copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
UMI Microform 3114116
Copyright 2004 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest Information and Learning Company


300 North Zeeb Road
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
UNDERSTANDING THE CRIMINAL MIND:

PSYCHOANALYTIC PERSPECTIVES REGARDING THE

ETIOLOGY AND TREATMENT OF VIOLENT PSYCHOPATHS

This clinical dissertation by Louis D. Monaco, M.S., has been approved by the
committee members signed below who recommend that it be accepted by the
faculty of the California School of Professional Psychology San Francisco Bay
Campus In partial fulfillment of requirements for the degree of

DOCTOR OF PSYCHOLOGY

Clinical Dissertation Committee:

Christopher D. Tori, Ph.D.


Chairperson

Murray Btfmes, Ph.D.

Date

ii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Copyright by Louis D. Monaco, M.S., 2004

All Rights Reserved

iii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
DEDICATION

To my family, I thank you for shaping me into who I am today. For showing me

that I could achieve whatever I put my mind to and for your love and support

throughout the years.

To Nicole Fisherman, Tiffany Brown, Jane Dube, Kathleen Anne Elizabeth Ellard

Harding, John Macneil, Gail Inwin, Cindy O'Connor, Sara Kendall, Kirsten

Halbrook, Shelley Curnow, Andrea Shelley, and Rana DeGil: My friends with

whom I have shared my ups and downs and who have supported, helped, loved,

cried, and laughed with me.

To all the incredibly thorough, mindful, and clinical experts in the field of

psychopathy who paved the way for my efforts to achieve the completion of this

dissertation.

I thank all of my professors and clinical supervisors at San Francisco State

University and the California School of Psychology because it is they who and

made it possible for me to be myself, to be able to achieve all that I have, and the

possibility to achieve all that I aspire to.

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

To Dr. Christopher Tori for his support, advice, good humor, and patience. From

him, I have learned a great deal about writing, research, and presenting clinical

material.

To Dr. Murray Bilmes for assisting me with the psychoanalytical theory portion of

this study, and for helping me to create a better dissertation.

To Dr. Margaret Singer for sharing her extensive experiences working with

psychopaths, and for agreeing to be a part of my dissertation committee. This

dissertation is dedicated to her memory.

To Dr. Carl Norris for his many editorial suggestions and for calmly guiding me

through the final phases of the dissertation and defense process.

Special thanks to the staff of Jail Psychiatric Services for their assistance.

Special thanks to the interns and staff at Manhattan Psychiatric Center and Kirby

Forensic Psychiatric Center for their expertise and support in this endeavor.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

Dedication............................................................................................................. iii

Acknowledgments.................................................................................................. v

List of Tables............................................................................................................ vi

I. Statement of the Problem.................................................................................... 1

II. Psychopathy: A Brief History.............................................................................. 16

III. Assessment and Diagnosis: Antisocial Personality Disorder


Vs. Psychopathy...........................................................................................34

IV. Psychoanalytic Theories about Psychopathy............................................... 52

V. Biological Contributions to Psychopathy...........................................................89

VI. Treatment of Psychopathy.................................................................................97

VII. Conclusion.......................................................................................................... 119

References................................................................................................................155

vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
LIST OF TABLES

Page

1. Hares ChildhoodSigns of Psychopathic Behavior......................................12

2. Brief History of How Psychopathy has been Understood.......................... 17

3. Kraeplins Psychopathic Categories.............................................................. 28

4. Cleckleys Psychopathic Categories............................................................... 30

5. Hare Psychopathy Checklist-R Factor Items and Descriptions...................49

6. Childhood Experiences of Psychopaths......................................................... 63

7. Meloys Defensive Structuring of the Psychopath......................................... 76

8. Kernbergs Classification of Personality Disorders


in which Antisocial Features are Prominent...................................................... 81

9. Kernbergs Antisocial Personality Disorder Symptoms............................... 82

10. Meloys Psychopathic Personality Organization........................................ 85

11. Martens Biological, Psychological, and Social Determinants of


Psychopathy................................................................................................ 95

12. Strasburgers Countertransference Responses in Working with


Psychopaths................................................................................................ 109

13. Meta-analysis Results for Psychopathy Intervention Studies....................115

14. Millon and Davis Ten Types of Psychopaths.............................................. 138

15. Psychopharmacological Treatments.............................................................142

vii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
1

Chapter I

Statement of the Problem

Why has this man done these terrible things? Because he is a


psychopath. And how do you know that he is a psychopath?
Because he has done these terrible things (Ellard, 1988, p. 387).

Psychopathy was the first personality disorder identified and described

by psychiatry, and the term was initially used to refer to those personalities

that were extreme variants of what was considered normal (Millon, Simonsen,

Birket-Smith, & Roger, 1998). Historically, there has been extensive debate

regarding the causes, essential features, and diagnostic criteria that identify

psychopathy (Void, Bernard, & Snipes, 1998). The confusion surrounding this

mental condition has been exacerbated by the use of terms with complicated

and contradictory implications to describe the personality organization and

behavioral characteristics of those who behave impulsively, violently, and

without remorse. The terms psychopath, sociopath, or psychopathic inferior

have been used often interchangeably in the psychiatric literature for some

time. The fluidity of this nomenclature and the shifting emphasis between the

moral, religious, and behavioral aspects of the disorder demonstrates the

complexity of the historical evolution of this construct.

Regarding the label itself, the diagnostic criteria and classification of

psychopathy has been, and remains to a large degree, uncertain. Today,

there is confusion about the relationship between the D S M -IV Antisocial

Personality Disorder diagnosis and the construct of psychopathy as first

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
2

defined in 1941 by Cleckley (Table 4) and later researched and quantitatively

measured by Hare (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001).

There have been many empirical studies, particularly over the past

decade, that have attempted to describe the range and severity of the

societal problems caused by the psychopaths behavior. For example,

Woodworth and Porter (2002) found that 15% - 25% of the federal offender

population is at high risk for recidivism and violence when they are identified

as psychopathic. Given the scope of this problem (i.e., more than 2 million

prisoners in the United States), it is surprising that an understanding of

psychopathic cognitive and affective functioning is so limited (Editorial, 2003).

This integrative literature review explores the development of the mind of the

violent psychopath from a psychoanalytic perspective. It examines the

interactions of biological, cognitive, social, and object relational factors that

seemingly result in violent psychopathic behaviors. I will begin by describing

an actual case of violent psychopathic behavior in order to illustrate the

frightening mystery of this problem.

Case Examples

To exemplify the dynamics of a psychopath, I selected the case of

Richard Kuklinski who was the son of a violent, emotionally absent, alcoholic

father. His mother, also quite violent, vented her resentment on her children.

Richard quickly learned to emulate violent behavior and, as a child, he

admitted to having tied two cats together and thrown them over a

clothesline in order to observe them mangling each other. He also kicked

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
dogs off roofs and tied them to the backs of buses. When asked during the

televised HBO interview (2002) what this made him feel, he replied,

excitement... but it wasnt much of a challenge.

Kuklinski had been living in New Jersey with his family (his wife

Barbara, two daughters and a son) but, unlike his own father, he spent much

of his free time with his wife and children. He frequently gave his family

spontaneous vacations, expensive jewelry, and new cars. Apparently, the

cost was not an issue. Anthony Bruno (1993) in The Iceman, (a biography

written with Kuklinski's full cooperation), noted that Kuklinski first killed

someone in 1949, when he was fourteen years old. Reportedly, Kuklinski

was protecting his territory against a bully and fatally beat his adversary,

which filled him with a sense of power. He now perceived himself as

someone special, and developed into a controlling man who did not tolerate

defiance or disrespect.

In a televised documentary on HBO (Ginsberg, 2002), Kuklinski

described his first premeditated murder as an adult. In Jersey City, New

Jersey, he had used a car bomb triggered by gasoline to kill a man. As he

walked away from the exploding car, he felt only cold satisfaction. He

detached himself from his victims, a style that he claims developed from

having to detach himself from the abuse his drunken father inflicted on him as

a boy. According to Kuklinski, no one would ever treat [me] with disrespect,

and he wanted them to know that the last thing they ever saw was me.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
4

Outside of his home Kuklinski was known as The Iceman, a

professional hit man. Bruno (1993) wrote that Kuklinski earned this name

because he had kept one of his victims in a freezer for over two years, out of

curiosity, in an attempt to see if he could disguise the victims actual time of

death. In nearly four decades of his brutal, cold-blooded killings, (and a body

count of over one hundred), the authorities were unable to capture Kuklinski.

He was finally apprehended by the undercover operations of the Bureau of

Alcohol, Tobacco, and Firearms and the New Jersey Attorney General's

Office. By the time of his trial in 1988, at age 53, he had reportedly killed

more than 100 men by shooting, stabbing, choking or poisoning them (Bruno,

1993). Kuklinski is currently incarcerated in Trenton State Prison and will not

be eligible for parole until he is 111 years old. The question for psychologists

is: Why did he become a man who could kill without remorse, guilt, or

hesitation?

This case example illustrates what is generally considered to be

severe, violent psychopathic behavior. To society this is someone who is

frightening and deranged, who represents the worst qualities of human nature

(Turvey, 1999). Although dramatic and frightening, Kuklinskis case is just

one example of violent psychopathic behavior. Other high-profile serial killers

have also been the center of clinical attention, and serve as continuing

reminders of the need to develop a better understanding the etiology of this

disorder.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5

Another example is John Wayne Gacy who worked as a clown, an

occupation that gave him access to young boys. Gacy lured his victims to

secluded places, and then typically, handcuffed, beat, raped, and recited

bible verses to them before eventually strangling them. Gacy was highly

organized in his killing, planning out each murder in detail. When he was

finally caught, the police found 30 bodies buried underneath his house

(Retrieved October 6, 2001 from http://www.mavhem.net/Crime/serial.html).

The case of Jeffrey Dahmer, who murdered 17 gay, African American

men, received worldwide attention. After meeting his victims at gay bars, he

brought them to his home, ostensibly for sex, then drugged, killed, and

sometimes ate them (Norris, 1992). It is reported that he attempted to make a

zombie out of one of his victims by lobotomizing him with a drill and pouring

acid into the drill holes (Schwartz, 1992). When Dahmer was finally

apprehended, the police found three decomposing bodies in 55-gallon acid

vats (Tithecott, 1997). They also found four severed heads, seven skulls,

several skeletons, and a penis (Jaeger & Balousek, 1991).

Another infamous case was Ted Bundy. Bundy was, by all accounts,

an attractive, intelligent, and charming man (Michaud, Aynesworth, & Bundy,

2000). He was also one of the most prolific serial killers of recent times.

Bundy killed at least 22 young women in several states by using his charm

and handsome appearance to attract women on college campuses and in

shopping malls. At times, Bundy would put his arm in a sling to generate

sympathy in order to lure his victims into his car before killing them.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
6

Cousins Kenneth Bianchi and Angelo Buono are known collectively as

the "Hillside Strangler." Bianchi and Buono impersonated police officers,

arrested prostitutes, and then raped and killed them (Newton, 2000). The

pair left their victims corpses in various poses in the hills of Los Angeles.

After his arrest, Bianchi attempted to malinger by faking a multiple personality

disorder in order to evade punishment, but he was unsuccessful. Afterward,

he wrote the following statement that both asks for our pity and denies

responsibility for his crimes:

It is profound to me that I have had to experience more


confusion and mistrust and insincerity in society. If only the
right people had been wise enough to follow through with their
responsibilities, during the years of forming me into the mold of
adulthood, I wouldnt be where I am now. Theres a sadness in
misunderstanding, an emptiness like a hollow egg. The egg
which can produce life in two ways, one in creation and one in
sustenance and not realizing the potential of either (quoted in
Meloy, 1988, pp. 181).

Some Characteristics of Psychopaths

Although gender is not a primary focus of this investigation, it is

noteworthy to mention that the majority of research studies and clinical

examples investigating the psychopathic disorder have been conducted with

male subjects. However, female psychopaths also exist. For example,

Nesca, Dalby, and Baskkerville (1999) investigated the social history and

personality dynamics of a severe female psychopath, and found that her

history included early antisocial behavior, poly-substance abuse, and sexual

acting out. The authors also conducted psychodiagnostic testing, which

indicated that this woman was often quite angry, tended to be sexually

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
7

aggressive, and had a personality structure organized along hysterical lines.

Their results also uncovered the presence of paranoid ideation and poor

reality testing when confronted with stressful situations. The authors

concluded, however, that the patient did not suffer from impulse control

deficits or pathological narcissism.

Salekin, Trobst, and Krioukova (2001) have suggested that the

disorder presents somewhat differently in females than males. Perhaps

because of this Antisocial Personality Disorder is diagnosed five times more

often in males than in females (Meloy, 2001). The difference may be due a

lower frequency of violent behavior among females than males, and rater

errors (i.e., not expecting psychopathy among females). For the purpose of

this review, however, only the violent behavior of male psychopaths will be

examined.

Some researchers have described the psychopath as impulsive and

unorganized, while others have suggested they are, in fact, quite clever and

able to effect long-term planning. Millon (1998) concluded that this apparent

dichotomy stems from stressful and conflictual life experiences that lead to

personality organization of opposites which render individuals capable of

heinous criminal behavior while, at the same time, maintaining an external

demeanor of calm and composure. Thus, he recommended that psychopathy

be conceptualized as a continuum, with less violent behavior (e.g., white-

collar crime, bribery, and ethics violations) at one extreme, and violent,

sadistic acts at the opposite end.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
8

Despite their often sadistic actions and manipulative intentions, many

violent psychopaths can present as quite personable and charming, which

makes them all the more dangerous. Violent psychopaths can be callous,

predatory, without conscience and feeling no guilt about the harm they do to

others. Turvey (1999) observed that, The real psychopath ranks as an

extremely dangerous person... Individuals of this type are often responsible

for perverse and murderous attacks on children frequently noticed in

newspapers. If true and full sadistic tendencies are combined with the

psychopaths lack of compunction, a formidable menace to others is likely to

emerge" (p. 84).

Despite the lurid and sensational nature of their actions,

psychopaths of this sort actually represent only a small percentage of

criminals, perhaps 15 to 20 percent (Hare, 1995; Pitchford 2001; Woodworth

& Porter, 2002). Those meeting the Cleckley/Hare criteria, discussed in detail

later in this review, probably account for about one percent of the

incarcerated population (Hare, 1993). Despite their relatively small number,

these individuals cause major societal problems with half of all serial rapists

suffering from psychopathy, while the recidivism rate of psychopaths is

roughly double that of other offenders (Hare, 1993).

Woodworth and Porter (2002) found that psychopaths are

responsible for more than 50 percent of the serious crimes committed and, in

addition, are responsible for the majority of violent crimes. Psychopaths also

commit twice as many aggressive acts as non-psychopaths, have higher

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
9

recidivism rates (Woodworth & Porter, 2002), and are more likely to repeat

past behaviors (Heinze & Paladino, 2000). For violent psychopaths, murder

is the most extreme expression of their psychopathic behavior, yet it is the

least studied and least understood aspect of the disorder (Woodworth &

Porter, 2002). The criminal justice system has responded to psychopathic

behavior with more restrictive laws and more prisons, yet there is no

consensus as to the provision of effective treatment or rehabilitation, mostly

because there are so few insights into the truly disturbed nature of the

psychopathic mind.

As violent crime continues to rise, social policy considerations are

pressing the need for effective treatment of the disorder (Meloy, 1988).

Salekin (2002) observed that ...continued problems with adult and juvenile

offending indicate that untreated offenders will continue to create serious

problems for society in both the short and long run...these problems are likely

to include violent crimes...stemming from individuals who lack important

human characteristics such as empathy and remorse (p. 80).

Law enforcement, too, is hoping that psychology may provide insights

as to the nature and development of psychopathy. Yet to date, few

meaningful strides have been made in understanding the development, and

perhaps more importantly, the treatment of juvenile delinquents or adult

psychopaths. This problem is particularly pressing because the harmful social

consequences of psychopathy begin early and are manifested throughout the

lifetime of the individual. Methvin (1997) observed that children aged 16 to 18

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
10

commit the majority of serious crimes in this country and that most of them

began before the age often.

A personal definition of the construct of psychopathy must be

described and applied throughout the remainder of this thesis. As with any

operational definition, some may disagree, however from my perspective,

psychopathy is a pervasive, developmental pathology characterized by

deficits in the development of the superego and object relational capacity for

attachment that is best understood as a variant of narcissistic personality

disorder with egosyntonic aggressive features.

Understanding the Mind of the Psychopath

Because developmental theories suggest there may be a genetic

predisposition to psychopathy, early intervention is important in order to

reduce the costs to society (Salekin, 2002). Early intervention, however,

requires early detection. Hare (1993) identified some characteristics of the

psychopathic disorder that are first detectable in childhood. These are

characterized as transgressions from what is considered normal

development. Early warning signs that appear to mark the onset of the

disorder are shown in Table 1. Another view is presented in the Diagnostic

and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV) which requires

evidence of a conduct disorder before age 15, which is defined as:

A repetitive and persistent pattern of behavior in which the basic


rights of others or societal norms or rules are violated, as
manifest by the presence of three or more of the following
criteria in the past 12 months, with at least one criterion present
in the past six months (DSM-IV, pg 00).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11

The criteria are: (a) often bullies, threatens, or intimidates others; (b) often

initiates physical fights; (c) has used a weapon that can cause serious

physical harm to others; (d) has been physically cruel to people; (e) has been

physically cruel to animals; (f) has stolen while confronting a victim (e.g.,

mugging, purse snatching, extortion, armed robbery); (g) has forced someone

into sexual activity; (h) has deliberately engaged in fire setting with the

intention of causing serious damage; (i) has deliberately destroyed others'

property (other than by fire setting); 0) has broken into someone else's house,

building, or car; (k) often lies to obtain goods or favors or to avoid obligations;

(I) has stolen items of nontrivial value without confronting a victim; (m) often

stays out at night despite parental prohibitions, beginning before age 13

years; (n) has run away from home overnight at least twice while living in

parental or parental surrogate home; and (o) is often truant from school,

beginning before age 13 years.

Despite the long history of the psychopathic disorder, the etiology of

psychopathy has eluded social scientists. The primary focus of researchers

and clinicians has been on identifying and describing the characteristics of

the disorder. During the past few decades, for example, the Federal Bureau

of Investigations (FBI) Behavioral Sciences Unit has methodically studied

psychopaths and their motivation for killing, and this has helped scientists

make substantial gains in identifying and characterizing psychopathy

(Anderson, 1994). The FBI has developed extensive training and literature on

how to "profile" (using crime scene evidence) the serial killers psychological

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
12

motivations for violence. According to Douglas (1996), profiling may be

defined as a technique for identifying the major personality and behavioral

characteristics of an individual based upon an analysis of the crimes he or

she has committed (p. 405). While profiling has been effective in helping to

apprehend criminals, it sheds little light on the etiological determinants of

psychopathy, and even less focused on the treatment of the disorder or how

to prevent it. Treatment and prevention remain the more difficult problems (J.

E. Douglas, 1996).

Table 1

Hares Childhood Signs of Psychopathic Behavior___________________________


1. Repetitive, casual, and seemingly thoughtless lying.
2. Indifference to, or inability to understand the feelings, expectations, or pain of
others.
3. Defiance of parents, teachers, and rules.
4. Continually in trouble and unresponsive to reprimands and threats of
punishment.
5. Petty theft from other children and parents.
6. Persistent aggression, bullying, and fighting.
7. A record of unremitting truancy, staying out late, and absences from home.
8. A pattern of hurting or killing animals.
9. Early experimentation with sex.
10.Vandalism and fire setting.____________________________________________
Note. From Without Conscience: The Disturbing World of the Psychopaths Among
Us (p. 158), by Robert Hare, 1993, New York: The Guildford Press.

Traditionally, the psychopath has been deemed incurable and

development of effective treatment models based on scientific findings has

been severely lacking (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Cleckley, 1988; Hare, 1993;

Millon et al., 1998; Rabin, 1979; Strasburger, 2001). Hare (1995) noted that

most interventions have been behaviorally oriented and of limited success. In

fact, there is some evidence to suggest that current treatment methods may

be no better than if the individual had no treatment at all. Cognitive behavioral

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
13

programs and other modes of treatment typically found in jails and prisons

are primarily designed for patients that can identify a problem behavior and,

with help, curtail it. Since most psychopaths have no regard for societal

values and do not view their behavior as problematic, these behavioral

interventions are rendered ineffective.

Hare (1995) further stated that psychopaths were almost four times

more likely to commit violent crimes after release from an intensive

therapeutic community program than were other patients, including

psychopaths who were left untreated. "They had learned enough psychiatric

and psychological jargon to convince therapists, counselors, and parole

boards that they were making remarkable progress, but they used that

knowledge only to develop new rationalizations for their behavior and better

ways to manipulate and deceive" (p. 2). When incarcerated, psychopaths

also display more violent and aggressive behaviors (Ogloff, Wong, &

Greenwood, 1990), are frequently segregated (McCord, 1982), and are more

likely to violate orders of conditional release (Wong, 1984).

The existing interventions are limited by the lack of a cogent

understanding of the psychological complexity of the psychopath including

the constellation of factors that contribute to the development of the disorder.

It is imperative to gain a better phenomenological understanding of the

psychopaths thought process and affective life which has been largely

ignored by professionals because of its frightening and disturbing content.

Along with this, better theoretical models are needed to elucidate the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
14

behavior, intrapsychic process, and intrinsic motivation involved in

psychopathy. To accomplish this, psychoanalytic theory presents itself as a

likely candidate to provide a comprehensive explanation of the motivating

forces that guide psychopathic behavior. Although Freud did not specifically

study psychopaths, psychoanalysts such as Cleckley (1988), Reich

(1925/1972), and Kernberg (1975) have made valuable contributions

regarding the origins and treatment of psychopathy.

This psychodynamic understanding may need to be supplemented by

neurobiological research. More recently, Meloy (1988) hypothesized that

there may be a psychobiological predisposition to the psychopathic disorder,

with deficient and conflictual primary object experiences that determine its

phenotypic expression (p. 6). The current psychoanalytic perspective is

increasingly open to the evidence that neuroscience presents. Research in

this area has already led to the identification of brain functions that affect

certain cognitive and affective processes in psychopaths. While some

evidence for a genetic etiology of psychopathy has been postulated, this area

of study remains in its infancy. Nevertheless, it is possible for dynamically

oriented formulations to incorporate these findings into the existing theoretical

understanding of the psychopathic personality structure.

Despite this complexity, it is the responsibility of psychologists to seek

an understanding of the disordered mind of the psychopath with the hope that

effective talking and pharmacological treatments as well as preventive

measures will one day be developed. Although promising research has

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
15

provided some hope of identifying individuals with this disorder, the search

continues for the underlying psychological processes supporting frightening

antisocial conduct.

In this dissertation, I will attempt to provide a psychoanalytical

perspective of the etiology of psychopathy by first presenting an integrative

summary of the historical development of the diagnosis. From there, I will

examine the psychoanalytic literature concerning the development of a

psychopathic personality structure. Then I will review current research

concerning the biological and environmental influences of psychopathy.

Finally, I will discuss treatment recommendations from an analytic

perspective.

It is hoped this analysis will contribute to clinical practice by providing

social scientists, mental health practitioners, and law enforcement officials

with a better understanding of the mind of the psychopath. This literature

review may also have heuristic value by stimulating the development of

theoretical thinking that may result in improved practice and innovative

research.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
16

Chapter II

Psychopathy: A Brief History

For many centuries, philosophers have described the personalities of

people displaying unique patterns of behavior generally regarded as breaking

social mores of their time. Theophrastus described what he called The

Unscrupulous Man:

The Unscrupulous Man will go and borrow more money from a


creditor he has never paid...When marketing he reminds the
butcher of some service he has rendered him and, standing
near the scales, throws in some meat, if he can, and a soup-
bone. If he succeeds, so much the better; if not, he will snatch
a piece of tripe and go off laughing (quoted in Millon et al.,
1998, p. 3).

The term psychopath and its meaning have been through quite a

number of formulations. The concept has been marked by much clinical

debate and shifting foci (e.g., on the individuals morality, volitional control,

and deterministic personality traits) as time has passed, and a great deal of

confusion still exists regarding the diagnostic criteria of and treatment

recommendations for the psychopathic disorder.

Changes in the Understanding of Psychopathy and its Causes over Time

In order to show how the understanding of the construct of

psychopathy has evolved, I will present a chronology of the changing

dimensions of psychopathy and a brief description of the unique contributions

of those individuals who were instrumental in developing these

understandings. Arrigo and Shipley (2001) categorized the fluid construct of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
17

psychopathy by tabulating the evolving philosophical and scientific viewpoints

of social condemnation and treatment prognosis as in Table 2 below.

Table 2

Brief History of How of Psychopathy has been Understood Part I


Date Theorist Nomenclature Description Social
Prognosis
Condemnation
1801 Pinel Manie sans Insanity without Morally neutral Moral
delire delirium treatment;
no bleeding

1812 Rush Moral Total perversion Enter social Tx preferred


alienation of of moral faculties condemnation in medical
the mind settings

1835 Prichard Moral insanity Deplorable defect Social No volitional


in personality castigation control;
intensifies should be
legal
defense

1891 Koch Psychopathic Strictly congenital Attempts to Depends on


inferiority personality types shed social the type;
condemnation chronic or
temporary

1897 Maudsley Moral Criminal class Criminal class Useless to


imbecility affected by status punish
cerebral deficits those that
cannot
control their
behavior

1904 Krafft- Morally Savages in Severe Without


Ebing depraved society condemnation prospect of
success -
asylums
indefinitely

1915 Kraepelin Psychopathic Most Moral Poor


personalities vicious/wicked; judgment/
born criminals, social
liars... condemnation

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
18

Brief History of How of Psychopathy has been Understood Part II


Date Theorist Nomenclature Description Social Prognosis
Condemnation
1941 Cleckley Psychopath Detached/ Pejorative Poor
Narcissistic
interpersonal style

1952 APAS Sociopathic More social Pejorative Poor


DSM personality perspective;
disturbance; Cleckleys
antisocial/disso features and
cial sociopath some criminal
behaviors

1968 DSM -II ASPD Still focused on Pejorative Poor


the personality
traits

1980 D S M -III ASPD Chronic violation Pejorative; Typically


of social norms includes most poor, but
offenders symptoms
diminish
with age

1985 Hares Psychopathy 2 factor model; Pejorative Poor; can


PCL/ reliable and valid become
PCL-R worse with
treatment

1994 DSM- IV ASPD Focused on Pejorative Poor, but


behavioral criteria equated with symptoms
criminal diminish
with
treatment
Note\ From The Confusion Over Psychopathy (I): Historical
Considerations, by B. Arrigo and S. Shipley, 2001, International Journal of
Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 45(3), pp. 325 - 344.

I will present a brief literature review describing the development of this

diagnostic category, starting with its inception in the early 19th century,

continuing through the contemporary contributions to the literature in this

area.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
19

19th Century Contributions to the Understanding of Psychopathy

Most writers on the topic of psychopathy credit French physician

Philippe Pinel as the first to scientifically study these individuals over 200

years ago. He coined the term manie sans delire, meaning madness without

confusion or insanity without delirium, to describe patients who presented

with impulsivity and violent behaviors yet were otherwise lucid with intact

reasoning abilities (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Hare, 1996; Lykken, 1995; Millon

& Davis, 1998; Rabin, 1979). Pinel (1801/1962) stated I was not a little

surprised to find many maniacs who at no period gave evidence of any lesion

of understanding, but who were under the dominion of instinctive and abstract

fury, as if the faculties of affect alone had sustained injury (p. 9).

Prior to Pinels observations, mental illness was believed to be an

impairment in the functioning of the brain, and insanity was considered to be

a loss of reason and intellect (Millon et al., 1998), but Pinel argued that one

could act impulsively, i.e., exhibit violent behavior, and cause harm to self

and others without the classic symptoms of mental illness. Further, he

insisted that these people knew full well that they were behaving irrationally.

It is worth noting that Pinel, for the first time, linked these individuals to

persons suffering from traditional mental disorders since both seemed unable

to control their symptoms (Muntz, 1998).

Over time, clinicians worldwide began to treat and describe patients

similar to those described by Pinel. In 1812, the American psychiatrist

Benjamin Rush (a signer of the Declaration of Independence) stated that

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
20

psychopaths had an innate, preternatural moral depravity...There is probably

an original defective organization in those parts of the body which are

preoccupied by the moral faculties of the mind (Toch, 1998, p. 112). While

Pinels description of this disorder was more affect-based, Rush believed

there was an underlying genetic, inherited etiology for the condition of

psychopathy to which the environment contributed situational factors that

favored or decreased the chance of its expression (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001).

Rush is also credited with being the first to turn psychopathy into something

socially condemnable (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Millon et al., 1998). He wrote:

The will might be deranged even in many instances of persons


of sound understandings...the will becoming the involuntary
vehicle of vicious actions through the instrumentality of the
passions. Persons thus diseased cannot speak the truth upon
any subject...their falsehoods are seldom calculated to injure
anybody but themselves (quoted in Millon et al., 1998, p. 4).

In 1835, British physician J.C. Pritchard spoke of moral insanity

(Lykken, 1995), and believed that psychopathic personalities were a form of

mental derangement (Rabin, 1979). He became the major spokesperson of

the view that there was a significant defect in the personalities of persons

with this condition that deserved social condemnation. According to Millon et

al. (1998), Pritchard also broadened the scope of the original syndrome by

including under the label moral insanity a wide range of previously diverse

mental and emotional conditions (p. 5). Pritchard described the

symptomology of psychopaths as follows:

There is a form of mental derangement in which the intellectual


functions appear to have sustained little or no injury, while the
disorder is manifested principally or alone in the state of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
21

feelings, temper or habits. In cases of this nature the moral or


active principles of the mind are strangely perverted or
depraved, the power of self-government is lost or greatly
impaired, and the individual is found to be incapable, not of
talking or reasoning...but conducting himself with decency and
propriety in the business of life (quoted in Millon et al., 1998, p.
6)

In addition, Pritchard believed that psychopathy was a disease, and

that despite their intellectual ability to understand their behavior, psychopaths

were overpowered by affections forcing them to behave in ways

unacceptable to society. Pritchards description of the symptoms and the

disorder accentuates the clear lack of impairment in intellect or reasoning,

and in particular, the absence of psychotic symptoms such as hallucinations

or delusions (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Millon et al., 1998).

Maudsley felt that punishment for psychopaths was not warranted

because they could not control their behavior, and further, incarceration

would not rehabilitate individuals suffering from the disorder (Toch, 1979).

Instead, he cited cerebral deficits as the root of psychopathy, and believed

there was a specific cerebral center underlying natural moral feelings

(Millon et al., 1998, p. 7). He wrote as evidence:

When we find young children, long before they can possibly


know what vice and crime means, addicted to extreme vice, or
committing great crimes, with an instinctive facility, and as if
from an inherent proneness to criminal actions...and when
experience proves that punishment has no reformatory effect
upon them - that they cannot reform - it is made evident that
moral imbecility is a fact, and that punishment is not the fittest
treatment of it (quoted in Toch, 1988, p.147).

Often overlooked in the historical reviews of psychopathy, Lombroso

developed a typology of born criminals, insane criminals, and criminaloids

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
22

(Arrigo & Shipley, 2001). In 1889, he described the criminaloid as individuals

whose mental and emotional makeup was such that under certain

circumstances they indulge(d) in vicious and criminal behavior (quoted in

Void, Bernard, & Snipes, 1998, p. 34). Lombroso proposed the notion that

someone could be a born delinquent, and added to the literature the idea

that there were certain physical characteristics that distinguish these people.

According to Lombroso, the psychopaths tendency toward criminality was

somehow linked to an innate return to pre-historic functioning. He stated,

constitutionally disposed criminal types displayed a notably large and

projective lower jaw, outstretched ears, retreating forehead, left-handed ness,

robust physique, precocious sexual development, tactile insensibility,

muscular agility, and so on... (quoted in Millon et al., 1988, p. 7).

Koch (1891) replaced moral insanity with the phrase psychopathic

inferiority, which characterized those who seemed to behave abnormally due

to biological factors, but who were not psychotic or insane (Ellard, 1988;

Lykken, 1995; McCord & McCord, 1964; Rabin, 1979). Koch described the

condition by noting that all mental irregularities, whether congenital or

acquired, that influence a man in his personal life and cause him, even in the

most favorable cases, to seem not fully in possession of normal mental

capacity. They always remain psychopathic, in that they are caused by

organic states and changes which are beyond the limits of physiological

normality (quoted in Millon et al., 1988, p. 8). Further, Koch divided the

psychopathic inferiority into three categories: (a) psychopathic disposition,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
23

which was characterized by sensitivity and tension; (b) psychic inferiority or

psychopathic taint, which manifested as impulsive anger and egocentricities;

and (c) psychopathic degeneration, which might be seen as borderline mental

states (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001). Despite his efforts to focus less on the moral

and social condemnation of the disorder and more on the biological causes of

abnormal behavior in psychopaths, the label itself continued to remain

pejorative (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Millon et al., 1998).

As the 19th century came to a close, there seemed to be a general

agreement among psychologists that there was a disorder in which patients

presented with marked impulsivity, socially deviant, and at times, violent

behavior. These persons also showed no evidence of impaired reality testing,

impaired intellectual abilities, or other deficits in functioning. What was not

clear yet was the diagnostic terminology to be used with these individuals.

Over the next century social scientists and clinicians expanded upon Pinels

initial work, hoping to understand the nature of this affliction.

20thCentury Contributions to the Understanding of Psychopathy

Adolf Meyer, convinced that American psychiatry had become

stagnant and hopeless, introduced the German psychiatric models to

American psychiatrists early in the 20th century, and was instrumental in

acquainting them with the construct of psychopathy (Werlinder, 1978). Meyer

believed that genetic predispositions played a large role in the development

of psychopathology. In 1905, he published a classification of

psychopathology in which he described the constitutional inferiority of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
24

psychopaths. These individuals were considered to be neurotic, with general

defects in their constitutional make-up and development (Werlinder, 1978).

Meyers writings about the constitutional inferiority condition came to replace

what was known in America as moral insanity.

Krafft-Ebing was probably among the first to comment that

psychopaths could not be treated successfully, and that those afflicted with

the disorder should be housed permanently in asylums in order to ensure

public safety (Toch, 1998). Ellard (1988) believed that Krafft-Ebings

sentiments were the beginning of a trend to link pathology with social

deviance and further promulgated the notion that the condition of

psychopathy was untreatable. Perhaps a more noteworthy contribution, in

terms of this review, is that Krafft-Ebing also introduced the terms sadism

and masochism into the psychological jargon, and wrote about what he

considered to be the violent psychopaths innate desire to humiliate and

hurt. Krafft-Ebing believed that this desire, considered an otherwise normal

element of male sexuality, would be heightened in psychopaths and thus

render them more likely to act upon this innate aggression (Millon et al.,

1998).

Kraepelin expanded upon the description of Kochs psychopathic

inferiority and categorized several different types of psychopathy that

essentially described the most devious, devastating, and most frequently

seen characteristics of the disorder (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001). He first

published these typologies in his landmark text Psychiatry: A Textbook for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
25

Students and Physicians and, throughout subsequent editions, he solidified

the characteristics and description of the psychopath as he developed

expertise in operationalizing the disorder (Kraepelin & Quen, 1990).

In the second edition of his text, Kraepelin (1887/1904) stated The

morally insane suffer congenital defects in their ability to restrain the reckless

gratification of...immediate egotistical desires (p. 281), and suggested there

might be some personality trait that develops in these individuals. In the fifth

edition of his text (1896) Kraepelin used the term psychopathic states,

which he believed to be trademark personality characteristics that remained

relatively consistent across the lifespan of the psychopath.

Kraepelin also believed that psychopathic states were degenerative

and likened to other syndromes such as obsessions, impulsivity, and sexual

perversions. Millon et al. (1998) stated that Kraepelins categorizations

effectively shifted the focus of psychopathy to a moral, pejorative, and social

deviation that had limited response to treatment. In the 1904 edition of his

textbook, Kraepelin created and defined four categories of psychopaths (see

Table 3), and Millon et. al. (1998) and Arrigo and Shipley (2001) noted, these

categories closely reflect what is now considered as Antisocial Personality

Disorder in the contemporary nomenclature.

By the eighth edition of his text (1915), Kraepelin began to narrow the

focus of his categories to those specific characteristics that were most often

seen in the institutional setting, and used the term psychopathic personality

to describe the amoral or immoral personality types of the psychopath

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
26

(Lykken, 1995), and considered them to be the most harmful to society

(Arrigo & Shipley, 2001). Instead of his previous concept of four categories

(Table 3), he now conceptualized psychopathy more broadly into two

categories. The first, morbid disposition, consisted of obsessives,

impulsives, and sexual deviants (Millon et al., 1998; Arrigo & Shipley, 2001).

The second category, personality peculiarities, was expanded into seven

different subtypes: (a) excitable, (b) unstable, (c) impulsive, (d) eccentric, (e)

liars and the swindlers, (f) antisocial, and (g) quarrelsome. Of particular note,

the antisocial type Kraepelin identified was the precursor to what is now

known clinically as Antisocial Personality Disorder. He described these

individuals as:

...the enemies of society...characterized by a blunting of the


moral elements. They are often destructive and
threatening...[and] there is a lack of deep emotional reaction;
and of sympathy and affection they have little. They are apt to
have been troublesome in school, given to truancy and running
away. Early thievery is common among them and they commit
crimes of various kinds (quoted in Millon et al., 1998, p. 10).

As behaviorism began to take hold in psychology, the term sociopath,

was often used interchangeably with psychopath. This stemmed from a

desire to root the etiology of the diagnosis in social conditioning as opposed

to organic causes. Still popular today, the term is sociopath is credited to

Birnbaum (1909), who believed that antisocial behavior only rarely stems

from inherent immoral traits of character; rather it reflects most often the

operation of societal forces that make the more acceptable forms of behavior

and adaptation difficult to acquire (Millon et al., 1998, p. 11).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
27

During this period, the term sociopath, was often used

interchangeably with psychopath. This stemmed from a desire to root the

etiology of the diagnosis in social conditioning as opposed to organic causes.

Still popular today, the term is sociopath is credited to Birnbaum (1909), who

believed that antisocial behavior only rarely stems from inherent immoral

traits of character; rather it reflects most often the operation of societal forces

that make the more acceptable forms of behavior and adaptation difficult to

acquire (Millon et al., 1998, p.11).

Schneider (1958) wrote about those individuals that performed criminal

acts, a behavior also evident in adolescence. He believed there were many of

these individuals who existed undetected in society or were politicians and

other powerful people. Schneider designated a particular type of

psychopathic personality as the impulsively violent type, that correlates to

the current conception of Antisocial Personality Disorder. He described those

suffering from this affliction as follows:

...personalities with a marked emotional blunting mainly but not


exclusively in relation to their fellows. Their character is a
pitiless one and they lack capacity for shame, decency,
remorse and conscience. They are ungracious, cold, surly,
and brutal in crime...The social moral code is known,
understood but not felt and therefore this personality is
indifferent to it (p. 126).

Cleckleys (1941) monumental contribution, The Mask of Sanity, hailed

the onset of modern concepts of psychopathy, and was a groundbreaking

work which attempted to identify observable behaviors and characteristic

actions of the psychopath (Table 4).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
28

Table 3

Kraeplins Psychopathic Categories


Term Description

Morbid liars and swindlers Glib and charming, but lacking in


inner morality and a sense of
responsibility to others; they
made frequent use of aliases,
were inclined to be fraudulent
con artists, and often
accumulated heavy debts that
were invariably unpaid.
Criminals by impulse Individuals who engaged in such
crimes as arson, rape, and
kleptomania, and were driven by
an inability to control their urges;
they rarely sought material gains
for their criminal actions.
Professional criminals Were neither impulsive nor
undisciplined; in fact, they often
appeared well mannered and
socially appropriate, but were
inwardly calculating,
manipulative, and self-serving.
Morbid vagabonds Strongly disposed to wander
through life, never taking firm
root, lacking both self-confidence
and the ability to undertake adult
responsibilities.
Note: From Psychopathy: Antisocial, Criminal, and Violent Behavior, by T.
Millon, E. Simonsen, and M. Birket-Smith (Eds.), 1998. New York: The
Guilford Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
29

For most of his career, Cleckley scrutinized the psychopathic

personality and his work remains widely accepted as a major contribution to

the field, sparking a new interest in the identification and assessment of

psychopathy (Muntz, 1998). Cleckley also recognized that the condition,

which he unsuccessfully attempted to label semantic dementia, was not

limited to those housed in jails, prisons, or mental institutions (Cleckley,

1988). Rather, there seemed to be a difference between criminal behavior

and true psychopathy. Cleckley believed that successfully psychopaths

never actually became part of the criminal justice system and were more

likely to be found as successful businessmen, politicians, or in schools, and

even seemingly normal families (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001). Cleckley

characterized noting, ...a very deep seated disorder often exists. The true

difference between them and the psychopaths who continually go to jails or

psychiatric hospitals is that they keep up a far better and more consistent

outward appearance of being normal (Cleckley, 1988, p. 198).

It is interesting to note that Cleckley also introduced the term

semantic aphasia, which he used to describe the psychopaths uncanny

ability to understand the concrete meaning of words without grasping the

affective charge they have. Muntz (1998) stated that Cleckley referred to this

phenomenon as knowing all the words to a song, but not the music (p. 14).

Cleckley analyzed and published detailed case studies of his

psychopathic patients, providing the first detailed clinical accounts combined

with a conceptual framework for psychologists to think about the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
30

development, presentation, and subsequent treatment of the psychopath.

Arrigo and Shipley (2001) stated that Cleckleys work led him to the

conclusion that what might be considered normal socializing experiences

were not motivating or effectual with the psychopath (p. 334).

Table 4

Cleckleys Psychopathic Characteristics_________________________________


1. Superficial charm and good intelligence
2. Absence of delusions and other signs of irrational thinking
3. Absence of nervousness or psychoneurotic manifestations
4. Unreliability
5. Untruthfulness and insecurity
6. Lack of remorse or shame
7. Inadequately motivated antisocial behavior
8. Poor judgment and failure to learn by experience
9. Pathologic egocentricity and incapacity for love
10. General poverty in major affective reactions
11. Specific loss of insight
12. Unresponsiveness in general interpersonal relations
13. Fantastic and uninviting behavior with drink and sometimes without
14. Suicide rarely carried out
15. Sex life impersonal, trivial, poorly integrated
16. Failure to follow any life plan

Note. From The Mask of Sanity, (5tn ed.), by H. Cleckley, 1988, p. 19. New
York: The C.V. Mosby Company.

This is not quite correct, instead Cleckley explored their intrapersonal

characteristics or inferred, non-observable, processes...such as lack of

judgment, impulsivity, an inability to feel remorse or guilt, an inability to learn

from punishment, and rationalizing or blaming others for ones behavior (p.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
31

334). This core belief shifted the focus away from criminal behavior and

highlighted the impact of heredity, the environment, and intrapsychic

psychological forces at work simultaneously in the psychopathic mind.

Cleckley (1988) described the psychopaths behavior quite eloquently:

The [psychopath] is unfamiliar with the primary facts or data


of what might be called personal values and is altogether
incapable of understanding such matters. It is impossible for
him to take even a slight interest in the tragedy or joy or the
striving of humanity as presented in serous literature or art.
He is also indifferent to all these matters in life itself. Beauty
and ugliness, except in a very superficial sense, goodness,
evil, love, horror, and humour have no actual meaning, no
power to move him. He is, furthermore, lacking in the ability
to see that others are moved. It is as though he were
colour-blind, despite his sharp intelligence, to this aspect of
human existence. It cannot be explained to him because
there is nothing in his orbit of awareness that can bridge the
gap with comparison. He can repeat the words and say
glibly that he understands, and there is no way for him to
realize that he does not understand (Cleckley, 1988, p. 90).

Rabin (1979) also noted that Cleckleys description of psychopathic

characteristics essentially described what the psychopath was not, i.e.,

specifically highlighting the notion that the psychopath was clearly not

psychotic, neurotic, or mentally retarded.

Hares Conception of Psychopaths

Robert Hare, described by Lykken (1995) as the most productive and

significant player among psychopathy researchers since 1965 (p. 166),

expanded on Cleckleys description of psychopathy. Hare (1995) defined and

described psychopaths as:

Intraspecies predators who use charm, manipulation,


intimidation, and violence to control others and to satisfy their
own selfish needs. Lacking in conscience and in feelings for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
32

others, they cold-bloodedly take what they want and do as


they please, violating social norms and expectations without
the slightest sense of guilt or regret...they form a significant
portion of persistent criminals, drug dealers, spouse and child
abusers, swindlers and con men, mercenaries, corrupt
politicians, unethical lawyers, terrorists, cult leaders, black
marketers, gang members, and radical political activists (p.
27).

Hare also added that these individuals are not delusional, psychotic, or

mentally ill in the traditional sense. Rather, he believed that, unlike psychotic

individuals, psychopaths are completely rational and fully cognizant of their

behaviors, know why they were engaging in them, and are freely choosing to

do so. Hare is also credited for his development of a valid instrument to

assess psychopathy. I will present a detailed and cogent description of Hares

assessment instrument in the following chapter.

Because of Cleckleys work, the term psychopath became popular in

both clinical and public domain as an explanation for and a cause of

depraved and frequent criminal behavior (Ellard, 1988, p. 387). However,

Cleckley did not have the final word. Today, terms psychopathy and

sociopathy are often used interchangeably, though the choice of term is

often used to avoid confusion with psychoticism and insanity...and reflects

the users views on whether the determinants of the condition are

psychological, biological, and genetic on the one hand or social forces and

early experience on the other (Hare, 1993. p. 23).

Much time has passed since Pinel documented what he called the

insane, but not confused, patients, since Cleckley advanced the clinical

understanding of the psychopathic mind and clinical research built upon his

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
33

work. Still, the debate surrounding the etiology, clinical characteristics,

identification, and treatment of psychopathy continues in psychology.

In many ways, the construct and definition of psychopathy changed

with the mores and norms of society and, in fact, so did the field of

psychology. With the onset of the categorization of mental disorders, there

had been much discussion about the essential characteristics and features of

psychopathy. Currently, the ongoing debate among researchers, clinicians,

and other professionals is focused on what the official diagnostic criteria for

psychopathy should be for inclusion in the DSM-V.

While psychologists know more about psychopathy than ever before,

thanks largely to the clinical research discussed in the following chapter,

there is still a great deal to understand about the diagnosis, assessment, and

effective treatment of this disorder.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
34

Chapter III

Assessment and Diagnosis:Antisocial Personality Disorder vs. Psychopathy

As the construct of psychopathy was evolving, the term became a

diagnostic wastebasket (Rabin, 1979. p. 323) for patients that did not

present in treatment as either neurotic or psychotic. Although general

agreement was beginning to coalesce about what characteristics

psychopaths displayed, psychologists, social scientists, and professionals

were still far from reaching consensus regarding the diagnostic criteria which

best described the disorder.

The DSM Definition of Psychopathy

In 1952, the first edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of

mental disorders (DSM) was published, and replaced what was becoming

known as psychopathy with the Sociopathic Personality Disturbance (Millon

et al., 1988; Rabin, 1979). While the new criteria did included much of

Cleckley's description of psychopathy, the prominence of the behaviorist

movement in the United States influenced the disorder to evolve into a more

behavioral concept which focused on deviant behaviors rather than internal

psychodynamics (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Meloy, 2001). McCord and McCord

(1964) believed this shift allowed for social and cultural considerations

contributing to the disorder to be taken into account. However, Arrigo and

Shipley (2001) explain that by focusing on deviant behaviors (p. 336) the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
35

Sociopathic Personality Disturbance categorized many different types of

criminals, thus made the disorder overly broad and inclusive.

Also included in the original version of the DSM were two subtypes of

Sociopathic Personality Disturbance, labeled the antisocial and the dyssocial

sociopaths. The antisocial type was characteristic of those individuals who

are always in trouble, profiting neither from experience nor punishment...they

are frequently callous and hedonistic, showing lack of responsibility...and lack

of judgment (Rabin, 1979, p. 323). The dyssocial sociopath was described

as a "professional criminal" (Smith, 1978b), or more simply put, a member of

organized crime.

The Revised DSM-II Definition

There were not many changes made to the criteria with the publication

of the D S M -II in 1968, however the category of dyssocial sociopath was

dropped and the DSM officially began to use the term Antisocial Personality

Disorder. Smith (1978a) believed the dyssocial subtype was easily

eliminated, as it represented only illegal behaviors and had nothing to do with

the true characteristics of psychopathy.

Ongoing DSM-III and DSM lll-R Revisions

Twelve years later, the D S M -III continued to use the term Antisocial

Personality Disorder, and now described it as a ...deeply ingrained

maladaptive patterns of behavior that are perceptibly different in quality from

psychotic and neurotic symptoms. Generally these are life-long patterns,

often recognizable by the time of adolescence or earlier (Arrigo & Shipley,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
36

2001, p. 337). The DSM-III diagnostic category practically eliminated the

personality traits that Cleckley (1988) described, and instead focused almost

entirely on illegal behaviors. Hare, Hart, and Harpur criticized the diagnostic

criteria as a rather radical break with clinical tradition, with clinical practice,

with earlier versions of the DSM, and excludes...such characteristics as

selfishness, egocentricity, callousness, manipulativeness, lack of empathy,

and so forth" (Hare, Hart, & Harpur, 1991, p. 392). Blackburn (1998) further

argued:

The current equation of psychopathic with antisocial


reflects a transmogrification of the meaning of the adjective
form psychologically damaged (i.e., psychopathological) to
socially damaging... and follows the moral insanity
tradition, insofar as psychological abnormality is inferred
from social deviance (p. 51).

The DSM diagnosis was originally intended to include all of the traits of

psychopaths, but Pitchford (2001) argued that because clinicians were not

thought to be able to assess personality traits competently, the DSM

definition focused instead on the antisocial and criminal behaviors that were

more easily observed and measured. This was a bias toward describing

mental illnesses in terms of their behavioral manifestations rather than the

underlying psychodynamic causes that became increasingly prevalent in later

editions of the DSM. Unfortunately, this emphasis on behavior alone blurred

the distinction between true psychopaths and criminals, and in many ways

effectively reinforced the belief that treatment with these type of offenders

would likely be ineffective (Blackburn, 1998).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
37

Hare (1996) argued that this shift in the conceptualization of

psychopathy from dynamic underpinnings to observable behaviors might

have been formulated in the service of greater reliability. Since behaviors are

more easily observed and measured than personality traits, it is easier to

agree on manifest behaviors instead of their complex etiology. However, this

striving for the reliable measurement of behaviors has essentially weakened

the construct validity of psychopathy (Hare et al., 1991; Scogan, 2000).

There was, in fact, much criticism regarding the shift in diagnostic

criteria adopted in the DSM-III and DSM-III-R. For example, Kernberg (1998)

wrote:

The shortcoming of the description of the antisocial


personality disorder in DSM-III-R, where the use of
excessively concrete, behavioral criteria instead of
personality traits - psychological, intrapsychic criteria - and
the heavy emphasis on criminal behavior, all risk coming to
an improper diagnosis...The criteria are certainly broad
enough to include practically all patients with antisocial
personality disorders who present predominantly aggressive
interactional patterns and criminal behavior...In their stress
on childhood antecedents they appropriately direct the
clinician to the childhood origins of this character pathology...
Unfortunately, however, in this very stress on the criminal
aspect of antisocial behavior they include delinquents with
very different personality makeup, and blur the distinction
between sociocultural and economic determinants of
delinquency on the one hand and psychopathology of the
personality on the other, (p. 316)

A particular oversight Kernberg highlighted regarding the DSM-

lll/DSM-lll-R criteria was the exclusion of the non-aggressive variety or the

predominantly inadequate or passive type of antisocial, whom he described

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
38

as possessing the dominant traits of parasitic and exploitive behaviors, as

opposed to overt behaviors which are more aggressive.

Also new to the DSM-III, and the subsequent revision, was the

requirement for a history of deviant behavior, labeled Conduct Disorder,

before the age of 15 (APA, 1987). For example, some of the deviant

behaviors required for Conduct Disorder were lying, hurting animals or

people, and fighting. Lykken (1995) and others have criticized this

requirement because children might also engage in these behaviors for other

reasons, such as abuse or neglect at home, rather than some biologically

predisposed link to adult psychopathic disorder. I will address these

developmental and biological determinants later in this review.

Hare, Hart, and Harpur (1991) argued that since the criteria for

Antisocial Personality Disorder were not weighted and the frequency or

severity of symptoms was not given serious consideration in the definitional

criteria, there could be little in the way of experimental evidence to support

the validity of the criteria adequately. Further, the researchers also argued

that:

All of those who fulfill the APD criteria may be antisocial, but
they may differ greatly in their motivations for being so and
in significant interpersonal, affective, and psychopathological
features, such as the capacity for empathy, remorse, guilt,
anxiety, or loyalty. Paradoxically, the criteria for APD appear
to define a diagnostic category that is at once too broad,
encompassing criminals and antisocial persons who are
psychologically heterogeneous, and too narrow, excluding
those who have the personality structure of the psychopath
but who have not exhibited some of the specific antisocial
behaviors listed for APD. (p. 393)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
39

Hares Psychopathy Checklist - Revised

In response to what Hare believed to be an unacceptable

categorization of the psychopath, he returned to Cleckleys original

psychopathic characteristics (Table 4) and began to address scientific

concerns by focusing his research on the development of more reliable ways

to identify psychopathic individuals. By doing so, Hare hoped that the

psychopathic personality could be identified and characterized more

accurately, and thus minimize the risk psychopaths pose to society.

Part of the early problems with assessing and diagnosing psychopathy

was a lack of research that was replicable and able to identify the traits and

characteristics believed to be psychopathic (Lykken, 1995). Hare ushered in

a dramatic increase in psychopathy research throughout the world (Meloy,

2001, p. 198). However, Hare did more than merely redefine the concept of

psychopathy. Through his years of empirical research, Robert Hare

developed a valid and reliable instrument to assess psychopathy (Arrigo &

Shipley, 2001). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) identifies

personality traits of psychopaths based on a cluster of interpersonal and

deviant behaviors. The PCL-R is regarded in the assessment literature as the

instrument of choice for identifying psychopathy (Conoley & Impara, 1995;

Marshall & Cooke, 1999; Woodworth & Porter, 2002), and the gold standard

for operationalizing psychopathy in multiple legal and clinical contexts that

call for assessment of an individuals capacity to benefit from treatment

(Skeem, Monahan, & Mulvey, 2002). Research scientists have also praised

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
40

the PCL-R and generally regard psychopathy as defined by the PCL-R as the

only reliable and valid construct for research and because it can be

generalized to a variety of populations (Martens, 2000). All the items of the

PCL-R have proven to be both empirically reliable and valid in determining

the degree of psychopathy present in any given person (Meloy, 2001).

The PCL-R is a 20-item scale that employs a two-factor approach to

psychopathy, and identifies both behavioral and affective characteristics by

scores derived from a lengthy, structured clinical interview and a review of

collateral and case history information. The items as described in the PCL-R

rating manual are found in Table 5.

For each of the 20 items, a rating of 0 (definitely does not apply), 1

(applies somewhat or only in a limited sense), or 2 (definitely does apply) is

given based on how present the trait or behavior is in the individuals history

or current presentation as defined by the PCL-R manual (Gacono & Meloy,

1994; Hare, 2003; Hare et al., 1991; Woodworth & Porter, 2002).

According to Hare (Hare et al., 1991), the items consistently fall into

two factors. Factor 1 (aggressive narcissism), or the passive variant,

describes the more affective and interpersonal characteristics, that are more

indicative of the traditional psychopathic traits Cleckley proposed. Factor 1

includes items such as lack of remorse, grandiosity, lying, lack of empathy,

superficial charm, glibness, selfishness, and egocentricity. Generally, the

items loading on this factor are what many typically consider the core

psychopathic traits. The structured clinical interview, direct observation, and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
41

collateral information from a review of the persons file provide the

information necessary to score the items in this factor (Westendorf, 2002).

Factor 2 (chronic antisocial behavior), or the active variant, describes

the chronic and versatile antisocial lifestyle, including delinquency as an

adolescent, substance abuse, impulsivity, parasitic lifestyle, and the other

more behavioral aspects of psychopathy. The information required to score

the items loaded on this factor will likely be obtained almost entirely from

records, since there should be extensive historical documentation of these

specific behaviors.

The PCL-R derives a score that represents the extent to which a

person is judged to match the characteristics of psychopathy (Hare et al.,

1991). The score can range from 0 to 40, with a score of the 30 or higher

indicating a severe psychopath. For research purposes, the scale can be

viewed as a continuum to define a degree of disturbance: mild (10-19),

moderate (20-29), and severe (30-40) (Meloy, 2001). Using this scale,

research has linked the severity of psychopathy with treatment failure,

violence and recidivism (Meloy, 2001; Millon et al., 1998). Interestingly,

Factor 1 scores have been found to be relatively static across the lifespan,

while Factor 2 items see a decline with age and seemingly correlate with the

Antisocial Personality Disorder (Hare, 1996). Thus, the psychopath does not

burn out as the typical Antisocial Personality Disordered criminal might.

The two-factor model has generated the most research and empirical

studies using the PCL-R, and has enhanced the assessment, management,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
42

and treatment of psychopathy (Cooke & Michie, 2001). There is evidence

that Factor 2 is a robust predictor of recidivism, with both factors playing an

equally important role in predicting violent recidivism (Hemphill, Hare, &

Wong, 1998). Factor 1 has been linked with planned predatory violence,

while factor 2 is linked with impulsive violent behavior (Hart & Dempster,

1997).

The PCL-R also has predictive value and has enabled researchers to

distinguish between the behavioral patterns of psychopaths and other

criminal offenders. Individuals who obtain high scores on the PCL-R and are

classified a psychopaths will begin their criminal careers sooner than non

psychopaths, commit a wider variety of crimes, and commit offenses at a

much higher rate than non-psychopaths (Shipley, 2001). Williamson, Hare,

and Wong (1987) studied the difference in the types of victims between

psychopaths and non-psychopaths, as defined by the PCL-R, and found that

non-psychopaths were more likely to commit homicide during a domestic

dispute, while psychopaths tended to kill unknown male victims. In addition,

Westendorf (2002) reported that the motivation for the cold-blooded crimes of

the violent psychopath was usually revenge, or at other times occurred under

the influence of substances. Hobson (2000) stated that disruptive behavior

during treatment groups was surprisingly correlated to a higher Factor 1

score, not Factor 2 as might be expected, as well as the efficacy of treatment

response.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
43

Limitations of the PLC-R

The PCL-R is not, however, without its skeptics or limitations. For

example, Salekin et. al., (2001) stated that despite over 60 years of research

of on psychopathy, including PCL-R investigations, most studies have

focused primarily on forensic patients with little emphasis on the

manifestation of psychopathy in the general public. Cooke and Michie (2001)

believed that while psychopathy as defined by the PCL-R, was a good

predictor of violent criminal behavior, failure on conditional release,

recidivism, and poor treatment response, the two-factor model developed by

Hare cold not be substantiated statistically. Instead, they proposed a 3-

factor, hierarchical model in which psychopathy is subordinate to 3 factors:

Arrogant and Deceitful Interpersonal Style, Deficient Affective Experience,

and Impulsive and Irresponsible Behavioral Style.

In response to the plethora of research using the PCL-R, and

undoubtedly the criticisms of the instrument as well, Hare (2003) released the

PCL-R Second Edition, which included normative data on both male and

female offenders, substance abusers, forensic patients, and African

Americans. In addition, it introduced four facets - two within each factor -

of psychopathy: Factor 1 was broken down into Interpersonal and Affective

facets while factor 2 was divided into Lifestyle and Antisocial facets. These

additions lend themselves to a much finer characterization of the overall

score, and are better able to capture the complexity of the disorder.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
44

Despite the utility of the revised PCL-R and the research it generated

regarding the correlation of the items with various psychopathic

characteristics, the DSM-IV, like its predecessor the DSM lll-R, chose to

abandon the theoretical assumptions on which the PCL-R was based.

DSM-IV Antisocial Personality Disorder vs. Psychopathy

Mounting criticisms of the DSM-III notwithstanding, there were only

slight changes made to the Antisocial Personality Disorder made in the DSM-

IV (1994), and the essential features that defined the disorder continued to

remain focused on antisocial behavior.

Pitchford (2001) stated that DSM has blurred the distinction

between psychopaths and criminals, and of course most of the latter are not

psychopaths (P. 29). By focusing on behavioral features which were easier

to determine than motivations, the characteristic which so clearly defined the

psychopath was removed as a diagnostic criteria. Typically, a psychopathic

individuals motivation for being psychopathic may be quite different, and with

varying degrees of features of the disorder (Hare et al., 1991). But if

motivations are disregarded the difference in behaviors between psychopaths

and other criminal offenders is often difficult to distinguish.

Antisocial Personality Disorder is described in the DSM-IV as:

A pervasive pattern of disregard for, and violation of, the


rights of others that begins in childhood or early
adolescence and continues into adulthood...This pattern
has also been referred to as psychopathy, sociopathy, or
dyssocial personality disorder. Individuals...fail to
conform to social norms with respect to lawful behavior.
They may repeatedly perform acts that are grounds for
arrest...such as destroying property, harassing others,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
45

stealing...They are frequently deceitful and manipulative


in order to gain personal profit or pleasure...They may
repeatedly lie, con others, or malinger...frequently lack
empathy and tend to be callous, cynical and
contemptuous of the rights of others (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 645).

The prevalence rate for the DSM-IV Antisocial Personality Disorder are

different than the rates of psychopaths, as identified by the Hare

Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001).

In the general population, approximately 5% of adult American males

will meet the criteria (Meloy, 2001). 50% - 80% of offenders and forensic

patients are diagnosed with ASPD, but only 15% - 30% of those same

individuals met the criteria for psychopathy (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Hare et

al., 1991; Hart & Hare, 1997). Thus, many people diagnosed with Antisocial

Personality Disorder may not be psychopathic at all (Hare, 1996; Pitchford,

2001). Psychopaths are a special population among those classified with an

ASPD. They are more likely to commit violent crime when compared with the

general population (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Hare, 1993; Monahan, 1998), and

are responsible for more than 50% of all serious crimes committed (Hare,

1993).

Pitchford (2001) thought that this confusion of terminology was

particularly troublesome from a research perspective, because the DSM-IV

associates Antisocial Personality Disorder with low socio-economic status,

while psychopathy did not appear to be linked to social disadvantage. Also,

the tendency to use the term Antisocial Personality Disorder interchangeably

with psychopathy in clinical discussions confused the distinction between the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
46

two disorders and created diagnostic problems for clinical practitioners and

those social scientists who were studying the disorder.

Arrigo and Shipley (2001) noted that identifying a true psychopath by

means of their being given an Antisocial Personality Disorder diagnosis was

quite different from identifying them using the traditional criteria for being

psychopathic. This definitional ambiguity has consequences more far

reaching than merely a desire for greater precision in clinical terminology. The

consequences of incorrectly diagnosing someone as a psychopath, or

alternatively failing to identify someone who is a psychopath, could cause

serious errors when deciding the most appropriate course of treatment or the

decision to withhold certain types of treatment.

While there have been many criticisms of the DSM diagnostic criteria

for psychopathy, few researchers have suggested a cogent alternative. Hare,

Hart, and Harpur (Hare et al., 1991) recommended the development of a

separate diagnostic category, Psychopathic Personality Disorder, which was

based on the PCL-R criteria. Meloy (1996) recommended the inclusion of

mild, moderate, or severe psychopathy and the PCL-R score after the ASPD

diagnosis on Axis II as a way to provide better diagnostic clarification. Yet,

the most comprehensive and plausible recommendation was made by Arrigo

and Shipley (2001):

W e maintain that psychopathy should be included as a


separate personality disorder, along with specific guidelines
on how it can be assessed. Furthermore, we suggest that
ASPD function on a continuum of mild, moderate, and
severe. Mild ASPD would reflect the current understanding
of ASPD, primarily with a behavioral orientation. It could be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
47

assessed by clinical interviewing and a criminal history


alone. However, if the PCL-R were given, it would indicate
a score of no more than 15. Moderate ASPD would
suggest a cluster of symptoms, including both personality
traits and behavioral symptoms as presented in both factors
1 and 2 of the PCL-R. The PCL-R score should range
between 16 and 29. Finally, a diagnosis of severe ASPD or
psychopathy would be obtained if an individual had a PCL-
R score of 30 or higher, and met the behavioral criteria
necessary for ASPD. This designation would be the most
cogent for evaluating the risk of violence to other offenders
and staff, likelihood of recidivism, and prognosis of
treatment (p. 411).

It is not clear what ramifications the current research and writings will

have on the next version of the DSM, however, support for revision of the

diagnosis becomes more widespread and is better substantiated by research

with each passing year. Despite the abundance of research, however, most

clinicians continue to define, assess, and treat psychopathy according to the

DSM-IV cookbook approach, and remain confused about what psychopathy

is and how it differs from Antisocial Personality Disorder (Arrigo & Shipley,

2001; Hare, 1996).

Review of the Historical Changes to the Psychopathy Construct

This review has provided a brief overview the development of the

construct of psychopathy from its first inception to its contemporary

interpretation. Over time the construct has undergone many changes as

societal norms, morality, religion, and research have separately and conjointly

helped to shape the evolution of psychopathy. According to Arrigo and

Shipley (2001), only two features of the psychopathy construct have

remained static over the years. First, the psychopath is not psychotic and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
48

has intact reality testing. Second, sadly, the psychopath has always been

considered untreatable.

Psychoanalytic theory, possibly more than any other, has consistently

produced plausible hypotheses about the etiology of the psychopathic

disorder. The next chapter will discuss the more prominent analytic writings

on the psychopathic disorder.

Leaff (1978) considered the psychodynamics of the psychopath to be

an area in which there have been varied and differing opinions as to the

etiology, in which sociology, criminology, and psychiatry interface, and in

which the role of hereditary and organic factors must be considered (p. 79).

This occurs because of a number of factors. The differences of opinion

concerning the etiology of this disorder stem not only from the different

theoretical orientations of the clinicians who have offered causative

explanations, but also from the complexity of the illness itself. The disorder

has a large number of symptoms and behavioral manifestations which do not

fall into easily related groupings and manifest at quite different levels of

intensity depending on the severity of the pathology. This variety has caused

considerable difficulty throughout the history of the development of the

psychopathy construct. The complexity of defining this illness leads even

clinicians with the same theoretical orientation to different conclusions

regarding its etiology depend on the symptoms of behavioral manifestations

that they consider most significant. In the next chapter, I will review

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
49

alternative psychoanalytic theories that have attempted to explain the origins

of the severe personality disorder known as psychopathy.

Table 5

Hare Psychopathy Checklist-R Factor Items and Descriptions


Item_______________________________ Description________

Factor 1: Interpersonal/Affective

1. Glibness/Superficial Charm A glib and facile individual who exudes


an insincere and superficial sort of
charm. Often an amusing and
entertaining conversationalist, is always
ready with a quick and clever
comeback, and is able to present
himself well.

2. Grandiose senses of self-worth An individual with a grossly inflated view


of his abilities and self-worth. Often
appears self-assured, opinionated, and
cocky.

4. Pathological Lying For this person, lying and deceit are a


characteristic part of his interactions
with others. He often lies for obvious
reasons, but deceiving others also has
some value.

5. Conning/Manipulative This item is more focused on the use of


deceit and deception to cheat, defraud,
or manipulate others. They use
schemes and scams for personal gain,
with disregard for their effects on
victims.

6. Lack of remorse or guilt No concern about what affect his


actions might have had on others. In
fact, may even be so bold as to outright
state that he/she has no guilt about
what they have done, and may blame
the victim for what has happened to
them.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
50

Table 5 (continued).

Characteristic Description

7. Shallow affect There is not the normal range of affect


that individuals usually display. Appears
as unemotional, or the emotions are
incongruent with actions.

8. Callous/Lack of empathy They totally disregard the rights of


others, and selfishly manipulate people
to get what they want.

9. Failure to accept responsibility Does not accept responsibility for what


They for their own actions do, and
rationalize behavior away. They may
blame the victim for their plight, but
always have an excuse for their deeds

Factor 2: Antisocial Behavior

3. Need for stimulation/ An individual that has a chronic and


proneness to boredom and excessive need for novel and
exciting stimulation, and in doing things
that are exciting, risky, or challenging.

9. Parasitic Lifestyle Avoids employment, and get what they


want from family, friends, or government
assistance. This stems from a belief that
they are supposed to have their needs
meet, no matter what.

10. Poor behavioral controls Often short-tempered, is unable to


tolerate frustration and may become
violent. Outbursts are usually short
lived, and out of proportion to the
context.

12. Early behavior problems Before the age of 12, these individuals
have already been caught lying,
stealing, setting fires, fighting, and
perhaps sexual activity.

13. Lack of realistic, long-term goals These individuals have no long-term


goals. They live day-to-day and dont
worry about the future very much.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
51

Table 5 (continued).

Characteristic Description

14. Impulsivity They do not think about what they are


doing, and do things because they feel
like it. They do not consider the
consequences of their actions.

15. Irresponsibility Habitually fails to follow through on


word, has no loyalty to friends or family,
defaults on loans and credit obligations
and exhibits behavior that may cause
risk to others (i.e. drunk driving).

16. Juvenile Delinquency History of antisocial behavior before the


age of 18, including criminal convictions.

19. Revocation of conditional release A violation of conditional release,


incurring new charges while on
conditional release, or an escape from
an institution.

Additional Items that do not load on either factor:

11. Promiscuous sexual behavior Engages in casual, impersonal sexual


encounters, with many of these types of
relationships at the same time. In fact,
may force themselves on victims and
have a history of sexual offenses.

17. Many short-term marital relationships These individuals have many marital or
live-in relationships that have some kind
of commitment.

20. Criminal versatility Charges or convictions for a variety of


criminal offenses such as theft, fraud,
drug offenses, sexual offenses,
kidnapping, etc.

a/_!_ H i
TU-. i i_n*.ii__ _i_i: /nad ed.), by R.
1
. n . JII J \
Note. From The Hare Psychopathy Checklist - Revised, (2n
Hare, 2003. Ontario: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
52

Chapter IV

Psychoanalytic Theories about Psychopathy

Among adult criminals, we must no doubt accept those


who commit crimes without any sense of guilt, who have
either developed no moral inhibitions or who, in their
conflict with society, consider themselves justified in
their actions (S. Freud, 1916, p. 333).

Beyond this brief statement, Sigmund Freud did not specifically

address the development of the diagnostic category now called psychopathy.

Generally, he described criminal behavior as the result of unconscious

Oedipal guilt which was relieved by projecting the guilt onto something real

and immediate, with the secondary benefit of serving as a mechanism for

punishing the self (Yochelson & Samenow, 1976). Freud (1916) further

wrote:

Analytic work then afforded the surprising conclusion that


such deeds are done precisely because they are
forbidden, and because by carrying them out the doer
enjoys a sense of mental relief. He suffered an
oppressive feeling of guilt, of which he did not know the
origin, and after he had committed a misdeed the
oppression was mitigated (p. 342)

As psychoanalysis began to coalesce in the United States during the

1920s, other theorists began to describe and publish writings from an

analytic orientation about their various experiences working with psychopathic

patients (Millon & Davis, 1998). What distinguishes these writings from those

discussed in Chapter II, is their attempt to not merely identify those with the

disorder and describe its characteristics, but instead to go beyond this in

order to understand psychopathy based on psychodynamic, biological,

developmental, the structural dynamic considerations. By providing an

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
53

account of the origins of psychopathy, it is hoped that insight could be gained

in the prevention and amelioration of the disorder. Futhermore,

psychoanalytic theory allows the complexity of the distorted mind of the

psychopath to be understood more fully, as there is a distinct focus the

developmental sequellae that are rooted in childhood experiences. Other

theories, such as cognitive behavioral theory, which is far too behaviorally

focused, lack the depth in which the psychopathic disorder must reach (i.e.

the unconscious, disturbed attachments, and defense mechanisms).

Early studies of the psychopathic personality centered on ego

deficiencies. Inadequate superego formation (the conscience) was seen as

the primary cause of psychopathy. Wilhelm Reich (1925/1972) believed that

the superegos functions were somehow unavailable because the ego was

over controlling, thus neither the ego nor superego could restrain powerful

impulses from the id. Door and Woodhall (1986) supported Reichs position

and believed that the earlier analytic understanding of psychopathy as a

combination of too much id combined with too little superego was superficial.

Instead, they agreed with Reich that psychopathy was a manifestation of

severe ego deficits. Blatt and Shichman (1981). They argued that the cause

of psychopathy was not an expression of superego deficit, but rather an

impairment of the development of basic ego functions. These analysts

believed that psychopaths cannot observe superego functioning (or their

conscience) because of their deficits in their ego functioning (Beliak,

Chassan, Gediman, & Hurvich, 1973).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
54

The belief that the ego and not the superego was the cause of

psychopathy did not go unopposed, however. Other psychoanalytic writers

theorized that psychopathy as developed from a failure on the part of the

individual to adequately introject parental and societal norms, which resulted

in inadequate superego formation (Smith, 1978a). Otto Fenichel (1945)

focused on the deficits of the superego and, in particular, how it was unable

to control id impulses, while Friedlander (1945) thought that psychopathic

expression in youngsters was under the dominance of the pleasure principle,

unguided by an adequately developed superego (Millon et al., 1998, p. 18).

According to Rabin (1979), one of the major criteria for the diagnosis

of psychopathy is a deficit in morality which could be the result of an

inadequate superego and an insufficiently developed acquired conscience.

Specifically, the psychopath has an inability to apply the moral standards of

society to his behavior, thus he cheats, lies, and steals because he has not

incorporated the rules of society and culture. The absence of guilt and

remorse, which are normal functions in a well-developed conscience, permits

the psychopath to continue his behavior indefinitely without experiencing

painful feelings.

Do psychopaths have any superego function at all? Dalmau (1961) did

not believe that psychopaths completely lacked a superego. He insisted that

psychopaths do show some evidence of possessing a superego, although it

would seem as though the ego and the id were joining forces to overpower

the superego despite the threat of punishment. Dalmau hypothesized that

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
55

parental and societal introjections in the psychopath actually did occur but

were not fully integrated in their personalities. Instead, he thought these

introjects became dissociated or repressed and, through the psychopaths

intrapsychic processes, were then projected in a symbolic attack against the

parents and society via his behavior:

Whether the mechanisms of projection, denial, undoing,


dissociation and reaction formation are all
responsible...is not as relevant as the fact that the
superego is functionally detached from the personality
and projected onto society...The core of the superego is
formed by the parents and what the child believes the
parents expect. Later on society extends the role of the
parents. The functional elimination of the superego then
becomes equated with a symbolic murder of father and
mother; the persistent attack against society also carries
the same symbolism beyond the intrapsychic structure.
(p. 445)

Thus, it was not the absence of a superego, but its continued

presence that was being continually attacked by the psychopath and this

resulted in anti-social behavior which was, at root, a continuing rebellion

against early parental influences. Dalmau (1961) frequently observed that

psychopaths displayed a pattern of getting into trouble due to their criminal

behavior, which often resulted in the loss of their freedom either by

incarceration or hospitalization. He believed this was symbolic behavior that

was representative of guilt feelings:

The psychopath in his symbolic behavior follows a


regressive pattern, from a raping genital sadistic attack
against society to the anal homosexual need to feel
screwed and then back to the oral dependent position
in a hospital, jail, or conditional custody. They use
reality as a vehicle for symbolic behavior, and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
56

transference conflicts are acted out in stage-like


representation (p. 446).

In other words, the psychopath was never able and ultimately did not wish to

completely vanquish the superego, but instead always wished to ultimately be

caught and punished to appease superego guilt.

Many authors have recognized the critical role of ego functions, and

subsequent superego development, but Johnson and Szurek (1952)

particularly stressed the deficits in these structures in their frequently cited

studies of antisocial adolescents. These authors observed not a generalized

weakness of the entire superego, but rather a lack of superego functions in

particular areas of behavior, which they described as superego lacunae.

Johnson and Szurek (1952) observed that the parents of these adolescents

transmitted their own poorly integrated id impulses to their children via

conscious or unconscious permissiveness or inconsistent parenting. Thus,

the childs superego lacunae developed in ways similar to the deficits in their

parents superegos. These parents superego lacunae had, in turn, been

derived from the unconscious transmissions of their own parents in an

intergenerational cascade of pathology. Leaff (1978) summarized this

phenomenon by concluding that, The concept of the superego lacunae

developed by Johnson and Szurek states that the identified patient is acting

out the unconscious impulse of the parent or parents. The neurotic needs of

the parent are met vicariously through the childs acting out. The parents

displace any self-punishment to the antisocial child through expression of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
57

hostility, and the family is maintained through the scapegoating of the sick

or evil members (p. 95).

Developmental and Object Relations Pathology

Aichorn (1925/1935) studied and delinquency and published

interpretations of the origins of delinquent behaviors from a psychoanalytic

viewpoint. In particular, he stressed the importance of oedipal identifications

and their role in the development of the ego ideal.

Aichorn described conscious and unconscious


struggles which raged within a youth when his efforts at
oedipal rebellion and resolution, as well as his efforts to
hold his father as a love object, failed. Acting out
behaviors were seen as symbolic equivalents.
Delinquency was explained partly by the unconquered
pleasure principle, and also by taking into consideration
the libidinal relationships which had not been dealt with
adequately in earlier childhood. He felt that when a
child was too strictly treated or when enough bad
experiences with reality had occurred too early, he was
unable at that stage of development to make the
necessary adjustments. Regression then occurred and
the pleasure principle achieved mastery (Leaff, 1978, p.
83).

Aichorn also noted that it was unlikely for a person to be born a psychopath.

He identified external factors, such as abusive parents, inconsistent parenting

styles, or separated parents that made adequate superego formation difficult

or impossible to achieve.

Abraham (1925/1935) also referred to several environmental

conditions that could greatly impact the development of antisocial traits in

children, especially those who are deprived of early nurturing or who were

frustrated in their developmental^ appropriate demands for love:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
58

Among delinquents (antisocial) we are more likely to


come across a different fate of the libido in early
childhood. It is the absence of love, comparable to
psychological undernourishment, which provides the pre
condition for the establishment of dissocial traits. An
excess of hatred and fury is generated which, first
directed against a small circle of persons, is later directed
against society as a whole (p. 304).

The Traumatic Origin of Psychopathic Impulsivity

A cornerstone trait of psychopathy with roots in ego and superego

development is the manifestation of violent impulsive and aggressive

psychopathic behavior. Shapiro (1965) described impulsive styles, which

Millon (1998) believed could be correlated with the current conceptualization

of Antisocial Personality Disorder. Shapiro wrote:

In many respects, the psychopath is the very model of the


impulsive style. He exhibits in a thorough and pervasive
way what for others is only a direction or tendency. He
acts on a whim, his aim is the quick, concrete gain, and
his interests and talents are in ways and means (Shapiro,
1965, p. 157).

Shapiros description of the impulsiveness of psychopaths was echoed by

Stone (1993) who described his observations of this quality in psychopaths in

the following manner:

Several attributes, personality traits among them, are


noted with unusual frequency in persons who murder.
Rebelliousness and aggressivity are common, as are
mendacity, entitlement, and social isolation.
Murderers are typically beset by surpluses of hatred
and impulsivity. These attributes, especially when
fueled by alcohol, conduce to ragefulness,
characterized by episodic outbursts of violent
behavior directed against others (p. 454).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
59

Eissler (1949) explained the development of this malevolent impulsivity

as originating from traumatic experiences perpetrated by a childs parents.

The traumatized child then identified with these abusive parents and this

identification served as the model for an ego ideal that the child would later

attempt to emulate. Trauma during childhood development interferes with the

process of identification by heightening awareness of the aggressive aspects

of the parents behavior so that the other facets of the parents are thrown into

shadow. The power of the traumatizing experience fixates the child on the

task of emulating the abusive parent in his relationships with others. Thus,

the traumatized ego refuses to accept certain behavioral norms, and permits

the expression of id impulses without interference or punishment by the

conscience.

Early Trauma and Its Consequences

What is traumatic for a child depends both on the individual child and

he childs age. Younger children are more susceptible to trauma than older

ones who have developed better skills at communication and self-regulation.

Toddlers and infants are the most sensitive of all. Anna Freud (1949)

stressed the crucial impact of the first year of life and, in particular, the

transition from primary narcissism to object love. In cases where the mother

was absent, neglectful, emotionally unstable, or ambivalent and was unable

to provide reliable emotional support and nurturance, the shift from the

narcissistic libido to object libido resulted in improper or deficient identification

and thus, the aggressive impulses were inadequately bound.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
60

Schmideberg (1949) reported clinical observations that suggested the

development of psychopathy was due, in large part, to disturbed object

relations, rather than superego deficit. She described psychopaths as

ambivalent and unstable, and suggested that the defense mechanism of

depersonalization was in important aspect of criminality. Further,

Schmideberg emphasized the role of anxiety in the antisocial activity,

suggesting that adaptation and ego functions were damaged during the

psychopaths childhood development, as were object relationships, as

evidenced by the inability to attach to caregivers.

Once these early developmental missteps had occurred, the pathology

became quickly became firmly entrenched. Dalmau (1961) argued that a

rejecting mother or overly strict father merely reinforce a pre-existing

psychopathy, and that even a psychopath brought up in a nurturing

environment would still be a psychopath because of his impaired intrapsychic

processes. The rejecting attitude of psychopaths parents is likely a

countertransference reaction to the psychopathic child:

The emotional damage in the psychopath may occur


anywhere form birth to Oedipus. It appears that the
true psychopath forms at a very early stage, which
may establish the basic destructive rage pattern
through the diffusion of the life and death instincts.
Any serious disruptive experience occurring at a
prelogical, preverbal level sets the stage for a conflict
of psychotic proportions. The child feels distress and
experiences this as an external attack that gives basis
to the paranoidal ideation and a punitive destructive
superego. The psychopath is afraid of positive
experiences; the fear of loss forces him to deny them
and to reinforce the paranoid position through defiant
provocation, hence the rejecting parents. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
61

psychopath uses parental punishment as justification


for further rage and reinforces the paranoid position
against the parents and later against all authority,
including school, society, and the law. It is invariably
the distortion of a reality situation that gives birth to
the basic feelings of rejection and rage, which then
becomes acted out and set the psychopathic pattern.
Punishment only reinforces the paranoid ideation and
nurses the hatred. The rejection by the parents is
usually well deserved on the basis of this behavior
and it should not be misinterpreted in a causal
relationship to psychopathy (p. 450).

Bender (1947) believed the psychopaths behavior was infantile and,

more specifically, pre-oedipal, pre-superego, and usually pre-narcissistic

citing evidence found in fledgling psychopaths:

There is an imitative, passive as i f quality to the


behavior of older children and adolescents. This is
because there is an inner drive to behave like a
human being. Whereas behavior in the normal child
arises from internal mechanisms, such as identification
process, object relationships, anxieties and symbolic
fantasy life, the psychopathic child has no such inner
life...It, therefore, copies the behavior of other
children, according to its maturation level and ability
(p. 75)

Similarly, Coriat (1927) described what he termed the constitutional

psychopath, who he believed was an antisocial person fixated at infantile

levels, had unresolved Oedipal conflicts, and has never learned to replace

the ego ideals of childhood with the ego ideals of society (quoted in Millon,

et. al 1988, p. 15).

The Role of Childhood Experiences on the Development of Psychopathy

Over the course of analytic investigation into the mind of the violent

psychopath, environmental factors, rather than intrapsychic deficits,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
62

contributing to the development of the disorder began to receive increasing

attention. The theory that psychopathy resulted primarily from external,

environmental factors was not without controversy. Theorists who opposed

this suggested there may not be any significant environmental factors that

contribute to psychopathic personality development at all (Marshall & Cooke,

1999). Both Cleckley (1988) and Hare (1993) discounted the significance of

environmental influences by arguing that there is little evidence to support the

claim that psychopathy is the result of any social or environmental factors

(Marshall & Cooke, 1999).

The importance of environmental influences was a matter that could

only be ultimately determined by empirical evidence. As the study of

psychopathy began to be scrutinized more closely, writers such as Meloy

(1988; 2001; 2002), Lykken (1995), McCord and McCord (1956), and others

soon hypothesized that, in fact, there was an inevitable contribution of

developmental sequellae that could be present in the young psychopaths

environment (Table 6). Patterson, DeBaryshe, and Ramsey (1989)

specifically presented evidence of a developmental model of psychopathy in

which a sequence of environmental experiences, beginning early in life and

continuing into adulthood, form the foundation of psychopathy.

Leaff (1978) opined that as the child witnesses continual arguing or

domestic violence between parents, the childs own aggressive impulses

cannot be fully integrated into their emotional life and will likely be acted out

in school:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
63

The home environment may present not only continual


battles between the parents but opportunity for sexual
stimulation from the parents and/or observation of
intercourse. The unmitigated aggression, already
difficult to deal with, leads to the conception of
intercourse as a violently aggressive, sadistic act which
is continued until one or both of the partners are
severely damaged...such children may be belligerent,
troublesome, always ready to fight. Their hate
relationships are most important to them, as they
represent love relationships; the hated enemy is their
unconscious representative of the sexual partner. They
may...continue to find direct expression in
characterologic and aggressive behaviors which
represent condensations of masochistic, sadistic,
scopophilic or exhibitionistic fantasies and/or fantasies
of aggressive intercourse (p. 88).

Table 6

Childhood experiences of psychopaths____________________


Author Findings

Lykken (1995) Interaction between biological


predisposition and parental
inadequacy. Inadequate or
incompetent parenting leads to
insecure attachment bonding.

Silverman (1943) Disturbed parent-child relationship is a


causal factor in psychopathy

McCord and McCord (1956) Parental conflict, neglect, and erratic


punishment are linked with subsequent
psychopathy

Cloniger, Reich, and Guze (1975) Marital separation and parental


deprivation often found in the
childhood of psychopaths

Pfiffner, L., McBurnett, K., and Families with fathers at home had
Rathouz, P. (2001) fewer paternal, maternal, and child
antisocial symptoms; antisocial
behavior more likely if the father is
absent or nonparticipating

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
64

Note: Taken from The Childhood Experiences of Psychopaths: A Retrospective


Study of Familial and Societal Factors by L. Marshall and D. Cooke, 1999,
International Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(3), 211 -225.

Leaff (1978) concluded that as the child witnesses continual arguing or

domestic violence between parents, the childs own aggressive impulses

cannot be fully integrated into their emotional life and will likely be acted out

in school:

The home environment may present not only continual


battles between the parents but opportunity for sexual
stimulation from the parents and/or observation of
intercourse. The unmitigated aggression, already difficult to
deal with, leads to the conception of intercourse as a
violently aggressive, sadistic act which is continued until
one or both of the partners are severely damaged...such
children may be belligerent, troublesome, always ready to
fight. Their hate relationships are most important to them,
as they represent love relationships; the hated enemy is
their unconscious representative of the sexual partner.
They may...continue to find direct expression in
characterologic and aggressive behaviors which represent
condensations of masochistic, sadistic, scopophilic or
exhibitionistic fantasies and/or fantasies of aggressive
intercourse (p. 88).

Marshall and Cooke (1999) compared childhood experiences of both

psychopaths and non-psychopaths, and found two factors to be highly

correlated with higher psychopathy scores on the PCL-R as described below:

The societal influences factor has a linear relationship,


suggesting that as the PCL-R score increases so does
the influence of societal experiences. In contrast, the
familial factor has a negative curvilinear relationship,
indicating that as the score on the PCL-R increased the
effect of the childhood variable decreases. This
suggests that low to moderate scores on the PCL-R are
strongly related to experiences within the family, while
high scores are less influenced by this factor. In other
words, the negative curvilinear effect suggests that the
biological influence may be stronger at the upper end of
the trait (p. 221).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
65

These familial factors included parental discipline, antipathy, discord, neglect,

and lack of supervision; physical, sexual, or emotional abuse; scapegoating;

negative school experience, performance; or negative social experience as

likely variables contributing to the childhood experience (p. 218).

Lykken (1995) suggested the interaction between biological factors,

(such as those described in the next chapter), and inadequate parenting may

lead to the development of psychopathy. He reasoned that inadequate or

incompetent parenting leads to insecure attachment bonding that forecasts

low levels of empathy, compliance, cooperation and self-control (p. 199).

From an analytic perspective, McWilliams (1994) described the

backgrounds of psychopaths as often rife with insecurity and chaos and

wrote:

One can find virtually no consistent, loving, adequately


protective family influences. Weak, depressed, or
masochistic mothers and explosive, inconsistent, or
sadistic fathers have been linked with psychopathy, as
have alcoholism and other substance abuse in the family
origin. Patterns of moves, losses, and family breakups
are also common (p. 156).

Patterson et al. (1989) agreed that family disruption could be an

important causal factor that promoted the development of psychopathic

behaviors. They initially studied antisocial behavior in juveniles and

hypothesized that traits associated with this disorder begin early in life and

continue into adulthood. Their research described how disrupted family

processes might produce antisocial behavior in a childs early years, which

later contributed to the childs becoming involved with a deviant peer group.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
66

Further, the study found evidence suggesting there are specific phenomena,

rooted in the home and social environment of the budding psychopath, that

influence the development of psychopathic personality in adolescence and

into adulthood. The factors that Patterson et al. (1989) suggested, are: (a)

basic training in the home, (b) social rejection and school failure, and (c)

deviant peer group membership. These factors, whose influence was

confirmed by other researchers, are described below.

Basic training in the home. Several studies have identified family

dynamics as a contributing external factor to early antisocial behavior. Both

Marshall and Cooke (1999) and Patterson (1989) found that families with

children who were considered antisocial could be described as having harsh

and inconsistent discipline, little parental involvement, and inadequate

monitoring and supervision of the childs activities. Additional factors were

also described by, Frodi et al., (2001) who emphasized the importance of

coercive parent-child interactions, a damaged or absent parent-child bond,

inconsistent parenting, neglect, and severe punishments as likely causes of

the development of psychopathy. Not surprisingly, Frodi also discovered that

higher scores on the PCL-R seemed to associate highly with a greater

number of convictions, more violent crime, and a higher likelihood of

childhood abuse.

Social rejection and school failure. Patterson et. al. (1989) believed

the childs behavior is likely to elicit reactions from his/her environment, which

may manifest as rejection by the childs peer group or by performing poorly in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
67

academic endeavors. This was later confirmed by Marshall and Cooke

(1999) who studied both adult psychopathic and non-psychopathic individuals

and found that poor school experience in childhood was associated with

psychopathy as an adult.

Deviant peer group membership. Patterson (1989) found that poor

parental supervision and peer group rejection were correlated positively with

the childs identification with a deviant peer group, which ultimately led to

delinquent acts and substance use. The deviant peer group then supports the

antisocial behavior. In essence, this can be thought of as a positive feedback

loop that maintains and amplifies the ongoing negative process.

A Psychodynamic Understanding of Childhood Influences on Psychopaths

While an identification of the external factors contributing to the

development of psychopathy are is important, in order to appreciate why

these factors are so influential requires an understanding of the

psychodynamics and internal world of the psychopath.

The importance of early parent-child interaction and bonding has been

examined extensively by psychoanalytic theory and empirical investigations.

Marshall and Cooke (1999) credit Bowlby as being the first psychiatrist to

suggest that early separations from attachment figures, or absence of

attachment figures, would predispose a person toward the development of

emotional coldness, or the affectionless character (p. 270), which resembles

could be a precursor to psychopathy.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
68

Bowlby (1944) attributed psychopathy, or what he termed the

Affectionless Character, to prolonged separation of a child from his mother.

Such a childs ability to sustain object-relationships is impaired and that child

has very limited control of his or her libidinal and aggressive impulses.

Bowlby observed that:

A child separated from his mother comes to crave both


for her love and for its accompanying symbols and this
craving, if unsatisfied, later presents itself as
stealing...the part played by aggression must not be
forgotten, for stealing not only enriches oneself, but
impoverishes and hurts others...If one has suffered great
deprivation oneself, one will feel inclined to inflict equal
suffering on someone else (p. 36).

The analytic understanding of psychopathy also offers an explanation

of how early attachment experiences influence how a psychopath manages

anxiety. In normal development, anxiety manifests in interpersonal

relationships to ensure the safety of a child when a stranger is introduced, or

during an actual or perceived separation of the maternal object (John Bowlby,

1988). For psychopaths, however, this anxiety is minimal or completely

absent (Meloy, 1988). Is this universally true?

The lack of anxiety appears in psychopaths appears to be a matter of

degree with a lower levels of anxiety corresponding to more serious forms of

the illness. Empirical research (Blackburn, 1998; Lykken, 1957; Ogloff et al.,

1990) has found that the presence of anxiety differentiates secondary

(anxious) psychopaths from primary (non-anxious) psychopaths, and is

considered a factor in children at risk for becoming psychopaths. Lynam

(1996) coined the term fledgling psychopath to describe the biological deficit

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
69

of the adolescent with features of callousness, lack of emotions, and the

minimal anxiety previously mentioned.

The lack of early attachment to an adequate primary caregiver

causes in the best cases an unbalanced, distorted, or conflicted ego ideal to

which the child relates only with extreme difficulty. Frodi et al., (2001) studied

24 psychopathic criminal offenders assessed with the PCL-R and found an

extensive over-representation of individuals who were dismissing of

attachment and attachment-related experiences, no secure individuals, and

an association between a higher psychopathy score and a family

constellation of a rejecting father and an emotionally very warm (idealized)

mother (p. 269).

In the worst cases, the child is left with a core of resentment that is

defended against by a reaction formation denying the value of the attachment

figure and hence temporarily reducing anxiety. Brody and Rosenfeld (2002)

described the development of psychopathy in terms of object relational

deficits, which resulted in the harboring of profound underlying psychosocial

damage including intense anxiety, anger, bitterness, and resentment (p.

408).

Bender (1947) argued that the main deficiency of the psychopath is an

inability to form relationships, identify empathically, and share in the

experiences of others. In her view, the developmental process in

psychopaths appears to be fixated at an early age (p.100). Benjamin (1993)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
70

characterized the interpersonal aspects of the antisocial personality

disordered individuals noting that:

There is a pattern of inappropriate and unmodulated


desire to control others, implemented in a detached
manner. There is a strong need to be independent, to
resist being controlled by others, who are usually held in
contempt. There is a willingness to use untamed
aggression to back up the need for control or
independence. The antisocial personality usually
presents in a friendly, sociable manner, but that
friendliness is always accompanied by a baseline position
of detachment. He or she doesnt care what happens to
self or others (p. 203).

Rabin (1979) confirmed this analysis. In his work, he identified certain

characteristics he believed to be present in the psychopaths object relational

life such as emotional immaturity, egocentricity, and impulsivity, and a

stunted capacity for love and emotional involvement.

Emotional immaturity, egocentricity, and Impulsivity. According to

Rabin, the young infants are only concerned with themselves and their own

wants and needs. They are egocentric, placing themselves in the center of

their own world. Their primary goal is the satisfaction and gratification of their

desires. Should the caretaker fail to satiate these immediate wishes, the

infant may become angry and lash out at the caretaker. As children mature

they become socialized and learn to delay gratification, to consider other

feelings, and to obey societal rules. In other words, they become socio-

centric.

The psychopath grows up physically and is often well


developed and attractive in appearance. He/she also
matures as far as formal intellectual functions are
concerned, and acquires many of the perceptual, motor,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
71

and cognitive skills necessary for the manipulation of the


environment. However, (s)he remains immature
emotionally and continues to be egocentric and impulsive,
being solely concerned with the immediate fulfillment of his
needs. As they can tolerate no delay or postponement and
cannot tolerate frustration when thwarted by the
environment, they remain rather childish in this respect.
As a rule, he/she proceeds to get what he wants when he
wants it, without consideration of others (Rabin, 1979, p.
327).

This characterization is similar to Shapiros (1965) description of what

he called the impulsive style. The distinctive quality of this subjective

experience revolves around an impairment of normal feelings of

deliberateness and intention. It is manifested in the nature of the experience

for these people of impulse or irresistible impulse, and of the significance of

whim in their mental lives (p 134). The psychopath does not learn or profit

from past experience because the capacity for self-control has never been

developed and he cannot inhibit his own behavior. Immediate and infantile

needs remain strong while control remains weak.

Stunted capacity for love and emotional Involvement. Rabin (1979)

referred to the affective life of the psychopath as an emotional poverty,

because of the lack of insight into the emotional state of others (and thus

ultimately themselves). Lacking an awareness of others feelings, the

psychopaths attention is directed inward rather than outward. Accordingly,

the psychopath suffers a genuine lack of empathy, and other people are

important only to the extent that they can be of use to the psychopaths own

self-gratifying ends. Generally, the psychopath is callous and insensitive other

people, and thus has no genuine intimate relationship aside from sexual

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
72

involvement with promiscuity as the rule and physical discharges that is very

detached from his partners.

Rabin ultimately concurred with his predecessors regarding the role of

the identification process in early personality development. According to

Rabin, the child begins this process by imitating the behavior of his parents

and by adhering to their demands which Rabin described as modeling

behavior. To the child, his parents are all-powerful. The child attempts to act

and become like them because this makes the child feel secure. The

behavioral expectations the parents try to instill in their child are usually the

standards of the community, society, and their culture. Traditionally, the child

internalizes these parental standards in order to avoid punishment and gain

the parents love and affection. As these standards become part of the childs

personality, the child learns to identify with society and humankind.

The unconscious, defensive operations, and affect. Franz Alexander

(1930b) was the first influential analyst to spend a great deal of time studying

and extensively writing about antisocial behavior. He developed the first

description of criminal behavior from a purely psychoanalytic perspective.

His publications, Psychoanalysis of the Total Personality (1930b) and The

Neurotic Character (1930a) described four levels of pathology: neurosis,

neurotic character, psychosis, and true criminality (Millon et al., 1998).

Alexander believed that one of these types, the neurotic character, was

equivalent to the psychopath because both tended to manifest their

unconscious conflicts behaviorally rather than trying to understand or

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
73

overcome them. Describing these types of character structures Alexander

stated:

Their conduct arises from unconscious motives which are


not directly accessible to their conscious
personality...Admonition, encouragement or punishment
coming from the environment is as useless as his own
resolution...A large proportion of such individuals, are
neurotically driven by unconscious motives, now to commit
a transgression, then to seek punishment, sooner or later
fall foul of the law...(quoted in Millon, et. al., 1988, p. 16).

In his later work, Alexander (1935) became aware of the effects that

the interplay between the environment, genetic predisposition, and

intrapsychic forces had on the development of psychopathy and set the

groundwork:

The emotional conflicts and deprivations of childhood, the


resentments of parents and siblings, find a powerful ally in
resentment against the social situation, and this combined
emotional tension seeks a realistic expression in criminal
acts and cannot be relieved by mere fantasy products that
are exhibited in neurotic symptoms...criminality in some
cases is a direct expression of protest against certain
depravations, a reaction of spite against certain members
of the family, the expression of jealously, envy, hostile
competition, all of which are strengthened by early
sufferings or the lack of love and support on the part of
adults...intense hostilities in such cases frequently create
strong guilt feelings, which in turn lead to an unconscious
need for punishment (p. 278).

In a reaction to anger at early or abusive caregivers, the psychopath is thus

torn between an aggressive emulation of them and unconscious feelings of

guilt which can manifest in a need to be caught and punished. If this is the

primary conflict of a psychopath, how is it defended against? Analytic authors

have offered several suggestions.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
74

Dalmau (1961) argued that the defense of projection predominated in

the psychopath, and that the instinctual behavior of the psychopath is quite

possibly a reaction formation against a massive, repressive superego. It is

the threat of this overwhelming superego that forces the psychopath to deny

its existence and project it symbolically onto society but even then he is not

free from it.

The so-called true psychopath shows little or no


manifestation of anxiety, guilt, remorse, or other superego
feelings. If there is a breakdown in ego defenses, it is
usually a brief psychotic episode with rapid reintegration of
defenses when deprived by hospitalization or jail of its
basic opportunity for acting-out operations...the
pathological defenses are so sturdily constructed that they
present the invulnerable, unassailable ego structure (p.
443).

McWilliams (1994), on the other hand, believed the primary defensive

operations in psychopaths were omnipotent control, projective identification,

acting out, and subtle dissociative processes. Of omnipotent control

McWilliams wrote, The need to exert power takes precedence over all other

aims. It defends against shame and, especially in brutal psychopaths,

distracts others from seeing the sexual perversions that often underlie

criminality (p. 153).

One way the defense of omnipotent control manifests in psychopaths

is via their flagrant bragging about their crimes and conquests, which is an

attempt to impress others with their abilities and power. Kernberg (1984)

referred to this as malignant grandiosity. Projective identification in the

psychopath may reflect a type of developmental arrest, and an inability to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
75

articulate his own thoughts and feelings thus forcing others to feel what he

himself cannot. Gacono and Meloy (1994) found similar defensive operations

such as devaluation, splitting, projective identification, and denial, and

Kernberg (1975) viewed these type of pre-oedipal defenses as part of the

borderline personality organization as well. The next to the last of the

defenses McWilliams identified, acting out, is virtually definitional of

psychopathy...they do feel anxiety, but act so fast to relieve themselves of

such a toxic feeling that the observer has no chance to see it (McWilliams,

1994, p. 155).

McWilliams (1994) also suggested that there is some type of

dissociative phenomenon that occurs in psychopaths which includes

minimizing mistakes or, in severe cases, amnesia for a particular crime. The

extent to which this occurs remains unclear however since psychopaths may

deny memory of an incident in order to manipulatively shirk responsibility for

it. Meloy (1988) saw disparities of this type as an interrelationship between

conscious choice and an unconscious defensive process in itself. For

example, there is a different internal and/or external goal sought between

conscious malingering and unconscious dissociative defenses as described

below in Table 7.

Many authors (Bender, 1947; Kernberg, 1998; Meloy, 1988) have

observed that, affectively, psychopaths function at the toddler stage, with the

only emotions available in their repartee being shame, envy, rage, frustration,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
76

or excitement. Meloy (2001) termed this phenomenon as part-object

emotions, which are quickly felt and just as quickly dispelled:

Once again, what is absent is most important. The


psychopath...will not evidence the more mature feelings
that necessitate whole-object relatedness and a capacity
for attachment. These emotions include anger, guilt, fear,
depression, sympathy, empathy, remorse, gratitude,
sadness, loneliness, and reciprocal joy - a range of feeling
that is broad, deep, and complex (p. 17).

It is difficult to imagine a way of looking at the world in which these

emotions, that seem so familiar to almost everyone, are not at all present.

Without, the world of the psychopath must be very strange indeed. There

remains some question as to the purpose of the psychopaths affective and

defensive positions; on one hand, they are adaptive and on the other,

defensive. Clearly more research is needed in this area.

Table 7

Meloys Defensive Structuring of the Psychopath


Conscious choice Unconscious Description
Defense
Deception Denial Deception is intentional, goal
directed, and gives false
information used to deceive.
Denial is a primitive mechanism
used against internal and/or
external reality.

Imitation Simulation Imitation is the intentional,


conscious mimicking of another
persons attitudes or behaviors.
Simulation is a magical and
omnipotent fusion between the
self- an' object percept; as-if
personalities.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
77

Table 7 (continued).

Object Control Projective Object control is the actual


Identification controlling of another person or
attempting to do so (usually via
projective identification).
Projective identification is a
process in which affective and
ideational components of the
individual are attributed to
another person.

Splitting Dissociation Dissociation is a defensive


process that expresses the
operation of splitting._____
Note. From The Psychopathic Mind: Origins, Dynamics, and Treatment,
1998, by J. Reid Meloy, p. 117-181. New Jersey: Jason Aronson, Inc.

Freedman, Kaplan, and Sadock (1975) underscored the interplay

between the developmental, psychodynamic, and genetic forces that

combine in the psychopath:

The evidence currently suggests that a predisposition to the


disorder is inherited. This implicates a constitutional
diatheses...in addition to biologic factors, the evidence also
points to environmental influences interacting with the
biological predisposition. The presence of certain forms of
parental deviance in the home clearly facilitates the
development of antisocial personality. Although this
influence may be partly genetic, the adoption work has
demonstrated that nongenetic parental influence is important
also (p. 1290).

Despite the many published works attempting to describe the etiology

and development of the violent psychopath from an analytic perspective, only

Meloy (1988; 2001) and Kernberg (1989) have been truly successful in both

thoroughly describing the disorder and generating a substantial following of

their theories in this area. Since their psychoanalytic paradigm is the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
78

foundation of much of the current thinking on the subject, their theoretical

postulates will be described in detail, and the breadth of their contributions,

particularly those aspects rooted in psychoanalytic theory, are fundamental to

this presentation.

Kernberg

Kernberg (1975) stated that psychopathy was at the severe end of the

continuum of the narcissistic personality disorder, and referred to the

development of a malignant narcissism (Geberth & Turco, 1997) which

involved five essential features of pathology in psychopaths: (a) narcissistic

personality structure, (b) overt antisocial behavior, (c) sadistic cruelty, (d)

egosyntonic aggression, and (e) reintrojection of primitive persecutory

superego features.

Arising from a belief that there had been an overemphasis in the

psychoanalytic literature on Freuds (1916) theory of criminality stemming

from unconscious guilt, Kernberg (1989) reinterpreted psychopathic behavior

as a reaction formation against unconscious guilt rather than the expression

of deficient superego development. Kernberg, perhaps more than any other

analyst, integrated the existing analytic literature in a way that formulated a

rich, detailed theory of psychopathic personality development. Kernberg

credited Johnsons (1949) and Johnson and Szureks (1952) description of

superego lacunae as influencing his own views, and began to focus on the

structural and dynamic aspects of antisocial personalities.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
79

Kernberg (1989) stated that the features of narcissistic personality

disorder are present in almost all patients diagnosed with psychopathy, plus

"a specific pathology of their internalized systems of morality (i.e. superego

functions) and a deterioration of their world of internalized object relations" (p.

315).

Kernberg also defined the predominantly aggressive aspects of the

psychopathic personality disorder from an analytic perspective:

These patients typically present a narcissistic personality


disorder. The typical symptoms of the narcissistic
personalities are in the area of pathological self-love:
excessive self reference and self-centered ness; grandiosity
and the derived characteristics of exhibitionism, an attitude
of superiority, recklessness, and overambitiousness;
overdependency on admiration; emotional
shallowness...these patients predominant symptoms are
inordinate envy (both conscious and unconscious);
devaluation of others as a defense against envy;
exploitativeness reflected in greediness, appropriation of
others ideas or property, and entitlement...the basic ego
state of these patients is characterized by a chronic sense of
emptiness, evidence of an incapacity to learn, a sense of
aloneness, stimulus hunger, and a diffuse sense of
meaningless of life. In addition, all of these patients present
some degree of superego pathology. Ordinary superego
pathology of narcissistic personalities includes the incapacity
to experience mournful, self-reflective sadness; the
presence of severe mood swings; and a predominance of
shame as contrasted to guilt in their intrapsychic regulation
of social behavior (p. 323).

Kernberg believed that psychopathy was the most severe disorder along the

continuum of narcissistic personality disorders (Table 8), since these

individuals possessed severe narcissistic features as well as an antisocial

orientation. According to Kernberg, psychopaths have a severe superego

pathology. They manifest symptoms as evidence of this which are

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
80

characterized by the combination of (a) a narcissistic personality disorder; (b)

antisocial behavior; (c) ego-syntonic aggression or sadism directed toward

others or toward oneself, the latter producing a perverse sense of triumph in

self-mutilation or suicide; and (d) a strong paranoid orientation (Millon et al.,

1998, p. 24). When the antisocial personality disordered individuals also

have a narcissistic personality disorder (Table 8), the more ego-syntonic

aggression, and the more the personality structure shifts from the narcissistic

into the antisocial, the more such aggressive behavior may become life-

threatening, and a subgroup of aggressive antisocial personalities may center

their criminal behavior on sexual assaults and murder (Meloy, 2002, p. 322).

Kernberg (1989) added that the most striking central feature of the disorder is

the absence of a capacity for remorse and authentic guilt feelings, and the

inability to imagine an ethical dimension in others. Even domesticated

animals sometimes seem to be exhibit manifestations of such guilt when they

transgress a known boundary.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
81

Table 8

Kernbergs Classification o f Personality Disorders


in which Antisocial Features are Prominent
Disorder Description

The Antisocial Personality Disorder The Psychopath; typical


symptoms of narcissistic personality,
with severe superego pathology; no
capacity for remorse or guilt feelings

Malignant Narcissism Narcissistic, but still have the


capacity for loyalty to and concern
for others or for feeling guilty

Narcissistic Personality Disorders Antisocial behaviors, but no ego-


with Antisocial Behavior syntonic sadism or overt paranoia;
have capacity for guilt,
can plan for the future; lack of
capacity for commitment to long
term relationships

Other Severe Personality Disorders Less negative prognosis; these


with Antisocial Features are patients with borderline
personality organization and non-
pathological narcissism

Neurotic Personality Disorders Freuds criminals from an


with Antisocial Features unconscious sense of guilt;
antisocial behavior occurs with in the
context of a neurotic personality
organization; excellent prognosis

Antisocial Behavior as Part of a Symptomatic Occasional antisocial behavior


Neurosis as a symptomatic neurosis part of
adolescence or in adjustment
disorder; social environment may be
fostering antisocial behavior

Dyssocial Reaction Normal and/or neurotic adjustment


to an abnormal social environment
or subgroup.

Note: From The Narcissistic Personality Disorder and the Differential Diagnosis of Antisocial
Behavior, by O. Kernberg, 1989, In: J. Reid Meloy, (Ed.), The Mark o f Cain: Psychoanalytic
Insight and the Psychopath, 2001, p. 315-337. New Jersey: The Analytic Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
82

Table 9

Kernbergs Antisocial Personality Disorder Symptoms


Symptom Description

Pathological Self-Love Excessive self-reference and self-


centeredness; grandiosity and the
derived characteristics of
exhibitionism, an attitude of
superiority, recklessness, and
overambitiousness; overdependency
on admiration; emotional
shallowness; and severe bouts of
insecurity alternating with a
predominant grandiosity.

Pathological Object Relations Inordinate envy (both conscious and


unconscious); devaluation of others
as a defense against envy;
exploitativeness reflected in
greediness, appropriation of others
ideas or property, and entitlement;
an incapacity to truly depend on
others in a mutual relationship; and
a remarkable lack of the capacity for
empathy with and commitment to
others.

Basic Ego State Characterized by a chronic sense of


emptiness, evidence of an
incapacity to learn, a sense of
aloneness, stimulus hunger, and a
diffuse sense of the
meaninglessness of life.

Superego Pathology The incapacity to experience


mournful, self-reflective sadness;
the presence of severe mood
swings; a predominance of shame
as contrasted to guilt in their
intrapsychic regulation of social
behavior; and a lack of an integrated
adult value system, reflected in the
persistence of childlike values.
Their self esteem depends on
physical beauty, power, wealth, and
admiration from others, in contrast
to such adult values as personal
capabilities, achievements,
responsibility, and relation to ideals.

Note: From The Narcissistic Personality Disorder and the Differential Diagnosis of
Antisocial Behavior, by O. Kernberg, 1989, In: J. Reid Meloy, (Ed.;, The M ark o f
Cain: Psychoanalytic Insight and the Psychopath, 2001, p. 315-337. New Jersey:
The Analytic Press.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
83

Kernberg also theorized about the likely psychodynamic

developmental origins of psychopaths as a result of their childhood

experiences:

These patients convey past experiences of savage


aggression from their parental objects, and frequently report
violence both observed and experienced in their early
childhood. They also convey a dramatic conviction of the
impotent weakness of any good object relation: the good are
weak and unreliable by definition, and the patient shows
rage, devaluation, and contempt against those vaguely
perceived as potentially good objects. The powerful, in
contrast, are needed to survive, but are unreliable in turn;
and they are invariably sadistic. The pain experienced in
having to depend upon powerful, desperately needed but
sadistic parental objects is transformed into rage, and
expressed as rage mostly projected, thus worsening the
sadistic image of powerful bad objects who become towering
sadistic tyrants. In this world...aggression is prevalent but
unpredictable, and this unpredictability precludes even a
secure submission to the sadistic tyrant and prevents the
patient from idealizing the sadistic value system of the
aggressor. This failure to achieve any idealization of
objects...prevents the antisocial patient from attempting a
masochistic submission to a predictable although sadistic
authority. The patient is deeply and totally convinced that
only his own power itself is reliable, and the pleasure of
sadistic control the only alternative to the suffering and
destruction of the weak (quoted in Meloy, 2001, p. 333).

Meloy

Meloy (1988; 2001) is, perhaps, the leading contemporary authority on

the psychoanalytic psychology of the psychopath, and has authored many

books and articles on the subject. Meloy built upon the seminal work of

Kernberg, by proposing a psychobiological understanding of the psychopath

(i.e., how nature and nurture interact to shape each other over time), which

consists of an inability of attachment, underarousal, and minimal anxiety.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
84

Like Kernberg, Meloy (1988) also believed that the psychopath suffered from

a pathological narcissism, and further distinguished the psychopathic

personality organization from the narcissistic personality, as shown in Table

8. Meloy (2001) defined the psychopathic disorder as:

A deviant developmental disturbance characterized by an


inordinate amount of instinctual aggression and the absence
of an object relational capacity to bond...psychopathy is a
process: a continuous interplay of factors and operations
that are implicitly progressing or regressing toward a
particular end point, a fundamental disidentification with
humanity...that can be understood as an aggressive
subcategory of narcissistic personality disorder (p. xvii).

Meloy cultivated significant support for the notion that there is both a

genetic predisposition to psychopathy, as well as intrapsychic factors that, in

combination, are related to the absence of empathy and sadomasochistic

qualities of the mind, emotion, and behavior.

Meloy (2001) combined these various dimensions and developed a

working definition of psychopathy from a psychobiological view that describes

the characteristics of psychopathy as:

No attachment, underarousal, and minimal anxiety


biologically anchor the foundation of the psychopath. As we
see later in adulthood, these substrates manifest in a
fearless and sensation-seeking lifestyle, one that is
unfettered by the constraints of an affectional bond,
excitability or worry, or fear of violating the rules of others (p.
8).

Meloy suggested that, from an analytic perspective, certain of the

psychological failures of the psychopath also involve biological deficits.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
85

Table 10

Meloys Psychopathic Personality Organization__________________________

1. The predominance of aggressive drive derivatives and the gratification of


aggression as the only significant mode of relating to others.
2. The absence of more passive and independent modes of narcissistic
repair.
3. The presence of sadistic or cruel behavior, implying the activation of
primitive persecutory introjects, or sadistic superego precursors.
4. The presence of a malignant ego ideal with developmental roots in a cruel
and aggressive primary parental object.
5. The absence of a desire to morally justify ones behavior, which would
imply the presence of superego precursors of a more socially acceptable
ego ideal.
6. The presence of both anal-eliminative and phallic exhibitionistic libidinal
themes in the repetitive interpersonal cycles of goal conflict with others,
the intent to deceive, the carrying out of the deceptive act, and the
contemptuous delight when victory is perceived.
7. The emergence of paranoid ideation when under stress, rather than a
vulnerability to depressive affect.

Note: From The Psychopathic Mind: Origins, Dynamics, and Treatment,


1998, p. 19. New Jersey: Jason Aronson, Inc.

Specifically, these are the failures of internalization, grandiose self and

omnipotent fantasies, primitive internalized object relations, and superego

abnormalities. I will briefly consider each of these.

Failures of internalization. As previously described, the psychopathic

individual has extreme difficulties with identifications and introjections.

Identifications are behaviors that are done to model after the object, while

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
86

introjections are internalized aspects of the object. For psychopaths, these

internalizations are completely absent, not available when wanted, or harsh

and unpleasant...the child may come to anticipate hard, aggressive objects

and may identify with such objects for both adaptive and defensive reasons

(Meloy, 2001, p. 10). Both Meloy (2001) and Kernberg (1984) referred to this

fantasy as the predator part-object, which becomes evident in the way

psychopaths take pleasure in dominating others. McWilliams (1994) referred

to this phenomena as being under the thumb of the psychopath.

Grandiose self and omnipotent fantasy. As previously stated, a

psychopathology of narcissism is the functional and affective core of

psychopathy upon which the house of psychopath is built (Meloy, 2001, p.

12). Meloy (2001) also said that:

The grandiose self-structure, a pathological formation rather


than a developmental fixation, has three fused, or
condensed, components: a real self, the actual specialness
of the child; an ideal self, a fantasized self-concept, which
compensates for severe oral frustration, rage, and envy; and
an ideal object, a fantasized image in the childs mind of a
completely loving and accepting parent, often at odds with
the actual behavior of the real parent (p. 11).

In other words, while reality testing remains intact for psychopaths, people

are merely objects designed to gratify their sexual or aggressive impulses

and have no other purpose. Psychopaths devalue others in order to feed

their own narcissism and to satisfy this omnipotent fantasy that they will not

be abused or criticized but rather will do that to others.

Primitive internalized object relations. Meloy (1988; 2001) described

developmental themes that distinguish psychopathy from Kernbergs (1984)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
87

narcissistic disturbance. Meloy believed that psychopathy should be thought

of as part of the borderline personality organization:

The developmental origins of the psychopathic personality


are characterized by a precocious separation from the
primary parent during the symbiotic phase of maturation;
failures of internalization that begin with an organismic
distrust of the sensory-perceptual environment; a
predominate, archetypal identification with the stranger self
object that is central to the conceptual self and object fusions
within the grandiose self-structure during the period of
separation-individuation; a failure of object constancy and a
primary narcissistic attachment to the grandiose self; and
states of relatedness that are aggressively and
sadomasochistically pursued with actual objects. This
coexistence of benign detachment and aggressively pursued,
sadistically toned attempts to bond is pathognomonic of the
psychopathic process (Meloy, 1988, p. 59).

Because the psychopath is organized at the borderline level, the self is

either all good or all bad, as are other people. However, given their

narcissism, usually they are all good and others are all bad. This goes for

their feelings as well so their own feelings are completely good while others

are totally bad. This dyadic structure, as opposed to the more mature

development of an id, ego, and superego, is classically psychopathic:

Repression must activate if there is to be a differentiated ego


and id, and ideal self-representations must integrate with
ideal object representations as an ego ideal if the
differentiation between ego and superego is to begin. In the
pre-Oedipal personality of the fledgling psychopath, which
continues into adulthood, there are only dyadic self- and
object representations, which are either condensed or
displaced somewhere else, usually outside the self (Meloy,
2001, p. 14).

Superego abnormalities. For the psychopath internalizations largely

fail so the ability to internalize the values, mores, and societal expectations

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
88

are also impaired. The adult psychopath is a valueless person...morals

responsibility is a moot question in psychopathy (Meloy, 2001, p. 15).

Further, there may be a process of identifying with badness or evil. When

this occurs, a sadistic quality emerges that is evident behaviorally, for

example, in the torturing of animals.

Although a psychoanalytic formulation of the development of

psychopathy is integral to the premise of this review, the importance of

biological aspects contributing to this paradigm warrants further investigation.

The following chapter summarizes the more poignant biological findings of

the psychopathic disorder.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
89

Chapter V

Biological Contributions to Psychopathy


Are psychopaths born or made? The house of psychopath
is built on a psychobiological foundation of no attachment,
underarousal and minimal anxiety (Meloy, 2001, p. 3)

Contemporary models, based on emerging research findings and

technological advances, incorporate not only a psychological basis of

psychopathy but also include a complex interplay between biological, social,

and developmental factors as well. Freuds drive theory is built upon the

notion that psychological processes have roots in biological functions, and

Hare (1993) observed there is evidence of genetic factors influencing brain

functions and personality structure. This impacts the way individuals interact

with the social environment.

Alexander (1935) believed that psychopathy had both psychological

and biological origins, which, when they converged, created a continuum of

psychopathy that he labeled neurosis, neurotic character, psychosis, and

criminality (p. 279). Kraepelin (1887/1904) also suspected a biological or

constitutional etiology of psychopathy, as evidenced by deficits in

psychopaths inability to enjoy a full range of affect and their lack of

adherence to moral values. These are characteristics that are quite

consistent over the course of a psychopaths lifespan.

Siever et. al., (1985) observed that psychopaths may demonstrate

lowered cortical arousal and more disinhibited motoric responses to a variety

of stimuli. Thus, persons with antisocial personality disorder are more likely

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
90

to act immediately than to reflect prior to their taking action, so that

internalization of societally sanctioned controls may be more problematic (p.

43). In persons identified as psychopaths, there appears to be a complex

interaction between psychological, environmental, and biological

predispositions to develop the full disorder. Psychopathy has already been

linked to family difficulties, such as physical punishment, inconsistent

discipline, and broken homes (Mitchell & Blair, 2000). Meloy (1988)

suggested there was a biological influence on psychological processes, and

by examining empirical research in this area he postulated two assumptions

about the biology of psychopaths. First, there seems to be a significant

relationship between criminality and psychopathy. Second, there also seems

to be a significant relationship between psychopathy and aggression,

whereby psychopaths generally commit more crimes than non-psychopaths.

Meloy hypothesized that the positive correlation between psychopathy,

criminality, and aggression are due, in part, to (a) neuroanatomical structure,

(b) genetic predisposition, (c) neurochemical sets, (d) hormonal influences,

and (e) autonomic reactivity. Each of these is considered below.

Neuroanatomical structures. Meloy cites Mednicks (1982) study in

which abnormal electroencephalogram measures were found in 25 to 50

percent of psychopathic individuals studied, while the incidence of these

abnormal readings in the normal population ranged from 5 to 20 percent.

Meloy also referred to Yeudalls (1977) research in which he discovered that

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
91

about 91 percent of psychopaths exhibited significant neuropsychological

impairment...implicating frontal and temporal area brain dysfunction (p. 22).

Genetic predispositions. Christiansen (1977) hypothesized that there

was a genetic influence in criminal behavior by studying monozygotic and

dizygotic twins, where he discovered a .69 concordance to criminality in

monozygotic twins, and a .33 concordance in dizygotic twins. An adoption

study by Schulsinger (1977)) discovered that psychopathic adoptees

relatives, who were unknown to the adoptee, as compared to non-

psychopathic adoptees, manifested more psychopathy than the normal

population (p. 23). This supports a biological influence for psychopathy which

may have a stronger influence than environmental factors.

Neurochemical Factors. Certain brain functions and physical

responses are different in psychopathic individuals than in non-psychopathic

individuals and this may provide a link between psychopathy to aggression

(Meloy, 1988). Meloy defined aggressive behaviors as falling into two

different categories: affective aggression that is the result of external or

internal threatening stimuli that evoke an intense and patterned activation of

the autonomic nervous system accompanied by threatening vocalizations and

attacking or defending postures (p. 25). The second form of aggression,

predatory aggression is directed toward the destruction of prey, usually for

food gathering in subhuman species (p. 410). Meloy (1988; 2001)

suggested that psychopaths are more apt to be predatorily aggressive, or

display primitive acts of violence. He cited this as a possible biological

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
92

source of the disorder, with increased levels the neurotransmitters of

serotonin, norepinephrine, dopamine, and acetylcholine as physiological

correlates of aggressive behavior.

Meloy found a strong and positive correlational relationship among

the psychopaths aggression, social dominance, and testosterone levels (p.

27) as well as evidence of reduced androgen levels that seem to increase

aggression.

Autonomic reactivity. Meloy hypothesized that psychopaths are

autonomically hyporeactive, at an electrical level (p. 31) and suggested that:

...The psychopathic individuals anticipation or experience of


unpleasant, negative affect will have little or no behavioral
consequence...insight may be imitated and quickly learned,
but will exist without an affective dimension such as shame,
guilt, or remorse...predominant aggressive-drive derivatives
will be less inhibited by the psychopathic personality despite
their aversive consequences for the individual and
others...and subsequent positive affect may strongly
reinforce these aggressive-drive derivatives. Punishment of
the psychopathic criminal exists only as a public projection (p.
33).

Hare (1970) reached similar conclusion through work he demonstrated

autonomic irregularities using skin conductance measures. These measures

send an electrical current through the fingers. If a subject is perspiring, thus

more nervous, the skin conductivity will increase, thus indicating increased

autonomic activity.

There are two structures in the brain that have been thought to play a

part in the development of psychopathy. Mitchell (2000) identified the

orbitofrontal cortex and the amygdala as possible candidates for links to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
93

psychopathy. According to Mitchell, damage to the orbitofrontal cortex,

located in the frontal lobe below the center of the forehead, has been linked

to severe behavioral problems, including aggression and rage reactions.

Lesions in this area have been known to cause increased anger and

aggressive behaviors in previously normal people. Damasio (1994) termed

individuals affected in this way acquired sociopaths. The amygdala, located

in the forebrain, is central to the processing of emotions and is critical to

mediating the fear response. From an early age, this region functions

differently in individuals with psychopathy (Mitchell, 2000). Blair and Frith

(2000) discussed possible impairments in neural substates that might cause

psychopathy. They concluded that the development of psychopathy is

associated with impairment in emotional processing, and that the

development of this kind impairment may be due to dysfunction within the

amygdala. When the amygdala is damaged, a person will not show fear or

startle responses. Mitchell (2000) further suggested that these findings might

provide evidence of a link between impaired fear and empathy responses

typical of a psychopath and amygdala functioning.

Physiological examinations of psychopaths have revealed other

biological abnormalities. For example, Patrick (1994) reported that

psychopaths have smaller heart rate changes and skin conduction in

response to fear-provoking sentences than non-psychopaths. Martens (2000)

suggested that psychopaths have dysfunctions in the limbic system that

affect the ability to inhibit behavior, even when it will lead to punishment.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
94

Further, he noted a marked negative relationship between monoamine

oxidase (MAO) activity on one hand and sensation seeking and impulsivity on

the other (p. 3), which are core features of the psychopathic disorder. He

concluded that psychopathy could be linked to a number of biochemical

abnormalities (which he defines as serotonin, monoamine oxidase, and

hormone dysfunctions), genetic and environmental influences, and

psychological and social manifestations (p. 406). In a review article of

research in this area to date, Martens summarized the salient findings,

presented in Table 11 below. Further, he described some brain injuries and

cerebrovascular disorders that can cause psychopathic personality changes,

as well as frontal lobe lesions or other disturbances in prefrontal functioning,

which may in psychopaths be related to the core features of impulsivity and

disinhibition (p. 410).

In total, these various measures of cortical underarousal (slow EEG

activity, low resting heart rate, and the skin conductance abnormalities)

appear to correlate with criminality (Marshall & Cooke, 1999; Meloy, 2001;

Raine, 1988). When taken together, they have a predictive power that can

override the influence of the environment, especially when the latter is what

we would consider normal, nurturing, and good enough (Meloy, 2001, p. 6).

Although there has been an abundance of research examining possible

biological causes for psychopathy, it should be noted that there has yet to be

a single or group of specifically identified biological underpinnings

consistently related to the psychopathic disorder. Lynam (1996) suggested

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
95

that both environmental difficulties and genetic factors play a role in the

development of psychopathy. Other contemporary researchers agree with

this position (Lykken, 1995; Marshall & Cooke, 1999). Understand the

mechanisms and the extent to which biological factors predispose an

individual toward psychopathy is essential to address the disorder. Blair and

Frith (2000) believe that until there is a complete understanding, on an

anatomical level, of what the psychopath is doing wrong, there is no way to

truly treat the psychopath.

Table 11

M a rte n s B iological, Psych o log ical, a n d S o c ia l D e te rm in a n ts o f P s y c h o p a th y


Cause Description
Brain Injuries and Dysfunctions Brain injuries, frontal lobe
lesions, disturbances in
prefrontal functioning,
dysfunctions in the limbic system
(affects the ability to inhibit or
disrupt ongoing behavior; cannot
learn from consequences).

MAO, 5-HIAA, and Hormone Dysfunctions Marked negative relationship


between MAO activity on one
hand and sensation seeking and
impulsivity on the other;
Association between low
serotonin function and
aggressive behavior; Inverse
relationship between 5-HIAA on
one hand and impulsivity,
irritability, hostility, and
aggression on the other.

Electroencephalogram (EEG) Abnormalities Association between


psychopathy and abnormal EEG
readings; stimulation seeking
behavior as a consequence of
pathological underarousal.

Low Autonomic Activity-Reactivity Low adrenaline excretion


strongly associate with persistent
antisocial behavior; Resting skin

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
96

Table 11 (continued)
conductance and heart rate -
characterized in psychopaths as
_________________________________________ underarousal._______________
N o te: From Antisocial and Psychopathic Personality Disorders: Causes,
Course, and Remission - A Review Article, by W. Martens, 2000, In te rn a tio n a l
J o u rn a l o f O ffe n d e r T h e ra p y a n d C o m p a ra tiv e C rim inology, 44(4), 406-430.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
97

Chapter VI

Treatment of Psychopathy
Dont forget these people. They have no one, yet they are
people. They are desperately lacking and in terrible pain.
Those who understand this are so rare; you must not turn
your back on them (Sturup (1951), quoted in Millon, 1998, p.
29)

Rabin (1979) some time ago asked, What can be done to prevent

this sort of disorder to begin with, and what can be done to modify and

change its course and continued path of destructiveness? Can the condition

be cured? (p. 342). In other words, how can psychopathy be prevented

from occurring or cured or partially ameliorated when it does occur?

Unfortunately, psychologists do not have much better answers to these vital

questions than the answers that were available at the time Rabin posed

them.

According to Lee (1999), at least some of the confusion and

controversy about treatment interventions stems from a lack of agreement

among clinical professionals regarding three critical issues:

The first issue concerns the nature of the psychopathic


condition and the specific class of persons to whom it applies.
The second issue relates to the most appropriate goals and
targets for the clinical management of the disorder, the form
treatment should take, and how successful treatment outcome
should be evaluated by the clinicians involved. The third issue
concerns the extent to which psychopathic behaviors are
treatable and to whether evidence of psychological change
during treatment implies reduced risk of re-engaging in such
behavior once treatment has culminated (p. 1).

The psychopath is often the least loved of patients (Strasburger,

1986) and traditionally, the psychopath has been deemed untreatable and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
98

incurable (Arrigo & Shipley, 2001; Carney, 1978; Cleckley, 1988; Hare, 1993;

Rabin, 1979; Strasburger, 1986), with the prognosis for effective

psychotherapeutic intervention as practically non-existent. Suedfled and

Landon (1978), for example, ironically noted that even a quick review of the

literature suggests that a chapter on effective treatment should be the

shortest in any book concerned with the psychopath. In fact, it has been

suggested that one sentence would suffice: No demonstrably effective

treatment has been found (Suedfeld & Landon, 1978, p. 347). In part, this

widespread consensus that treatment is ineffective may reflect a general

aversion for the psychopathic personality and therefore a reluctance to

engage it. Vaillants (1975) once observed that, The defense mechanisms

that underlie sociopathy seem...as unbearably gross to the observer as a

strong cigar in a crowded elevator (p. 75).

Nevertheless, it surprising that so little progress has been made

regarding the treatment of psychopaths since Rabin (1979) summed up the

matter almost twenty five years ago. Rabin observed that:

Imprisonment of the psychopathic criminal has not aided


much in his rehabilitation. Violation of parole and recidivism
are the rule rather than the exception, according to the
evidence we have. Modes of temporary segregation from
the community other than imprisonment have been similarly
unsuccessful in the long range. Hospitalization in psychiatric
institutions is usually of brief duration, frequently on an
emergency basis. Quite often, psychopaths shuttle back
and forth from penal to psychiatric institutions until paroled
or discharged, to resume the antisocial behavior for which
they were originally incarcerated. Quite often, prison
authorities feel that these prisoners have psychological
problems which should be treated in a hospital setting, but
psychiatric institutions return the men to prison, for past

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
99

experience has shown that the ordinary hospital regime,


including individual counseling or group therapy as well as
various other ancillary methods of treatment, is of little value
in attempting to change the psychopath (p. 343).

Efforts to treat this misunderstood and often dangerous population

have been quite frustrating, and traditional interventions have almost always

proven ineffectual (Frodi et al., 2001). The psychopath has a tendency to

attribute his problems to the external environment making thus

psychotherapy with this population challenging (Doren, 1987). Hare (1993)

reported another major problem in treating psychopaths is that psychopaths

do not consider themselves to have psychological or emotional problems,

and subsequently they see no reason to change their behavior to adhere to

societal norms. Similarly, McCord and McCord (1964) stated, ...the typical

psychopathic personality seems singularly resistant to change. He lacks a

desire for change and the anxiety, which most therapists believe is a

prerequisite for treatment. Most psychopaths see nothing wrong with

themselves and, therefore, no reason to change (p. 118). Karpman (1946)

agreed with this calling the psychopath a virtually incurable case, requiring

indefinite institutionalization rather than temporary punishment (p. 285).

Clearly, there are numerous challenges facing the mental health

professional treating psychopathically disturbed individuals, and many

differing views about the efficacy of different techniques and treatment

approaches. This is not to say that all psychopaths are completely immune to

psychotherapy, however, treatment efficacy has been difficult and

problematic to measure since psychopaths generally see treatment as a way

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
100

to secure particular goals such as discharge or early release from prison and

are prone to feign improvement.

Taking a more pessimistic stance than some of his contemporaries,

Meloy (2001) cites the lack of analytic writings on therapeutic techniques for

the treatment of psychopathy as a lack of clinical or empirical evidence that

psychopaths will benefit from any form of psychodynamic therapy, including

the expressive or supportive psychotherapies, psychoanalysis, or various

psychodynamically oriented group psychotherapies (p. 183).

Although this paper attempts to understand the psychological and

interpersonal dynamics underlying psychopathy, there is little

empirical research that combines both what is already known about

psychopathy and how to treat the disorder effectively. Psychoanalytic

models seem to provide the greatest hope of shedding light on the poor

prognosis and lack of suitable treatment models and, minimally, play a

large part in the development of future clinical interventions.

Psychoanalytic Treatment

Analytic knowing has charted the expectable but invariably


malignant course of transference and countertransference
and also contributed to empirical diagnosis to discern the
patient for whom psychoanalysis offers no hope (Meloy,
2001, p. 202).

Among the first to focus on the treatment of psychopaths was Aichorn

(1935), who described three types of antisocial behavior, each warranting

different methods of treatment. First, he recommended that psychopaths

who were driven by guilt originating in the superego have a wish to be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
101

punished to alleviate the guilt feelings could be benefit from psychoanalytic

treatment. Second, therapists treating individuals who identified with their

antisocial parents needed to encourage a strong positive transference to the

therapist, who should use a variety of incentives including unconditional

forgiveness and admiration, and even gifts (Wolman, 1980). Aichorns third

type was the primitive delinquent, who had no superego. He suggested a

structured environment and non-psychoanalytic psychotherapy for these

individuals.

According to Toch (1979) there are several determinants of patient

responsiveness to psychoanalytic treatment modality: (a) the dominant role

of irrational motives, pressures of which the individual may not be aware,

which can manifest themselves in devious or indirect ways; (b) deficits that

can occur in the egos mediation of reality, or the egos capacity to control

affects or drives; (c) the presence of several layers of motivation; (d)

problems with values which can be tenuous or tyrannical; (e) the complexity

that is produced by internal conflicts; and (f) cues that the persons past is

alive in his reactions to present circumstances (p. 191).

Kernberg believed it is crucial, from a diagnostic, prognostic, and

therapeutic viewpoint to carefully evaluate (a) the presence or absence of

some form of pathological narcissism; (b) the extent to which superego

pathology dominates - that is, where the patient falls in the continuum from

the narcissistic to the antisocial personality disorders; (c) the intensity of ego-

syntonic aggression, whether directed toward the self (in the form of suicidal,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
102

parasuicidal, and/or self-mutilating and self-destructive behavior) or against

others (in the form of physical violence, homicidal tendencies, or a life-

endangering sadistic perversion); (d) the severity of paranoid trends; and

finally (e) the stability of reality testing (Kernberg, 1998, p. 376). This careful

assessment, which Kernberg cautioned to not attempt to carry out alone in a

closed room (p. 378), has been used by clinicians to determine the degree of

a persons antisocial tendencies in order to make predictions about the

quality of their object relations and their ability to benefit from

psychotherapeutic treatment (Kernberg, 1998; McWilliams, 1994).

Kernberg (1989) believed the most important feature to assess for

treatability was the extent to which lives were at stake; either the patients life,

the lives of other people, or the therapists life. Despite this attempt to

identify those individuals with antisocial tendencies who might benefit from

psychotherapy, Kernberg even suggested the prognosis for psychopathic

patients to be practically zero. He stated:

It is important to keep in mind that some patients cannot be


helped (at least in the concrete situation of any particular
psychotherapeutic engagement), and that, in the end, it
cannot be only the therapist who wishes to help patients
whose major gratification in life is the destruction of those
who are attempting to help them (p. 391).

On the other hand, to say that psychopaths never recover at all

flies in the face of change that occurs with some.

While the more extreme psychopaths are unlikely to benefit from

therapy, those who are less disturbed may find some benefit. Meloy believed

there is an inverse relationship between level of psychopathic disturbance

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
103

and treatment efficacy, i.e., the greater the psychopathic disturbance in the

individual, the more likely psychotherapy will have no effect:

Treatment should not proceed if the patient is a primary


psychopath. Interpersonal and intrapsychic features that
contraindicate any form of treatment include sadistic behavior
in the patients history that resulted in serious injury, maiming,
or death of a victim; the need to justify or rationalize such
behavior or a complete absence of any remorse; intelligence
greater or less than two standard deviations from the mean; a
historical absence of attachment without depression; and the
presence of an atavistic fear of predation felt by clinicians
when with the patient (Meloy, 2001, p. 188).

The degree of damage that a psychopath has sustained is determined

by the extent of ego or superego dysfunction. Severe damage to these

faculties occurs most frequently when individuals are quite young. Bender

believed there was a certain point in the normal development of the

personality after which analytic treatment could not be used to:

repair the infantile deprivation because the psychopath was


not able to engage in a transferrential relationship that was
required for successful education and treatment. Once the
early childhood has been passed without the adequate
opportunity for normal relationships and personality
development, the organization of the personality permits no
modification (Bender, 1947, p. 76).

Strasburger (1986) suggested that degree of insight and openness

required for analytic treatment is unrealistic for the psychopathic personality,

and because their behavior is characteristically callous and without empathy

there would inevitably be an incapacity to maintain the therapeutic

relationship:

These people are impulsive, unable to tolerate frustration


and delay, and have problems with trusting. They take a
paranoid position or externalize their emotional experience.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
104

They have little ability to form a working alliance and poor


capacity for self-observation. Their anger is frightening.
Frequently they take flight. Their relations with others are
highly problematic. When close to another person they fear
engulfment or fusion and loss of self. At the same time,
paradoxically, they desire closeness; frustration of their
entitled wishes to be nourished, cared for, and assisted
often leads to rage. They are capable of a childs primitive
fury enacted with an adults physical capabilities, and action
is always in the offing (Strasburger, 1986, p 191).

Transference

It is only in psychoanalytic treatment that reliving distorted or disturbed

has a curative effect, and thus the psychopath will have to relive, through

transference and countertransference, these early conflicts. Meloy (2001)

stated that psychopaths would adopt one of four transference positions in

treatment that were originally characterized by Kohut (1971). This stems from

their core narcissism. The patient will either seek to idealize (I want to

worship), or to mirror (I want to be worshiped), or to twin (I want to imitate), or

to merge (I want to control) (p. 184). In a traditional therapeutic situation it is

likely, that the primitive need to control the analyst will emerge and the

discomfort is experienced as being under his thumb (McWilliams, 1994). In

response to this threatening hostility the therapist will likely feel that working

with the psychopath is like walking on eggshells to avoid what is

catastrophically imagined as rageful, explosive, and potentially violent affect,

perhaps a complimentary and fearful identification with how the psychopath

felt as a child (Meloy, 2001, p. 184). Vaillant (1975) suggested that from the

very beginning of the treatment the therapist must aggressively take control

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
105

of the relationship, structure it, and define its limitations in order to establish

adequate boundaries on the therapeutic frame.

Meloy (1988, 2001) also identified other predictable resistances that

would likely occur when working with a psychopath. These included

manipulative cycling, deceptive practices, malignant pseudo-identification,

and sadistic control. He described manipulative cycling as:

A goal conflict, an intent to deceive, a successfully carried


out deceptive act, and a subsequent feeling of
contemptuous delight...the process of which unconsciously
defends against envy and oral rage, which is likely felt when
the analyst is initially idealized; it purges devalued introjects
that are then projectively identified into the analyst and thus
retain the homeostasis of the grandiose self-structure
(Meloy, 2001, p. 184).

Deceptive practices, or manipulation, are to be expected when working with

psychopaths, and Kernberg (1992a) wrote:

I have coined the term psychopathic transference to refer to


periods in the treatment when...conditions of deceptiveness
and their projection prevail. In my view, it is essential to
explore such transferences in great detail, and to resolve
them interpretatively before proceeding with other materials
(p. 18).

The psychopath uses deception in an attempt to devalue the therapist, which

wards off anxiety, and leaves the therapist feeling suspicious. Malignant

pseudo-identification is used to control the therapist and appeals to the

psychopaths own narcissistic tendencies. An example of this would be when

the psychopath attempts to imitate certain emotions during treatment, using

all the right words about certain feelings, but without any actual insight about

what they actually feel like or mean. The analyst is left feeling distant and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
106

skeptical, yet confused by the absence of any true empathic responses in

himself (Kernberg, 1992b). Sadistic control, a component of malignant

narcissism was first discussed by Kernberg (1984) and later described in

more detail by Meloy (2001) as:

...ranging from verbal devaluation of the process to blatant


forms of psychological and physical aggression. It is a
clinical manifestation of the prey-predator dynamic in which
all relationships, including the one with the analyst, are
defined by dominance and submission. The analytic
question is the degree to which the desire for sadistic control
by the patient is ego syntonic, or riddled with conflicts and
dependencies (p. 185).

When the analyst attempts to treat this syntonic sadism, it can have serious

implications for the therapy including actually physical danger. McWilliams

(1994) stated that the psychopaths transference to the therapist is a

projection of his or her internal predation, the assumption that the clinician

intends to use the patient for selfish purposes (p. 159). According to

McWilliams, this was the result of not being able to understand the interest or

generosity of the therapist, having been deprived any kind of gratifying

emotional experiences as a child.

Countertransference

The clinical treatment of psychopathy can elicit a variety of responses

from the therapist, including anger, empathy, hatred, fear and other intense

negative emotions (Weiss, 1988). Kernberg (1989) suggested that therapists

working with psychopathic patients might be tempted to either just accept the

patients statements, or fully reject them in what he called a protective

pseudoneutrality, which may be a way to devalue the patient. Kernberg

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
107

recommended taking a true ambivalent stance; moral but not moralizing, fair

but not naive, and confronting but not aggressive.

Strasburger (1986) wrote a great deal about countertransference

reactions in therapists working with psychopaths, and described six typical

responses: fear of assault or harm, helplessness and guilt, loss of

professional identity, denial of danger, rejection of the patient, and the rageful

wish to destroy (Table 12). Strasburger discussed the therapeutic

pessimism that may be evoked in the therapist during treatment:

It is difficult to be sympathetic toward delinquent, substance


abusing, and sometimes violent individuals...Clinical staff do
not like their repeated demands for help, coupled with their
insistence that their troubles always originate outside
themselves. Their infantile manner, omnipotent demands,
and lack of introspection cause them to be rejected. Their
hedonism and requirements for instant gratification often
clash head-on with the personal values of the clinical staff
(quoted in Meloy, 2002, p. 298).

Because the behavior of these types of clients can be so disgusting

and undesirable, they are often dismissed as just psychopaths, and their

inner world can be ignored by the clinician in a dehumanizing fashion - just

as the psychopath does to the therapist. Vaillant (1975) stated the pejorative

term, psychopath, is accurate only insofar as it describes the back of a

patient fleeing therapy. If a psychiatrist sees the same patient in a prison

hospital, he may doubt that such a disorder exists (p. 75).

Meloy (2001) and McWilliams (1994) described a therapeutic

nihilism, or a certain cynicism in the analyst which can occur when working

with clients who have even mild antisocial histories. This countertransference

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
108

manifests as a dismissive or derogatory attitude toward any patient who

reports an illegal act or has been accused, or convicted, of a crime (Meloy,

2001, p. 185). Conversely, the therapist may believe, mistakenly, that there

is a therapeutic alliance or attachment to them when what is occurring is

simply faking on the part of the psychopath This type of countertransference

usually stems from the therapists wish to believe that the psychopath does

have some ability to form a bond (Lion, 1978). McWilliams (1994), with great

insight, described the therapists reaction to this as:

Shock and resistance to the sense that ones essential


identity as a helper is being eradicated. The naive
practitioner may succumb to the temptation to try to prove
helpful intent. When that fails, hostility, contempt, and
moralistic outrage toward the psychopathic person are
common reactions. These unempathic feelings in
ordinarily compassionate people should be understood,
paradoxically, as a kind of empathy with psychopathic
psychology: The client is unable to care about the therapist,
and the therapist finds it almost as hard to care about the
client...if one can tolerate the experience of internal
coldness and even hatred, one will get a unpleasant but
useful glimpse of what it is like to be a psychopathically
organized person (p. 159).

Often, clinicians describe having a physical sensation when meeting

with psychopathic patients as a fear of physical harm that manifests as

goose bumps, piloerection, or the skin crawling. Meloy (2001) specifically

noted that:

These are phylogenetically old, biologically based autonomic signals of


impending predatory danger. They may prompt a variety of systemic
disturbances in the analyst, including gastrointestinal and
cardiopulmonary reactivity, and may be signaling an actual future
threat (p. 186).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
109

Melloy surveyed mental health and criminal justice professionals, and

found this countertransference response occurred in a majority of the

respondents. Its universality suggested that it reflected an important and

perhaps primitive survival mechanism. McWilliams (1994) thought it

important to tolerate these reactions and avoid minimization of the actual

threats these patients pose.

Table 12

S tra s b u rg e rs C o u n te rtra n s fe re n c e R e s p o n s e s in W o rkin g with P s y c h o p a th s


Response Description

Fear of assault or harm The therapists frustration can produce anger,


which is projected as a fear of ambush. Fear
can lead to the abandonment of talking in favor
of seclusion, restraint, or medication. Fears of
assault usually diminish with experience.

Helplessness and guilt The therapist may feel his efforts to help
rejected while he himself is devalued. It is a
difficult, slow-moving task. Helplessness and
guilt about the
patients lack of improvement, manifest as
either emotional withdrawal or as over
responsibility for what happens to the patient.

Loss of professional identity The patient disowns his problems and ascribes
them to the therapist, and the therapist may
come to feel that he owns them. Unwilling to
experience the dysphoria, the therapist
attempts to wriggle free of the patients
attributions. Devaluation by the patient can
evoke feelings of worthlessness, fear,
depression, rage, guilt, shame, and envy.

Denial of danger It may lead to a failure to elicit information


about weapons, lethal skills, past criminal or
violent acts and the therapist may counter-
phobically place himself in a dangerous
position.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
110

Rejection of the patient The therapist must maintain a delicate balance


between a talion rejection of the patient and a
masochistic submission to unreasonable
demands. Rejection might be a lack of
emotional investment, boredom, hatred, or
emotional withdrawal.

The rageful wish to destroy May be reactively mirroring the emotion of the
patient, or via identification with the aggressor.
The patients defiant, remorseless, threatening
attitude provokes its punitive reaction.

Note. From The Treatment of Antisocial Syndromes, by L. Strasburger, 1986, In W. Reid,


D. Dorr, J. Walker, and J. Bonner (Eds.), Unmasking the psychopath: Antisocial personality
and related syndromes. New York: W. W. Norton and company.

Successful psychotherapy requires the therapist to show acceptance

of the subject, yet the necessary focus on the patients destructiveness may

generate an intense countertransference reaction that can sometimes

develop into a fear of the patient (Blackburn, 1993).

Leaff (1978) observed that while treatment demands with psychopathic

patients are many, these patients actually might be treatable if they had at

least some adequate developmental history. By treatment, Leaff meant any

interventions from psychoanalysis to supportive counseling as well as the use

of institutionalization, whether in a hospital or prison setting, as well as

medication:

The therapist is buffeted by demands from the patient and


the external world, as well as by intense transference and
countertransference reactions. The patent, as well as his
family, may subvert treatment; provocativeness and acting
out may bring extreme pressure to bear from the
environment, or evoke inner feelings of discouragement and
failure in the therapist. The patients devaluation of the
therapist may lead to feelings of depletion. Covert and overt
hostility may engender counterhostility. Repeated use of
externalization and hostile expectations may make engaging
the patient difficult; the patients constant conscious and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
111

unconscious rage toward the therapist may lead to


termination of treatment, increased acting out or even
assault of the therapist. Alternatively, the therapists values
are threatened. He may attempt to become the patients
superego or his own instinctual drives may be so
stimulated by the patient that the therapist becomes
anxious, does not function effectively, and/or withdraws from
the treatment situation (Leaff, 1978, p. 115).

Dalmau (1961) was a bit more optimistic, and a perhaps a bit naive, in

recommending a psychoanalytic approach for the treatment of psychopathy

with specific focus on the interpretation of psychopathic behavior in symbolic

terms. He believed that one of the greatest difficulties in treating the

psychopath is understanding the symbolic meaning of his behavior and

managing the overwhelming malignant transference.

The true psychopath is more difficult to reach because the


prelogical level of trauma makes the defenses almost
impregnable. The psychotic intensity of the conflict is too much
of a threat to the ego and analysis is avoided realistically. If the
premise of the massive overwhelming superego directly
proportional to the instinctual forces is correct, then therapy
poses the threat of internalization and acceptance of psychic pain
(Dalmau, 1961, p. 451).

Skeem et al (2002) cited the emotional detachment of psychopaths as

an obstacle preventing them from establishing a therapeutic alliance with the

therapist as a reason for treatment failure, ...interpersonal and affective

features of psychopathy like manipulativeness, pathological lying, shallow

affect, and denial of responsibility are obstacles in accomplishing empirically

validated characteristics of effective psychotherapy such as therapeutic

cooperation, self-exploration, cognitive confrontation, affective intimacy,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
112

patient engagement, reciprocal attachment, and openness (Skeem et al.,

2002, p. 578).

Current Research and Treatment Modalities

Salekin (2002) reviewed 42 treatment studies on psychopathy and

concluded that there is little scientific basis for the belief that psychopathy is

an untreatable disorder (p. 79). Salekin found several problems with the

existing research in this area. First is widespread disagreement concerning

the definition and characteristics of psychopathy. Second, the etiology of

psychopathy is not well understood. Finally, there are too few empirical

studies of the treatment of psychopaths with little follow up investigations

after treatment (p. 79).

The results of Salekins meta-analysis (Table 13) found that

psychoanalytic therapy was, in fact, an effective modality in the treatment of

psychopathy. Salekin (2002) reported that the average success rate was

59% based on 17 studies and 88 psychopaths, and found that after

psychoanalytic treatment these individuals (between 8 and 55 years old)

improved. By improved he means a decrease in lying, an increase in

remorse and empathy, and improved relations with others (p. 105). He

added, this finding indicates that insight-oriented therapies may decrease

psychopathic traits and that psychopathic individuals gained some awareness

of their general approach to life through this therapeutic approach (p. 101).

Further, Salekin added for the most effective programs the treatment

consisted of an average of four sessions of individual therapy per week for at

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
113

least one year, and when augmented with group psychotherapy was

preliminarily beneficial. Salekins research has challenged the prevailing

consensus in psychology that treatment for the psychopath is a lost cause

(Skeem et al., 2002). Agreeing with Salekin, Frodi et. al. (2001) asserted that

when a psychopath has regular sessions over an extended period, this can

provide a secure base from which psychopaths can access different

aspects of their internal world, address transference and countertransference

issues, and eventually facilitate their development of empathy for others.

(Frodi etal., 2001, p. 281)

Aside from Salekins meta-analysis, an extensive literature search did

not produce many studies on using analytic group psychotherapy with

psychopaths. Cox (1998) described a small (6 member), modified group-

analytic psychotherapy approach to treating psychopaths at Broadmoor

Hospital, where about 25% of the entire patient population have been

diagnosed as psychopathic. Interestingly, patients there are hospitalized

without limit of time, which implies that it is only when patients are

considered psychodynamically integrated, socially stable, and safe that they

are transferred to less secure conditions (p. 395). Because the participants

have violent histories, Cox stated that the only place to work analytically in

groups with these patients is in the secure setting of prisons or hospitals.

Some research indicates that insight-oriented therapies actually make

psychopaths (but not non-psychopaths) more likely to recidivate, possibly

because psychopaths can use psychotherapy sessions to develop their skills

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
114

in psychological manipulation, and because they see no need to change their

personalities. (Hare, 1993; McCord & McCord, 1964).

It appears that the least effective modality for treating psychopaths is

the therapeutic community. Despite this therapeutic communities have been

and continue to be used quite often with this population. Salekin believes that

the reason for the low level of success is that therapeutic community

programs are not designed to target the symptoms of the psychopath and do

not have sufficient contact with psychologists as part of their design (Salekin,

2002, p. 101). Without the psychological insight of mental health

professionals, therapeutic communities can be easily manipulated. Harris,

Rice, and Cormier (1992) reported that psychopaths tended to exploit

unstructured programs such as therapeutic communities. This can be easily

done since many psychopaths are often quite verbally facile.

Ogloff et al. (1990) speculated that the therapeutic communities may

fail to provide effective treatment for psychopaths because often psychopaths

initially seek treatment for secondary gain, such as conditional release, but

once treatment has begun they tend to be less motivated, put less effort into

the program, and are eventually removed - either for lack of motivation or

behavioral and security concerns. The psychopath starts to become more

aware of the impact of his self-destructive and irresponsible behaviors.

These insights may be alien and uncomfortable to him, and he escapes this

difficult environment by acting up, (p. 187)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
115

Lee (1999) described other problems with the therapeutic community

approach to the treatment of psychopathy:

On closer look at the data, for instance, there are a


noticeably high number of reported incidents of attempted
escape by psychopathic subjects living in this type of
environment, as well as dangerous and life-threatening
group behavior.... psychopathic patients can abuse the
freedoms they are granted within TCs or cliques have
developed among patients which then result in dangerous
behaviour, such as the severe incident that occurred in the
early spring of 1991, in which six unsupervised patients set
fire to a couch and narrowly avoided killing another
patient...(p. 23).

Table 13

Meta-analysis Results for Psychopathy


Intervention Studies
Type of Psychotherapy # of studies Total SampleSuccess Rate
1. Psychoanalytic 17 88 .59
2. Cognitive-Behavioral 5 246 .62
3. Therapeutic Communities 8 371 .25
4. Actional Procedures 1 20 .88
5. Eclectic 2 62 .86
6. Pharmacotherapy 2 10 .70
7. ECT 2 9 .22
8. Personal Construct 1 46 1.0
9. Rational Therapy 1 1 1.0
10. Psychodrama 1 1 1.0
11. Not Specified 1 6 .17
12. Control 8 287 .20
Note: From Psychopathy and Therapeutic Pessimism: Clinical Lore or
Clinical Reality? by R. Salekin, 2002, Clinical Psychology Review, 22, p. 79-
112. Elsevier Science, Inc.

Rice, Harris and Cormier (1992) conducted a retrospective evaluation

of a peer operated, intensive group therapy (80 hours per week), therapeutic

community that ran from 1968 - 1978. They measured the criminal recidivism

of the patients involved in the study and discovered that psychopaths

(defined by Hares PCL-R M = 19) had higher recidivism rates (90%) than

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
116

controls who received no treatment at all (87%) - and, worse yet, those

treated were more likely to recidivate violently.

This surprised the researchers because the program was explicitly

designed to effect positive changes in the psychopathic personality based on

a solid theoretical background provided by the existing literature, and it

provided extensive opportunities for patients to gain insight into their own

behavior and to learn to be caring and empathic (Rice et al., 1992, p. 408).

This is the study that is most typically used to support the statement

previously addressed that therapy may make psychopaths worse (Hare,

1993; Skeem et al., 2002).

Another study of the efficacy of therapeutic communities for

psychopaths was done by Ogloff et al. (1990) with psychopaths (PCL-R > 27)

in a therapeutic community. The authors concluded that psychopaths tend to

show less clinical improvement, they are less motivated in trying to change

their behaviors, and they have a higher attrition rate (p. 186). Overall, it

would seem that therapeutic communities instead of having a beneficial effect

merely facilitate a psychopaths ability to manipulate and exploit others by

placing them in interaction with one another, which fosters criminality,

recidivism, and could be a way for them to learn new ways to commit violent

crimes (Ogloff et al., 1990; Rice et al., 1992).

More recently, Skeem et al., (2002) studied civil psychiatric patients

with psychopathy and found that when these patients received more

treatment sessions (seven or more) during a 10-week period, they were

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
117

approximately three times less likely to be violent during the subsequent 10-

week period than those who received fewer (six or less) sessions. This is

quite contrary to other studies, and led the authors to conclude that

psychopaths do benefit from adequate treatment regimens.

Rockwell (1978) contends that in the absence of clear understanding

of the etiology of psychopathy, treatment of the antisocial personality is at

best empirical, and stated:

The consistent theme throughout seems to be a focus on


locking the psychopath into a stable milieu in which the
therapists countertransference is looked at in great detail.
The previous pessimism and hopelessness of therapists are
unwarranted, and yet we cannot be sure that what we do can
be shown to be helpful (p. 140).

Virtually all the research on the treatment of psychopathic patients has

suggested that those working with this group of patients will experience a

great deal of difficulty in reducing symptoms and improving the personality

deficiencies of psychopaths. As noted earlier, the research in the area of

analytic intervention is relatively scarce and has overall limited by

methodological deficiencies. Lee (1999) succinctly summarized the

problematic aspects of current research methodologies when he said that in

the research there were very few:

...experiments using controls and adequate follow-up periods


and too many relying upon recidivism statistics as a measure
of success, when these are notoriously inaccurate. It has also
been difficult to make comparisons between the different
studies carried out in this field, because so many of them use
different diagnostic criteria for their subjects and monitor their
progress with incompatible assessment devices (p. 25).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
118

Despite the recent interest in clinical research of effective treatment

methods for psychopathy, there is still no indication that psychology has

made any important advances in treatment of the psychopathic disorder.

However, even if effective therapeutic techniques are never achieved, the

knowledge that the field of psychology has already amassed or will obtain

in the future through continued research, will greatly assist those working

in the medical, legal, and educational, mental health professions to

develop more effective methods to manage and address the behavior and

internal world of the violent psychopath.

Shortcomings not withstanding, it still remains clear from this review

that psychopathic patients are difficult to treat in psychotherapy. However,

there is currently a growing consensus that not all psychopaths should be

considered untreatable. There are clearly gradations of the disorder that may

be amenable to some therapeutic interventions. One of the most exciting

findings of the research reviewed in this paper was that longer periods of

treatment or more frequent sessions produced better results in psychopathic

patients. Also, with the limited evidence now available, it appears that

psychoanalytic oriented treatment does appear to be effective with

psychopathic patients since they do show improvement in terms of symptom

reduction when they receive the traditional, long term therapy 3 or 4 times per

week.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
119

Chapter VII

Conclusion

Historical Overview

This review has considered the historical backdrop and theoretical

efforts that have attempted to provide an understanding of the often violent,

aggressive, and impulsive nature of the psychopath. Although Pinel first

described the psychopath in 1801 over one hundred years ago, there is today

less understanding of psychopathy than any other personality disorder.

Because of this and because of the great harm persons with this illness can

do to society psychopathy present an important and difficult challenge for the

psychological profession.

Over the past 25 years, the fields of psychology, psychiatry, social

science, and the legal professionals have developed and researched a

number of theoretical and practical hypotheses regarding the identification,

assessment, and diagnosis of the psychopathies. Most contemporary

clinicians appear to hold a view of psychopathy, perhaps influenced more by

the media rather than formal training or exposure to this population, that sees

psychopaths as simply untreatable. The psychological research literature that

has been considered in this dissertation suggests that this is not the case.

Treatments with some types of psychopaths do seem to be successful,

though such work is both difficult and expensive. This may be a truth that

society does not want to hear. If psychopathy is indeed a treatable mental

illness, then perhaps it is incumbent upon society which has assumed

custody of many psychopaths and locked them away in prisons, to afford

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
120

them access to this expensive, long term treatment to relieve their continuing

suffering.

Further Research Required

For psychologists, clearly there is a clinical and ethical responsibility to

pursue research focused on treatment interventions with these patients. This

responsibility is not simply to their potential psychopathic patients.

Psychologists also have a responsibility to the public at large, since violent

psychopaths are the most dangerous and have the highest rates of recidivism

(Harris, Rice, & Cormier, 1991). Just as clearly the society at large has a

responsibility to fund the research that will help prevent and treat this disorder

rather than simply accepting the havoc that psychopaths wreck on civil

society and later punishing the offender whose guilt is often mitigated by the

damaging abuse and developmental neglect for he had no responsibility.

Skeem, Monahan, and Mulvey (2002) state that the premise of psychopathy

as untreatable has numerous clinical implications given the scarcity of

mental health care resources, the number of legal contexts that call for

assessment of treatability, and the explosion of research on psychopathy and

violence risk over recent years (p. 577). Obviously, this premise has equally

momentous social implications that are not independent of its clinical

implications.

To this point in time, an enormous amount of resources have been

focused on creating instruments to assess for psychopathy and on clarifying

the diagnostic criteria of this disorder. Without these efforts, successful

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
121

treatment programs could not be developed. Despite this, it appears that the

current DSM-IV definition of Anti-Social Personality Disorder is rather limited

in its ability to distinguish psychopaths from other social offenders because of

its exclusive focus on behavioral criteria. This problem will hopefully be

corrected in the near future. Once the illness has been given a reliable

definition the next task for psychologists should be to engage in

methodologically sound research and the development of practical

interventions for the treatment of psychopathy.

Research directions in this are numerous. For example, Rockwell

(1978) suggested that research of the following phenomenon related to the

psychopathic disorder required further explanation, and may provide valuable

information on the development of effective treatment modalities:

1. There is a 5:1 to 10:1 preponderance of males to females in diagnosis

of psychopathy. There are close associations among male

psychopathy, female hysteria, and parental psychopathy and

alcoholism.

2. Core or primary psychopathy is a distinct syndrome with no proved

effective treatment.

3. Much of what is written about psychopathy is value-laden description.

4. There appear to be familial and environmental predicates which are

often contradictory in their proposed impact.

5. There is a burn out phenomenon affecting about 1/3 or more of

persons diagnosed as psychopathic, usually between the ages of 30

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
122

and 40.

6. There is failure to explain, in terms of learning theory, the psychopaths

peculiar insensitivity to learning and to generalization.

7. The psychopath is probably neuropsychologically deviant from

normals.

8. One can argue for evidence of significant genetic influence.

9. There seems to be an increased frequency of psychopathic behavior

in Western society as a whole. (Rockwell, 1978, p. 141).

It has been suggested by more than one professional (Kernberg, 1989;

Meloy, 2001, etc.) that the lifelong institutionalization of the psychopath,

either in psychiatric hospitals or prisons should be considered the treatment

of choice for the particularly violent psychopaths. Arguments against this,

however, are based on ethical concerns and highlight the current state of

treatment models in effect. Opposing this position, Lees (1999) argued that:

...it is unethical to keep a psychopathic patient on an indeterminate


hospital sentence, when the condition attached to release is the
amelioration of their disorder. Indeed, the system as it stands has
encouraged a high level of deception among psychopathic patients,
who are aware that if they fail to demonstrate improvement in their
psychiatric symptoms, they will languish in hospital for a far longer time
than would be demanded by legal punishment for their offence (p. 25).

In essence, psychiatric and hospitals by their very nature of treating

the psychopath as untreatable, have encouraged psychopathic patients to lie

about and minimize their behavior and to be deceptive in therapy. Thus,

partially in response to this social policy, psychopaths have developed such

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
123

an awful reputation in the mental health system that hospitals are refusing to

admit them because they consider psychopathy as untreatable (Lee, 1999).

Evaluating Treatment Options for Psychopaths

Although there is still no clear treatment of choice for psychopaths,

some of the most recent research is decidedly skeptical of the long held

belief that the psychopath is completely untreatable (Blackburn, 2000; Losel,

1998; Salekin, 2002; Skeem et al., 2002). Given all the dogma in the

literature, it is easy to understand the pessimism in treating this population.

However, Skeem, et al. (2002) cite little compelling evidence to support this

blanket assumption (p. 578), and identify two quite problematic flaws in the

research which supports the claim that psychopathy is untreatable: (a) most

studies of treatment outcome and psychopathy focus on heterogeneous

groups of individuals with antisocial personality disorder and are quite

uninformative about those with Cleckleyan traits per se; and (b) of the studies

that specifically focus on psychopathy, few are prospective, include control

groups, and describe their treatment programs (p. 579)

Blackburn also disagreed with the traditional conclusion that

psychopaths are untreatable and stated First, while classical psychopaths

have been shown to respond poorly to some traditional therapeutic

interventions, it has yet to be established that nothing works with this group.

Second, some offenders with personality disorders do appear to change with

psychological treatment (Blackburn, 1993, p. 202).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
124

Trying to research treatment effectiveness with this population is

difficult for the reasons cited above and due to some contradictory findings.

Skeem, et al. (2002) interestingly have reminded us that existing research

indicates not whether psychopaths are treatable, but how responsive select

groups of psychopaths have been to the treatments we have studied. In

PCL-R based research, the modal treatment studied is a TC and the modal

groups of psychopaths are correctional or forensic inmates (Skeem et al.,

2002, p. 581).

Why do psychopaths not improve with treatment? Perhaps not

because the treatment is inappropriate, but because it is not enough. Are

these patients not improving because they receive insufficient doses of

treatment? It is quite likely that psychopaths actually need more intensive

treatment before results (i.e. change) can be achieved (Skeem et al., 2002).

This is suggested by Salekin (2002) who found a strong correlation between

the amount and duration of treatment and what researchers considered

partially successful. Skeem, et. al. (2002) address this concern quite well

when they note that:

It is possible that psychopaths who drop out of treatment are


more likely to recidivate than those who do not drop out,
regardless of the effects of treatment. Nevertheless, if
intensive treatment is required to effectively alter embedded
personality patterns, two clear pathways to poor outcome for
psychopathy are premature dropout or termination and
provision of inadequate treatment. Identifying whether either
of these pathways characterizes psychopaths relatively poor
outcomes would have crucial implications for designing
standard outpatient care. Given the mental health care
markets emphasis on inexpensive treatment, psychopaths
are unlikely to receive outreach services or long-term

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
125

treatment. This is not a problem if treatment as usual is


ineffective for patients with psychopathy, regardless of
treatment intensity. However, if traditional psychiatric services
are effective for patients with psychopathy, particularly or only
when they are intensive, it would be prudent to provide
increased services to psychopaths to decrease this
syndromes considerable toll on society (p. 583).

Early intervention, too, seems to be a key factor in treating

psychopathy. Although not the focus of this research, Fongay and Target

(1996) inferred that intensive psychoanalytic treatment with children was

somewhat effective. Bender (1947), too, believed that preventative measures

during the early infantile stages, such as avoiding deprivation and maintaining

continuous and close relationships within a family could ameliorate the

development of psychopathy in adulthood. Marshall and Cooke (1999) also

suggested early, targeted interventions for children with both familial and

societal risk factors could reduce the risk of psychopathic development.

Regarding the clear developmental hallmarks of the psychopathy, Lee (1999)

stated:

This implies that psychopathic symptoms are detectable


before the condition gets to the adult stage and that it might
be possible to treat predispositional signs in childhood or
adolescence before they have had a chance to develop.
Indeed, there is a growing amount of evidence that childhood
conduct problems are related to adult psychopathy...it is
surprising, however, that little in the way of preventative
methods have been tried with those suffering from the
disorder (p. 26).

Given that psychoanalytic treatment is rooted in a rich history of

developmental theory, and preferably done with two or more treatment

sessions per week, it seems a likely candidate to succeed where other

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
126

methods have failed. Again, more research in this area is called for before

psychopaths are dismissed as untreatable.

In a personal communication with Salekin (personal communication,

May 13, 2003) regarding the existence of comprehensive research using

intensive psychoanalytic treatment of psychopaths he stated, I do think there

is merit in the analytic approach. I do not know of any other studies on this at

the moment and I think analytic would be hard to find given the lack of

funding for research in this area (2003). In this current environment of

managed care, it is not difficult to imagine the hesitancy in funding such a

lengthy endeavor. Yet without doubt, the victims, and their families, of

Richard Kuklinski, John Wayne Gacy, Jeffrey Dahmer, Ted Bundy, Kenneth

Bianchi, and Angelo Buono would agree there is cause for funding the proper

assessment, diagnosis, and treatment of the violent psychopath.

This study has provided a historical review of the psychological

literature regarding the clinical definition, causative etiology, and therapeutic

treatment of psychopathy. This review indicates that there is a growing

consensus that this disorder is treatable, though the most successful

treatments have required intensive, long term therapy. Because of this, I have

made recommendations for the psychoanalytically informed, long-term

treatment for persons suffering from this disorder. Again, it is only

psychoanalytic treatment that takes a person back to problems rooted in

childhood, through a process of curative experiences via the transference

and countertransference. To be meaningful, these recommendations need to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
127

be considered in relation to the social context in which they must occur since

it is the larger society that will either support and pay for them or alternatively

pay for the damage that untreated psychopaths will cause.

Is There a Moral Imperative for Treatment?

From a moral viewpoint, I believe that once a treatment is discovered

that will relieve an ailment which causes great suffering or death, it is

inhumane to withhold it. Since workable treatments for psychopathy have

been demonstrated, there exists a social responsibility and moral imperative

to treat those afflicted with disorder in order to relieve their suffering and to

prevent these persons from doing harm to civil society. Clearly, this moral

imperative is not today universally acknowledged. Currently, many millions of

people with AIDS in economically disadvantaged countries are being allowed

to die because wealthy nations choose to profiteer rather than provide life

saving medications to those will die without them. With many more people

dying of this treatable disease than all that have been killed in the wars of the

last century, is it realistic to hope that society will fund expensive treatments

for psychopaths who do not complain that they are suffering? Are not

psychopaths, when they are locked away in institutions, as invisible as the

African children with AIDS are to European and American public awareness?

In contemporary political climate that is guided in large part by

economic considerations and expediency, the more persuasive argument for

treatment is economic advantage rather than a moral imperative. Wealthy

nations began to provide AIDS medications to poorer countries when it was

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
128

recognized that the ongoing loss of population would destabilize the

governments of these nations, causing regional and global disruptions of

trade. Similarly, treatment for psychopaths will likely be funded when the

costs of not treating these individuals are more widely recognized. That this

will happen is inevitable. When it will occur is difficult to predict. However, the

range and severity of the societal problems caused by the psychopaths

behavior is far reaching, with heinous and violent crimes at the severe end of

this disorder. At some point society will recognize that these crimes are too

costly to its order and sense of meaning not to be prevented in every way

possible. That point will occur when society places a significantly greater

value upon itself than at present.

The Range of Psychopathic Behavior and Limits of Psychological Knowledge

While the scope of this problem is broad, the psychological

understanding of psychopaths is really still quite limited. What is known is that

there is an interaction of biological, cognitive, social, and object relational

factors that result in violent psychopathic behaviors. These behaviors range

from a callous disregard for the rights and feelings of others to the most

heinous criminal acts. The dramatic case examples cited in the first chapter -

Kuklinski, Gacy, Dahmer, Bundy, Bianchi and Buono - illustrate the extent of

the pain and suffering that criminal psychopaths may cause. Violent

psychopaths such as these are callous and predatory. They have no

conscience and feel no guilt about their behavior. This makes them

extremely dangerous. Luckily, psychopaths such as those depicted in this

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
129

study represent only a small percentage of criminals, perhaps 15 to 20

percent. Nevertheless these individuals are responsible for more than 50

percent of the serious crimes committed and the majority of violent crimes.

While the legal system has responded to psychopathic behavior with

more restrictive laws and more prisons, Psychology has yet to develop and

operationalize effective treatment or rehabilitation. Advances in this area

have been limited by the inability to integrate all of the knowledge currently

available concerning psychopaths into a cogent, insightful paradigm of the

truly disturbed nature of the psychopathic mind. The work of both social

scientists and clinicians has focused too primarily on the identification and

classification of psychopaths and too little attention has been given to

longitudinal studies that could delineate its etiology or comparative studies of

alternative treatment modalities.

Today, however, this picture is gradually beginning to change. Now,

thanks in large part to psychoanalytic theory, psychopathic clinical study and

research has begun to focus on juvenile delinquency, noting that the

development of adult psychopathic disorder likely has roots in childhood

development and impaired object relations. These manifest as aggressive

childhood behaviors that show a striking callousness and lack of concern for

others.

Contemporary Directions for Defining Psychopathy

In the earlier chapters I have examined how psychopathy evolved from

a morally neutral construct to a pejorative term equated with criminality. For

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
130

the past several decades, contemporary psychologists and social scientists

have struggled to make this construct more precise. Cleckleys (1941) book,

The Mask of Sanity, was a major contribution to this effort that identified

many of the observable behaviors and characteristics of the psychopaths.

Cleckley the first to recognize that psychopathy was not limited to criminals or

psychiatric patients. Successful psychopaths might also be found among

the ranks of successful businessmen or politicians. The damage the

individual may cause to society may make the crimes of typical criminal

psychopaths pale by comparison.

Cleckley provided psychologists with a list of criteria for diagnostic

purposes. This increased the visibility and awareness of psychopaths and led

to an increased number of publications and research. Robert Hare extended

Cleckleys work, confirming that psychopaths are not delusional, psychotic, or

mentally ill in the traditional sense. He demonstrated that, unlike psychotic

individuals, psychopaths are completely rational and fully cognizant of their

behaviors, know why they were engaging in them, and are freely choosing to

do so. Hare operationalized his understanding by creating an instrument to

assess psychopathy, the Psychopathy Checklist - Revised (PCL-R).

Psychopathy vs. Antisocial Personality Disorder

Because of Cleckleys and Hares work, the term psychopath became

popular in both clinical and public domains. However, not all researchers use

Clecklys criteria and, as a result, there is a great deal of literature that is

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
131

incongruent because they purport to be measuring psychopathy yet when

examined more closely the criteria for inclusion is different across studies.

With the advent of the PCL-R, general agreement was beginning to

coalesce about what characteristics psychopaths displayed, but even today

psychologists and researchers are still far from reaching consensus regarding

the diagnostic criteria which best described the disorder. Despite the

controversy surrounding the Antisocial Personality Disorder construct as it

appears in the DSM-IV, and the suggestions of numerous psychologists

about the psychopathic personality, the APA field study team further

confounded an already disorganized psychological construct by ignoring the

advocates of those calling for separate diagnostic categories for psychopathy

and Antisocial Personality Disorder.

I agree with those (Meloy, 2001; Arrigo & Shipley, 2001) who would

call for the APA field study committee to include psychopathy as a separate

personality disorder, or at least in some way differentiated it from Antisocial

Personality Disorder, in the next version of the DSM. I believe this DSM

definition of psychopathy should include recommendations on how to assess

the disorder by using the PCL-R, with cutoff scores for mild, moderate, and

severe.

In my opinion there are compelling reasons why the next version of the

DSM really needs to address the issue of Antisocial Personality Disorder as

different from psychopathy. This must be done primarily to reduce or

eliminate the diagnostic confusion among clinicians who find it difficult to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
132

differentiate between the two terms. Unless psychopaths are accurately

identified they cannot be adequately treated. Because criminals, sociopaths,

and antisocial types have all incorrectly received the label of psychopath

confusion is generated regarding what appropriate treatment an individual

should receive. This confusion sometimes extends to treatment efficacy

studies because it confounds the subject inclusion criteria.

Despite the preponderance of research to the contrary, psychopathy is

still used interchangeably with other concepts such as juvenile delinquency,

antisocial behavior, sociopathy and criminality. Many psychologists and

social scientists refer to one of the terms, but are actually describing another.

In forensic settings, Antisocial Personality Disorder is amazingly over

diagnosed by psychologists, particularly when the patient continually acts out.

Thus, while social scientists and select psychologists may have become

familiar with the psychopathy nomenclature, the fact remains that the majority

of clinicians still have little understanding about the etiology of the disorder.

This is an area where psychologists should claim some responsibility, and

begin to educate themselves and other mental health clinicians.

How Better Diagnostic Criteria Can Help Treatment - Primary Prevention

Psychoanalytic theory has much to contribute to the study and

prevention of psychopathy, because it offers a comprehensive theory of

personality development and a rich terminology for describing personality

disorders. For example, Johnson and Szurek (Johnson & Szurek, 1952)

observed that the parents of adolescents transmitted their own poorly

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
133

integrated id impulses to their children thorough conscious or unconscious

permissiveness or inconsistent parenting. Aichorn (Aichorn, 1935) identified

external factors, such as abusive parents, inconsistent parenting styles, or

separated parents that made adequate superego formation difficult or

impossible to achieve. Other contemporary writers have also argued that

there are identifiable developmental sequellae that form the foundation of the

psychopathic disorder which begins early in life. It may also be that there is

some biological predisposition to psychopathy that predisposes some

individuals with a heightened sensitivity to these negative developmental

pressures. If and when such organic correlates are identified they can be

added to the already know social environmental pressures as factors that will

help in the early identification of those predisposed to this disorder.

The problem of psychopathy needs to be addressed at three levels of

prevention. The first is primary prevention. That consists of the early

detection of those predisposed to the disorder and the application of some

treatment to prevent its initial manifestation. To accomplish this some sort of

early screening process is necessary. While the least expensive screening

process would only consider children who have demonstrated behavioral

problems, many of these would be detected only late in development and

those in lower socioeconomic levels would likely be less frequently identified.

A broad and early screening process would likely be more effective in

detecting the ongoing precipitants of this disorder while they have had only a

limited time to deform a childs ability to relate to others. Perhaps all children

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
134

should be tested as part of their regular pediatric care with Mary Ainsworths

Strange Situation (Ainsworth, 1978). Children who are insecurely attached

could be identified and their parents offered parenting classes and individual

psychotherapy if they had attachment issues. The development of these

children could then be closely monitored and those who seem to be

developing early signs of oppositional or conduct disorders could be referred

to caregivers with special training in attachment disorders. At a minimum,

these children who might develop into psychopaths could be identified at an

early age and then be given various interventions that would help them

develop their conscience and feelings for others. Such treatments at an early

age would be much more effective and less expensive than treating

psychopathy and its consequences after it has fully developed. As primary

prevention, early identification and intervention of psychopathy is required in

order to counteract the predisposition and external factors of children in

chaotic families with incompetent parenting. If biological predispositions for

this disorder are eventually demonstrated, then perhaps there may one day a

medical intervention that can supplement psychological treatments.

Hopefully, interventions during childhood may effect change before

permanent adverse personality and behavioral patterns can manifest. If the

known precipitation condition for psychopathic personality disorders can be

reduced or eliminated, particularly in those children who are insecurely

attached, significant progress may be made in the primary prevention of this

disorder. However, implementing these recommendations will require an

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
135

increase the priority American society gives to the care of children. The

chaotic family environment that fosters the development of conduct

disorders, juvenile delinquency, and ultimately psychopathy is difficult to

monitor without unpopular programs such as parental licensure. Yet, the

relationship between parents and children, particularly in infancy, is the most

important and critical to ego and superego development, the ability to

empathize, delay gratification, and modulate anxiety and affect. Societys

response, however, has been to build more prisons rather than address the

problem in its beginning stages of infancy and adolescence. Psychopaths

begin their destructive careers earlier than other offenders, and this behavior

is relatively stable across the lifespan (unlike prototypical antisocial

offenders, who experience a decline, or burn out effect, of their antisocial

behavior in their 30 - 40s), treatment interventions as early as possible are

critical.

Is such a reversal of Americas neglect of its children on the

foreseeable horizon? I believe it is. In terms of many social programs such

as universal health care and child care, the United States currently lags

behind the European democracies, however, in order to compete with them

it must continue to revise its public policies in the direction of their more

progressive stance.

Secondary Prevention

Secondary prevention of psychopathy involves preventing the

reoccurrence of its manifestations once it has initially occurred. This requires

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
136

gaining greater definitional clarity concerning these manifestations

themselves. The appropriate secondary prevention treatment requires an

intervention that is appropriate to the specific psychopathic behaviors

involved. Because psychopathic behaviors are quite variable, this may be

quite challenging. While the examples of psychopaths I presented in the first

chapter were high profile, dramatic, and extreme, many writers have

suggested that psychopaths may, in fact, exhibit unique symptomology

resistant to identification or classification by current standards of diagnosis.

While this study has focused on what some believe to be the only type of

psychopath (the violent psychopath) there may be and likely are different

kinds of psychopaths with different behavioral manifestation that require

different and specialized treatment interventions.

As an example of this diversity of psychopathic presentation, Millon

and Davis (Millon & Davis, 1998) described ten subtypes of psychopathy,

based on their premise that different people exhibit a wide variety of

psychopathic characteristics that stem from different life experiences.

Although they report many similarities among psychopaths, specifically a

marked self-centeredness and disdain for the needs of others (p. 161),

there are also a number of features that are sufficiently different to justify sub

classification. Mealey (Mealey, 1995) classified these into what he termed

primary and secondary psychopaths:

primary sociopaths are biologically contraprepared to learn


empathy and consequently demonstrate psychopathic
behaviour at an early stage, whereas secondary
sociopaths encounter a combination of risk factors such as

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
137

a large number of siblings, low socio-economic status,


urban residency, low intelligence and poor social skills.
These variables contribute to the development of
secondary sociopathy in a two-stage process involving
initially parental neglect, abuse, inconsistent discipline, and
punishment as opposed to rewards. In the second stage
children may be at a social disadvantage because of poor
social skills and may therefore interact primarily with a peer
group comprised other unskilled individuals, including
primary sociopaths (p. 534).

The idea that there may be different types of psychopathy may help

explain why there has been such difficulty in the development of the

concept, why there have been persistent problems in reaching agreement

about the assessment and diagnosis of the disorder, and why there has

been continuing difficulty in developing successful treatment programs.

Unlike other personality disorders, psychopathy may require different

treatment regimens depending upon the etiology of the disorder in particular

individuals (i.e. primary or secondary origins), as well as the type,

prominence and potential for danger that various classes of symptoms

present.

Psychoanalytic understanding of psychopathy has definitely been

enhanced by neurobiological research, and the current psychoanalytic

perspective is increasingly open to further neuroscientific evidence. Research

in this area has already led to the identification of brain functions that affect

certain cognitive and affective processes in psychopaths. While some

evidence for a genetic etiology of psychopathy has been postulated, this area

of study remains in its infancy.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
138

Table 14

Millon and Davis 10 Subtypes of Psychopaths_______


Type Description

The unprincipled psychopath This is similar to the DSM-IV Narcissistic


Personality Disorder. Behavior within
the law, and will not usually present for
clinical treatment. Generally, they exhibit
little integrity, have no social
conscience, and seemingly have no
superego functions. Their interpersonal
and family difficulties are projected onto
others, which makes their interactions
malicious and vindictive.

The disingenuous psychopath Make a superficial good impression, but


are ultimately seen as unreliable,
impulsive, and moody. This group is
similar to Histrionic Personality Disorder
in that they constantly seek attention
and excitement, and can use seductive
means to get what they want. They tend
to have shallow relationships and are
crafty and scheming.

The risk-taking psychopath If one suffers from this disorder, craving


for risk is common. Unlike other sub-
types, risk-taking psychopaths are not
highly motivated by material rewards.
They tend to act before thinking, behave
in an uncontrolled manner, and do not
take the consequences of their actions
into account.

The covetous psychopath This category is very similar to the DSM-


IV Antisocial Personality Disorder.
Those with the disorder feel that life
has not given them their due, so they
take that which they believe they have
been deprived. They exhibit acts of
theft or destruction and violate social
norms to compensate for perceived
inequities.

The spineless psychopath This category is similar to the avoidant


and dependent personality disorders,
being insecure and cowardly, but having
a desire to prove that they are not weak.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
139

Table 14 (continued).
They become violent in order to
overcome their fear and to psychically
protect themselves against unconscious
fantasies. The view of

self is helplessness, being cornered,


empty, and fragile.

The explosive psychopath These people exhibit sudden,


unpredictable rage, often directed at
family members. They lose control
without provocation at perceived
mistreatment or resentments from
others who then must be destroyed.

The abrasive psychopath The characteristics of this disorder


include the purposeful antagonization of
others, and taking pleasure in the
degradation of them. They claim to be
highly principled, and will fault others for
taking opposing views. These principles,
however, obscure their hostile and
combative character.

The malevolent psychopath This category includes those who are


ruthless, particularly when they perceive
themselves to have been mistreated in
some way. These individuals are
suspicious of others and waste no time
punishing their betrayers without feeling
guilt or remorse. Many murderers are of
this type.

The tyrannical psychopath These individuals attack those they


perceive as weak, and this excites them
a great deal. These attacks are often
brutal, intending to make their victim
fear them. They often show no mercy,
and they seek submission in their
victims who are carefully selected to
ensure compliance.

The malignant psychopath Individuals in this category seek power,


and although they try to abuse other
people, it usually does not work. Their

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
140

abuse is usually met with retaliation,


which only serves to enrage them and
thus seek retribution.

Note: From "Psychopathy: Antisocial, Criminal, and Violent Behavior," by T. Millon,


E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith, and R. Davis, 1998. New York: The Guilford Press.

Nevertheless, it is possible for dynamically oriented formulations to

incorporate these findings into the existing theoretical understanding of the

psychopathic personality structure.

Physiologically Based Treatments for Secondary Prevention

The study of psychopathy, as psychology itself, has been assisted by

emerging technologies that permitted physiological research. Studies of

psychopaths have shown possible genetic and frontal lobe deficits. This work

needs to be expanded to determine if there are physiological causes of

psychopathy that may be treated biologically.

Presently, however, there are pharmacological interventions that might

be used in conjunction with analytic treatment. According to Lee (Lee, 1999),

the most common forms of medication used with personality disordered

patients are neuroleptics, antidepressants, lithium, benzodiazepines,

psychostimulants and anticonvulsants. These medications and their intended

effects are summarized in Table 15 below. Of particular interest, when the

danger of violent behavior is immediate, there seems to be a reduction of

arousal using the neuroleptics (Faulk, 1994; Lee, 1999). I would like to

greatly emphasize, however, that medication is only one possible aspect of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
141

treating psychopathy, and should be considered complimentary to analytic

psychotherapeutic interventions.

Psychodynamic Treatment Options

The most controversial aspect of psychopathy is whether or not those

with the disorder are amenable to treatment. Although psychopathy

treatment has been viewed as unsuccessful by some (Hare, 1996), other

researchers have found that certain theoretical approaches have been

successful in reducing some of the more flagrant psychopathic

characteristics, as well as a reduction in the rates of recidivism (Salekin,

2002 ).

Psychoanalytic interventions administered in proper doses, (i.e., four

times per week or more) fared much better in achieving those reductions

than other therapies. I would like to suggest that effective treatment is

possible, but given the current limitation of resources, both financial and in

adequately trained staff, administrators need to make a commitment to

address this in order to make any headway in the provision of treatment to

this difficult population. The question of cost inevitably becomes a factor in

developing a model such as this. Given the skewed relationship between

money and psychology, it seem highly unlikely that funding for such a model

will become readily available at any point in the near future; yet, it seems

imperative that attention be focused in this direction. If it is not society will

bear the alternative and inescapable cost of the damage these severely

disturbed individuals will do by the criminal acts.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
142

Table 15

Psychopharmacological Treatments
Drug Effect

Neuroleptics Tranquilizing effect on disturbed


behavior, tension, anger, and
hostility. Can also be administered
in crisis situations.

Antidepressants MAOIs have been used in


personality disordered Individuals to
reduce anger, impulsivity, and
interpersonal sensitivity. There are
serious side effects from MAOIs,
and they should only be used after a
failed trial of lithium.

Lithium Often used with psychopaths;


reduces impulsive, explosive, and
unstable behavior. Some are neither
impulsive nor undisciplined but they
often appeared well mannered and
socially appropriate, however, they
were inwardly calculating,
manipulative, and self-serving.

Benzodiazepines Helpful during episodes of severe


disturbance or overwhelming
anxiety; also for patients with history
of aggression and/or behavioral
problems.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
143

Table 15 (continued)

Psychostimulants Reduce feelings of tension and


dysphoria; useful when psychopathic
behavior is related to adult
development of ADHD; even if there
is not a direct relationship between
ADHD and psychopathy, the two
conditions share some overlapping
genetic component.

Anticonvulsants Behavioral dyscontrol exhibited by


psychopaths could be linked to a
disorder of the limbic system;
emerged from
electroencephalography (EEG)
studies of psychopaths. The
incidence of EEG abnormality is
thought to be highest in aggressive
psychopaths with violent histories
and explosive rage.

Note: From "The Treatment of Psychopathic and Antisocial Personality


Disorders: A Review," by J. Lee, 1999. Retrieved on October 23, 2003 from
http://www.ramas.co.uk.

Personal Clinical Experience with Psychopaths

In my experience, psychopaths in most involuntary settings are, for the

most part, unmanageable by the staff of psychiatric institutions. For

example, when asked to recommend treatment interventions for a

behaviorally disruptive psychopath who had coyly been found not guilty by

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
144

reason of insanity and placed on a ward of patients with profoundly

psychotic disorders, the suggestions offered by this provider was not

encouraging to the staff. I began by noting that the few effective treatment

interventions that had been found to be successful were not likely to work

within the construct of the therapeutic community paradigm and

psychopharmacological treatments available at the institution. The most

important contribution I was able to make was to suggest modalities of

treatment that likely would not work, or might exacerbate his symptoms.

Often when one can be of no positive benefit, one can at least prevent

further harm from being done. That is the point of Hippocrates first principle

of medicine, premium non nocere, first do no harm.

Difficulties Treating Psychopaths in Clinical Settings

Ineffective treatment models are not to be solely blamed for the

inability to cure psychopathy. Provider countertransference reactions to

psychopaths, their acting out behavior, their lack of remorse, and their

manipulative and conning scams also contribute to poor treatment

outcomes. Because of this there is a prejudice against this population within

the mental health field so that even a proper diagnosis of psychopathy is not

always used as a treatment recommendation, but is interpreted pejoratively

by clinicians. In typical clinical settings, psychopaths are generally rare.

Because of this there is staff are often ignorant of the characteristics of

psychopathy, and their implications for treatment and staff interaction.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
145

Multidiciplinary teams, in my experience, are wholly unequipped to

handle a true psychopath in their midst. Such a circumstance often results in

arguments, splitting of staff, and staff being manipulated by the psychopath.

Sometimes staff unfamiliar with psychopathic behaviors will suggest

potentially dangerous interventions such as allowing the psychopath to

participate in groups with fragile and easily manipulated patients. Because of

the general lack of understanding of psychopathic behavior by many mental

health professionals who have not worked with them, I believe it is essential

that psychologists play a vital role in the education of other mental health

professionals concerning the characteristics and special treatment needs of

psychopaths in institutional settings. This would be better done through

outreach and direct teaching rather than through journal articles.

Psychologists should also participate more actively in research by applying

clinical case studies to the development of new assessment instruments.

They also have a responsibility both to their patients and to society to

investigate treatment interventions based on empirical and clinical knowledge

and to report these to others both informally and through journal articles.

Tertiary Prevention - A Need for Controlled Treatment Settings

Tertiary prevention is the amelioration of an already chronic condition

so that its ongoing symptoms are significantly reduced. This can occur even

though the underlying illness cannot be cured or eliminated entirely. In terms

of the criminal psychopathic populations who are housed in the prison system

or locked mental facilities, the goal would be reducing the behaviors which

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
146

make them difficult to control by staff and difficult to associate with by fellow

prisoners.

For tertiary prevention, I believe that there should be more

experimental treatment settings. These could be specialized units where

psychopaths can be placed to help further research. They would be located

both in the prison system and in specialized inpatient units in large mental

hospital settings. Having a number of these units in a coordinated and

centrally funded research program under federal sponsorship would go a long

way toward making a beginning to systematically investigating the treatment

options for this illness. Unless such a coordinated effort is established, the

diversity of this population and the complexity and duration of the treatment

they require is likely to frustrate any local or piecemeal attempts to examine

treatment alternatives. Since the goal of tertiary prevention is to allow these

psychopaths to be managed well in confinement thereby reducing staff

problems, there should be ample financial justification for this type of

research. What appears to be needed is some initial evidence that tertiary

prevention has financial benefits, followed by a funding request for a

comprehensive tertiary prevention research program within the federal prison

system.

An example of what might be done in tertiary prevention is Gunns

(1988)(1998) recommended that treatment focus on the select symptoms of

psychopathy such as grandiosity or manipulativeness, within the context of

the specialized units staffed with professionals trained to work with

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
147

psychopaths. The goal would be to reduce the frequency and severity of

particular symptoms. When enough symptoms were reduced, treatment

could focus on making these individuals aware of their underlying

psychodynamic conflicts.

Certainly developing a experimental therapeutic units such as that

would be costly, require time to establish, and have to recruit and retain train

staff knowledgeable about and dedicated to working with this difficult

population. The impetus for such an experimental program of tertiary

prevention with psychopaths is unlikely to come from the federal prison

management bureaucracy since they lack the confidence that such a

program will be effective. I believe that psychologists who have the expertise

in developing and scientifically validating new treatment alternatives should

be at the forefront of developing effective treatment modalities. In particular,

the American Psychological Association should strongly support this type of

research and lobby for it on a national level. Access to mental health

treatment should be the right not just of those who society favors, but also of

those who are locked away in the prisons because of the consequences of

their mental illness.

Other Psychopathic Populations

Although I mentioned psychopathic females only briefly in this study,

there is limited body of psychological literature that considers this particular

population. The trend seems, at least from my experience and conversations

with other clinicians, to diagnose psychopathic females with Borderline

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
148

Personality Disorder. This is somewhat an artifact of the present DSM for

Antisocial Personality Disorder which appears biased towards males since it

has as criteria behaviors that are less uncommon among women.

Perhaps the population of psychopaths receives the least attention

from psychologists are those psychopaths that reside in the general public,

individuals who are cunning and manipulative enough to avoid legal

involvement. Cleckley was the first to suggest that psychopathy was often

prevalent in the community and need not manifest as criminal behavior. To

describe this, he coined the term successful psychopath for those people

who achieve positions of power because of their psychopathic personality

traits. Hare (1993) also opined that politicians, doctors, lawyers, and even

university students could be psychopaths. He called these white collar

psychopaths, since they were intelligent and adept at manipulating others

without actually breaking the law. Hare estimated that 1% of the population

are white collar psychopaths, however, my experience suggest this is an

underestimation. It is ironic, then, that Antisocial Personality Disorder is so

heavily behaviorally weighted and used interchangeably with the psychopathy

construct when, in fact, it is least representative of the original concept.

Successful Psychopaths

This widespread misunderstand of the true prevalence of psychopathy

illustrates the need for further research to differentiate the variety and types

of psychopathy. By the 5th edition of his book, The Mask of Sanity, Cleckley

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
149

(1976) had already envisioned the dichotomy between the obvious and

hidden psychopaths when he said:

The true difference between them (white collar psychopaths)


and the psychopaths who continually go to jails or to
psychiatric hospitals is that (the former) keep up a far better
and more consistent outward appearance of being normal.
This outward appearance may include business or
professional careers that continue in a sense of successful,
and which are truly successful when measured by financial
reward or by the casual observers opinion of real
accomplishment. It must be remembered that even the
most severely and disabled psychopath presents a technical
appearance of sanity, often one of high intellectual
capacities and not infrequently succeeds in business or
professional activities for short periods, sometimes for
considerable periods (p. 191).

There are only a few studies, however, that examine the psychopathic

disorder in the general population. Most research has been done almost

exclusively using forensic samples. While this concentration of focus on the

criminal psychopath has increased psychological knowledge of the extreme

and obvious manifestations of this disorder, it has at the same time limited

our knowledge of the full spectrum of the psychopathic condition. Additional

research is called for on successful psychopaths because the damage they

do, while generally less dramatic, is often just as pernicious and certainly

more widespread than their better known criminal counterparts.

In one of the few studies done on successful psychopaths, Salekin,

Trobst, and Krioukova (2001) found that psychopaths in the community might

be able to delay gratification when it suit them in order to continue their

behavior successfully without legal intervention. In other words, they might

be able to adapt their behavior to avoid legal involvement or even to learn

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
150

from experience. This would be considerably different from the criminal

psychopath who is generally so impulsive that he is unable to avoid being

captured.

I believe these successfully psychopaths are far more dangerous to

society than the violent psychopaths already incarcerated or in psychiatric

hospitals, since their potential damage to the general public is much greater.

Thus, there is an immediate need to focus more research and clinical

attention on screening for psychopaths in outpatient settings. This may help

detect these dangerous people before their behavior causes harm or

escalates into acts of violence.

Successful psychopaths differ from violent psychopaths in many

ways. First, there is a difference between the type of violence perpetrated by

psychopaths and successful psychopaths or nonpsychopaths (Meloy 1988;

Hare, 2003). Psychopathic violence is more likely to be predatory, or

instrumental, since it is usually motivated by an internal desire to obtain an

object or goal. The act is often callous and calculated with little or no emotion.

Psychopaths of this type often elicit that piloerection response in therapists,

and dynamically, their violent behavior may represent an attempt to dominate

or control.

Second, in contrast to successful psychopaths the victims of violent

psychopaths are usually not friends or family members, since they tend to be

unable to maintain long lasting relationships. Many murders and assaults

committed by nonpsychopaths have occurred during a domestic dispute or in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
151

the heat of the moment with females that they knew. By contrast, a violent

psychopaths victims are usually males that they do not know, with revenge or

retribution as the motive. This excites them and makes them feel powerful,

and inflates their self esteem.

Because these types of psychopaths are so different, simply

identifying an individual as a psychopath has limited utility with respect to

treatment. It is necessary to differentiate between the successful or white

collar psychopath and the more violent psychopath. How can this be done?

The white collar psychopath would likely present with higher scores on

Factor 1 of the PCL-R while the violent psychopath would score higher on

factor two. While both would have certain symptoms of psychopathy their

treatment goals would be dramatically different. A problem with previous

research has been a failure to distinguish between these two populations

when setting inclusion criteria for subject participation. Thus, in many of the

studies previously discussed, particularly those relying on PCL-R cutoff

scores for inclusion in treatment groups, an treatment intervention has been

administered to two very different types of psychopaths each of which

required a different treatment approach. I believe that violent psychopaths are

more amenable to cognitive behavioral therapies in which their acting out and

impulsive behavior can be addressed, while the more narcissistic, shallow,

and manipulative psychopath would benefit from psychoanalysis targeting

superego deficits.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
152

Considerations for Forensic Psychologists

Forensic psychologists have additional factors to consider in the

assessment, diagnosis, and treatment of psychopathy. Research on

psychopathy and the PCL-R have played a huge role in the assessment of

sexually violent predators (SVPs), have influenced civil commitment laws in

many states, as well as informed public policy. Using the PCL-R has shown

that a high proportion of rapists that are psychopaths. This has been used to

validate the connection between psychopathy and callous, instrumental

violence. Because of this, there is public perception that there is more

diagnostic clarity than is often the case. Psychologists must be sensitive to

this misperception and when necessary try to educate the public and the

professionals who deal with this population on the complexity of both

diagnosis and treatment issues. In particular, extreme care must be accorded

to all diagnoses. As Shipley (2001) recommended:

Forensic psychologists must also be aware of their own biases and

sensitive issues when confronted with this type of client. A diagnosis of

ASPD or psychopathy must not be found arbitrarily based on a therapists

countertransference with a difficult client. This is a label that can have

important and damaging implications that could follow the offender for life. A

psychological evaluation of an offender for a parole hearing that includes

ASPD or psychopathy can decrease the chances of early release (p. 360)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
153

Conclusion

Successful treatment interventions will require a collaborative process

between psychologists, social scientists, and policy makers, in order to

implement actual clinical programs that develop from empirical investigations.

To date, there have been few programs developed from theoretical models or

from suggestions made in research studies. For this work to begun

professional organizations of those concerned with this issue must make an

effort to educate the public and lobby lawmakers regarding the need for a

coordinated, sustained program of treatment development for individuals

afflicted with this disorder.

With Meloy (1988) and Kernberg (1989) I am doubtful whether

severely disturbed psychopaths can benefit from the current treatment

approaches. Hopefully new alternatives will become available for this

population at some point in the future. Within the spectrum of psychopathy

there is considerable variability of symptomology with some individuals and

some symptoms being more amenable to interventions. This seems

particularly true of fledgling psychopaths, or delinquent children. Many have

made strong arguments that helping children develop sufficient ego and

superego functions could prevent the later development of psychopathy yet

there is no organized movement toward developing the comprehensive

prevention and treatment programs that are required. Instead, clinical

attention has bee focused largely on the identification and classification of

adult psychopaths.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
154

Future research needs to focus on characterizing how non-criminal

psychopaths are different from criminal psychopaths. Comparing these two

groups may provide a better explanation of the disorder and its etiology. Until

this is achieved successful treatment options, however well funded, are likely

to have only marginal success. Treatment is often most effective if it takes

place in either the earlier stages of a disorder or when the disorder has not

developed to its fullest potential. Thus, treatment programs oriented toward

children and program for those successful psychopaths who remain among

us in society at large may provide clues on how to help those most seriously

affected with this disorder. Future research should attempt to determine what

factors distinguish the psychopaths in the community from those in forensic

settings. Knowing this would be the key to successful secondary and tertiary

prevention programs.

This study has identified the large gaps that exist in the psychological

understanding of this disorder. It has reviewed the literature on the genetic,

psychodynamic, neurological, biochemical and sociocultural factors that may

play a role in its development I have argued that there is a compelling and

urgent need for additional research to prevent and ameliorate this disorder

and that such research must be carried out in a comprehensive, systematic

manner. To do this will require a public commitment that hopefully will arise

when there is better understanding of the suffering of both those who are

afflicted by disorder and their victims.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
155

References

Abraham, K. (1925/1935). Die Geschichte eines Hochstaplers im Lichte


psychoanalytischer Erkenntnis. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 4, 570-587.

Aichorn, A. (1935). Wayward youth. New York: Viking Press.

Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of


the strange situation. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Alexander, F. (1930a). The neurotic character. International Journal of


Psycho-Analysis, 11, 292-311.

Alexander, F. (1930b). The psychoanalysis of the total personality: The


application of Freud's theory of the ego to the neuroses. New York:
Nervous and Mental Disease Pub. Co.

Alexander, F., & Healy, W. (1935). Roots of crime: psychoanalytic studies.


Oxford, England: Knopf.

Anderson, J. (1994). Genesis of a serial killer: Fantasy's integral role in the


creation of a monster. Unpublished manuscript, IO.

APA. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-III-R


(3rd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Arrigo, B., & Shipley, S. (2001). The confusion over psychopathy (I):
Historical considerations. International Journal of Offender Therapy
and Comparative Criminology, 45(3), 325-344.

Beliak, L., Chassan, J., Gediman, H., & Hurvich, M. (1973). Ego function
assessment of analytic psychotherapy combined with drug therapy.
Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 157(G), 465-469.

Bender, L. (1947). Psychopathic Behavior Disorders in children. In J. Meloy


(Ed.), The Mark of Cain: Psychoanalytic insight and the psychopath
(pp. 63-78). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.

Benjamin, L. S. (1993). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of personality


disorders. New York: Guilford Press.

Birnbaum, K. (1909). Die psychopathischen Verbrecker. Leipzig, Germany:


Thieme.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
156

Blackburn, R. (1993). Clinical programs with psychopaths. In K. Howells & C.


R. Holling (Eds.), Clinical approaches to mentally disordered offenders.
Chichester, England: Wiley, Inc.

Blackburn, R. (1998). Psychopathy and the contribution of personality to


violence. In T. Millon, E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith & R. Davis (Eds.),
Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (pp. 372 - 392).
New York: Guilford Press.

Blackburn, R. (2000). Treatment or incapacitation? Implications of research


on personality disorders for the management of dangerous offenders.
Legal & Criminological Psychology, 5(1), 1-21.

Blair, J., & Frith, U. (2000). Neurocognitive explanations of the antisocial


personality disorders. Criminal Behaviour & Mental Health, 10, S66-
S81.

Blatt, S., & Schichman, S. (1981). Antisocial behavior and personality


organization. In C. Kayne & M. Zimmerman (Eds.), Object and Self: A
Developmental Approach. New York: International Universities Press.

Bowlby, J. (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: Their characters and homelife.


International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 25, 19-53.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy


human development. New York: Basic Books.

Brody, Y., & Rosenfeld, B. (2002). Object relations in criminal psychopaths.


International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
Criminology, 46(4), 400-411.

Bruno, A. (1993). The Iceman: The true story of a cold-blooded killer. New
York: Delacorte Press.

Carney, F. (1978). Inpatient treatment programs. In W. H. Reid (Ed.), The


psychopath: A comprehensive study of antisocial disorders and
behaviors (pp. 261-285). New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Christiansen, K. (1977). A preliminary study of criminality among twins. In S.


A. Mednick & K. O. Christiansen (Eds.), Biosocial bases of criminal
behavior. New York: Wiley.

Cleckley, H. M. (1988). The mask of sanity (5 ed.). New York: C. V. Mosby.

Conoley, J., & Impara, J. (1995). Review of Hare's psychopathy checklist


revised. Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
157

Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the Construct of Psychopathy:


Towards a Hierarchical Model. Psychological Assessment, 13(2), 171-
188.

Coriat, R. (1927). Discussion of the constitutional psychopathic inferior.


American Journal of Psychiatry, 6, 686-689.

Cox, M. (1998). A group-analytic approach to psychopaths: "The ring of


truth." In T. Millon, E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith & R. Davis (Eds.),
Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (pp. 393-407).
New York: Guilford Press.

Dalmau, C. (1961). Psychopathy and psychopathic behavior: A


psychoanalytic approach. Archives of Criminal Psychodynamics, 4,
443-455.

Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes'error: Emotion, rationality and the human


brain. New York: G.P. Putnam.

Doren, D. M. (1987). Understanding and treating the psychopath. New York:


Wiley.

Dorr, D., & Woodhall, P. (1986). Ego dysfunction in psychopathic psychiatric


inpatients. In W. Reid, D. Dorr, J. Walker & J. Bonner (Eds.),
Unmasking the psychopath: Antisocial personality and related
syndromes. New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

Douglas, J., & Olshaker, M. (Eds.). (1996). Mindhunter: Inside the FBI's Elite
Serial Crime Unit. New York: Simon and Schuster.

Douglas, J. E. (1996). Journey into Darkness: Pocket Books.

Editorial. (2003, June 3). U.S. prison population largest in world. The
Baltimore Sun, p. 12A.

Eissler, K. (1949). Some problems of delinquency. In K. Eissler (Ed.),


Searchlights on delinquency. New York: International Universities
Press.

Ellard, J. (1988). The history and present status of moral insanity. Australian
and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 22, 383-389.

Faulk, M. (1994). Basic Forensic Psychiatry (Second ed.). Oxford, England:


Blackwell Scientific Publications.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
158

Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. New York:


Norton.

Fongay, P., & Target, M. (1996). Predictors of outcome in child


psychoanalysis. Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association, 44,
27-78.

Freedman, A., Kaplan, H., & Sadock, B. (Eds.). (1975). Comprehensive


textbook of psychiatry. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, Co.

Freud, A. (1949). Certain types and stages of social maladjustment. In K.


Eissler (Ed.), Searchlights on delinquency (pp. 3 - 25). New York:
International Universities Press.

Freud, S. (1916). Some character types met with in psychoanalytic work (Vol.
14). London: Hogarth Press.

Friedlander, K. (1945). Formation of the antisocial character. Psychoanalytic


Study of the Child, 1, 189-203.

Frodi, A., Dernevik, M., Sepa, A., Philipson, J., & Bragesjo, M. (2001).
Current attachment representations of incarcerated offenders varying
in degree of psychopathy. Attachment & Human Development, 3(3),
269-283.

Gacono, C. B., & Meloy, J. R. (1994). The Rorschach assessment of


aggressive and psychopathic personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Geberth, V., & Turco, R. (1997). Antisocial personality disorder, sexual


sadism, malignant narcissism, and serial murder. Journal of Forensic
Sciences, 42(1), 49-60.

Ginsberg, A. (Writer) (2002). The iceman and the psychiatrist. In G. Monet


(Producer), America Undercover. UDA: HBO.

Gunn, J. (1988). Psychopathy: An elusive concept with moral overtones. In T.


Millon & E. Simonsen (Eds.), Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and
violent behavior (pp. 32-39). New York: Guilford Press.

Hare, R. (1970). Psychopathy: Theory and research. New York: Wiley.

Hare, R. (1993). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the


psychopaths among us. New York: Pocket Books.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
159

Hare, R. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come.


Criminal Justice and Behavior, 23(1), 25-54.

Hare, R. (2003). Hare Psychopathy Checklist - Revised (PCL-R): 2nd Edition


(Vol. Technical Manual). Toronto, Canada: MHS.

Hare, R., Hart, S., & Harpur, T. (1991). Psychopathy and the DSM-IV criteria
for antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
100(3), 391-398.

Harris, G., Rice, M., & Cormier, C. (1991). Psychopathy and violent
recidivism. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 625-637.

Harris, G., Rice, M., & Cormier, C. (1992). An evaluation of a maximum


security therapeutic community for psychopaths and other mentally
disordered offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 16, 399-412.

Hart, S., & Dempster, R. J. (1997). Impulsivity and psychopathy. In C. D.


Webster & M. A. Jackson (Eds.), Impulsivity: Theory, assessment, and
treatment (pp. 212-232). New York: Guilford Press.

Hart, S., & Hare, R. (1997). Psychopathy: Assessment and association with
criminal conduct. In D. Stoff & J. Breiling (Eds.), Handbook of
antisocial behavior, (pp. 22-35). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Heinze, M., & Paladino, G. (2000). Profiling and managing psychopaths.


Paper presented at the Forensic Mental Health Association of
California Annual Conference, Pacific Grove, CA.

Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., &Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and recidivism:


A review. Legal & Criminological Psychology, 3(1), 139-170.

Hobson, J., Aylesbury, E., Shine, J., & Roberts, R. (2000). How do
psychopaths behave in a prison therapeutic community? Psychology,
Crime & Law, 6(2), 139-154.

Jaeger, R. W., & Balousek, M. (1991). Massacre in Milwaukee: The macabre


case of Jeffrey Dahmer (1st ed.). Oregon, Wl: Waubesa Press.

Johnson, A. (1949). Sanctions for superego lacunae of adolescents. In K. R.


Eissler (Ed.), Searchlights on delinquency: new psychoanalytic studies
(pp. 225-245). Oxford, England: International Universities Press.

Johnson, A., & Szurek, S. (1952). The genesis of antisocial acting out in
children and adults. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 21, 323-343.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
160

Karpman, B. (1946). Psychopathy in the scheme of human typology. Journal


of Nervous & Mental Disease, 103, 276-288.

Kernberg, O. F. (1975). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism.


New York: Jayson Aronson.

Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe Personality Disorders: Psychotherapeutic


Strategies. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Kernberg, O. F. (1989). The narcissistic personality disorder and the


differential diagnosis of antisocial behavior. In J. Meloy (Ed.), The
Mark of Cain: Psychoanalytic insight and the psychopath (pp. 315-
337). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.

Kernberg, O. F. (1992a). Aggression in personality disorders and


perversions. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Kernberg, O. F. (1992b). Psychopathic, paranoid and depressive


transferences. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 73(1), 13-28.

Kernberg, O. F. (1998). The psychotherapeutic management of psychopathic,


narcissistic, and paranoid transferences. In T. Millon & E. Simonsen
(Eds.), Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior, (pp.
372-392). New York: Guilford Press.

Koch, J. (1891). Die psychopathischen Minderwertigkeiten. Ravensburgh,


Germany: Maier.

Kohut, H. (1971). Analysis of the self. New York: Universities Press.

Kraepelin, E. (1887/1904). Psychiatrie: Ein lehbruch. Leipzig, Germany:


Barth.

Kraepelin, E., & Quen, J. M. (1990). Psychiatry: A textbook for students and
physicians (Vol. 1). Canton, MA: Science History Publications/U.S.A.

Leaff, L. (1978). The antisocial personality: Psychodynamic implications. In


W. Reid (Ed.), The psychopath: A comprehensive study of antisocial
disorders and behaviors. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Lee, J. (1999). The Treatment of Psychopathic and Antisocial Personality


Disorders: A Review. Retrieved November, 17, 2003, from
http://www.ramas.co.uk/

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
161

Losel, F. (1998). Treatment and management of psychopaths. In D. Cooke,


A. Forth & R. Hare (Eds.), Psychopathy: Theory, research and
implications for society. London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Lykken, D. T. (1957). A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality.


Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 55, 6-10.

Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence


Erlbaum Associates.

Lynam, D. R. (1996). Early identification of chronic offenders: Who is the


fledgling psychopath? Psychological Bulletin, 120(2), 209-234.

Marshall, L. A., & Cooke, D. J. (1999). The childhood experiences of


psychopaths: A retrospective study of familial and societal factors.
International Journal of Personality Disorders, 13(3), 211-225.

Martens, W. H. J. (2000). Antisocial and psychopathic personality disorders:


Causes, course, and remission--a review article. International Journal
of Offender Therapy & Comparative Criminology, 44(A), 406.

McCord, W. (1982). The psychopath and milieu therapy: A longitudinal study.


New York: Academic Press.

McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1956). Psychopathy and delinquency. New York:
Grune and Stratton.

McCord, W., & McCord, J. (1964). The psychopath: An essay on the criminal
mind. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.

McWilliams, N. (1994). Psychoanalytic diagnosis: Understanding personality


structure in the clinical process. New York: Guilford Press.

Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary


model. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 18, 523-599.

Mednick, S., Pollock, V., Volavks, J., & Gabrielli, W. (1982). Biology and
violence. In M. Wolfgang & N. Weiner (Eds.), Criminal Violence.
Beverly Hills: Sage.

Meloy, J. (1988). The psychopathic mind: Origins, dynamics, and treatment.


Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.

Meloy, J. (1996). The dangerous psychopath. On Psychiatry: Audio Digest


Foundation.

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
162

Meloy, J. (2001). The mark of Cain: Psychoanalytic insight and the


psychopath. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.

Meloy, J. (2002). The "polymorphously perverse" psychopath: Understanding


a strong empirical relationship. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 66(3),
273-289.

Methvin, S. W. (1997). Women's choices and the risk of poverty: Case


studies of breaking the cycle. New York: Garland Pub.

Michaud, S. G., Aynesworth, H., & Bundy, T. (2000). Ted Bundy:


Conversations with a killer. Irving, TX: Authorlink Press.

Millon, T., & Davis, R. D. (1998). Ten subtypes of psychopathy. In T. Millon,


E. Simonsen, Birket-Smith, M. Davis & R. D (Eds.), Psychopathy-
Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (pp. 161-170). New York:
Guilford Press.

Millon, T., Simonsen, E., Birket-Smith, M., & Roger, D. (1998). Psychopathy:
Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior. New York: Guilford Press.

Mitchell, D., & Blair, J. (2000). State of the art: Psychopathy. Psychologist,
13(7), 356-360.

Monahan, J. (1998). Major mental disorders and violence to others. In D.


Stoff, J. Breiling & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior
(pp. 92-100). New York: John Wiley.

Muntz, A. (1998). Object relations and defense mechanisms of psychopathic


and nonpsychopathic female offenders: A descriptive study (Vol. UMI
No. 9933992): Dissertation Abstracts International.

Nesca, M., Dalby, J. T., & Baskerville, S. (1999). Psychosocial profile of a


female psychopath. American Journal of Forensic Psychology, 17(2),
63-77.

Newton, M. (2000). The encyclopedia of serial killers. New York: Checkmark


Books.

Norris, J. (1992). Jeffrey Dahmer. New York: Windsor Pub. Corp.

Ogloff, J., Wong, S., & Greenwood, A. (1990). Treating criminal psychopaths
in a therapeutic community program. Behavioral Sciences and the
Law, 8, 181-190.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
163

Patrick, C. J. (1994). Emotion and temperament in psychopathy.


Psychophysiology, 31(4), 319-330.

Patterson, G., DeBaryshe, B., & Ramsey, E. (1989). A developmental


perspective on antisocial behavior. American Psychologist, 44(2), 329-
335.

Pinel, P. (1801/1962). A treatise on insanity (D. Davis, Trans.). New York:


Hafner.

Pitchford, I. (2001). The origins of violence: Is psychopathy an adaptation?


The Human Nature Review, 1, 2 8 - 3 6 .

Rabin, A. (1979). The antisocial personality: Psychopathy and sociopathy. In


H. Toch (Ed.), Psychology of crime and criminal justice (pp. 322-346).
Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press, Inc.

Raine, A. (1988). Antisocial behaviour and social psychophysiology. In H. L.


Wagner (Ed.), Social psychophysiology and emotion: Theory and
clinical applications (pp. 231-250). Oxford, England: John Wiley &
Sons.

Reich, W. (1925/1972). Character analysis (V. R. Carfagno, Trans. 3rd ed.).


New York: Simon and Schuster.

Rice, M., Harris, G., & Cormier, C. (1992). An evaluation of a maximum


security therapeutic community for psychopaths and other mentally
disordered offenders. Law and Human Behavior, 1, 399-412.

Rockwell, D. (1978). Social and Familial Correlates of Antisocial Disorders. In


W. Reid (Ed.), The Psychopath: A comprehensive study of antisocial
disorders and behaviors. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Salekin, R. (2002). Psychopathy and therapeutic pessimism: Clinical lore or


clinical reality? Clinical Psychology Review, 22(1), 79-112.

Salekin, R., Trobst, K., & Krioukova, M. (2001). Construct validity of


psychopathy in a community sample: A nomological net approach.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 15(5), 425-441.

Schmideberg, M. (1949). The analytic treatment of major criminals:


Therapeutic results and technical problems. In K. R. Eissler (Ed.),
Searchlights on delinquency: New psychoanalytic studies (pp. 174-
189). Oxford, England: International Universities Press.

Schneider, K. (1958). Psychopathic personalities. London: Cassell.

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
164

Schulsinger, F. (1977). Psychopathy: Heredity and environment. In S. A.


Mednick & K. O. Christiansen (Eds.), Biosocial bases of criminal
behavior. New York: Wiley.

Schwartz, A. E. (1992). The man who could not kill enough: The secret
murders of Milwaukee's Jeffrey Dahmer. Secaucus, NJ: Carol Pub.
Group.

Scogan, T. (2000). Assessing psychopathy with the MMPI-2: The


development of a supplementary scale (Vol. UMI No. 3008560):
Dissertation Abstracts International.

Shapiro, D. (1965). Neurotic styles. New York: Basic Books.

Shipley, S. (2001). Attachment, psychopathy, and women who commit


predatory homicide: A case study investigation (Vol. UMI No.
3043009): Dissertation Abstracts International.

Siever, L., Klar, H., & Coccaro, E. (1985). Biological response styles: Clinical
implications.

Skeem, J., Monahan, J., & Mulvey, E. (2002). Psychopathy, treatment


involvement, and subsequent violence among civil psychiatric patients.
Law and Human Behavior, 26(6), 577-603.

Smith, R. J. (1978a). Personality and psychopathology: A series of


monographs, texts, and treatises. New York: Academic Press.

Smith, R. J. (1978b). The psychopath in society. New York: Academic Press.

Stone, M. H. (1993). Abnormalities of personality: Within and beyond the


realm of treatment. New York: W. W. Norton & Co, Inc.

Strasburger, L. (1986). The treatment of antisocial syndromes: the therapist's


feelings. In W. H. Reid, D. Dorr, J. Walker & J. Bonner (Eds.),
Unmasking the psychopath: Antisocial personality and related
syndromes. New York: W .W. Norton and Company, Inc.

Strasburger, L. (2001). The treatment of antisocial syndromes: The


therapist's feelings. In J. R. Meloy (Ed.), The mark of Cain:
Psychoanalytic insight and the psychopath (pp. 297-313). Hillsdale,
NJ: Analytic Press, Inc.

Sturup, G. (1951). Krogede skaebner. Copenhagen, Denmark: Munksgaard.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
165

Suedfeld, P., & Landon, P. (1978). Approaches to treatment. In R. Hare & D.


Schalling (Eds.), Psychopathic Behaviour: Approaches to Research
(pp. 347 - 376). Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Tithecott, R. (1997). Of men and monsters: Jeffrey Dahmer and the


construction of the serial killer. Madison: University of Wisconsin
Press.

Toch, H. (1979). Psychology of crime and criminal justice. New York: Holt,
Rinehart, and Winston.

Toch, H. (1998). Psychopathy or antisocial personality in forensic settings. In


T. Millon, E. Simonsen, M. Birket-Smith & D. Roger (Eds.),
Psychopathy: Antisocial, criminal, and violent behavior (pp. 144-158).
New York: Guilford Press.

Turvey, B. E. (1999). Criminal profiling: An introduction to behavioral


evidence analysis. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.

Vaillant, G. E. (1975). Sociopathy as a human process: A viewpoint. Archives


of General Psychiatry, 32(2), 178-183.

Void, G. B., Bernard, T. J., & Snipes, J. B. (1998). Theoretical criminology


(4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

Weiss, J. (1988). Some reflections on countertransference in the treatment of


criminals. Psychiatry, 61, 172-177.

Werlinder, H. (1978). Psychopathy: A history of the concepts: Analysis of the


origin and development of a family of concepts in psychopathy.
Philadelphia: Coronet Books.

Westendorf, M. (2002). Disciplinary institutional infractions: The role of


psychopathy (Vol. UMI No. 3041294): Dissertation Abstracts
International.

Williamson, S., Hare, R., & Wong, S. (1987). Criminal psychopaths and their
victims. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 19(4), 454-462.

Wolman, B. B. (1980). The sociopathic personality. New York: Brunner/Mazel


Publishers.

Wong, S. (1984). The criminal and institutional behaviours of psychopaths.


Ottawa, Canada: Programs Branch Ministry of the Solicitor General.

Reproduced with permission o f the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
166

Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal


homicides as a function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology, 117(3), 436-445.

Yeudall, L. (1977). Neuropsychological assessment of forensic disorder.


Canadian Mental Health, 25, 7.

Yochelson, S., & Samenow, S. E. (1976). The criminal personality (Vol. 3).
New York: Jayson Aronson.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen