Sie sind auf Seite 1von 17

NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

V I E W
E
Review in Advance first posted online
R

S
on April 12, 2012. (Changes may
still occur before final publication
online and in print.)
C E
I N

A
D V A

The Attention System of the


Human Brain: 20 Years After
Steven E. Petersen1 and Michael I. Posner2
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

1
School of Medicine, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, Missouri 63110;
email: sep@npg.wustl.edu
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

2
Department of Psychology, University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403-1227;
email: mposner@uoregon.edu

Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012. 35:7389 Keywords


The Annual Review of Neuroscience is online at
neuro.annualreviews.org alerting network, executive network, orienting network,
cingulo-opercular network, frontoparietal network
This articles doi:
10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
Abstract
Copyright  c 2012 by Annual Reviews.
All rights reserved Here, we update our 1990 Annual Review of Neuroscience article, The
Attention System of the Human Brain. The framework presented in the
0147-006X/12/0721-0073$20.00
original article has helped to integrate behavioral, systems, cellular, and
molecular approaches to common problems in attention research. Our
framework has been both elaborated and expanded in subsequent years.
Research on orienting and executive functions has supported the ad-
dition of new networks of brain regions. Developmental studies have
shown important changes in control systems between infancy and child-
hood. In some cases, evidence has supported the role of specic genetic
variations, often in conjunction with experience, that account for some
of the individual differences in the efciency of attentional networks.
The ndings have led to increased understanding of aspects of pathol-
ogy and to some new interventions.

73

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

systems that could be affected by attention. The


Contents second concept is that attention utilizes a net-
work of anatomical areas. The third is that these
INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
anatomical areas carry out different functions
THE ORIGINAL NETWORKS . . . . . 74
that can be specied in cognitive terms. The
Alerting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
most unique aspect of our original article, which
Orienting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
separated it from the many excellent summaries
Executive . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
of the attention literature appearing in the An-
ELABORATIONS OF THE
nual Review of Neuroscience in the years since,
FRAMEWORK . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
is the discrete anatomical basis of the attention
Alerting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
system: divided into three networks, each repre-
Orienting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
senting a different set of attentional processes.
Executive Control . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

We believe that these important concepts are


EXTENDING THE
still operative. Here, we try to update the frame-
FRAMEWORK . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
work of our earlier Annual Review article (other
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

Self-Regulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82
summaries are available in Posner (2012a,b).
Differences in Network Efciency . . 83
In this review, we outline some of the ma-
Training . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
jor advances related to our framework that have
Evolution . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
taken place in the past 20 years. First, we rein-
FUTURE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
troduce the three original networks of the at-
tention system. We examine the nature of these
networks and how the ideas related to them
INTRODUCTION have evolved. The second part of the article
Twenty years ago, when neuroimaging was in explores additions to the original conception.
its infancy, we summarized the current state Two new networks are proposed with their
of knowledge on attention in the 1990 volume functional descriptions, and new methods for
of the Annual Review of Neuroscience (Posner & understanding interactions between them. The
Petersen 1990). At that time, most available ev- third part of the article indicates how the ideas
idence was from behavioral studies of normal have been extended to related topics, for exam-
adults or patients with varying forms of brain in- ple, in tying genetic variations to individual dif-
jury. However, the ability to image brain activ- ferences in network efciency and in examining
ity with positron emission tomography seemed the development of attention in childhood.
to hold great promise for the physiological anal-
ysis of mental processes, including attention. In THE ORIGINAL NETWORKS
our review, we were able to integrate ndings of
The three networks we described in 1990
the initial imaging studies. We never imagined
included an alerting network, which focused
that the growth of cognitive neuroscience over
on brain stem arousal systems along with
the subsequent 20 years would make it possible
right hemisphere systems related to sustained
to revisit our analysis, with 4,0006,000 imag-
vigilance; an orienting network focused on,
ing papers on attention or cognitive control and
among other regions, parietal cortex; and an
nearly 3,500 citations of our original review.
executive network, which included midline
The original review suggested three basic
frontal/anterior cingulate cortex. Each of these
concepts about attention systems. The rst is
networks is explored below.
that attention systems are anatomically sepa-
rate from processing systems, which handle in-
coming stimuli, make decisions, and produce Alerting
outputs. We emphasized the sources of the at- The concept of arousal goes back to the clas-
tentional inuences, not the many processing sic work of Moruzzi & Magoun (1949) on the

74 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

role of the brain stem reticular system in main- Alerting


taining alertness (Figure 1, for macaque brain).
As more became known of the neuromodula-
tory systems of the brain stem and thalamus,
it was necessary to qualify the general concept
of arousal into more differentiated components.
Within cognitive psychology, a major emphasis
has been on producing and maintaining optimal
vigilance and performance during tasks; this is
the sense of alertness that we discussed in our
1990 article.
One approach to the study of alerting is to
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

use a warning signal prior to a target event to Locus coeruleus:


norepinephrine
produce a phasic change in alertness. The warn-
ing cue leads to replacing the resting state with
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

a new state that involves preparation for detect-


Figure 1
ing and responding to an expected signal. If a
speeded response is required to the target, re- The locus coeruleus projections of the alerting system shown on a macaque
brain. The diffuse connections interact with other, more strongly localized
action time improves following a warning. This systems. The alerting system also includes regions of the frontal and parietal
improvement is not due to the buildup of more cortices (not shown). Reproduced from Aston-Jones & Cohen (2005).
accurate information about the target, which
is not changed by the warning signal, but the modality or location. Although the arguments
warning signal does change the speed of orient- in the original review included discussion of
ing attention and thus responding to the signal. the pulvinar and the superior colliculus, most
Several other methods have been used to of our focus was on visual selection and on the
study tonic alertness. These include changes parietal cortex as part of a posterior attention
over the course of the day (circadian rhythm). system (Figure 2a). Consensus in the imaging
Reaction times are usually longer in the early literature now indicates that frontal as well as
morning and decline over the course of the day posterior areas are involved in orienting. For
only to rise again during the night and peak in example, human and animal studies have impli-
the early morning (Posner 1975). These mea- cated the frontal eye elds (FEF) in this process
sures reect other diurnal changes such as body (Corbetta et al. 1998, Thompson et al. 2005).
temperature and cortisol secretion. A long es- In addition, parietal areas have been impli-
tablished approach to tonic alertness is to use cated in related forms of processing. This pro-
a long and usually rather boring task to mea- cessing can be concrete as in the specication
sure sustained vigilance. Some of these tasks of directed motor or eye movements (Lindner
have grown out of the job of radar opera- et al. 2010) or more abstract as movements
tors looking for near threshold changes over across a number line (Hubbard et al. 2005). In
long periods of time. Vigilance tasks rely heav- fact, the specicity of parietal regions in terms
ily on mechanisms of the right cerebral cortex of sensory versus motor processing is a major
(Posner & Petersen 1990). Both classical lesion point of contention. Nonetheless, most would
data and more recent imaging data conrm that agree the functions of the parietal lobe are not
tonic alertness is heavily lateralized to the right restricted to orienting to sensory stimuli but in-
hemisphere. volve other related processes.

Orienting Executive
The orienting network is focused on the In our original article, the third major system
ability to prioritize sensory input by selecting a was presented under the heading of target

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 75

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

b Executive control
dACC/msFC mCC
a Orienting
Precuneus Precuneus
aPFC
IPS/SPL FEF
Thalamus Thalamus

dFC IPS dFC


dlPFC dlPFC
IPL
aPFC aPFC
TPJ VFC
(IPL/STG) (IFg/MFg) al/fO al/fO
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

Dorsal attention system: Frontoparietal control system:


top-down visuospatial moment-to-moment task
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

Ventral attention system: Cingulo-opercular system:


bottom-up reorienting task set maintenance

c Grouping of regions using resting state functional connectivity MRI

Figure 2
(a) The dorsal and ventral orienting networks (after Corbetta & Shulman 2002). The dorsal attention network (light green) consists of
frontal eye elds (FEF) and the intraparietal sulcus/superior parietal lobe (IPS/SPL). The ventral attention network (teal) consists of
regions in the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and the ventral frontal cortex (VFC). (b) Two networks of the executive control system.
The circled region indicates the original member of the executive control system from Posner & Petersen (1990). The remaining
regions come from the elaboration of the original cingulo-opercular system (black) and the addition of the frontoparietal system ( yellow)
(adapted from Dosenbach et al. 2007). (c) Resting-state correlation reecting separate control systems. The gure illustrates three views
of the brain (left, dorsal view; middle, tilted lateral view; right, medial view). These separable resting networks are consistent with the
distinctions based on functional criteria exhibited in panels a and b: dorsal attention ( green), ventral attention (teal ), cingulo-opercular
(black), frontoparietal ( yellow) (adapted from Power et al. 2011).

detection. The main reason for this was not to the limited capacity of the attention system,
that target detection itself is a major atten- and to awareness itself, and has often been
tional process, but that the moment of target called focal attention. One might think of focal
detection captures awareness in a very specic attention as the entry to the conscious state,
way. Although it is possible to monitor for which may involve widespread connections
targets in many processing streams without from the midline cortex and the anterior cingu-
too much difculty, the moment of target late cortex (ACC) (Figure 2b) to produce the
detection produces interference across the global work space frequently associated with
system, slowing detection of another target consciousness (Dehaene & Changeux 2011).
(Duncan 1980). This set of processes is related We associated target detection and awareness

76 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

of the target with the medial frontal cortex and described above. There has also been a signi-
the adjacent ACC. This brain region has been cant amount of elaboration or evolution of the
highly studied by imaging experiments partly ideas during that timeframe. The next three
because of its frequent activation. sections review some of the studies deepening
Although one of us (S.P.) has vacillated sig- or expanding our understanding of the original
nicantly on this original idea over the past networks.
20 years, it seems that the idea is still relevant.
One of the reasons is that the ACC and related
regions have been reliably activated when there Alerting
is conict [e.g., a requirement to withhold a Our understanding of the physiology and
dominant response to perform a subdominant pharmacology underlying the alerting system
response (Botvinick et al. 2001)]. The argument has changed signicantly. For example, strong
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

has been extended to include a role for these evidence relates the neuromodulator nore-
areas in the regulation of both cognition and pinephrine (NE) to the alerting system. A warn-
emotion (Bush et al. 2000). ing signal is accompanied by activity in the lo-
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

The most compelling argument for a focal cus coeruleus, the source of NE (Aston-Jones
attention explanation comes from the activity & Cohen 2005). Warning-signal effects can be
found in the medial frontal/anterior cingulate blocked by drugs such as guanfacine and clono-
in such diverse operations as perception of dine, which decrease NE release (Marrocco &
either physical (Rainville et al. 1997) or social Davidson 1998). Drugs that increase NE re-
(Eisenberger et al. 2003) pain, processing of lease can also enhance the warning-signal ef-
reward (Hampton & ODoherty 2007), moni- fect. The NE pathway includes major nodes in
toring or resolution of conict (Botvinick et al. the frontal cortex and in parietal areas relating
2001), error detection (Dehaene et al. 1994), to the dorsal but not the ventral visual pathways
and theory of mind (Kampe et al. 2003). These (Morrison & Foote 1986).
different demands all activate this region, in To examine the specicity of these effects
most cases in conjunction with the anterior to the warning signal, researchers used a cued
insula. Some investigators advocate a separate detection task with humans, monkeys, and
role for the system for each of the comparisons rats (Beane & Marrocco 2004, Marrocco &
above (e.g., as part of a pain or reward system), Davidson 1998) to separate information about
but, as we argue below, we support a more where a target will occur (orienting) from when
comprehensive view that captures more of it will occur (alerting). To accomplish this, one
the results, including focal attention and the of four cue conditions was presented prior to
regulation of processing networks. Since the a target for a rapid response. By subtracting a
original article, this network has also taken double cue condition, where the participant is
on an even more extensive role in executive informed of when a target will occur but not
control on the basis of ndings showing multi- where, from a no cue condition, they receive a
ple top-down control signals in these regions. specic measure of the warning inuence of the
This more complex functional and anatomical signal. When the cue that indicates the targets
network is discussed in the executive control location is subtracted from an alerting cue,
section below. the difference represents effects of orienting.
Results of drug studies with humans and
monkeys both show that NE release inuences
ELABORATIONS OF THE alerting effects, whereas drugs inuencing
FRAMEWORK the neuromodulator acetylcholine (Ach) affect
The intervening 20 years since our original orienting but not alerting. Studies have shown
article have produced a surprising amount of that individual differences in alerting and
support for the basic outlines of the framework orienting are largely uncorrelated (Fan et al.

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 77

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

2002) and that orienting improves to the same event-related fMRI, Corbetta & Shulman
degree with a cue regardless of the level of (2002) showed that two brain systems are re-
alertness. These results suggest a great deal lated to orienting to external stimuli as illus-
of independence between these two functions trated in Figure 2a. A more dorsal system in-
(Fernandez-Duque & Posner 1997). However, cluding the FEFs and the interparietal sulcus
these systems usually work together in most followed presentation of an arrow cue and was
real-world situations, when a single event often identied with rapid strategic control over at-
provides information on both when and where tention. When the target was miscued, subjects
a target will occur (Fan et al. 2009). had to break their focus of attention on the cued
The changes during the time between warn- location and switch to the target location. The
ing and target reect a suppression of ongo- switch appeared to involve the temporoparietal
ing activity thought to prepare the system for junction (TPJ) and the ventral frontal cortex
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

a rapid response. In the central nervous sys- and was identied with the interrupt signal that
tem there is a negative shift in scalp-recorded allowed the switch to occur.
EEG, known as the contingent negative varia- The dorsal system included the well-studied
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

tion (CNV) (Walter 1964), which often begins parietal regions but added a small set of frontal
with the warning signal and may remain present locations as well, particularly in the FEFs.
until the target presentation. This negative po- Some have argued that covert attention shifts
tential appears to arise in part from the ante- are slaved to the saccadic eye movement system
rior cingulate and adjacent structures (Nagai (Rizzolatti et al. 1987), and neuroimaging
et al. 2004) and may overlap the event-related studies using fMRI have shown that covert and
response to the warning stimulus. The negative overt shifts of attention involve similar areas
shift may remain present as a standing wave over (Corbetta et al. 1998). However, single-unit
the parietal area of the contralateral hemisphere physiology studies in the macaque suggest
(Harter & Guido 1980). If the target interval is important distinctions at the level of cell
predictable, the person may not show the CNV populations, with some cells in the FEFs active
until just prior to target presentation. during saccades and a distinct but overlapping
An extensive imaging study (Sturm & population of cells involved in covert shifts of
Willmes 2001) showed that a largely common attention (Schafer & Moore 2007, Thompson
right hemisphere and thalamic set of areas are et al. 2005). The cells responsible for covert
involved in both phasic and tonic alerting. An- shifts of attention also seem to hold the location
other imaging study, however, suggested that of cues during a delay interval (Armstrong et al.
the warning signal effects rely more strongly 2009). The two populations of cells are mixed
on left cerebral hemisphere mechanisms (Coull within the FEFs and, at least to date, have not
et al. 2000, Fan et al. 2005). This could rep- been distinguished by fMRI. However, the
resent the common ndings described above physiological data indicate that covert attention
on hemispheric differences in which right lat- is distinct from the motor system governing
eralized processes often involve slower effects saccades, even though they clearly interact.
(tonic), whereas left hemisphere mechanisms As suggested by the FEF studies, it is impor-
are more likely to be involved with higher tem- tant to be able to link the imaging and physi-
poral (phasic) or spatial frequencies (Ivry & ological results with other studies to provide
Robertson 1997). The exact reasons for differ- more details on local computations. One strat-
ences in laterality found with tonic and phasic egy for doing so is to study the pharmacology
studies are still unknown. of each of the attention networks. Cholinergic
systems arising in the basal forebrain appear to
Orienting play a critical role in orienting; lesions of the
In a series of imaging experiments us- basal forebrain in monkeys interfere with ori-
ing cuing methodology in combination with enting attention (Voytko et al. 1994). However,

78 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

it appears that the site of this effect is not in between modalities. In many cases, orienting
the basal forebrain per se, but instead involves to a location will provide priority not only to
the superior parietal lobe. Davidson & Mar- the expected modality but also to information
rocco (2000) made injections of scopolamine present at the same location from other modal-
directly into the lateral intraparietal area of ities (Driver et al. 2004), indicating how closely
monkeys. This area corresponds to the human the sensory systems are integrated within the
superior parietal lobe and contains cells inu- orienting network.
enced by cues about spatial location. The in- How can the sources of the orienting net-
jections have a large effect on the monkeys work described above inuence sensory com-
ability to shift attention to a target. Systemic putations? Anatomically, the source of the ori-
injections of scopolamine, an anticholinergic, enting effect lies in the network of parietal,
have a smaller effect on covert orienting of at- frontal, and subcortical areas mentioned above.
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

tention than do local injections in the parietal However, the inuence of attention is on the
area. Cholinergic drugs do not affect the abil- bottom-up signals arriving in sensory-specic
ity of a warning signal to improve the mon- areas: for vision, in the primary visual cortex
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

keys performance, so there appears to be a dou- and extrastriate areas moving forward toward
ble dissociation, with NE involved mainly in the temporal lobe. That this remote inuence
the alerting network and Ach involved in the involves synchronization between activity in the
orienting network. These observations in the more dorsal attention areas and activity in the
monkey have been conrmed by similar studies more ventral visual areas is an inuential idea
in the rat (Everitt & Robbins 1997). It is es- (Womelsdorf et al. 2007). The synchronization
pecially signicant that comparisons in the rat apparently leads to greater sensitivity in the vi-
studies of cholinergic and dopaminergic mech- sual system, allowing faster responses to visual
anisms have shown that only the former inu- targets and thus improved priority for process-
ence the orienting response (Everitt & Robbins ing targets.
1997, Stewart et al. 2001). In addition to synchronization, single-unit
The more ventral network including the physiology studies conducted within ventral vi-
TPJ (Figure 2a) seemed to be more active fol- sual areas suggest that as items are added to a
lowing the target and was thus identied as part visual scene they tend to reduce the ring rate of
of a network responsive to sensory events. It cells responding to the target stimulus. Atten-
is strongly right lateralized and lesions in this tion to a target seems to reduce the inuence of
area are central to the neglect syndrome, al- other competing stimuli. This idea was impor-
though the interaction of TPJ with more frontal tant in the development of biased competition
and dorsal brain areas is also critical (Shulman theory (Desimone & Duncan 1995). This the-
& Corbetta 2012). Researchers generally agree ory sees attention as arising out of a winner-
about the membership of the major nodes of the take-all competition within various levels of
orienting network on the basis of both spatial sensory and association systems. fMRI studies
cuing and visual search studies (Hillyard et al. conrm that attention to a stimulus can occur
2004, Wright & Ward 2008). prior to its arrival, changing the baseline neu-
Perhaps more surprising is that the brain ral and blood oxygen leveldependent (BOLD)
areas involved in orienting to visual stimuli response, and that the overall BOLD activity is
seem to overlap strongly (within fMRI res- affected in ways consistent with the biased com-
olution) with those involved with orienting petition theory (Desimone & Duncan 1995).
to stimuli in other modalities (Driver et al.
2004). Although attention operates on sensory-
specic modalities according to the incoming Executive Control
target, the sources of this effect appear to be Our third original network has been elabo-
common. There are also important synergies rated considerably. As noted above, our original

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 79

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

focus was on midline regions of the medial transient signals at the beginning of blocks,
frontal cortex and anterior cingulate. We sug- whereas medial frontal/cingulate cortex and bi-
gested that activity found during the perfor- lateral anterior insula also showed sustained
mance of tasks was related to focal attention be- maintenance signals across task conditions. Al-
cause trial-related activity in these regions was though these experiments identied a set of re-
greater for targets than for nontargets, for con- gions that could be involved in top-down task
ict more than for nonconict trials, and for level control, they provide no evidence of the
errors more than for correct trials. We argued relationships between regions.
that such a system might be very useful for pro- Another experiment (Dosenbach et al.
ducing top-down regulation, thus its relation- 2007) looked for functional correlations (at
ship to executive control. This role of the ACC rest) between regions that showed some or
in top-down control was based on rather slim all of these putative control signals, with the
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

evidence at the time but seems to still seems idea that these functional connections may
to be accurate and plays an important role in dene the systems-level relationships between
two prominent theories of executive control the regions. Lateral frontal and parietal re-
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

prominent in the current literature. One the- gions that showed primarily start-cue activity
ory stresses the role of the ACC in monitoring correlated well with each other (Figure 2c).
conict and in relation to lateral frontal areas The midline and anterior insular regions
in resolving the conict (Botvinick et al. 2001, that showed additional sustained activity also
Carter & Krug 2012). A different view arguing correlated well with each other (Figure 2c),
for two different top-down control networks is but these two sets of regions did not correlate
based on extensive studies of the specic aspects strongly with each other.
of the ACC during task performance and corre- These results suggested there are two sepa-
lations with other regions at rest (Figure 2b,c) rable executive control networks. Detailed ev-
(Dosenbach et al. 2006, 2007). idence for this view is found in Dosenbach
Support for two separate executive control et al. (2008). The frontoparietal network ap-
networks arises from studies designed to dis- pears to be distinct from the orienting net-
cover signals related to top-down task control. work discussed previously, whereas the cingulo-
Such signals might include those related to task opercular network overlaps with the original
instructions that are transient at the beginning executive network. If this view is correct, there
of a task block (Figure 3). Transient block are two relatively separate executive networks.
transition signals had been seen in earlier work Although the best imaging evidence shows that
(Donaldson et al. 2001, Fox et al. 2005, Konishi the orienting and frontoparietal executive net-
et al. 2001) with many different interpretations. works are separate in adulthood, they may have
A second type of activity is sustained across a common origin in early development (see
the trials of the task, putatively related to the Self-Regulation, below).
maintenance of task parameters/top-down This breakdown of executive control into
control (Figure 3). The third type of signal is two separate networks is anatomically simi-
related to performance feedback; an example of lar to an inuential idea pertaining to cogni-
such feedback would be systematic differences tive control (Botvinick et al. 2001, Carter &
between correct and incorrect trials (Figure 3). Krug 2012). However, this cognitive control
Dosenbach et al. (2006, 2007) studied 10 view favors a single unied executive system
different tasks (including visual and auditory in which lateral prefrontal cortex provides top-
words and visual objects as stimuli, with many down control signals, guided by performance-
different decision criteria, such as semantic, monitoring signals generated by midline struc-
timing, and similarity judgments) searching for tures. Although the cognitive control view and
evidence of these signal types. Lateral frontal the ideas shown in Figure 2 are anatomically
and parietal regions appeared to emphasize similar, several specic functional differences

80 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

a start +

+ stop

Sustained

fixate fixate
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

Cue Correct Errors Cue


Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

trials

b Task-related (adjusted) Trial-related


0.4 0.4
Error
Correct

0.3 0.3

0.2 Sustained maintenance signal 0.2


% change

% change
Transient
0.1 task- 0.1
initiation
signal

0 0

0.1 0.1
1 11 21 31 41 1 7
Time (MR frames) Time (MR frames)

Figure 3
Executive control signals. The top panel shows three putative executive control signals: a task initiation signal in yellow, a task-
maintenance signal in red, and activity related to correct (black) and error (blue) trials (adapted from Dosenbach et al. 2006). Regions
showing differences in error versus correct trials are considered to be computing or receiving performance feedback. The bottom gure
shows activity in the left anterior insula during a task that contains all the putative signals (plus a transient transition signal at the end of
the block of trials). MR, magnetic resonance.

remain. In the dual network view (Dosenbach stable background maintenance for task perfor-
et al. 2008), the two executive systems act rel- mance as a whole. The frontoparietal system,
atively independently in producing top-down in contrast, showing mostly start-cue signals, is
control. The cingulo-opericular control sys- thought to relate to task switching and initiation
tem shows maintenance across trials and acts as and to adjustments within trials in real time.

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 81

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

Both the cognitive control view and the dual The addition of two separate orienting net-
networks view explain a considerable amount works and two separate executive networks
of extant data, but we believe there are several raises the possibility that additional control net-
reasons to choose the latter formulation. works will be elaborated in the future. How-
First, lesion studies in both humans and an- ever, for several reasons, we do not expect the
imals seem to indicate separate aspects of con- number of control networks to be much larger
trol. Large lesions of the frontal midline of- than the number described here. The study
ten result in akinetic mutism in which people of many complex systems, from ecosystems to
are capable of carrying out goal-directed activ- protein-protein interactions, seems to indicate
ities but do not do so. On the other hand, pa- that these systems follow a rule of hand and
tients with more laterally placed lesions, includ- have approximately ve controlling variables
ing the dorsolateral prefronal cortex (DLPFC) (ranging from three to seven) (Gunderson &
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

often exhibit perseverations with an inability to Holling 2002). For example, the maintenance
switch from one set to the other. In a com- of upright balanced posture appears to be con-
pelling set of macaque experiments, Rossi et al. trolled by at least three separate systems: vi-
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

(2007) showed that a complete unilateral resec- sion, the vestibular system, and kinesthetic joint
tion of the DLPFC and an interruption of the sensors. These systems act relatively indepen-
corpus callosum resulted in a unilateral inabil- dently and have different spatial and temporal
ity to switch sets but an intact ability to adopt a characteristics. From this perspective, the pres-
sustained set, consistent with the human lesion ence of ve relatively separate attention net-
data. works appears reasonable. A second argument
A second difference between the dual net- in favor of this view is an empirical one. In
work and cognitive control views is concerned a recent large-scale study of resting state net-
with the directionality of relationships. The works (Power et al. 2011), with effectively all
cognitive control view requires a timing dif- the brain represented, all the cortical networks,
ference between the midline monitoring pro- found by the more piecemeal approaches de-
cesses and the DLPFC implementation regions scribed above, are present.
within a trial. The two-network account is tol-
erant of ordering effects because the two net-
works operate separately. Two quite different EXTENDING THE FRAMEWORK
sets of data argue that cingulo-opercular in- One of the gratifying outcomes of our original
volvement is often at the end of or after the publication has been the many ways that these
trial. The rst is from studies of single-unit ideas inspired a large number of studies. We
activity in the ACC in macaques (Ito et al. review extensions of the framework into new
2003). During a saccade countermanding task, areas related to attention networks.
investigators found neurons that signaled er-
rors and unexpected rewards after trial com-
pletion. Second, a recent human imaging study Self-Regulation
by Ploran et al. (2007) used a slow reveal task. The ability to control our thoughts, feelings,
During visual information processing, activ- and behavior in developmental psychology
ity progressively increased with increasing vi- is called self-regulation; with adults it is
sual information across several seconds in the often called self-control (see sidebar on Will,
DLPFC. This preceded late activity in the ACC Self-Regulation, and Self-Control for further
and anterior insula. These results are consistent denitions). Neuroimaging presents strong
with the hypothesis that the ACC may often evidence that conict tasks such as the Stroop
serve to monitor the consequences of actions, effect activate common areas of the anterior
and they are inconsistent with a more rigid cingulate gyrus: the dorsal portion for more
directionality. strictly cognitive tasks and the ventral area for

82 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

emotion-related tasks (Botvinick et al. 2001, 2011). These ndings show that self-regulation
Bush et al. 2000). Although the cingulate is a psychological function crucial for child so-
anatomy is much more complex, the division cialization, and they suggest that it can also be
into cognitive and emotion-related areas has studied in terms of specic anatomical areas and
been supported by more detailed anatomical their connections by examining the develop-
studies (Beckmann et al. 2009). ment of executive attention networks.
Support for the voluntary exercise of self-
regulation comes from studies that examine ei- Differences in Network Efficiency
ther the instruction to control affect or the con- Although everyone has the attention net-
nections involved in the exercise of that control. works described above, there are also individual
For example, the instruction to avoid arousal
during processing of erotic events (Beauregard
WILL, SELF-REGULATION, AND
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

et al. 2001) or to ward off emotion when look-


ing at negative pictures (Ochsner et al. 2002) SELF-CONTROL
produces a locus of activation in midfrontal
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

and cingulate areas. If people are required to Several names have been applied to the voluntary control of
select an input modality, the cingulate shows emotion and cognition. During child development, these func-
functional connectivity to the selected sensory tions are often called self-regulation. This name provides a clear
system (Crottaz-Herbette & Menon 2006) and contrast to the regulation that occurs through the caregiver or
in emotional tasks to limbic areas (Etkin et al. other external sources. In adults, the same set of voluntary func-
2006). tions is frequently called self-control. Regulation may also oc-
Both behavioral and resting state functional cur through nonvoluntary means, for example, by fear or by the
data suggest substantial development of the ex- calming aspects of drugs or therapy. In all cases, self-control or
ecutive attention network between infancy and self-regulation appears to be an ability to control reexive or oth-
childhood. A study of error detection in seven- erwise dominant responses to select less dominant ones.
month-old infants and adults (Berger et al. Conict tasks: The Stroop effect involves the conict between
2006) shows that both ages use the anterior cin- the task of naming the color of ink of conicting color names
gulate area, but the usual slowing following an (e.g., the word GREEN presented in RED INK). The Stroop
error does not seem present until about three and other conict-related tasks have been used to measure the
years of age ( Jones et al. 2003). We recently ability to select the less dominant response. Because the classic
proposed (Posner et al. 2012, Rothbart et al. Stroop effect requires reading, other conict tasks such as spa-
2011) that during infancy control systems de- tial conict, anker conict, and pictorial conict have also been
pend primarily on the orienting network as de- used. Imaging studies with adults suggest that the conict in these
scribed previously. During later childhood and tasks have a common anatomy (Fan et al. 2003a).
into adulthood, the time to resolve conict cor- Anatomy: The use of imaging has provided some evidence
related with parent reports of their childs abil- of a common brain network that is involved in all these senses
ity to control his or her behavior (effortful con- of control. This network includes anterior cingulate (Bush et al.
trol, EC) (Posner & Rothbart 2007, Rothbart 2000) and anterior insula (Dosenbach et al. 2007; Sridharan et al.
et al. 2011). The correlation between conict 2007, 2008) and also includes areas of the prefrontal cortex when
scores and parent reports of EC form one ba- inhibition of dominant responses is a strong feature (Fan et al.
sis for the association between self-regulation 2003a). The common involvement of the anterior cingulate in
and executive attention. EC is also related to attention and both emotion and cognitive control has provided
the empathy that children show toward others one basis for the argument that the executive attention network is
and their ability to delay an action as well as to critical to these various functions. The brain activation of conict-
avoid such behaviors as lying or cheating when related tasks such as the Stroop has also been common to studies
given the opportunity. High levels of EC and of attention and aspects of control.
the ability to resolve conict are related to fewer (Continued )
antisocial behaviors in adolescents (Rothbart

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 83

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

attention networks, takes advantage of the


Age: Self-regulation has been a concept used mainly in de- association between different neuromodulators
velopmental psychology, whereas the terms cognitive control, and attention networks to examine specic
self-control, and willpower are usually applied to adults. There genetic alleles (e.g., related to dopamine)
appears to be no strict dividing line. A new nding is the impor- to examine individual performance on the
tant role of the orienting system in providing some of the control appropriate network (see Green et al. 2008 for
in infants and in young children (Posner et al. 2012, Rothbart review). As one example, the ANT has been
et al. 2011). Even in adults, no doubt orienting to new sensory used to examine individual differences in the
stimuli or thoughts can be a self-control mechanism. efciency of executive attention. A number
Future: The much broader term executive function is applied of polymorphisms in dopamine and serotonin
in psychology to self-control as well as the ability to solve prob- genes have been associated specically with
lems, shift tasks, plan ahead, and implement goals. Although con- the scores on executive attention (Green et al.
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

ict resolution has been studied widely with normals, the anatomy 2008). This work is still just getting started,
of other functions remains to be thoroughly explored. and reports are conicting. One reason for the
conict may be that genetic variations are also
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

inuenced by environmental factors.


differences in the efciency of all brain net- Genetic modulation by environmental fac-
works. The Attention Network Test (ANT) has tors is perhaps clearest for the dopamine 4 re-
been used to examine the efciency of atten- ceptor gene (DRD4), which has been associated
tion networks (Fan et al. 2002). The task re- with the executive network in adult imaging
quires the person to press one key if the cen- studies (Fan et al. 2003b). Data at 1820 months
tral arrow points to the left and another if it showed that quality of parenting interacted with
points to the right. Conict is introduced by the 7 repeat allele of the DRD4 gene to in-
having ankers surrounding the target point- uence the temperamental dimensions of im-
ing in either the same (congruent) or opposite pulsivity, high-intensity pleasure and activity
(incongruent) direction as the target. Cues pre- level, and all components of sensation seeking
sented prior to the target provide information (Sheese et al. 2007). Parenting made a strong
on where or when the target will occur. There difference for children with the 7 repeat al-
are strong individual differences in each atten- lele in moderating sensation seeking but not for
tion network and there are surprisingly low cor- those children without this allele. At 34 years
relations between these network scores (Fan of age, the DRD4 gene interaction with par-
et al. 2002), although the networks interact in enting was related to childrens EC, suggesting
more complex tasks and in everyday life (Fan that executive attention may be the mechanism
et al. 2009). for this interaction. One study found that only
Normal functions including attention are those children with the 7-repeat of the DRD4
undoubtedly inuenced by many genes in showed the inuence of a parent training inter-
complex interaction with epigenetic and envi- vention (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al. 2008),
ronmental factors. Most studies have involved suggesting that the presence of the DRD4 7
various pathologies and have not centered on repeat allele may make the child more suscep-
common human functions; hence relatively tible to environmental inuences (Bakermans-
little is known about the full range of genes Kranenburg & Van IJzendoorn 2011, Belsky &
involved in attention networks. One strategy Pluess 2009, Sheese et al. 2007). This joint in-
would be to use emerging genomic and uence of environment and genetics seems to
epigenomic technologies to carry out studies continue into adulthood (Larsen et al. 2010).
of large cohorts using various attention tasks
as phenotypes to determine genes that relate Training
to performance differences. A more limited Because parenting and other cultural factors
approach, based on what is known about interact with genes to inuence behavior, it

84 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

should be possible to develop specic training in which von Economo neurons are found (cin-
methods that can be used to inuence under- gulate and anterior insula) are also shown to be
lying brain networks. Two forms of training in close communication during the resting state
methods have been used in the literature. One (Dosenbach et al. 2007). It is not clear, however,
involves practice of a particular attention net- if the distribution of Von Economo neurons
work. Several such attention training studies and the cingulo-opercular network are overlap-
have shown improved executive attention func- ping or closely juxtaposed (Power et al. 2011).
tion and produced changes in attention-related Some evidence indicates that the frequency of
brain areas (Klingberg 2011, Rueda et al. 2005). this type of neuron increases in human devel-
The practice of a form of meditation has been opment between infancy and later childhood
used to change the brain state in a way that im- (Allman et al. 2005). These neurons may pro-
proves attention, reduces stress, and also im- vide the rapid and efcient connectivity needed
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

proves functional connectivity between the an- for executive control and may help explain why
terior cingulate and the striatum (Tang et al. self-regulation in adult humans can be so much
2007, 2009). stronger than in other organisms.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

Evolution FUTURE
The ACC is a phylogenetically old area of the It has been exciting for us to see the expan-
brain. Comparative anatomical studies point to sion of work on networks of attention over the
important differences in the evolution of cingu- past 20 years. We now have the opportunity to
late connectivity between nonhuman primates go from genes to cells, networks, and behavior
and humans. Anatomical studies show great ex- and to examine how these relationships change
pansion of white matter, which has increased from infancy to old age. In development, the
more in recent evolution than has the neo- number of active control systems increases and
cortex itself (Zilles 2005). One type of pro- their inuence changes.
jection cell called a Von Economo neuron is Although much has been learned, many
found only in higher apes and a few other so- questions remain unanswered. We are hopeful
cial species, but they are most common in hu- that the study of attention will continue
mans. In the human brain, the Von Economo to provide greater understanding of how
neurons are found only in the anterior cingu- control develops typically and in pathology
late and a related area of the anterior insula (Posner 2012a, Posner et al. 2011) and will
(Allman et al. 2005). This neuron is likely im- provide promising leads for translating basic
portant in communication between the cingu- research into interventions to aid children and
late and other brain areas. The two brain areas families.

DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
The authors are not aware of any afliations, memberships, funding, or nancial holdings that
might be perceived as affecting the objectivity of this review.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This article was supported in part by grant HD060563 to Georgia State University subcontracted
to the University of Oregon, Rothbart et al. 2011;. Prof. Mary K. Rothbart made important
contributions to the research and writing of this review. This article was also supported by NIH
grants NS32797 and 61144 and the McDonnell Foundation.

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 85

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

LITERATURE CITED
Allman JM, Watson KK, Tetreault NA, Hakeem AY. 2005. Intuition and autism: a possible role for Von
Economo neurons. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9:36773
Armstrong KM, Chang MH, Moore T. 2009. Selection and maintenance of spatial information by frontal eye
eld neurons. J. Neurosci. 29:1562129
Aston-Jones G, Cohen JD. 2005. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive
gain and optimal performance. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28:40350
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH. 2011. Differential susceptibility to rearing environment
depending on dopamine-related genes: new evidence and a meta-analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 23:3952
Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Pijlman FT, Mesman J, Juffer F. 2008. Experimental
evidence for differential susceptibility: dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates
intervention effects on toddlers externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol.
44:293300
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

Beane M, Marrocco RT. 2004. Norepinephrine and acetylcholine mediation of the components of reexive
attention: implications for attention decit disorders. Prog. Neurobiol. 74:16781
Beauregard M, Levesque J, Bourgouin P. 2001. Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

J. Neurosci. 21:RC165
Beckmann M, Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MFS. 2009. Connectivity-based parcellation of human cingulate
cortex and its relation to functional specialization. J. Neurosci. 29:117590
Belsky J, Pluess M. 2009. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental inuences. Psychol.
Bull. 135:885908
Berger A, Tzur G, Posner MI. 2006. Infant brains detect arithmetic errors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
103:1264953
Botvinick MM, Braver TS, Barch DM, Carter CS, Cohen JD. 2001. Conict monitoring and cognitive control.
Psychol. Rev. 108:62452
Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI. 2000. Cognitive and emotional inuences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends
Cogn. Sci. 4:21522
Carter CS, Krug MK. 2012. Dynamic cognitive control and frontal-cingulate interactions. See Posner 2012b,
pp. 8998
Corbetta M, Akbudak E, Conturo TE, Snyder AZ, Ollinger JM, et al. 1998. A common network of functional
areas for attention and eye movements. Neuron 21:76173
Corbetta M, Shulman GL. 2002. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat.
Rev. Neurosci. 3:20115
Coull JT, Frith CD, Buchel C, Nobre AC. 2000. Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical
distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia 38:80819
Crottaz-Herbette S, Menon V. 2006. Where and when the anterior cingulate cortex modulates attentional
response: combined fMRI and ERP evidence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18:76680
Davidson MC, Marrocco RT. 2000. Local infusion of scopolamine into intraparietal cortex slows covert
orienting in rhesus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 83:153649
Dehaene S, Changeux JP. 2011. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron
70:20027
Dehaene S, Posner MI, Tucker DM. 1994. Localization of a neural system for error detection and compen-
sation. Psychol. Sci. 5:3035
Desimone R, Duncan J. 1995. Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 18:193222
Donaldson DI, Petersen SE, Ollinger JM, Buckner RL. 2001. Dissociating state and item components of
recognition memory using fMRI. Neuroimage 13:12942
Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Cohen AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. 2008. A dual-networks architecture of
top-down control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 12:99105
Dosenbach NUF, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, et al. 2007. Distinct brain networks for
adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104:1107378
Dosenbach NUF, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, et al. 2006. A core system for the
implementation of task sets. Neuron 50:799812

86 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

Driver J, Eimer M, Macaluso E, van Velzen J. 2004. Neurobiology of human spatial attention: modulation,
generation, and integration. See Kanwisher & Duncan 2004, pp. 267300
Duncan J. 1980. The locus of interference in the perception of simultaneous stimuli. Psychol. Rev. 87:272300
Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD, Williams KD. 2003. Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion.
Science 302:29092
Etkin A, Egner T, Peraza DM, Kandel ER, Hirsch J. 2006. Resolving emotional conict: a role for the rostral
anterior cingulate cortex in modulating activity in the amygdala. Neuron 51:87182
Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. 1997. Central cholinergic systems and cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 48:64984
Fan J, Flombaum JI, McCandliss BD, Thomas KM, Posner MI. 2003a. Cognitive and brain consequences of
conict. Neuroimage 18:4257
Fan J, Fossella J, Sommer T, Wu Y, Posner MI. 2003b. Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention
onto brain activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100:740611
Fan J, Gu X, Guise KG, Liu X, Fossella J, et al. 2009. Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of
attentional networks. Brain Cogn. 70:20920
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. 2005. The activation of attentional networks.
Neuroimage 26:47179
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. 2002. Testing the efciency and independence of
attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14:34047
Fernandez-Duque D, Posner MI. 1997. Relating the mechanisms of orienting and alerting. Neuropsychologia
35:47786
Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Barch DM, Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. 2005. Transient BOLD responses at block
transitions. Neuroimage 28:95666
Green AE, Munafo MR, DeYoung CG, Fossella JA, Fan J, Gray JR. 2008. Using genetic data in cognitive
neuroscience: from growing pains to genuine insights. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9:71020
Gunderson LH, Holling CS. 2002. Panarchy: Understanding Transformations in Human and Natural Systems.
Washington, DC: Island
Hampton AN, ODoherty JP. 2007. Decoding the neural substrates of reward-related decision making with
functional MRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104:137782
Harter MR, Guido W. 1980. Attention to pattern orientation: negative cortical potentials, reaction time, and
the selection process. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 49:46175
Hillyard SA, Di Russo F, Martinez A. 2004. The imaging of visual attention. See Kanwisher & Duncan 2004,
pp. 38190
Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S. 2005. Interactions between number and space in parietal cortex.
Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6:43548
Ito S, Stuphorn V, Brown JW, Schall JD. 2003. Performance monitoring by the anterior cingulate cortex
during saccade countermanding. Science 302:12022
Ivry R, Robertson LC. 1997. Two Sides of Perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Jones L, Rothbart MK, Posner MI. 2003. Development of inhibitory control in preschool children. Dev. Sci.
6:498504
Kampe KK, Frith CD, Frith U. 2003. Hey John: signals conveying communicative intention toward the
self activate brain regions associated with mentalizing, regardless of modality. J. Neurosci. 23:525863
Kanwisher N, Duncan J, eds. 2004. Attention and Performance XX: Functional Brain Imaging of Visual Cognition.
Oxford, UK: Oxford Univ. Press
Klingberg T. 2012. Training working memory and attention. See Posner 2012b, pp. 47586
Konishi S, Donaldson DI, Buckner RL. 2001. Transient activation during block transition. Neuroimage 13:364
74
Larsen H, van der Zwaluw CS, Overbeek G, Granic I, Franke B, Engels RC. 2010. A variable-number-of-
tandem-repeats polymorphism in the dopamine D4 receptor gene affects social adaptation of alcohol use:
investigation of a gene-environment interaction. Psychol. Sci. 21:106468
Lindner A, Iyer A, Kagan I, Andersen RA. 2010. Human posterior parietal cortex plans where to reach and
what to avoid. J. Neurosci. 30:1171525
Marrocco RT, Davidson MC. 1998. Neurochemistry of attention. In The Attentive Brain, ed. R Parasuraman,
pp. 3550. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 87

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

Morrison JH, Foote SL. 1986. Noradrenergic and serotoninergic innervation of cortical, thalamic and tectal
visual structures in Old and New World monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 243:11728
Moruzzi G, Magoun HW. 1949. Brainstem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalogr.
Clin. Neurophysiol. 1:45573
Nagai Y, Critchley HD, Featherstone E, Fenwick PB, Trimble MR, Dolan RJ. 2004. Brain activity relating
to the contingent negative variation: an fMRI investigation. Neuroimage 21:123241
Ochsner KN, Bunge SA, Gross JJ, Gabrieli JD. 2002. Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive
regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14:121529
Ploran EJ, Nelson SM, Velanova K, Donaldson DI, Petersen SE, Wheeler ME. 2007. Evidence accumulation
and the moment of recognition: dissociating perceptual recognition processes using fMRI. J. Neurosci.
27:1191224
Posner MI. 1975. Psychobiology of attention. In Handbook of Psychobiology, ed. M Gazzaniga, C Blakemore,
pp. 44180. New York: Academic
Posner MI. 2012a. Attention in the Social World. New York: Oxford Univ. Press
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.

Posner MI. 2012b. Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention. New York: Guilford


Posner MI, Petersen SE. 1990. The attention system of the human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 13:2542
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

Posner MI, Rothbart MK. 2007. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological
science. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58:123
Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, Voelker P. 2012. Control networks and neuromodulators of early
development. Dev. Psychol. In press
Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Vogel AC, Church JA, et al. 2011. Functional network organization in the
human brain. Neuron 72:66578
Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, Bushnell MC. 1997. Pain affect encoded in human anterior
cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science 277:96871
Rizzolatti G, Riggio L, Dascola I, Umilta C. 1987. Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical
meridians: evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia 25:3140
Rossi AF, Bichot NP, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG. 2007. Top down attentional decits in macaques with
lesions of lateral prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 27:1130614
Rothbart MK. 2011. Becoming Who We Are. New York: Guilford
Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, Rueda MR, Posner MI. 2011. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early
life. Emot. Rev. 3:20713
Rueda MR, Rothbart MK, McCandliss BD, Saccomanno L, Posner MI. 2005. Training, maturation, and
genetic inuences on the development of executive attention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102:1493136
Schafer RJ, Moore T. 2007. Attention governs action in the primate frontal eye eld. Neuron 56:54151
Sheese BE, Voelker PM, Rothbart MK, Posner MI. 2007. Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation
in dopamine receptor D4 to inuence temperament in early childhood. Dev. Psychopathol. 19:103946
Shulman GL, Corbetta M. 2012. See Posner 2012b, pp. 11327
Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Chafe CH, Berger J, Menon V. 2007. Neural dynamics of event segmentation in
music: converging evidence for dissociable ventral and dorsal networks. Neuron 55:52132
Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. 2008. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching
between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105:1256974
Stewart C, Burke S, Marrocco R. 2001. Cholinergic modulation of covert attention in the rat. Psychopharma-
cology 155:21018
Sturm W, Willmes K. 2001. On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage
14:S7684
Tang YY, Ma Y, Fan Y, Feng H, Wang J, et al. 2009. Central and autonomic nervous system interaction is
altered by short-term meditation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106:886570
Tang YY, Ma Y, Wang J, Fan Y, Feng S, et al. 2007. Short-term meditation training improves attention and
self-regulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104:1715256
Thompson KG, Biscoe KL, Sato TR. 2005. Neuronal basis of covert spatial attention in the frontal eye eld.
J. Neurosci. 25:947987
Voytko ML, Olton DS, Richardson RT, Gorman LK, Tobin JR, Price DL. 1994. Basal forebrain lesions in
monkeys disrupt attention but not learning and memory. J. Neurosci. 14:16786

88 Petersen Posner

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print
NE35CH04-Petersen ARI 16 March 2012 15:43

Walter G. 1964. The convergence and interaction of visual, auditory, and tactile responses in human non-
specic cortex. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 112:32061
Womelsdorf T, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Singer W, Desimone R, et al. 2007. Modulation of neuronal
interactions through neuronal synchronization. Science 316:160912
Wright RD, Ward LM. 2008. Orienting of Attention. Oxford/New York: Oxford Univ. Press
Zilles K. 2005. Evolution of the human brain and comparative cyto- and receptor architecture. In From Monkey
Brain to Human Brain, ed. S Dehaene, J-R Duhamel, MD Hauser, G Rizzolatti, pp. 4156. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books
by Stanford University - Main Campus - Green Library on 06/11/12. For personal use only.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012.35. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org

www.annualreviews.org The Attention System of the Human Brain 89

Changes may still occur before final publication online and in print

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen