Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Bekesy Audiometry in

Analysis of Auditory Disorders


JAMES JERGER

Less than 14 years has elapsed since hears it. By connecting a pen-writing
Bekesy's original description of a self- system to the attenuator a graphic
recording audiometer (2). Within this representation, or tracing, of the sub-
period, however, the technique of ject's successive threshold crossings
'Bekesy audiometry' has rapidly gained may be obtained.
the stature of a major clinical and The Bekesy technique is particu-
research tool in audiology'. larly useful in psychoacoustics. It lends
Bekesy audiometry refers to a itself admirably, for example: to the
method in which the subject traces his measurement of temporary threshold
own auditory threshold by means of shift following acoustic stimulation
a suitable self-recording audiometer. and has been so employed by several
The threshold tracing signal may be investigators (6, 8, 10, 12, 15, 20, 31,
either a fixed frequency or a gradually 32, 33, 34). It finds use in the measure-
changing frequency, and the signal ment of pure-tone masking (3, Y).
may be either continuous or periodi- The present paper is concerned, how-
cally interrupted in time, but the ever, only with Bekesy audiometry as
essence of Bekesy's method is, first, a clinical tool in the evaluation of the
that the signal intensity is always hearing impaired. In the majority of
changing at a constant rate, and second, papers concerned with the clinical ap-
that the subject determines the direc- plication of Bekesy audiometry, meas-
tion of this change by alternately urement and description have been
pressing and releasing a key that re- confined almost exclusively to the
verses the direction of a motor-driven width or amplitude of the audiometric
attenuator. He is instructed to press tracing. This distance or width may
this key when he just hears the tone be expressed either in decibels or in
and to release it when he just-no-longer number of threshold crossings over a
given frequency span. In the graphic
James Jerger (Ph.D., Northwestern Uni- form of the Bekesy audiogram it is
versity, 1954) is Associate Professor of Audi- most easily visualized as the amplitude
ology, Northwestern University. A portion
of this article is based on a paper presented of the oscillating trace. Bekesy (2), in
at the 1959 Convention of the American his original paper, noted that the
Speech and Hearing Association, Cleveland. amplitude became greatly diminished
This research was supported by research
grant B-1310 from the National Institutes of
in subjects with hearing loss accom-
Health, Public Health Service, and by the panied by loudness recruitment. He
United States Air Force under Contract assumed that the tracing amplitude
AF 41 (657)-185, monitored by the School of represented the first just-noticeable-
Aviation Medicine, USAF, Brooks Air Force
Base, Texas. difference (JND) in loudness and con-
Volume 3, No. 3 275 September 1960

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
276 Journal o[ Speech and Hearing Research

cluded that a reduction in its size was progression toward higher and higher
compatible with the presence of an threshold intensity over time in retro-
abnormally rapid rate of loudness cochlear lesion.
growth with intensity (that is, loud- The present paper concerns the rela-
ness recruitment). However, Bekesy's tionship between Bekesy audiometry
assumption that the amplitude repre- and site of lesion within the auditory
sents the first JND has been questioned system. Unfortunately, almost every
by Hirsh, Palva, and Goodman (9), previous writer has confused this issue
who feel that the amplitude actually with a quite separate question, the re-
represents the variability about the lationship between the Bekesy tracing
absolute threshold. and the presence or absence of loudness
In any event, subsequent papers on recruitment. It must be emphasized,
Bekesy audiometry have dealt primarily therefore, that the present paper is not
with the amplitude aspect of the audio- concerned with how Bekesy audiom-
metric tracing (1, 7, 11, 17, 18, 21, 22, etry relates to loudness recruitment,
23, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 35, 36). The only with how it relates to site of
major point of view in this respect is lesion within the auditory system.
best exemplified by the very thorough
monograph of Lundborg (21). This Procedure
investigator obtained Bekesy audio-
grams on 50 normals, 25 cases of acous- Subjects. This report is based on the
tic trauma, 26 cases of Meniere's disease, Bekesy audiograms of 434 subjects
and 21 cases of diverse retrocochlear tested at the Hearing Clinic of the
lesion. He then classified the audiograms Northwestern U n i v e r s i t y M e d i c a l
into four types based on the tracing School over a three-year period. The
amplitude. There appeared to be a subjects were referred from various
rather precise relationship between sources for audiological evaluation. The
type of Bekesy tracing and site of majority were referred by otologists, a
lesion. Markedly reduced amplitude small number by neurologists and neuro-
was characteristically present in cases surgeons in the Chicago area. Although
with p r e s u m a b l y c o c h l e a r lesion no formal attempt at random selection
(acoustic trauma and Meniere's disease) was made, the series is fairly representa-
but characteristically absent in cases tive of the otologic case load in a large
with retrocochlear lesion. hospital environment. In most cases
Bekesy audiometry was performed as
In recent years increasing attention
part of a larger battery of auditory
has been given to another aspect of the
tests typically administered in a three-
Bekesy tracing, the change in threshold
hour test session. Although tracings
intensity over time as the subject traces
were ordinarily obtained on both ears,
threshold at a fixed frequency (4, 14,
subsequent analysis is confined to results
16, 19, 26, 27, 28, 37). Kos (16), Lierle obtained on only one ear of each sub-
and Reger (19), Jerger, Carhart, and
ject.
Lassman (14), and Yantis (37) have
shown very little change over time in Apparatus. All of the tracings on
presumably cochlear lesion, but marked which this report is based were ob-

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Jerger: Bekesy Audiometry, Auditory Disorders 277

tained w i t h a single Bekesy audiometer When I put these earphones on, you are
(Grason-Stadler, M o d e l E-800). T h e going to hear a beeping sound in your
ear. As long as you don't do anything the
rate of attenuation change was always sound will keep getting louder. But you
2.5 db per second, and the rate of fre- can make it fade away by holding down
q u e n c y change was always one octave this switch. When you let up on the switch
the sound will get louder again. Now,
per minute. T h e instrument offered here is what I want you to do. Listen
the option of a test signal that was very carefully, and, as soon as you hear
either continuous or periodically inter- the beeping sound, hold this switch down
until you can't hear it any more. As
r u p t e d in time. In the latter case, the soon as the beeping sound is gone, let up
interruption rate was 2.5 ips. on the switch until it comes back. Then,
as soon as you hear it again, hold the
T h e results r e p o r t e d below involve switch down until it goes away again,
t w o kinds of tracing, subsequently re- and so forth. The idea is ~o keep going
ferred to as 'conventional' and 'fixed- back and forth from where you can just
hear the beeping sound to where you
f r e q u e n c y ' tracings. In conventional can just not hear it any more. Never
tracings, the f r e q u e n c y of the test signal let the sound get very loud and never
m o v e d gradually u p w a r d from 100 to let it stay away too long. Hold this
switch down as soon as you hear the
10 000 cps. In fixed-frequency tracings sound, then let it up as soon as the sound
the f r e q u e n c y was preset and never is gone.
changed as the subject traced his thresh- F o llo win g these instructions a tracing
old over a t hree-mi n u t e period. was made w i t h the periodically inter-
In either case, a complete test always r u p t e d (I) test signal. A t the termina-
consisted of two separate tracings. In tion of this tracing the subject was
one the signal was periodically inter- reinstructed as follows:
rupted, in the second it was continuous Now we are going to do the same thing
in time. Both tracings, i n t e r r u p t e d and again, but this time the sound will be
steady instead of beeping on and off.
continuous, w ere always made on the Your job is still the same. Hold the switch
same piece of graph paper wi t h t w o down as soon as you hear the steady
sound, and let it up as soon as the steady
different colors of ink. It has been sound goes away.
f o u n d convenient to symbolize these F o llo win g these instructions a tracing
t w o conditions b y the letters T for was made w i t h the continuous (C)
i n t e r r u p t e d and 'C' for continuous in test signal. T h i s test order, interrupted
subsequent portions of this report. first and continuous second, was used
in all subjects. Instructions w e r e iden-
Method. A relatively rigidly stand- tical for either conventional or fixed-
ardized p r o c e d u r e of test administra- frequency tracings. When verbal
tion was initially designed, b u t could c o m m u n i c a t i o n was not possible, in-
not be followed rigorously in all structions we r e effected t h r o u g h pan-
subjects due to the occasional subject tomime.
whose ability to understand speech was
e x t r e m e l y limited. In a n y event, the Findings
following instructions w e r e used w h e n - A n initial a t t e m p t was made to ana-
ever verbal c o m m u n i c a t i o n was pos- lyse and score these Bekesy audiograms
sible: quantitatively. Various indices, such as

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
278 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research

the width of the continuous tracing in of fixed-frequency tracings. Through-


db, the number of threshold crossing out these and subsequent figures, green
per quarter octave, the difference be- denotes the interrupted (I) tracing and
tween tracing width at high and low red denotes the continuous (C) tracing.
frequencies, the difference between Type I. The type I relationship is
continuous and interrupted tracing characterized by an interweaving or
widths, and the difference between superposition of continuous and inter-
continuous and interrupted mid-points, rupted tracings, and by a tracing width
were evaluated, all with exceedingly which is constant over frequency and
discouraging results. It soon became averages about 10 db. There is, how-
apparent that the range of individual ever, considerable variation about this
variability on any absolute aspect of the mean value. Tracing widths as small as
Bekesy audiogram could be quite sub- 3 db and as large as 20 db are not un-
stantial. A good example is the width common.
of the continuous tracing. In most In the ease of fixed-frequency trac-
Meniere's patients it is, to be sure, quite ings, the type I relationship is reflected
small at high frequencies. On the other in two interweaving, horizontal trac-
hand many young adults with oto- ings.
sclerosis show tracing widths consider-
Type lh Type II tracings differ from
ably narrower than a large number of
type I in two respects. First, the con-
older Meniere's patients. There were,
tinuous tracing drops below the inter-
indeed, significant group tendencies in
rupted at high frequencies, but never
this quantitative analysis, but the degree
to a substantial extent. The gap seldom
of overlap among groups appeared to
exceeds 20 db and ordinarily does not
limit severely the use of any quantita-
appear at frequencies below 1000 cps.
tive measure as a reliable means of
Second, the width or amplitude of the
differentiating site of lesion. A similar
continuous tracing is often quite small
conclusion was reached by Landes (17).
(3 to 5 db) in these higher frequencies.
On the other hand, a qualitative This narrowing of the width or ampli-
judgment of the patterning or relation- tude of the continuous tracing is, of
ship between the interrupted and the course, the classical Bekesy sign
continuous tracings seemed to have thought by many to indicate the pres-
important diagnostic value. There ap- ence of loudness recruitment.
peared to be a unique relationship be- In fixed-frequency tracing the type
tween continuous and interrupted II result is quite clear-cut. The inter-
tracings corresponding to site of lesion rupted tracing is, again, horizontal and
within the auditory system. of normal width, but the continuous
One may distinguish four basic types trace drops from 5 to 20 db below the
of relationship, labelled, respectively, interrupted, ~vithin the first minute;
type I, type II, type III, and type IV. thereafter, it maintains a fairly stable
They are illustrated in Figures 1 and level. There is a reliable difference be-
2. Figure 1 shows the four types in the tween interrupted and continuous trac-
ease of conventional tracings, Figure ings but the difference is relatively
2 the corresponding types in the case small and remains quite constant after

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
]erger: Bekesy Audiometry, Auditory Disorders 279

2O 2O
e~

.E 40

.~ 6(3 60

80 := 80

I00 I00
125 ?.SG 500 IK 2K 4K 8K 1?.5 ?_50 500 IK 2K 4K 8K
Frequency in cps Frequency in cp$

TYPE I TYPE ~"

20 2O

.G
40 v " ~ 40 ., AA,AI AAAAaAA,k~ A 9

I
60 "v vvv [
9 60 VVV ' vVV~SA%
80 %~' = 80

v'~/~ I00
I00
125 250 500 IK 2K 4K 8K 125 250 500 IK ?.K 4K 8K
Frequency in cp$ Frequency in cp$

TYPE lIT TYPE ~Z


FIGURE 1. The four types of conventional Bekesy audiograms. Green represents threshold
tracing for a periodically interrupted tone, red for a continuous tone.
the first 60 seconds of tracing. Further- 150 cps and drop to a level as much
more, the difference appears only at as 40 to 50 db below the interrupted
mid- and high frequencies (that is, tracing. The width of the continuous
above 500 to 1000 cps). tracing ordinarily remains, however,
quite normal.
Type IlL Type III tracings are quite In type III fixed-frequency tracings
dramatic. The continuous tracing drops the interrupted tracing is horizontal but
below the interrupted to a remarkable the continuous drops very rapidly
degree. Furthermore, the two curves and ordinarily does not stabilize at all.
may diverge at relatively low frequen- Typically, the continuous tracing be-
cies (100 to 500 cps). It is not uncom- gins at the same level as the interrupted
mon to observe the continuous tracing but describes a rapidly descending trace
break away at a frequency as low as to the limit of the equipment. A 40-to-

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
280 Journal o[ Speech and Hearing Research

250 4K 250 4K

20 2O
.r
40
~ w w m
.~ 4o
60 W~Vv~
-r
80 8O

I00 I00
0 2 4 6 8 12 0 2 4 6 8
Tune in minutes Time in minutes

TYPE T TYPE 1T

I 1
250 IK 4K 250 4K

20 20
5: .c
_~ 40
Wv~ ] 40 ~/W Wvw V V ~ W
"~ 60 60 ~,A?,,^.,.~JV

80 80

I00
O 2 4 6
t 8
\
I0 12
I00
0 2 4 6 8 IO 12
Time in minutes Tlme~minu~
TYPE ~l" TYPE~
FIGURE 2. The four types of fixed-frequency Bekesy tracings. Green is interrupted; red,
continuous.

50-db drop within as little as 60 seconds resembling a type II in this respect.


is not unusual. The tracing width may or may not
become abnormally small, further add-
Type IV. Type IV tracings more
closely resemble type II than type III ing to possible confusion with type II.
but differ in one important respect. At mid- and high frequencies there
The continuous tracing falls consist- may even be some overlap between C
ently below the interrupted at fre- and I. The distinguishing feature, how-
quencies below 500 cps. At higher ever, occurring in both conventional
frequencies the continuous may fall a and fixed-frequency tracings, is the
constant distance below the interrupted, gap between C and I at relatively low

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
]erger: Bekesy Audiometry, Auditory Disorders 281

frequencies (100 to 500 cps). Type IV


tracings differ from type III tracings .
in that C ordinarily does not show a
precipitous drop over time. 20
The vast majority of Bekesy tracings -~
can be fitted into one of these four 40
categories quite reliably. There are,
~9 60 ,t
8O

2O I00
'10
125 250 500 IK 2K 4K OK
Frequency in cps
40
A

~9 60 i j
I
250 4K
80 I ,

I00 2O
125 250 500 IK 2K 4K 8K
Frequency in cps .r
=9 4 0
A

I I I 9~" 60
IK 4K
I I
80

20 I00
2 4 6 8 I0 12
40 yw | i.,.- w
Time in minutes

FIGURE 4. Conventional and fixed-frequency


80 Bekesy tracings in a 42-year-old male with left
Meniere's disease: A, conventional tracings; B,
fixed-frequency tracings. Loudness recruit-
IOO ment, as measured by the alternate binaural
2 4 6 8 I0 12 loudness balance test, was present at 1000 and
4000 cps but absent at 250 cps. T h e PB score
Time in mbutm at SL --- 25 db was 24%. Bekesy tracings
B are type II.

FtGUR~ 3. Conventional and fixed-frequency


Bekesy tracings in a 31-year-old female with however, a small number that, for one
left otosclerosis: A, conventional tracings; reason or another, do not appear to fit
B, fixed-frequency tracings. Loudness re-
cruitment, as measured by the alternate
any of the four classic patterns. They
binaural loudness balance test, was absent at may be designated by the label 'ques-
250, 1000, and 4000 cps on the test ear. T h e tionable.' In some of these, excessive
PB score at S L - - 2 5 db was 100%. Bekesy
tracings are type I. tracing width (30 to 40 db) obscures

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
282 ]ourllal of Speech and Hearing Research

the relationship between C and I. In


others the conventional and fixed-
frequency results are contradictory,
and, in still others, high-pitched tinnitus
20
appears to invalidate the continuous
tracing. There is no unique common- .~ 4O t
t
6o
.1-

8O

20 'If'
I00
125 250 500 IK 2K 4K 8K
"i6 4O
,AN Frequency in cps
.! vv/v'
.I~ 60
I I
250 4K
80 . I

I00 2O
125 250 500 v\lK ?-K 4K 8K
Frequerfcy in cps
4O
_=
60 ~A AAAAL~
I 8
250 4K
8O

20 I00
2 '4 6 8 I0
.r
"rime in minutes
4o

1
B
"~ 60 FIGURE 6. Preoperative c o n v e n t i o n a l a n d

8O

I00
I~ fixed-frequency Bekesy tracings in a 51-year-
old female with a surgically confirmed left
acoustic neurinoma: A, conventional tracings;
B, fixed-frequency tracings. Loudness recruit-
ment, as measured by the alternate binaural
2 4 6 8 I0 12
loudness balance test, was absent at 250, I000,
"lime in minutes and 4000 cps. The PB score at SL = 25 db
was 58%. Bekesy tracings are type IV.

FIGt:RE 5. Preoperative c o n v e n t i o n a l a n d ality to these questionable tracings.


fixed-frequency Bekesy tracings in a 47-vear- They seem, instead, to reflect a general
old female with a surgically confirmed right
acoustic neurinoma: A, conventional tracings;
lack of validity. T h i s category en-
B, fixed-frequency tracings. Loudness recruit- compasses only a relatively small per-
ment, as measured by the alternate binaural centage of tracings and does not seem
loudness balance test, was absent at 4000 cps.
The PB score at SL -- 25 db was 26%.
to be unique to any particular etiology
Bekesv tracings are type III. or site of lesion.

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Jerger: Bekesy Audiometry, Auditory Disorders 283

Illustrative Cases. Figures 3, 4, 5, and cps is quite dramatic. On the left, or


6 illustrate these four basic types of unaffected, ear, C and I are horizontal
tracings as they occur in actual sub- and overlap. On the right, or affected,
jects. Figure 3 shows Bekesy tracitlgs ear, however, I is stable but C drops
in a case of unilateral otosclerosis. Con- over 60 db to the limit of the equipment
ventional tracings are type I. The con- in less than 60 seconds.
tinuous tracing and the interrupted The fact that such a phenomenal
tracing overlap, and the width or ampli- drop should occur at all is remarkable.
tude of the continuous tracing remains That it should occur for a frequency
essentially normal (that is, about 10 as low as 250 cps is even more remark-
db). Fixed-frequency tracings are also able. Exploration at lower frequencies
type I. They show essentially horizontal in this subject revealed the same steadily
tracings, with the continuous and inter- progressive drop in the C trace at 100
rupted interweaving at all test fre- cps, the lowest frequency obtainable
quencies. from the equipment.
Figure 4 shows test results in a case Figure 6 illustrates the type IV trac-
of unilateral Meniere's disease. Here, ing in another surgically-confirmed
the Bekesy tracings are clearly type II. acoustic neurinoma. Neither conven-
On the conventional tracing C breaks tional nor fixed-frequency tracings
away from I at about 500 cps and re- show the steady decline typical of a
mains 10 to 15 db below I out to 8000 type III tracing. At the same time the
cps. Throughout this range the width relatively large gap between C and I
of the C tracing is quite small. The at very low frequencies clearly differ-
fixed-frequency tracing at 4000 cps entiates this from a type II tracing pat-
shows the characteristic initial drop of tern. In this particular ease the C
10 to 20 db in the C trace, followed by tracing width is relatively small, but
a relatively stable level. In this particu- other type IV tracings show a quite
lar case, the width of the C tracing is normal width.
relatively small, but this is not invari- Distribution of Patterns. In order to
ably the case in type II fixed-frequency study the generality of this apparent
tracings. relationship between type of Bekesy
Figure 5 shows Bekesy tracings in tracing and site of lesion within the
a subject with a right acoustic neuri- auditory mechanism, all Bekesy audio-
noma. Here, one sees a relatively ex- grams obtained on subjects with hear-
treme example of the type III Bekesy ing loss in the Northwestern University
tracing. On the conventional tracings Hearing Clinics were categorized ac-
C never does overlap I. Even at 125 cps cording to type.
it runs about 35 db below I, and the Table 1 shows the number of subjects
disparity increases with frequency. At within each of the four categories for
1000 cps the C trace has dropped to various etiological subgroups. In the
the limit of the equipment, whereas case of the acoustic neurinoma group,
the I trace is at a hearing level of about classification is based on surgical con-
10 db. firmation. All other classification by
The fixed-frequency tracing at 250 subgroup is based on the medical

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
284 Journal of Speeclo and Hearing Researcb

TXBLE 1. Distribution of the four Bekesy types (I, II, III, IV) and of unclassifiable tracings (?) accord-
ing to presumed etiology of the hearing loss in 434 subjects.

Etiology Tracings Total


I II III IV ?

Normal Hearing 33 0 0 0 0 33
Otosclerosis 50 2 0 0 2 54
Otitis Media 6 0 0 0 0 6
Other Conductive Loss 9 0 0 0 0 9
Meniere's Disease 4 26 0 1 1 32
Noise Induced Loss 7 15 0 0 0 22
Acoustic Neurinoma 0 0 6 4 0 10
Unknown Sensorineural Loss 54 119 0 12 10 195
Presbycusis 24 15 0 2 3 44
Otosclerosis Plus Sensorineural Loss 2 10 0 1 0 13
Sudden Onset of Loss 1 1 10 4 0 16

Total 190 188 16 24 16 434

diagnosis supplied by staff members of pathology, suggests a fairly strong re-


the Department of Otolaryngology of lationship between type of Bekesy
the Northwestern University Medical tracing and site of lesion. In lesions of
School. the middle ear (otosclerosis, otitis
Included in this series of 434 ears are media) the type I tracing predominates.
69 presumably conductive lesions pri- In cochlear lesion (Meniere's, noise-
marily due to otosclerosis and otitis induced) the type II tracing predom-
media, 54 presumably cochlear lesions inates although some fall into the type
due to Meniere's disease and prolonged I category. No Meniere's case ever
noise exposure, 10 known eighth nerve showed a type III tracing. In eighth
lesions due to acoustic neurinoma, and nerve lesion (acoustic neurinoma) type
four subgroups in which the site of III and type IV tracings predominate.
lesion is less well understood. One No acoustic neurinoma ever gave a
of these, the sensorineural unknown type II tracing.
group, constitutes the largest single In view of this compelling relation-
subgroup with 195 subjects. An ad- ship, the results of the analysis in etio-
ditional 16 subjects from this group logic subgroups of more obscure origin
are treated separately because of a are of interest. As might be expected,
history of relatively sudden onset of the majority of sensorineural unknowns
loss in one ear, without subsequent are type II, suggesting cochlear lesion.
fluctuation. Finally, there are 44 sub- This is also true of otosclerosis ac-
jects with presbycusis, and 13 subjects companied by secondary sensorineural
with advanced otosclerosis accompanied IOSS.
by secondary sensorineural loss. Almost one-third of the sensorineural
Examination of subgroups in Table unknowns, however, show type I trac-
1, for which there is relatively good ings and 12 show type IV tracings.
agreement concerning the locus of This relatively ill-defined group may

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
]erger: Bekesy Audiometry, Auditory Disorders 285

possibly include at least two and pos- Finally, it should be observed that
sibly three distinctly different kinds the present results are in accord with
of sensorineural loss. In presbycusis the the previous findings of Dix and Hood
situation is even more provocative. (4), Kos (16), Lierle and Reger (19),
Here, there are actually more type I and Yantis (37).
than type II tracings. It may be appropriate to cite two
Contrary to expectation, hearing loss possible bases for the lack of agreement
of sudden onset is primarily type III between the present results and the
and type IV, suggesting eighth nerve previous findings of Lundborg and of
rather than, or perhaps in addition to, Palva. First, the discrepancy may be
cochlear lesion. due to a simple artifact of instrumenta-
tion. Lundborg (21) states that, in his
Discussion Bekesy audiometer, attenuation changed
In certain respects the present results in 2-db steps, and Palva (24) states that
do not seem to be in very good agree- his audiometer changed in 1-db steps.
ment with the findings of some previous It may be that the momentary transient
investigators. Lundborg (21), for ex- energy introduced by each abrupt
ample, apparently observed nothing change in level made their continuous
like the present type III tracings in any stimuli more like the interrupted than
of his 21 cases of retrocochlear lesion. the continuous stimulus used in the
His Bekesy thresholds were apparently present study. In the Bekesy audiom-
in good agreement with the results of eter used in the present experiment,
conventional threshold audiometry. Nor successive changes in level were less
do Palva's (27) results on 39 cases agree than 0.25 db. This distinction between
with the present findings in fixed-fre- virtually continuous change and change
quency tracings. After four minutes of in small, discrete steps may very well
threshold tracing, there was a change be an exceedingly important variable.
of more than 10 db in only one of Jerger and Bucy (13) showed, for ex-
Palva's 33 perceptive losses. He con- ample, that only very brief silent inter-
eluded (26) that 'an abnormal loss in vals (10 to 20 msec) between successive
sensitivity is not common enough to short tones were sufficient to maintain
give reliable clues to differential diag- a stable horizontal tracing in a patient
nosis.' who readily demonstrated a type III
tracing under continuous stimulation.
The present findings are far more
encouraging. They show clear evidence Second, it should be observed that
of pathological adaptation (types II, with the exception of Dix and Hood
III, and IV) in 226 of 332 sensorineural (4), who used different instrumenta-
losses (68 % ). Furthermore, the manner tion, no previous investigator, to the
in which pathological adaptation ap- author's knowledge, has compared the
pears to be related to site of lesion sug- continuous threshold tracing with the
gests that Bekesy audiometry has the corresponding interrupted threshold
potential to become an exceedingly tracing. Apparently, all previous work-
sharp tool in the differential diagnosis ers have employed only a continuous
of hearing disorders. stimulus for either conventional or

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
286 Journal of Speech and Hearing Research

fixed-frequency tracing. The present ear are characterized by one relation-


results, however, suggest that the com- ship, lesions of the cochlea by a second,
parison between C and I is the key to and lesions of the eighth nerve by a
fruitful interpretation of Bekesy trac- third and fourth.
ings. Type III continuous tracings are,
to be sure, so dramatic that they are Summario in Interlingua
easily recognized, but they are com- Un analyse qualitative de quatro
paratively rare. In the vast majority of centos trenta-quatro audiogrammas de
cases showing pathological adaptation Bekesy suggere clue le major parte del
(type II) the magnitude of the effect audiogrammas pote esser placiate in
is not great (5 to 20 db). It occurs, un de quarto categorias. Le base del
furthermore, so rapidly that, if one categorisation es le relation inter audio-
makes measurements only at one minute grammas de stimulos tonal que es inter-
intervals and seeks only a shift in the rupte periodicamente e stimulos tonal
continuous threshold, he is likely to que es continue. Lesiones del aure medic
observe very little evidence of dra- demonstra un reIation, lesiones del
matic adaptation over time. When the coelea demonstra un secunde, e lesiones
continuous tracing is compared with del nervo octave un tertie e quarte.
its interrupted counterpart, however, Editor's note: For the interest of Journal
readers, the author has prepared the above
the abnormality is readily recognized. Summary in Interlingua, an international aux-
Another aspect of interpretation that iliary language developed by the International
deserves re-emphasis is the relationship Auxiliary Language Association, 420 Lexing-
ton Ave., New York 17. As of 1960, 17 Amer-
between pathological adaptation and ican and five foreign journals are printing
frequency. Again, when marked adap- summaries in Interlingua; two American
tation occurs (type III), it may journals are being edited completely in Inter-
lingua; seven international congresses thus far
generally be observed at almost any have furnished summaries of all papers in
frequency with measurable hearing. Interlingua. The core of this language is the
But such tracings are, again, compar- vast number of internationally identical tech-
nical terms already in existence in the various
atively rare. In the more commonly national tongues of western culture. A recent
encountered type II tracing, pathologi- UNESCO survey indicated that of all existing
cal adaptation is very definitely a high- languages, Interlingua has the widest range
frequency phenomenon. The manner in of immediate intelligibility. English is second.
The Journal will print other summaries in
which the difference between C and I Interlingua when these are provided by the
relates to frequency is, in itself, a quite authors.
stable characteristic of the over-all type References
II pattem. 1. BA~s, J. L., and MtnmiNs, C. J., Re-
Summary cruitment testing in hearing and its
implications. Arch. Otolaryng., 58, 1953,
A qualitative analysis of 434 Bekesy 582-592.
audiograms suggests that most tracings 2. BEI~SY, G. % A new audiometer. Acta
can be placed into one of four cate- Otolaryng., 35, 1947, 411-422.
3. B~v~, R. C., and HIRSH, I. J., Masking
gories. The basis for categorization is of tones by bands of noise. J. acoust.
the relationship between tracings of Soc. Amer., 28, 1956, 623-630.
periodically interrupted and continu- 4. DIx, M. R., and HooD, J. D., Modem
developments in pure tone audiometry
ous tonal stimuli. Lesions of the middle and their application to the clinical diag-

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx
Jerger: Bekesy A u d i o m e t r y , A u d i t o r y Disorders 287

nosis of end-organ deafness. Proc. R. Sac. 300 verified cases and the Btk~sy audio-
Med., 46, 1953, 992-994. gram in the differential diagnosis. Acta
5. EHMER, R. H., Masking patterns of tones. Otolaryng., Suppl. 99, 1952.
J. acoust. Sac. Amer., 31, 1959, 1115-1120. 22. MISKOLCZY-FODOR, V. F., The Btk~sy
6. EPSXEIN, A., and SCHtSBERX, E. D., Re- difference limen in bone conduction and
versible auditory fatigue resulting from recruitment. (in German) Pract. Oto.
exposure to a pure tone. Arch. Oto- Rhino. Laryng., 19, 1957, 282-288.
laryng., 65, 1957, 174-182. 23. MSLZZa, F., and NENZELIUS, C., An ac-
7. HEI~COCK, L., The measurement of cumulation of cases of neurogenous hear-
auditory recruitment. Arch. Otolaryng., ing impairment. Acta Otolaryng., 47,
62, 1955, 515-527. 1957, 158-166.
8. Hmsn, I. J., and BmCER, R. C., Audi- 24. PALVA, T., Absolute thresholds for con-
tory-threshold recovery after exposures tinuous and interrupted pure tones. Acta
to pure tones. J. acoust. Sac. Amer., 27, Otolaryng., 46, 1956, 129-136.
1955, 1186-1194. 25. PALVA, T., Cochlear vs. retrocochlear
9. Hmsn, I. J., PALVA, T., and GOODMAn, lesions. Laryngoscope, 68, 1958, 288-299.
A., Difference limen and recruitment. 26. PALVA, T., Recruitment testing. Arch.
Arch. Otolaryng., 60, 1954, 525-540. Otolaryng., 66, 1957, 93-98.
10. Hmsu, I. J., and WARD, W . D., Recovery 27. PALVA, T., Recruitment tests at low
of the auditory threshold after strong sensation levels. Laryngoscope, 66, 1956,
acoustic stimulation. ]. acoust. Sac. Amer., 1519-1540.
24, 1952, 131-141. 28. PALVA, T., Self-recording threshold audi-
11. HORMIA, A. L., Difference limen of in- ometry and recruitment. Arch. Otolaryng.,
tensity in hearing impairment due to 65, 1957, 591-602.
craniocerebral injury. Laryngoscope, 68, 29. RANTA, L. J., Acoustic and vestibular
1958, 808-813. disturbances following streptomycin-
12. Hvcrms, J. R., Auditory sensitization. treated tuberculous meningitis in children.
J. acoust. Sac. Amer., 26, 1954, 1064-1070. Acta Otolaryng., Suppl. 136, 1958.
13. JF~C~ER,J., and BvcY, P., Audiologic find- 30. RECEa, S. N., A clinical and research
ings in an unusual case of eighth nerve version of the Bekesy audiometer.
lesion. J. Hud. Res., (in press). Laryngoscope, 62, 1952, 1333-1351.
14. JERGER, J., CARHART, R., and LASSMAN, 31. REcv.a, S. N., and LIERta~, D. M., Changes
JoY~, Clinical observations on excessive in auditory acuity produced by low and
threshold adaptation. Arch. Otolaryng., medium intensity level exposures. Trans.
68, 1958, 617-623. Amer. Acad. Ophtbal. Oto-laryng., 58,
15. Kovax, L. L., Threshold recoveries for 1954, 433-438.
continuous and interrupted pure tones 32. Ri3~b L., Actions of vitamin A on the
following auditory fatigue. ]. acoust. Sac. human and animal ear. Acta Otolaryng.,
Amer., 27, 1955, 201. 44, 1954, 502-516.
16. Kos, C. M., Auditory function as re-
33. SCHULXrmSS,G. v., Evaluation of hearing
lated to the complaint of dizziness.
impairment due to industrial noise. Arab.
Laryngoscope, 65, 1955, 711-721.
17. LANDES, B. A., Recruitment measured by
Otolaryng., 65, 1957, 512-520.
automatic audiometry. Arch. Otolaryng., 34. TRIX'rmoE, W. J., Temporary threshold
68, 1958, 685-696. shift as a function of noise exposure
18. LxD~.~r, G., Loss of hearing following level. J. acoust. Sac. Amer.~ 30, 1958,
treatment with dihydrostreptomycin or 250-253.
streptomycin. Acta Otolaryng., 43, 1953, 35. WEDE~V_.a% E., Auditory tests on new-
551-572. born infants. Acta Otolaryng.~ 46, 1956,
19. LmRLE, D. M., and Ra~v.R, S. N., Experi- 446-461.
mentally induced temporary threshold 36. WEDE~-BV_at% E., Hereditary background
shifts in ears with impaired hearing. Ann. of auditory impairment; laboratory de-
Oto. Rhino. Laryng., 64, 1955, 263-277. tection of heterozygotes of deafness; a
20. Lxv_at~, D. M., and RECV.R, S. N., Further Bekesy-audiometric examination of par-
studies of threshold shifts as measured ents with children deaf from birth.
with the B~k~sy-type audiometer. Ann. Acta Otolaryng., 49, 1958, 451-452.
Oto. Rhino. Laryng., 63, 1954, 772-784. 37. YANXIS, P. A., Clinical applications of
21. Ltrt~BOR% T., Diagnostic problems con- the temporary threshold shift. Arch.
r acoustic tumors, A study of Otolaryng., 70~ 1959, 779-787,

Downloaded From: http://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/ by a La Trobe Univ User on 10/25/2016


Terms of Use: http://pubs.asha.org/ss/rights_and_permissions.aspx

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen