Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31

www.elsevier.com/locate/neures

Circadian fluctuation of time perception in healthy human subjects


Kenichi Kuriyama a,b, Makoto Uchiyama b,*, Hiroyuki Suzuki b,c, Hirokuni Tagaya b,
Akiko Ozaki b, Sayaka Aritake b, Yuichi Kamei d, Toru Nishikawa a,
Kiyohisa Takahashi e
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Tokyo Medical and Dental University, Yushima, Bunkyo-Ku, 113-0034 Tokyo, Japan
b
Department of Psychophysiology, National Institute of Mental Health, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Kohnodai, Ichikawa 272-0827,
Japan
c
Graduate School of Humanities, Nihon University, Sakurajosui, Setagaya-ku, Tokyo 156-8550, Japan
d
Department of Psychiatry, Kohnodai Hospital, NCNP, Kohnodai, Ichikawa 272-8516, Japan
e
National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Ogawahigashi-cho, Kodaira 187-8551, Japan

Received 6 September 2002; accepted 9 December 2002

Abstract

Previous studies suggested that various psychophysiological factors have influences on human time perception. In particular,
working memory loads, time of day, body temperature, and mood were known as important modifiers of time perception. The
purpose of this study is to elucidate factors affecting the short-term time perception under controlled condition. Fourteen healthy
young male adults participated in this study. Time perception sessions (TPS) were conducted 4 times at 0900, 1300, 1700 and 2100 h.
The TPS consisted of five 10-s time production trials under five different conditions (control trial, those with reward, and 3 different
dual-load working memory tasks). Subjective status was assessed using visual analogue scales (VAS). To verify a participants
vigilance state, an alpha attenuation coefficient (AAC) was calculated. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA for produced time
revealed a significant main effect of session, but no effect of task or interaction. Although produced time was not correlated with
AACs or VAS scores, there was a significant negative correlation between produced time and core body temperature. These results
suggest that human short-term time perception may be more influenced by circadian rhythm than working memory load or
psychophysiological status.
# 2003 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd and the Japan Neuroscience Society. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Time perception; Circadian rhythm; Interval timing clock; Working memory; Mood; Core body temperature; Time production test

1. Introduction is located in the suprachiasmatic nuclei of the hypotha-


lamus and synchronized to the light /dark cycle (Moore-
Animal studies have suggested that organisms have Ede et al., 1983). In contrast to the circadian clock, the
two endogenous timekeeping systems; a circadian clock interval timing clock counts the number of signals that
that provides time of day and an interval timing clock generated in certain intervals (Gibbon and Church,
that measures passage of time like a stopwatch (Virgi- 1981; Roberts and Holder, 1984). These clocks play a
nia, 1996). The circadian clock is driven by self- crucial role in long lasting natural selection because the
sustaining oscillator with a period of about 24 h, which ability to anticipate risks and opportunities that occur in
the environment could increase the chance for the
animal to survive.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: /81-47-375-4756; fax: /81-47-375-
It has been well-documented that humans can per-
4771. ceive the passage of time without referring to an external
E-mail address: macoto@ncnp-k.go.jp (M. Uchiyama). clock or stopwatch, whether duration to be perceived
0168-0102/03/$ - see front matter # 2003 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd and the Japan Neuroscience Society. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0168-0102(03)00025-7
24 K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31

was longer (long-term time perception) or shorter However, there may be another possible explanation.
(short-term time perception). Previous studies on long- That is, psychophysiological status such as mood,
term time perception, duration to be perceived was alertness, or tiredness, or changes in the environment
widely ranged from minutes to hours (Mischel et al., such as light, posture or exercise may produce diurnal
1969; Watts and Sharrock, 1984; Zakay, 1992; Campbell fluctuation in perceived time, providing that diurnal
et al., 2001; Conti, 2001; Rammsayer et al., 2001), fluctuation of the time perception documented in the
whereas those on short-term time perception concerned previous studies may have been a consequence of non-
a perceived period of less than a minute (Treisman, circadian origin.
1963; Rammsayer and Vogel, 1992; Fortin et al., 1993; Here we investigate short-term time perception in
Fortin and Breton, 1995; Ivry, 1996; Fortin and healthy human and study factors affecting it. In the
Rousseau, 1998; Fortin and Masse, 1999; Casini and present study, we focused on the effects of working
Ivry, 1999; Rammsayer et al., 2001). Though various memory load and psychophysiological status on time
mechanisms of long-term time perception have been perception and those of diurnal fluctuations, as well as
postulated, there was significant controversy on the the possible interaction among those factors. To differ-
nature and functions because definitions of long-term entiate the influence of these factors clearly, we utilized a
time perception were apparently diverse from study to regimen, in which a constant resting wakefulness with
study, and because different study protocols were used semi-recumbent position was kept for 17 h, so that we
in the previous studies (Mischel et al., 1969; Watts and excluded potential confounding factors that may have
Sharrock, 1984; Zakay, 1992; Campbell et al., 2001; influences on the participants psychological and physi-
Conti, 2001; Rammsayer et al., 2001). In contrast, the cal status and may mask the differences of the experi-
experimental settings in which short-term time percep- mental setting.
tion was investigated were similar and comparable in the
previous studies (Treisman, 1963; Rammsayer and
Vogel, 1992; Fortin et al., 1993; Ivry, 1996; Fortin and 2. Methods
Rousseau, 1998; Rammsayer et al., 2001). Moreover,
recent progresses in functional imaging technique have 2.1. Participants
suggested that certain regions of the brain were acti-
vated when short-term time perception tasks were Fourteen healthy young male volunteers aged 18/24
loaded (Maquet et al., 1996; Harrington et al., 1998; years (mean age9/S.D., 20.99/1.8 yr) participated in the
Pouthas et al., 1999; Schubotz et al., 2000), though present study. A physician and a psychiatrist examined
variations and changes of perceived time were not them and found that no participants had a history of, or
monitored in these studies because of the methodologi- suffered from, neurological or psychiatric disorders, or
cal limitations. had a history of using any psychoactive drugs. They
In the previous reports on short-term time perception, were asked to abstain from caffeine, nicotine, and
it was indicated that psychological or cognitive factors alcohol for a week prior to the experiment. They were
influenced human time perception. Some researchers instructed to keep to a regular sleep /wake schedule and
have reported that short-term memory or working to record sleep logs for 2 weeks. We confirmed the
memory tasks that were loaded simultaneously with a validity of their sleep logs by using an ambulatory wrist
short-term time perception task had an effect on activity recorder (Actiwatch-L, Mini-Mitter co., Inc.
perceived time (Fortin et al., 1993; Fortin and Breton, Bend, OR) for a week prior to the experiment, and
1995; Fortin and Rousseau, 1998; Fortin and Masse, found that the participants had regular sleep /wake
1999). Enhancement of subjects attention to temporal habits without marked weekday /weekend differences
information has been reported to give more accurate in sleep schedule. The experimental protocol was
short-term time perception (Zakay, 1992; Casini and approved by the Intramural Research Board of Na-
Ivry, 1999; Rammsayer et al., 2001). tional Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, and each
In chronobiological studies, it was reported that participant gave his informed consent after the nature,
short-term time perception seems to fluctuate across the purpose, and possible risks of the experiment had
the day (Aschoff, 1998; Aschoff and Daan, 1997; been explained in detail. All the experiment was
Morofushi et al., 2001). Under the free-running condi- performed at the time isolation laboratory of National
tion, produced time (10-s) has been reported to change Center of Neurology and Psychiatry.
along with core body temperature, which is a possible
marker of circadian rhythm. During the subjective day, 2.2. Experimental design
produced time (10-s) decreased from the morning into
the night (Aschoff, 1998). These findings might suggest 2.2.1. Overview
that short-term time perception may be modulated by a Experimental schedule is illustrated in Fig. 1. The
circadian oscillator. participant entered a two-day laboratory experimental
K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31 25

Fig. 1. A schematic schedule of the two-day study is shown. The participant was instructed to retire to temporal isolation facilities at 2300 h (Day 1)
and to stay in bed until 0700 h (Day 2) under complete darkness (B/0.1 lx). Thereafter, they were enforced to keep awake in a semi-recumbent
position under room light conditions (180 lx) until 2400 h (Day 2). Time production sessions (TPS) were carried out 4 times (0900, 1300, 1700 and
2100 h) on day 2. The TPS consisted of VAS, AAT, and five TPTs under five different psychological conditions; TPT without any tasks (control
task), that with a reward (5000 ) for accuracy of the perception, that with memory task of 8-digit numbers, that with memory task of 4 words, and
that with memory task of a figure of Bentons memory test. Three memory tasks were designed as dual-load tasks. The tasks were presented to the
participant in a randomized crossover manner. At the beginning to each TPS, VAS and AAT were administered.

session. On the first day (Day 1), the participant arrived electrode system, electrooculograms (left and right),
at the laboratory at 1900 h. Setting of rectal temperature chin surface electromyogram, and electrocardiogram.
sensor (inserted 10 /15 cm from the anus) and attach- During TPP, the participant was enforced to keep
ment of EEG electrodes were completed between 2000 awake quietly without any amusements. The investiga-
and 2200 h. The participant was instructed to retire at tors monitored the participants status via a video
2300 h and to stay in bed until 0700 h on the second monitoring system and polygraphic recordings, and
experiment day (Day 2) when he was awakened by the enforced them to stay awake. The participant was
technician. During this period, polysomnographic re- restricted to meet people except for meals.
cordings were conducted under complete darkness (B/
0.1 lx). Thereafter, the participant was enforced to keep 2.2.2. TP Sessions
awake on a semi-recumbent bed under room light The TPS consisted of psychological assessment by
conditions (180 lx) until 2400 h. During this period on visual analogue scales (VAS), alpha attenuation tests
Day 2, time perception protocol (TPP) was carried out. (AAT), and five time production trials (TPT) under five
During the TPP, TP session (TPS) was conducted 4 different task conditions: (1) TPT without any tasks
times at 0900, 1300, 1700 and 2100 h. Iso-caloric meals (control task), (2) one with reward (5000 ) for accuracy
(470 kcal) were given an hour before every TPS (0800, of the produced time, (3) one with a memory task
1200, 1600 and 2000 h). Non-sparkling mineral water consisting of 8-digit numbers, (4) one with a memory
was supplied at any time according to the participants task consisting of 4 different words (common nouns
requests. During the study, the ambient temperature and written in three different font styles of the Japanese
humidity in the time isolation facility were controlled at cursive (hiragana ) syllabary), and (5) one with a memory
24.09/0.5 8C and 609/5%, respectively. Throughout the task of a figure of Bentons Memory test. The memory
study, no time cues or information on the exact numbers task was designed as a dual-load regimen, so that each
of TPSs and meals were given to the participant. Rectal task load would require processing in working memory
temperature measurement and polygraphic recordings simultaneously with the TPTs (Baddeley and Hitch,
were carried out continuously throughout the study. 1974; Baddeley, 1982, 1986). These trials were given to
Temperature data were measured every 2 min and the participant in a randomized crossover manner. All
stored telemetrically in a computerized monitoring the tasks were performed on a laptop computer with a
system (Vital Sense, resolution 0.02 8C, Mini-Mitter 14-in. color liquid crystal monitor. The participant was
Co. Inc., Bend, OR). Polygraphic recordings were instructed to produce 10 s by pressing a space key on the
performed throughout the study by using a polygraphic computer. The answers of the memory tasks were
recorder (Neurofax, Nihon Kohden, Tokyo, Japan). provided by pressing a key on the numerical keyboard
Polygraphic recordings consisted of C3 /A1, C4 /A2 of the computer. The screen was placed at 50 cm from
and O1 /A1 EEGs in conformity with the 10 /20 the participants eyes. All the data were transmitted via
26 K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31

a local network system and were stored in the host explanation. Produced time in the training trials ranged
computer. Prior to the study, they trained on TPTs to from 9.40 to 14.04 s (11.459/1.14, mean9/S.D.). For all
understand the operation of the PC-based test battery; performance tasks, participants were instructed to per-
between 1900 and 2000 h on Day 1, a TP session was form as quickly as possible, but without sacrificing
carried out after the investigator gave a detailed accuracy.

Fig. 2. TPS was conducted under five different conditions. Produced time was measured as the interval between keypress to begin and keypress to
end. This procedure was operated under following conditions: (A) memory task condition. The participant hits a space key to start the trial, this
keypress triggered the presentation of memory sets. It disappeared between 5 and 15 s to avoid itself influenced on the participants judgment of a 10-
s time production. After the TPT, several choices of the memory targets were presented and urged to select the correct answer. The memory task of
numbers and figure of Bentons Memory test had four choices (a and b), and the task of words had nine choices (c). (B) Control task condition. TPT
without any additional tasks was carried out. (C) Reward task condition. At the beginning of TPT, instruction of reward condition was presented to
the participant. The reward was given to the participant when his produced time was within the range of 109/0.5 s. The results of time production
were not announced until the end of all the study. In this trial, no additional task was carried out. Each condition was presented in a randomized
manner. The intervals between trial conditions were set up 70 /100 s to avoid being used by the participant as a ruler interval.
K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31 27

2.3. Measures Very Sleepy on the right. The participant was asked to
draw a vertical mark on the line at the point corre-
2.3.1. TP trials sponding to their present status. The VAS scores were
Produced time was measured as the interval between obtained by measuring the distance of the mark from
keypress in each different condition. The TPTs are the left end of the line (higher scores being associated
illustrated in Fig. 2. The memory tasks (Fig. 2A) was with more intense feelings of each state).
designed to be loaded simultaneously during time
production. After pressing a space key to start the trial,
the participant was instructed to memorize the targets 2.4. Statistical analyses
displayed on the screen for 5/15 s and to retain them
until the end of the trial. The participant was required to To reduce inter-individual covariance, AACs and
select the correct targets among several choices. The VAS scores were standardized by using Fishers Z-
memory task for the numbers and for the figure of transformation, and core body temperature was trans-
Bentons Memory test had four choices (Fig. 2A (a and formed to the mean deviations.
b)), and the memory task for words had nine choices Two-factor 4 /5 (session-by-task) repeated measures
(Fig. 2A c). The inter-trial interval ranged from 70 to ANOVAs were used to analyze the produced time.
100 s. In the control task (Fig. 2B), the participant was Greenhouse-Geisers adjustment factor (epsilon) was
instructed directly to enter the TPT. In the reward task calculated to adjust the degrees of freedom.
(Fig. 2C), the amount of reward for accurate produced Endogenous circadian phase of the core body tem-
time was presented prior to the TPT. A reward (5000) perature data was assessed by fitting of dual-harmonic
was given to the participant when his produced time was cosine curves (periods, 24 and 12 h) with a least squares
within the range of 109/0.5 s. method (Uchiyama et al., 1995; Kubota et al., 2002;
Information concerning the accuracy of time produc- Tagaya et al., 2002) using a software package (Kaleida-
tion and the results of the memory tasks was not given Graph, Synergy Software, Reading, PA). The nadir of
to the subject until the end of the study. the fitted curve was regarded as the circadian phase
marker. The nadir of the core body temperature was
2.3.2. Alpha attenuation test 05419/0122 (mean9/S.D.), suggesting the participants
At the beginning of each TPS, AAT was adminis- circadian phase markers were normal range. Pearsons
tered. In the AAT, three 1-min artifact-free EEG correlation coefficients were calculated between pro-
recordings with eyes-open and those with eyes-closed duced time and other measures.
were obtained. The EEG data were digitized using a SPSS ver.10.0J for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL)
sampling rate of 200 Hz. A half-amplitude low-fre- was used for all the statistical analyses. All statistical
quency filter was set at 0.5 Hz, and high frequency filter
at 50 Hz. The digitized data was stored on an optical
disk and analyzed offline with an EEG spectral analyses
package (FOCUS; MEGIS software, GmbH). Power
spectra of eyes-opened and eyes-closed EEG recordings
derived from O1 to A1 were analyzed by using a fast
Fourier transformation (FFT) within the alpha fre-
quency band (8 /12 Hz) using a bin size of 0.5 Hz. The
FFT analyses were conducted under 5.120-s Welch
tapered windows with 2.620-s overlap. This yielded 24
windows per 1-min epoch. The bins were averaged
across the 8/12 Hz frequency range to produce an
estimate of alpha power in squared microvolts. The
ratio of the mean eyes-closed to mean eyes-opened alpha
power was defined as the alpha attenuation coefficient
(AAC).
Fig. 3. The results of the TPTs of a representative participant are
shown. The horizontal axis represents time of day. The vertical axis
2.3.3. Visual analogue scale represents values of produced time. Black squares () represent
Subjective psychophysiological status was assessed produced time under control task condition. Black circles (m) are
using VAS following the AAT in each TPS. The VAS produced time with WM tasks of 8-digit numbers. Black triangles (?)
are produced time with WM tasks of 4 words. Open circles (k) are
consisted of seven items, which were alertness, mood,
produced time with WM tasks of figures. Open triangles (^) are
energy, tiredness, tension, motivation, and irritability. produced time with rewards. The produced time decreased as time goes
The VAS was presented as a horizontal line 100 mm in from 0900 to 1700 h regardless of presence of memory tasks or rewards
length, labeled for example Very Alert on the left and condition.
28 K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31

analyses were two-tailed. The level of statistical signifi-


cance was set at P B/0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Overview

The results obtained from a representative participant


are shown in Fig. 3. Produced time was longer than 10 s
in the first two sessions and was decreased toward the
evening (Fig. 3(a)). The difference in trial condition did
Fig. 5. Relation between produced time and core body temperature
not seem to influence the produced time.
together with the regression line are shown. The horizontal axis
The overall error rates for memory tasks obtained in represents standardized scores of produced time. The vertical axis
all participants were 0.018% (1/56; at the fourth session) represents mean deviated values of rectal temperature. Standardized
for number, 0.018% (1/56; at the second session) for produced time and standardized core body temperature were signifi-
Bentons figure, 0.018% (1/56; at the second session) for cantly negatively correlated (r//0.44, P B/0.001).
word, confirming that most of the dual-loaded memory
tasks were properly performed.

3.2. Effect of trial condition and time of day 3.3. Correlation with core body temperature, AACs and
VAS scores
Two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed a
significant main effect of session (F (3, 11) /7.27, P / 3.3.1. Core body temperature
0.005), but no effect of trial condition (F (4, 10) /0.65, A significant negative correlation was found between
P /0.56) or interaction (F (12, 2) /1.70, P /0.15). produced times and core body temperature (r //0.44,
Thus, produced times were averaged across condition. P B/0.001; Fig. 5).
Produced times decreased with progression of the
sessions (Fig. 4).
3.3.2. Alpha attenuation coefficients
No significant correlation was found between pro-
duced times and AACs (r //0.061, NS).

3.3.3. Visual analogue scale scores


No significant correlation was found between pro-
duced times and VAS scores (Alertness: r //0.074,
Mood: r /0.283, Energy: r/0.038, Tiredness: r//
0.236, Tension: r //0.013, Motivation: r /0.158, Irrit-
ability: r/0.028, NS).

4. Discussion

In the present study, short-term time production


showed significant diurnal fluctuations, in which re-
markable over-production in the morning decreased
Fig. 4. Diurnal change of produced times are shown. The horizontal towards the evening. However, time production was
axis represents time of day. The vertical axis represents values of not influenced by the working memory tasks that were
produced time. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA of produced simultaneously loaded to the participant. In addition,
time revealed a significant main effect of session (F (3, 11)/7.27, P /
the vigilance level measured by using AAT or VAS
0.005). Each data point represents the mean produced time of all the
conditions. Bars represent standard errors of mean. The produced time
scores did not affect the time production, whereas core
which were averaged across condition decreased from the morning till body temperature at the TPS showed a significant
evening. negative correlation with produced time.
K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31 29

Recent studies have suggested that there are potential In contrast, about 70 years ago, when knowledge in
underlying psychophysiological factors on the diurnal human circadian organization had not been established,
fluctuations in human time production: (1) accumula- Hoagland described that a pathologically high skin
tion of fatigue or sleepiness according to the length of temperature in patients with various infectious diseases
the wakefulness (Phillips, 1977; Rammsayer, 1989), (2) was associated with prolongation of time estimation,
an effect of circadian rhythm that was synchronized to presumably regardless of circadian timing (Hoagland,
day /night transition (Aschoff and Daan, 1997; Aschoff, 1933). Recently, a similar observation has been reported
1998; Campbell et al., 2001; Morofushi et al., 2001), and by Hancock (1993). He found a significant but weak
(3) differences in psychological status at the time negative correlation between manipulated intra-auricu-
production session, especially mood (Mischel et al., lar temperature and produced times, though shortening
1969; Watts and Sharrock, 1984). of produced time was not marked even when the intra-
We used an AAT to evaluate vigilance level at the auricular temperature was seriously elevated. These
time production test. The AAC, a ratio of EEG alpha studies may indicate that there are possible direct effects
power values with eyes-closed to that with eyes-opened, of body temperature on time perception. However,
has been widely utilized in human psychophysiological produced time obtained from patients suffering from
studies and recognized as a robust measure to evaluate infectious diseases seemed to be influenced rather by
the vigilance level objectively (Saletu and Grunberger, multiple pathological conditions than head temperature
1984; Niedermeyer et al., 1989). Studies on diurnal alone, since infection may cause not only the elevation
fluctuations of vigilance have revealed that longer of body temperature but also the releases of cytokines
sustained wakefulness was associated with lower vigi- that have marked effects on brain function (Dantzer,
lance level (Higuchi et al., 2001). Our results showing 2001; Owens and Babcock, 2002).
that AAC did not have a significant correlation with In the present study, participants were instructed to
produced time suggest that the diurnal fluctuation in memorize numbers, words, and figures before the
initiation of time production, and maintain them while
produced time was unlikely to be a consequence of
the participants were producing time. This procedure
changes in vigilance level.
was considered to provide a dual task loading. That is,
We conducted continuous measurement of rectal
maintaining these memory targets was likely to occupy
temperature throughout the study, and found that a
the working memory areas in the brain (McCarthy et al.,
significant negative correlation between the produced
1994; Manoach et al., 1997). In the present study, the
time and rectal temperature. Rectal temperature mea-
simultaneous load on the working memory areas,
sured under the strictly controlled conditions, in which
however, did not alter the time interval that the
ambient temperature and illumination, body movement,
participants produced. In contrast, Fortin et al. have
and posture of the participant, and feeding were kept
conducted a similar dual task procedure in searching
constant, was reported to represent an oscillation of the
effects on produced time and found that memory tasks
circadian pacemaker (Czeisler et al., 1990), suggesting presented during time production test were associated
that the diurnal fluctuations in produced time may with a prolongation of produced time as compared with
reflect changes in the output of the circadian pacemaker. trials without memory tasks (Fortin and Breton, 1995;
In the present study, the experimental protocol was Fortin and Masse, 1999). However, their results may
carefully designed to minimize effects of non-circadian have been due to uncontrolled chronobiotic effects and
factors. Thus, the fluctuation of core body temperature other behavioral confounding factors. Our results re-
obtained in the present study was likely to represent an vealed that the time of day had a more robust effect on
output of the circadian pacemaker. Aschoff, conducting produced time in comparison with simultaneously
a time production test in human subjects exhibiting free- loaded memory tasks, even though there could have
run in the temporal isolation unit, has stressed that there been a scanty effect of such memory tasks that Fortin et
may be a circadian effect on short-term time perception, al. have reported.
though his study was not completely free from non- Recently, functional imaging techniques were utilized
circadian confounding factors that may have effects on in identifying neural networks related to time produc-
short-term time perception, such as psychological status, tion (Harrington et al., 1998; Pouthas et al., 1999;
or mental activities. In his experiments, longer or shorter Schubotz et al., 2000). Maquet et al. have suggested that
produced times were associated with lower or higher time production process would be related to the function
core body temperatures, respectively (Aschoff, 1998). To of the prefrontal cortices (BA 10, 46) in their PET study
further confirm those potential effects of the circadian (Maquet et al., 1996). These areas have been reported to
pacemaker on short-term time perception which ob- have a functional relationship with working memory
tained in his study and the present study, a longer process (Clifford et al., 2000). These functional imaging
observation under the well-controlled condition is studies suggested that time production process may be
necessary. related to a working memory function. Our results that
30 K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31

working memory loads have no significant effect on an Baddeley, A.D., 1982. Implications of neuropsychological evidence for
theories of normal memory. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol.
accuracy of produced time suggest that the prefrontal
Sci. 298 (1089), 59 /72.
cortices might not be the interval timing clock itself, Baddeley, A.D., 1986. Working Memory. Oxford University Press,
though it played an important role as a part of neural New York.
networks that were involved in participants attempt to Campbell, S.S., Murphy, P.J., Boothroyd, C.E., 2001. Long-term time
produce time, since their study did not concern the estimation is influenced by circadian phase. Physiol. Behav. 72 (4),
accuracy of produced time due to different objectives of 589 /593.
Casini, L., Ivry, R.B., 1999. Effects of divided attention on temporal
their studies.
processing in patients with lesions of the cerebellum or frontal lobe.
Mood had been believed as an important factor in Neuropsychology 13 (1), 10 /21.
considering subjective passage of time. As one may Clifford, B.S., Susan, I., Richard, F., 2000. Integration of sensory and
imagine, in ones daily life time is likely to pass faster in motor function: the association areas of the cerebral cortex and the
a pleasant situation than in an unpleasant situation. cognitive capabilities of the brain. In: Eric, R.K., James, H.S.,
Mischel et al. and Watts et al. have reported that Thomas, M.J. (Eds.), Principles of Neural Science, fourth ed..
Principles of Neural Science, pp. 357 /361.
subjective passage of time ran slower in the condition in Conti, R., 2001. Time flies: investigating the connection between
which an emotionally uncomfortable task was provided, intrinsic motivation and the experience of time. J. Pers. 69 (1), 1 /
in comparison with that without such a task (Mischel et 26.
al., 1969; Watts and Sharrock, 1984; Angrilli et al., Czeisler, C.A., Johnson, M.P., Duffy, J.F., Brown, E.N., Ronda, J.M.,
1997). In the present study, we did not find any effects of Kronauer, R.E., 1990. Exposure to bright light and darkness to
treat physiologic maladaptation to night work. N. Engl. J. Med.
mood and other psychological measures on time pro-
322, 1253 /1259.
duction. A possible explanation for the different results Dantzer, R., 2001. Cytokine-induced sickness behavior: mechanisms
may be attributed to our study design in which the and implications. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 933, 222 /234.
behavioral and psychological conditions were strictly Fortin, C., Rousseau, R., Bourque, P., Kirouac, E., 1993. Time
controlled, giving minimal changes in the psychological estimation and concurrent nontemporal processing: specific inter-
statuses across 4 sessions. Moreover, enforced motiva- ference from short-term-memory demands. Percept. Psychophys.
53 (5), 536 /548.
tion for accuracy of produced time using economical
Fortin, C., Breton, R., 1995. Temporal interval production and
rewards did not affect the accuracy, providing another processing in working memory. Percept. Psychophys. 57 (2),
explanation that produced time may be beyond voli- 203 /215.
tional control. Fortin, C., Rousseau, R., 1998. Interference from short-term memory
processing on encoding and reproducing brief durations. Psychol.
Res. 61 (4), 269 /276.
Fortin, C., Masse, N., 1999. Order information in short-term memory
Acknowledgements and time estimation. Mem. Cognit. 27 (1), 54 /62.
Gibbon, J., Church, R.M., 1981. Journal of experimental psychology:
The authors would like to appreciate the assistance of animal behavior processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process.
Kayo Shibui, M.D., Ph.D. and Kyuja Kim, M.D., 7 (2), 87 /107.
Ph.D. in designing the study. This study was supported Hancock, P.A., 1993. Body temperature influence on time perception.
J. Gen. Psychol. 120 (3), 197 /216.
in part by a Research Grant for Nervous and Mental Harrington, D.L., Haaland, K.Y., Knight, R.T., 1998. Cortical
Disorders (11 /3) and a Health Science Grant networks underlying mechanisms of time perception. J. Neurosci.
(12080701) from the Ministry of Health, Labor and 18 (3), 1085 /1095.
Welfare, and a Grant-in-aid for Scientific Research Higuchi, S., Liu, Y., Yuasa, T., Maeda, A., Motohashi, Y., 2001.
(13470200) by the Ministry of Education, Science and Diurnal variations in alpha power density and subjective sleepiness
while performing repeated vigilance tasks. Clin. Neurophysiol. 112
Culture.
(6), 997 /1000.
Hoagland, H., 1933. The physiological control of judgements of
duration. Evidence of a chemical clock. J. Gen. Psychol. 9, 267 /
References 287.
Ivry, R.B., 1996. The representation of temporal information in
Angrilli, A., Cherubini, P., Pavese, A., Mantredini, S., 1997. The perception and motor control. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 6 (6),
influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept. Psycho- 851 /857.
phys. 59 (6), 972 /982. Kubota, T., Uchiyama, M., Suzuki, H., Shibui, K., Kim, K., Tan, X.,
Aschoff, J., Daan, S., 1997. Human time perception in temporal Tagaya, H., Okawa, M., Inoue, S., 2002. Effects of nocturnal
isolation: effects of illumination intensity. Chronobiol. Int. 14 (6), bright light on saliva melatonin, core body temperature and sleep
585 /596. propensity rhythms in human subjects. Neurosci. Res. 42, 115 /122.
Aschoff, J., 1998. Human perception of short and long time intervals: Manoach, D.S., Schlaug, G., Siewert, B., Darby, D.G., Bly, B.M.,
its correlation with body temperature and the duration of wake Benfield, A., Edelman, R.R., Warach, S., 1997. Prefrontal cortex
time. J. Biol. Rhythms 13 (5), 437 /442. fMRI signal changes are correlated with working memory load.
Baddeley, A.D., Hitch, G.J., 1974. Working memory. In: Bower, G.H. Neuroreport 8 (2), 545 /549.
(Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Maquet, P., Lejeune, H., Pouthas, V., Bonnet, M., Casini, L., Macar,
Research and Theory, vol. 8. Academic Press, New York, pp. 47 / F., Timsit-Berthier, M., Vidal, F., Ferrara, A., Degueldre, C.,
89. Quaglia, L., Delfiore, G., Luxen, A., Woods, R., Mazziotta, J.C.,
K. Kuriyama et al. / Neuroscience Research 46 (2003) 23 /31 31

Comar, D., 1984. Brain activation induced by estimation of Rammsayer, T.H., Vogel, W.H., 1992. Pharmacologic properties of
duration: a PET study. Neuroimage 3 (2), 119 /126. the internal clock underlying time perception in humans. Neurop-
McCarthy, G., Blamire, A.M., Puce, A., Nobre, A.C., Bloch, G., sychobiology 26 (1 /2), 71 /80.
Hyder, F., Goldman-Rakic, P., Shulman, R.G., 1994. Functional Rammsayer, T.H., Hennig, J., Haag, A., Lange, N., 2001. Effects of
magnetic resonance imaging of human prefrontal cortex activation noradrenergic activity on temporal information processing in
during a spatial working memory task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 91 humans. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B 54 (3), 247 /258.
(18), 8690 /8694. Roberts, S., Holder, M.D., 1984. What starts an internal clock? J. Exp.
Mischel, W., Grusec, J., Masters, J.C., 1969. Effects of expected delay Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 10 (3), 273 /296.
time on the subjective value of rewards and punishments. J. Pers. Saletu, B., Grunberger, J., 1984. Early clinical pharmacological trials
Soc. Psychol. 11 (4), 363 /373. with a new anti-epileptic, milacemide, using pharmaco-EEG and
Moore-Ede, M.C., Czeisler, C.A., Richardson, G.S., 1983. Circadian psychometry. Methods Find Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 6 (6), 317 /330.
time-keeping in health and disease. I. Basic properties of circadian Schubotz, R.I., Friederici, A.D., von Cramon, D.Y., 2000. Time
pacemakers. N. Engl. J. Med. 309, 469 /476. perception and motor timing: a common cortical and subcortical
Morofushi, M., Shinohara, K., Kimura, F., 2001. Menstrual and
basis revealed by fMRI. Neuroimage 11 (1), 1 /12.
circadian variations in time perception in healthy women and
Treisman, M., 1963. Temporal discrimination and the indifference
women with premenstrual syndrome. Neurosci. Res. 41 (4), 339 /
interval. Implications for a model of the internal clock. Psychol.
344.
Monogr. 77 (13), 1 /31.
Niedermeyer, E., Krauss, G.L., Peyser, C.E., 1989. The electroence-
Tagaya, H., Uchiyama, M., Shibui, K., Kim, K., Suzuki, H., Kamei,
phalogram and mental activation. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 20 (4),
Y., Okawa, M., 2002. Non-rapid-eye-movement sleep propensity
215 /227.
Owens, T., Babcock, A., 2002. Immune response induction in the after sleep deprivation in human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 323, 17 /
central nervous system. Front. Biosci. 1 (7), 427 /438. 20.
Phillips, J.R., 1977. Relationship of field-dependence */independence Uchiyama, M., Mayer, G., Okawa, M., Meier-Ewert, K., 1995. Effects
to posture and judgment of time duration. Percept. Mot. Skills 44 of vitamin B12 on human circadian body temperature rhythm.
(3 Pt. 1), 931 /940. Neurosci. Lett. 192, 1 /4.
Pouthas, V., Maquet, P., Garnero, L., Ferrandez, A.M., Renault, B., Virginia, M., 1996. Setting a biological stopwatch. Science 271, 905 /
1999. Neural bases of time estimation: a PET and ERP study. 906.
Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 50 (Suppl.), 598 /603. Watts, F.N., Sharrock, R., 1984. Fear and time estimation. Percept.
Rammsayer, T.H., 1989. Dopaminergic and serotoninergic influence Mot. Skills 59 (2), 597 /598.
on duration discrimination and vigilance. Pharmacopsychiatry 22 Zakay, D., 1992. The role of attention in childrens time perception. J.
(Suppl. 1), 39 /43. Exp. Child. Psychol. 54 (3), 355 /371.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen