Sie sind auf Seite 1von 21

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602


www.elsevier.com/locate/aos

The interplay between cost accounting knowledge


and presentation formats in cost-based decision-making
Eddy Cardinaels *

Tilburg University, Department of Accountancy, P.O. Box 90153, 5000 LE Tilburg, The Netherlands

Abstract

Most studies on cost-based decision-making examine the prot impact of cost reports that rely on dierent methods
to allocate costs. In practice, rms cost reports often employ the same cost allocation method with subtle variations in
the way that the cost data are presented. This paper examines experimentally the prot impact of a cost reports pre-
sentation format in relation to a decision makers level of cost accounting knowledge. Using a customer protability
report prepared using activity-based costing and presented in either a tabular or a graphical format, participants ana-
lyze a complex pricing and resource allocation task that aects rm protability. The results suggest a strong relation
between presentation format and cost accounting knowledge. Specically, decision makers with a low level of cost
accounting knowledge attain higher prots when they use a graphical format in comparison to a tabular format. More
surprisingly, graphs (versus tables) have an adverse eect on prots for users with a high level of cost knowledge. This
result has broad implications: in order to facilitate the decisions of a variety of users of accounting data (e.g. managers,
external investors, etc.), rms may need to adapt the presentation format of their accounting data to the level of
accounting sophistication of the users.
 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction based decision-making (e.g. Briers, Chow, Hwang,


& Luckett, 1999; Drake, Haka, & Ravenscroft,
The performance eects of dierent types of 1999; Gupta & King, 1997; Waller, Shapiro, &
cost reports (variable versus absorption costing, Sevcik, 1999). This paper presents the results of
volume-based versus activity-based costing) in an experiment conducted to study how dierent
relation to a number of contextual variables is representations of identical underlying cost data
the key focus of several previous studies on cost- aect cost-based decision-making and rm prot-
ability. Specically, I nd unique evidence suggest-
*
Tel.: +31 13 4668231; fax: +31 13 4668001. ing that the prot impact of tabular versus
E-mail address: e.cardinaels@uvt.nl graphical representations of activity-based costing

0361-3682/$ - see front matter  2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.aos.2007.06.003
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 583

(ABC) data is dependent upon the accounting Finally, although there is a strong belief that
sophistication of the user, i.e. his or her level of decision makers should benet from graphical rep-
cost accounting knowledge. resentation (Harvey & Bolger, 1996), research that
Studying the joint eects of presentation format compares the relative impact of graphical versus
and a decision makers level of accounting knowl- tabular formats remains inconclusive (Vessey,
edge is important for several reasons. First, dier- 1991). In an attempt to resolve the controversy, a
ent managers clearly have dierent levels of few studies suggest looking at individual dier-
accounting knowledge (Dearman & Shields, ences among the users of information (Amer,
2001; Stone, Hunton, & Wier, 2000). Firms infor- 1991; Chandra & Krovi, 1999; Ganzach, 1993).
mation systems provide managers with reports The current study sheds light on this debate by
that range from traditional tabular formats to testing whether accounting knowledge as a mana-
graphical displays (So & Smith, 2002; Sullivan, gerial characteristic helps to explain when certain
1988). Many managers use elaborate cost reports report formats are associated with stronger perfor-
for their daily decisions. Others encourage the mance than others in a cost-based decision task.
use of easy-to-understand graphs (Mooney, To investigate these joint considerations, I con-
Rogers, & Wright, 2000; Remus, 1987; Yates, duct an experiment with presentation format as
1985) in the belief that a graphical representation the between subjects factor. I measure a partici-
(versus a tabular format) makes cost data accessi- pants level of accounting knowledge, in addition
ble to all members of the rm, irrespective of their to some common control variables, using research
level of accounting knowledge. In spite of evidence instruments suggested in prior studies (Bonner &
of dierential managerial knowledge, however, the Lewis, 1990; Cloyd, 1997; Dearman & Shields,
extant literature does not indicate how managerial 2005). I create a complex task in which the partic-
decision-making and, in turn, rm prots are ipants realized prot depends upon both price and
aected (Sprinkle, 2003) when information is pre- resource allocation decisions for a heterogeneous
sented in dierent report formats to decision mak- set of customers. All participants receive ABC-dri-
ers with dierent levels of accounting knowledge ven customer protability data, presented in either
(Haynes & Kachelmeier, 1998; Libby, 1981). a tabular or a graphical format (multicolored bar
Second, recent studies in accounting only charts and trend charts). I measure prot perfor-
address the separate eects of expertise and report mance objectively as the dierence between a par-
format (Sprinkle, 2003), though such studies nd ticipants realized prot and the maximum prot
that both aect cost-based decision-making. For that could be achieved in performing the task.
instance, Dearman and Shields (2001) show that After controlling for dierences in ability and
the level of a managers cost accounting knowl- work experience, I nd evidence of an eect reversal
edge is linked with the ability to correct for vol- across knowledge levels: decision makers with a low
ume-based cost bias, and Bucheit (2003) shows level of cost accounting knowledge perform better
that investment decisions change when cost with a graphical ABC format, and decision makers
reports explicitly contain the cost of capacity with a high level of cost knowledge obtain superior
(compared to reports that do not). There is also prots with a tabular ABC format. Further evi-
some evidence that suggests the interaction of the dence indicates that graphical formats tend to
two variables. For instance, Vera-Munoz, Kinney, reduce task complexity for a low-knowledge deci-
and Bonner (2001) show that the impact of alter- sion maker, whereas tables support the information
native task representations depends upon the deci- search of a more knowledgeable user. This result
sion makers expertise. However, they employ only provides important theoretical and practical
tabular reports based on either historical earnings insights, suggesting that (1) cost accounting knowl-
or historical cash ows. Thus, the impact of tabu- edge is a crucial managerial characteristic that
lar versus graphical representation of identical should be taken into account when a rm presents
data in relation to a decision makers knowledge cost reports to a decision maker, and (2) managerial
is an open question. cost accounting knowledge and data representation
584 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

jointly determine the direction on rm protability. Even research that focuses on task characteris-
This has broad implications for the many informa- tics (both task type and task complexity) to address
tion ows within the rm. To convey accounting this controversy (Jarvenpaa, 1989; Remus, 1987;
data to both internal and external parties (e.g. man- Vessey, 1991) oers mixed results (Hwang, 1995).
agers, investors), an exclusive focus on one format Vessey (1991) argues that the representation of
is probably not advisable. Instead, rms can facili- the problem should match the type of task (cogni-
tate decision-making by adapting the presentation tive t) to enhance decision-making. Graphical for-
format to the level of the accounting sophistication mats present spatial information (Vessey, 1991):
of their intended audience. they tend to emphasize relationships in the data
and provide a holistic view by presenting the data
at a glance. Such formats are most appropriate
Prior research when decision makers have to compare alternatives
or tasks that require integration of data (Vessey,
Although accounting systems vary greatly with 1991). In contrast, tables present symbolic infor-
respect to how they package the same kind of infor- mation: by emphasizing discrete values, they pro-
mation, systematic research on this issue remains vide an analytical view and facilitate the
scarce (Haynes & Kachelmeier, 1998; Luft, 1994). extraction of specic values (item-by-item evalua-
Most prior work examines the prot impact of cost tion) and the evaluation of changes in variables
reports that are based on dierent assumptions, (Mackay & Villarreal, 1987; Vessey, 1991). Tables
such as activity- versus volume-based costing or should be more appropriate when the task requires
absorption versus variable costing (e.g. Drake the manager to extract and act on discrete data val-
et al., 1999; Gupta & King, 1997; Waller et al., ues (Vessey, 1991). Nonetheless, the evidence on
1999). This study is the rst to disentangle the dif- task type remains inconclusive. For instance, Amer
ferential impact of tabular versus graphical repre- (1991) and Frownfelter-Lohrke (1998) show little
sentations of identical ABC cost data. The unique dierence in decision accuracy between bar charts
contribution of this study is that it links dierential and tables for tasks that are either spatial or sym-
information representation to the decision makers bolic in nature. Also, the argument that graphical
level of accounting sophistication. Nevertheless, formats are preferable for complex tasks (because
most evidence on either the eects of presentation analytical processing is dicult in complex tasks,
format or accounting knowledge is found in sepa- graphical formats that stimulate integrative pro-
rate research streams. In what follows I review this cessing become more interesting, even for symbolic
literature and I provide arguments for why we need tasks) receives little support (Hwang, 1995; Vessey,
to consider the joint eects of knowledge and pre- 1991). Studies in accounting have shown benecial
sentation formats. eects of graphs (e.g. Stock & Watson, 1984;
With regard to information representation, Wright, 1995) as well as favorable eects of tables
some studies focus on salience eects. Bucheit for tasks and questions that are suciently com-
(2003) nds that capacity investment decisions plex (Davis, 1989; So & Smith, 2004).
are suboptimal if cost reports explicitly contain One explanation for these disparate results is
capacity costs (versus reports that exclude these that user characteristics are ignored (Chandra &
costs). Maines and McDaniel (2000) show that Krovi, 1999). Amer (1991, p. 3031) argues that
non-professional investors are inuenced by com- subject variation could explain the failure to obtain
prehensive income information when it appears certain eects of presentation formats. This paper
in a separate statement, as opposed to when it is is the rst to test whether specic knowledge (i.e.
masked in stockholders equity. In contrast to sal- cost accounting knowledge) is a critical individual
ience eects, evidence on the performance eects of factor that can explain when graphics versus tables
various presentation formats (Vessey, 1991) and increase or decrease performance (i.e. prots from
the belief that graphs are superior to tables (Har- cost-based decisions). Knowledge can inuence
vey & Bolger, 1996) remains inconclusive. the internal memory representations of a decision
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 585

maker and could therefore be linked to the external ables, which require a large amount of processing
presentation format. Theory suggests that more (Bonner, 1994; Tan et al., 2002). Participants
knowledgeable subjects possess internal schemata simultaneously decide on price and resource allo-
that help them to better retrieve and search for rel- cations for a heterogeneous set of customers. The
evant data, along with internal rules for appropri- task requires participants to compare dierent cus-
ately weighing and acting on these data (Libby & tomers in terms of cost and revenue potential, to
Luft, 1993; Rose & Wolfe, 2000; Spilker, 1995). search for relevant information in an ABC-driven
Knowledge has previously been studied in rela- report, and to explore the sensitivity of the results
tion to task complexity (e.g. Bonner, 1994). Dear- to various decision rules. I argue that the direc-
man and Shields (2001) show that managers with tional eect on prots of a tabular versus a graph-
more cost accounting knowledge perform better ical ABC presentation format depends on the
in a judgment task that requires complex evalua- users level of cost accounting knowledge: formats
tions of the level of cost distortions introduced in that benet a low-knowledge user may hamper the
a volume-based costing system. Other studies performance of a knowledgeable user.
argue that the eects of having more advanced
knowledge structures on the participants perfor- Representational congruence and the cognitive
mance in more complex tasks is dependent upon burden
other conditional factors such as accountability,
problem solving ability, and motivation (e.g. The theory of representational congruence
Cloyd, 1997; Dearman & Shields, 2005; Tan & (Arnold, Collier, Leech, & Sutton, 2004; Chandra &
Kao, 1999; Tan, Ng, & Mak, 2002). Yet, no study Krovi, 1999) predicts a more favorable eect
has explored whether the eect of knowledge on on decision performance when the external presen-
performance in a complex task depends on the tation format matches the users cognitive model or
way we present information (i.e. presentation for- internal representation (Chandra & Krovi, 1999).
mat) to the decision maker. A mismatch leads to a high cognitive load and a
Only one recent study by Vera-Munoz et al. less eective information retrieval, negatively
(2001) suggests that knowledge may be related to aecting the decision makers performance. Build-
the external representation of the problem. They ing on this work, the decision aid literature suggests
show that when cash-ow data are presented in opportunities to reduce the cognitive load by
an inappropriate format (i.e. in a statement of his- restoring the t between the user and the decision
torical earnings), experienced managers (with a aid (Rose & Wolfe, 2000; Rose, Douglas, & Rose,
stronger knowledge base) are better able to deter- 2004). In particular, some authors suggest that in
mine the relevant cash-ow items in advising cli- comparison to tables, graphical formats can reduce
ents than are less experienced managers. a decision makers cognitive burden. Stock and
Experience level has no eect, however, when the Watson (1984) and Moriarity (1979) argue that
information is presented appropriately (i.e. in a graphical data allow the decision maker to trigger
cash ow statement). However, this nding is analogue graphical representations that are stored
based on tabular formats only and the underlying in memory. These representations facilitate data
data is based on dierent assumptions (earnings retrieval and information processing. Similarly,
versus cash ow). Also, the performance conse- Wright (1995) argues that graphical data help
quences (in terms of prots) are not assessed. reduce information overload by highlighting pat-
terns in the data, promoting the perception and
acquisition of information relationships in short-
Theory and hypothesis development term memory. Here, I presume that these benets
of graphics apply mainly to decision makers with
I study the joint eects of cost accounting a limited level of cost accounting knowledge.
knowledge and presentation format in a complex Cost accounting knowledge is likely to be cru-
decision task with multiple cues and decision vari- cial when decision makers receive a tabular ABC
586 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

report that contains very specic ABC-related ers, or those who look at the entire problem, ben-
information. The advantage of greater cost et from graphs that emphasize an overview of the
accounting knowledge is that it typically leads to same data (Lucas, 1981; Vessey, 1991).
more advanced internal schemata (internal repre- There are reasons to believe that greater knowl-
sentations) that are stored around various ABC edge in a particular domain leads to an analytical
cost concepts (Dearman & Shields, 2001). These focus. Factual knowledge can evolve into more
internal schemata help decision makers search abstract (analytical) representations rather than
for the relevant details within a tabular ABC cost surface-level representations in memory (Ander-
report (external representation). Decision makers son, 1990). Because an analytical approach takes
with less accounting knowledge, in contrast, lack eort, a high degree of knowledge in a domain is
the sucient internal ABC representations to needed to engage in analytical processing (Alba &
make a match and retrieve specic information Hutchinson, 1987, p. 418). Consistent with an ana-
from a tabular ABC format. Based on the theory lytical focus, knowledgeable users perform a more
of representational congruence (Chandra & Krovi, focused information search prior to solving a prob-
1999), a graphical ABC format is likely to reduce lem (Cloyd, 1997; Wood & Lynch, 2002) and access
the cognitive burden of less knowledgeable deci- rules to distinguish between more and less relevant
sion makers by providing a t to analogue graph- information (Bonner, 1990; ODonnell, Koch, &
ical representations that are stored in memory Boone, 2005). Conversely, Chi, Feltovich, and Gla-
(Moriarity, 1979; Stock & Watson, 1984; Wright, ser (1981), as cited in Vera-Munoz et al. (2001, p.
1995). Hence, this t should facilitate data retrie- 409), suggest that novices as opposed to experi-
val and in turn their performance should improve. enced physicists, tend to focus more on surface fea-
tures (heuristic style). Also, earlier work by
Decision styles of users and the search for Benbasat and Schroeder (1977) suggests that users
information with low-knowledge focus on an overview and
screen all available reports, whereas knowledgeable
The above theory says little about the perfor- decision makers search for specic details by
mance eects of graphical and tabular formats requesting a limited number of specic reports.
for knowledgeable users. Such users have the An unanticipated nding in Desanctis and Jar-
appropriate internal schemata to eciently use venpaa (1989) hints at the possibility that graphical
the tabular ABC data for prot improvement. If formats do not t the way certain users process
they use a graphical representation, on the other information: some users in their study started to
hand, they may make parallel ts to analogue convert bar charts into numerical tables. In my
graphical representations stored in memory in study, I predict lower performance with graphics,
the same way as their less knowledgeable counter- particularly for high-knowledge decision makers,
parts. According to Umanath and Vessey (1994, p. given that knowledge in a domain leads to an
797), this may imply that the decisions of sophisti- analytical focus. Because graphics emphasize an
cated and unsophisticated users would not dier overview of the data, changes in certain variables
appreciably with a graphical representation. In may go unnoticed; and given that knowledgeable
this section, I introduce the notion of the decision users look for specic details (item-by-item pro-
style (Lucas, 1981; Sullivan, 1988) to argue that cessing) we might expect them to perform better
graphical formats in comparison to tables could with tables (Mackay & Villarreal, 1987). The fact
actually produce adverse performance eects for that graphics tend to lower decision times by
the knowledgeable decision maker. According to providing an overview (Hwang, 1995; Painton &
Lucas (1981), analytical decision makers, or those Gentry, 1985) may further lower the performance
who specically look for details, benet from of high-knowledge decision makers by impeding
tables. Tables indeed oer an analytical view of detailed analytical processing.
the data that facilitates item-by-item evaluation On the basis of the above arguments, I hypoth-
(Vessey, 1991). In contrast, heuristic decision mak- esize the following:
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 587

HYPOTHESIS The prot performance for pleted at least two accounting courses in which
decision makers with high (low) cost accounting ABC is covered. The advantage of using student
knowledge is higher (lower) given a tabular ABC subjects and knowledge pre-tests is that I can better
presentation format than given a graphical ABC capture the participants knowledge component
presentation format. without the noise of expertise acquired via work
experience (Libby, 1995; Rose & Wolfe, 2000; Uma-
nath & Vessey, 1994). Nonetheless, further tests
Experimental method control for self-reported work experience (see Sec-
tion Control variables) as 93% of my participants
The experimental task consists of a complex indicated some relevant part-time work experience,
management accounting problem. Participants with a mean of just over three years of self-reported
improve prots by conducting eight trials of price experience. Participants typically applied for part-
and resource allocation decisions for three custom- time jobs corresponding to their studies (e.g. in a
ers of a case company. Presentation format is typical business setting) and one-quarter of them
manipulated as a between subjects factor: partici- interned with a Big-four accounting rm or with a
pants receive ABC-based customer protability large controller department of a private rm.
reports presented in either a tabular or a graphical To ensure that participants were motivated, they
format. Prior to administering the task, I measure received a participation fee of six euros along with
each participants level of knowledge, in addition a lottery ticket that gave them a chance to win one
to common control variables such as ability and of four 50-euro bonus prizes (Bonner, Libby, &
work experience, using instruments similar to those Nelson, 1997). To stimulate ample cognitive eort
in Bonner and Lewis (1990), Dearman and Shields, in both the pre-tests and the experimental task, par-
2005, and Cloyd (1997). I assume that these vari- ticipants were informed that the chances of receiv-
ables are randomly distributed across the between ing this bonus increased with realized prot in the
subjects factor.1 All experimental materials are task as well as with their knowledge and ability test
issued via computer in the following order. First, scores.2 Two ve-point Likert-scaled items for
participants provide demographic information, motivation (in the debrieng questionnaire)
which includes a measure of their work experience. showed that on average participants were highly
Second, participants complete two pre-tests to mea- motivated (mean = 4.30, std. dev. = 0.58, a =
sure their cost accounting knowledge and general 0.62), with no signicant dierences across presen-
ability. Third, the experimental task is administered. tation format and knowledge (all ps > 0.28).
Finally, participants complete a debrieng ques-
tionnaire. The next subsections describe the partici- The experimental task and procedures
pants, the experimental task and its procedures, and
the measurement of the relevant variables. Participants play the role of a rms manager
and review descriptions of the case company and
Participants
2
The participants were 55 students enrolled in a It is not uncommon to also reward for knowledge and
four-year business program at a large west-Euro- ability scores (e.g. Vera-Munoz, 1998; Rose & Wolfe, 2000). In
my experiment, ticket numbers could appear 2, 6, 11, 15, 20, 21,
pean university. On average, participants were 25, or 30 times (practical range: 225 tickets) in the lottery using
22.0 years old (most students were about to start the following scheme. The person with the highest combined
or nish their nal year). All students had com- score in the knowledge and ability test receives 10 tickets, the
next 50% all score ve tickets, the rest each receive one ticket.
The experimental task would be similar to that above. The
1
In order to assess the validity of this assumption, t-test person with the highest mean prot realized over the eight
results indicate no signicant dierences (all ps > 0.14) in the decision trials receives 20 tickets, the next 50% receive 10
mean ability and knowledge test scores as well as the mean level tickets, the rest each receive one ticket. The bonus prices would
of work experience across the two levels of presentation format. be drawn and paid out one week after the experiment.
588 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

its customers. The rm is an exclusive distributor and (2) the task is unstructured (Vera-Munoz
of a crucial product that is sold to three major cus- et al., 2001). Participants know that customers
tomers, denoted A, B, and C, respectively. Partic- are heterogeneous in their required level of sales
ipants are told that customers vary in revenue support. Nevertheless, the cost reports only deli-
potential and in customer-specic support costs ver updated cost information in each trial. Consis-
(delivery, logistics, and sales visit costs). Partici- tent with Christensen and Demski (1995) and
pants receive a detailed task description, together Datar and Gupta (1994), the ABC report does
with an initial ABC-driven customer protability not perfectly reect the true costs, nor are the
report that presents a situation with ample room underlying cost parameters or functions revealed.
for prot improvement.3 Depending upon the In order to improve prots, participants must
assigned condition, the cost report presents the make comparisons across the dierent customers
same underlying ABC data (calculations in the and explore several decision rules in each trial to
Appendix) in either a tabular or a graphical format retrieve, evaluate, and weigh the dierent items
(multicolored bar charts showing absolute data in their report.
and data on a per unit/percentage basis). Fig. 1
displays the examples. Participants are instructed
Operational measures of the variables
to carefully review this information.4 Their objec-
tive is to improve prots by altering price (pi)
Performance metrics
and resource allocation (xi) for each customer
Table 1 contains an overview of the main vari-
within a given range (pi 2 [80, 140] and xi 2 [30,
ables used in the analyses. Consistent with Waller
90]). The ABC report is updated after each of
et al. (1999), I measure each participants perfor-
the eight trials. As Fig. 1 shows, prior decisions
mance as the deviation of his or her realized prot
and outcomes are stored in either a trend chart
from the optimal rm-wide prot (averaged over
(graphical format) or a table.
eight trials). I also compute the mean deviation
As the Appendix shows, both prices and
of actual price and resource allocation decisions
resource allocations aect prots and there is an
for Customers A, B, and C against optimal price
optimum level of each at which prots are maxi-
and resource allocation decisions. The resulting
mized. The task is relatively complex: (1) prots
metrics in Table 1 are labeled as PROFIT and
are aected by two simultaneous decisions, requir-
DECISION, respectively. Smaller values signify
ing more mental capacity (Bonner, 1994; Tan
better performance. The hypothesis test is based
et al., 2002) than a task with one decision variable,
on the prot deviation (PROFIT); sensitivity
checks also explore the DECISION metric.

3
The initial ABC report displays starting prices of 98, 97, and Presentation format
104 euros, while sales visits are xed at 52, 60, and 54 euros for
Customers A, B and C, respectively. This provides room for
Twenty-eight participants receive their ABC
prot improvement: Firm-wide prot for the starting values is report and previous decision outcomes in a tabular
only 1,614,542, whereas the maximum prot (see the Appendix) format. The remaining 27 participants receive the
equals 3,036,145 (achieved at Pa = 115.5, Pb = 124.9, Pc = 97.6, same data in a graphical format (multicolored
xa = 62.0, xb = 49.4, and xc = 83.1). Students are not given any bar charts of the ABC-data and trend charts for
information about the optima.
4
It is important to note that the computer tracks the time
the previous decision outcomes; e.g. Hwang,
that participants use to review the problem description and the 1995; Jarvenpaa, 1989). Both formats are used in
initial ABC report, as supplementary analyses use this time as a ABC software applications (Mooney et al., 2000;
proxy for information search (see Section Supplementary So & Smith, 2002).
analysis). The rationale is that the time expended on It is important to note that the bar charts con-
information gathering prior to problem solving can provide
indications on how subjects with dierent levels of knowledge
tain most, but not all of the items in the table.
and presentation formats approach the problem (see Section Because of computer screen space limitations, the
Theory and hypothesis development). charts would become too crowded if all items of
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 589

Fig. 1. Actual screenshots of the initial ABC-report issued prior to the task for the two levels of presentation format (between subjects
factor).
590 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

Table 1
Summary of variables operational denitions and descriptive statistics
Variable (Denition) Mean Standard Min. Max. Median
N = 55 deviation
Dependent variablesa
PROFIT (prot deviation: mean distance of the total 761,491 292,732 307,758 1,447,754 773,486
realized prot to the optimal prot averaged over 8 trials)b
DECISION (deviation of decisions: mean distance of the 74.60 16.46 39.7 104.0 76.4
actual decisions for Customer A, B, and C to the optimal
values averaged over 8 trials)
Independent variables
KNOWLEDGEb
KNOWSCORE (score on the test for the level of cost 3.84 1.18 2 6 4
accounting content)
KNOW (mean split: high versus low level of accounting 0 1
knowledge) Low High
PRESENTATION (presentation format of ABC-data; 0 1
either graphical displays or tables)c Table Graphic
Control variables
ABILITY (general ability test score)d 7.00 1.96 1 11 7
WORKEXP (a participants self-reported relevant 3.06 2.37 0 9 2
part-time work experience in number of years)
a
PROFIT used the following formula: Rj(P *  Pj)/8 with P* optimal rm-wide prot and Pj a participants realized rm-wide
prot in trial j = 1, . . . ,8. DECISION used the following formula: RiRj(jxi*  xijj + jpi*  pijj)/8 with pi*, xi* optimal solutions and xij,
pij a participants decision choices for customer i = A, B, C in trial j = 1,. . ., 8. Lower scores represent better performance.
b
The theoretical range of KNOWSCORE is from zero to six. For the mean split variable KNOW, 23 (32) subjects were classied in
the low (high) accounting knowledge condition.
c
Twenty-eight (27) subjects received the ABC report in a tabular (graphical) format. The two reporting formats are displayed in
Fig. 1.
d
The theoretical range of ABILITY is from 0 to 11.

the table were shown, and the eectiveness of the whereas tables display the specic gures.5
graph (Davis, 1989, p. 497; Tufte, 2001) would Although bar charts do not convey the amount
decline. Nevertheless, I expect the two formats to of resources consumed per customer (e.g. volume
be essentially identical in information content. In drivers for sales visits, deliveries, and stock pick-
particular, most of the items are maintained in ings), customer costs are calculated by multiplying
the graphical format, including the most crucial the amount of resources by a xed driver rate and
information (i.e. the customer-specic costs of hence they have the same information content as
sales visits, internal logistics, and product delivery) resources consumed (the two cues are perfectly
and the few omitted items convey little new infor-
mation. Both the tables as well as the bar charts 5
Bar charts give the dierent customer cost categories about
reveal that support costs are higher for Customer the same color and use a dierent color for the purchasing cost.
B relative to purchasing costs and that the oppo- Consistent with the features described in the literature
site is true for Customer C. Inherent to the features (Umanath & Vessey, 1994) the bar charts provide an overview
of presentation formats (Jarvenpaa, 1989; So & and make certain trends (e.g. relative consumption of customer
costs across customers) more visible. These trends must be
Smith, 2004), the graphical format visualizes the determined by the participant in a tabular format. Nevertheless,
amount of costs per category by distances in the in a tabular format subjects have better access to the discrete
space vector (without attaching a discrete value), values in comparison to bar charts.
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 591

correlated). In the same way, sales volume is not and are less important, as the participants goal is
shown in the bar chart as volume is highly corre- to increase prots.
lated with revenues. For the total outcome feed-
back, I keep only the most relevant item in the Accounting knowledge
graphical format, namely total prots. Other totals Following prior research (Bonner & Lewis,
in the tabular format can be automatically derived 1990; Dearman & Shields, 2001; Dearman &

Table 2
Hypothesis test
Panel A: ANCOVA-models with LN(PROFIT) as dependent measurea
Model 1: Controlling for ability and workexp Model 2: Controlling for ability, workexp and
W K interaction
Source of variation Mean square F-stat p-value Mean square F-stat p-value
Factors
Presentation (P) 0.00146 0.01 0.923 0.01094 0.07 0.787
Know (K) 0.10341 0.67 0.417 0.51393 3.47 0.069*
PK 0.63676 4.12 0.048** 0.75938 5.12 0.028**
Covariates
Ability (A) 0.42559 2.75 0.103 0.35940 2.42 0.126
Workexp (W) 0.24561 1.59 0.213 0.37611 2.54 0.118
WK 0.45124 3.04 0.087*
R2 = 0.176; N = 55 R2 = 0.225; N = 55
Panel B: Mean by mean comparison of the Presentation (P) by Know (K) interactionb

Model 1: Interaction P x K (F=4.12, p=.048**) Model 2: Interaction P x K (F=5.12, p=.028**)

880000 808915 880000 804553


(13.603) (13.598)
742561 725387
800000 N=15 800000 N=15
(13.518) (13.494)
.017** .015**
N=19 Graphic N=19 Graphic
720000 .108 720000 .056*

.224 .047**
640000 N=8 640000 .057*
.153
648425 N=8
(13.382) N=13 Table 560000 604644 N=13 Table
560000
582432 (13.312) 579436
(13.275) (13.270)
480000 480000

400000 400000
LOW HIGH LOW HIGH
a
Variable denitions in Table 1. **, * Indicate signicance levels of 5 or 10%. The test uses the mean split variable for knowledge
(know) to allow for a cell-by-cell comparison of the P K interaction (see Panel B). Model 1 controls for the covariates work
experience and ability. Model 2 adds the WxK interaction as an additional covariate (experience matters more at lower-levels of cost
knowledge).
b
Detailed analyses of the P K interaction of the models in Panel A. The horizontal axes represent the knowledge categories (low
versus high). The full lines represent the two presentation formats (tables versus graphics). The vertical axes display the mean prot
deviation (LN of prot between brackets) evaluated at covariates workexp (mean = 3.06) and ability (mean = 7.00). A lower prot
deviation represents better performance. N indicates the number of participants in each cell. Dotted arrows and associated gures
represent the direction and the p-value of a one-sided mean-by-mean comparison. **, * represents signicance at the 5 or 10% level.
a
Indicates that the eect of p = 0.108 becomes signicant at the 10% level (p = 0.097) when the last trial is excluded from the analysis
(end-trial eect).
592 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

Shields, 2005), I measure a participants knowl- p < 0.01). Sensitivity checks will also repeat the tests
edge as the number of correct answers to six multi- with the actual knowledge score (KNOWSCORE).
ple-choice questions that are intended to assess the
subjects level of cost accounting knowledge.6 The Control variables
questions are adapted from test banks of cost and Prior studies hypothesize positive relationships
management accounting textbooks and are deni- between a decision makers ability and audit perfor-
tional in nature. They assess conceptual knowl- mance (Bonner & Lewis, 1990; Libby & Luft, 1993).
edge on ABC- and cost-related accounting topics Other studies suggest that work experience leads to
such as the eects of product complexity on prod- expertise (experience creates opportunities to gain
uct costing, the eects associated with specic cost additional knowledge) that can aect performance
allocation methods, the eects of value-added (Cloyd, 1997; Libby, 1995). Consistent with Vera-
costs and various types of cost drivers, the ABC Munoz (1998) and Cloyd (1997), I make no a priori
cost driver levels, and some statements on more predictions about dierences in ability and work
elaborate ABC-systems. experience among the participants. However, I
Table 1 shows a large variation in the partici- introduce these variables to statistically control
pants knowledge, with scores (KNOWSCORE) for their eects on a participants task performance.
ranging between two and six. Studies with actual Work experience (WORKEXP) is measured by
managers tend to show similar variations in knowl- participants self-reported relevant part-time work
edge (Dearman & Shields, 2001; Stone et al., 2000), experience in years. Table 1 shows that WORK-
and reliability of the measure is comparable to other EXP has a mean of 3.06 years, with a range of zero
studies on accounting knowledge.7 To disentangle to nine years of experience. Most of the participants
the eects of presentation format by knowledge cat- (92.7%) reported some work experience.
egory, the hypothesis tests will utilize a mean split Similar to prior studies (e.g. Bonner & Lewis,
metric (KNOW) to classify participants into a 1990; Bonner, Davis, & Jackson, 1992; Cloyd,
low- or a high-knowledge subgroup (see Bierstaker, 1997; Dearman & Shields, 2005), general ability
2003). Table 2 shows that about 23 participants, or (ABILITY) is measured as the number of correct
42%, fall into the low-knowledge subgroup. Their answers on an 11-item test derived from sections
average knowledge score of 2.65 is statistically of prior graduate record examinations (GREs),
dierent from the score of 4.69 for participants with questions on problem solving, analytical abil-
in the high-knowledge subgroup (t = 12.09; ity, and data interpretation. The mean ABILITY
score reported in Table 1 is seven. Again, the reli-
ability of this test is satisfactory in comparison to
6
One question is dropped because of interpretation problems prior studies.8
as indicated by a few participants.
7
Bonner and Walker (1994) argue that reliability measures
are not appropriate for accounting knowledge constructs. Experimental results
Knowledge is typically an omnibus construct whereby each
item of a test measures a separate subconstruct of the overall
concept. Therefore, such tests often achieve a low level of In the rst subsection below, I describe the
reliability. Depending on the items included, ex post reliability results for the hypothesis on the joint consider-
of the current studys test range from a = 0.15 to a = 0.47, with ations of knowledge and presentation format on
reliability of the full six-item test equal to 0.21. These levels are prot performance. The second subsection reports
similar to those in studies with a comparable number of items.
Tan and Kao (1999) and Tan and Libby (1997) report a levels
8
ranging from 0.19 to 0.43 for auditing task knowledge or 0.39 The ex post reliability of the items in the current ability test
for technical accounting knowledge. Dearman and Shields is a = 0.61. Dearman and Shields (2005) report a reliability of
(2001) obtain levels of 0.29 to 0.45 for similar constructs of cost a = 0.52, while both Bonner et al. (1992) and Cloyd (1997)
accounting content. Bonner and Lewis (1990) do not report a achieve a level of a = 0.63 for comparable ability tests. The
levels for their knowledge tests. The mean score of 3.84 (64% mean score of seven, or 63.6% correct answers, is comparable to
correct answers) is similar to that of prior studies (e.g. Cloyd, the ability test scores reported in prior studies (e.g. Cloyd,
1997). 1997).
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 593

some sensitivity checks. Finally, the third subsec- The interaction between knowledge and presen-
tion further explores some motivations for the tation format is signicant at the 5% level in both
joint eects of knowledge and presentation format. models. The test provides strong evidence of an
eect reversal (the interaction is signicant; the
Hypothesis test main factors are either not signicant or only mar-
ginally signicant). In line with my hypothesis, the
I perform an ANCOVA analysis using PROFIT performance of decision makers with a high (low)
as the dependent measure. Consistent with Waller degree of accounting knowledge is higher (lower)
et al. (1999) and Gupta and King (1997), I employ with tabular ABC presentation formats than with
a log-linear relation specication. The model graphical presentation formats. The plots of the
includes the presentation format factor and the interaction in Panel B of Table 2 conrm that
mean split variable KNOW as an additional factor. the prot deviation for high-knowledge decision
Specically, four unique cell means can be created makers is signicantly larger for a graphical
low KNOW or high KNOW, and tabular or graph- ABC format than for a tabular ABC format
ical format from which cross-cell comparisons (p = 0.047 and p = 0.057 in Models 1 and 2,
can be made to better understand the interaction respectively). Conversely, participants with a low
between knowledge and presentation format. Work level of cost accounting knowledge perform better
experience and general ability serve as covariates. when using graphics rather than tables. This eect
The results are reported in Table 2, along with the is signicant in Model 2 (p = 0.056), which con-
detailed tests of the interaction. trols for the fact that a low-knowledge decision
Panel A of Table 2 reports two models. Model 1 maker benets from work experience (W K
is an ANCOVA that controls for the main eects interaction). The insignicant eect of presenta-
of ability and work experience. Model 2 also tion format in Model 1 (p = 0.108) also becomes
includes the interaction of work experience and signicant (p = 0.097) when the last trial is
knowledge. This additional covariate is included excluded from the analysis (end-trial eect). In line
because the correlations suggest a strong eect of with the theory on representational congruence
work experience on the prot error for less knowl- (see Section Theory and hypothesis develop-
edgeable decision makers (r = 0.459, p < 0.03), ment), improved cost accounting knowledge
while for high-knowledge decision makers this especially matters with a tabular format (p =
eect is insignicant (r = 0.0267, p = 0.88). Fol- 0.017 and p = 0.015 in Models 1 and 2, respec-
lowing Libby (1995), who suggests potential inter- tively). Participants with low cost accounting
actions between indirect expertise (acquired via knowledge presumably have more diculty in
work experience) and knowledge, Model 2 statisti- extracting data from a tabular ABC format due
cally controls for this eect.9 to a lack of appropriate internal ABC schemata.
Conversely, accounting knowledge is not signi-
cant under a graphical ABC format (p > 0.22 and
9
Libby (1995) and Tan and Kao (1999) also suggest that p > 0.15 in Models 1 and 2, respectively) because
interactions of knowledge and ability may aect performance a graphical format is benecial for the low-knowl-
(especially when the task is very complex and accountability is edge decision maker but detrimental for users with
high, see Tan & Kao, 1999). Tan and Libby (1997) further study more knowledge.
the eects of ability and knowledge at dierent levels of
experience. Accordingly, I perform an ANCOVA analysis
Note that the ndings do not suggest a dier-
adding all the possible interactions of ability, knowledge, and ence in information content across formats.
work experience; the results are not qualitatively altered in that Although bar charts do not capture all the items
the P K interaction still remains signicant (while the extra of a table, low-knowledge users with graphs in
covariates are not signicant). Although theory does not my experiment can realize the same prot as their
provide guidance about the interactions of ability, work
experience, and presentation format, I also include these as
knowledgeable counterparts that use a table (i.e.
covariates in a further test. The P K interaction remains the two cells with the best performance). P-values
signicant (the covariates are again not signicant). of this comparison are not signicant (p > 0.28 and
594 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

p > 0.40 in Models 1 and 2, respectively; compari- (Model 1 includes work experience and ability;
son not shown in Panel B of Table 2). This sug- Model 2 additionally includes the interaction of
gests that the graphical format maintains the work experience and knowledge). I follow the pro-
relevant items that are crucial for prot improve- cedure of Aiken and West (1991) whereby presen-
ment. Furthermore, I would probably not nd a tation format is dummy-coded (tabular format =
negative prot eect for tabular formats with 0; graphical format = 1), and knowledge is mean-
low-knowledge decision makers if the extra items centered in both main eect and the multiplicative
of a table revealed new information. terms to avoid multicollinearity problems (see
Cloyd, 1997; Tan & Kao, 1999). Consistent with
Sensitivity checks my hypothesis, the parameter estimate for the
interaction term of presentation format and
Results remain similar when I use the log of knowledge is signicant in Models 1 (b = 0.191;
DECISION as a dependent variable. The interac- p = 0.05) and 2 (b = 0.186; p = 0.06).
tion of presentation format and knowledge remain I determine the nature of this signicant interac-
signicant (p = 0.07 in Model 1; p = 0.04 in Model tion by calculating simple eects of presentation for-
2). Simple presentation format eects at high or mat at one standard deviation above and below the
low levels of knowledge corroborate the eect mean of knowledge (Aiken & West, 1991; Pedhazur,
reversal: low-knowledge participants perform bet- 1982).11 For participants with high-knowledge
ter with graphs than with tables (smallest p = (mean plus one standard deviation), presentation
0.05, cf. p = 0.06 for the prot deviation) whereas format has a positive sign signifying higher prot
the opposite is true for high-knowledge partici- deviations (or lower performance) under a graphi-
pants (smallest p = 0.08, cf. p = 0.05 for the prot cal format than under a tabular format. The eect
deviation).10 I also test whether the eect of the is signicant in Model 1 (b = 0.278, p = 0.05) and
interaction between presentation format and Model 2 (b = 0.229, p = 0.08). For participants with
knowledge (P K) on prot persisted over trials. low-knowledge (mean less one standard deviation),
A repeated measure test (on the log of prot in the eect of presentation format is negative in both
each trial) does not reveal any signicant three- models, consistent with the prediction that graphi-
way interaction among trial, presentation format, cal formats lead to better performance (lower prot
and knowledge (F = 1.26, p > 0.27 in Model 1; deviation), although the eect is only signicant in
F = 1.13, p > 0.34 in Model 2). Conditional means Model 2 (b = 0.216, p = 0.08; cf. Model 1:
per trial conrm that tables are always superior to b = 0.177, p = 0.13).
graphical formats for the high-knowledge decision Panel A of Table 3 plots regression lines for
maker, where the opposite holds true for the low- Models 1 and 2 (predicted values of the log of
knowledge decision maker. prot were transformed to prot deviations).
Additionally, I explore the interactive eect of Panel B of Table 3 displays the simple eects and
presentation format and knowledge on perfor- the signicance levels (p-values) of presentation
mance (i.e. log of prot) by using the actual score format at each level of knowledge, which is equiv-
on the knowledge test (KNOWSCORE) as a con- alent to establishing regions of signicance (see
tinuous variable into two regressions that use the Aiken & West (1991, p. 132), for a more detailed
same control variables as in the previous section description). The results in Panel B of Table 3

10 11
The statistical tests increase in power if I consider that none Consistent with Pedhazur (1982, p. 440), the decomposition
of the suggested variables has an eect on the decision errors for of the interaction in simple eects of presentation format at
Customer A (negative adj. R-square for all models, ps high or low-knowledge or at each specic knowledge score (as
individual betas all >0.24). The eect reversal becomes highly reported in Table 3 below) is based on one-tailed statistics to
signicant when the analysis of decision is based on the minimize the type II error. Based on the theory, I also predict
resulting sum of decision errors on Customers B and C (the specic directions for the eect of presentation format at high
least and the most protable customers, respectively). or low levels of accounting knowledge.
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 595

Table 3
Sensitivity checks with KNOWSCORE (actual score) as covariate in Models 1 and 2
Panel A: Plots of the regression lines in Models 1 and 2 and p-value of P K interactiona

1000000 Model 1: Interaction P x K (p=.05**) 1000000 Model 2: Interaction P x K (p=.06*)


900000 900000
Graph
Profit deviation

mean Graph

Profit deviation
800000 800000 mean

700000 700000

600000 600000
Point of intersection: Tab Point of intersection: Tab
500000 3,576 500000 3,816
400000 400000

300000 300000

200000 200000
2 3 4 5 6 2 3 4 5 6
Knowledge Knowledge

Panel B: Eect of presentation format at each level of the knowledge scoreb


Model 1: Estimate, sign. level, eect size Model 2: Estimate, sign. level, eect size
Knowledge score b (format) p-value Dierence in prot b (format) p-value Dierence in prot
Dev. (graph-tab) Dev. (graph-tab)
2 0.30184 0.08* 228545.2 0.33777 0.05* 252933.0
3 0.11039 0.21 79108.9 0.15172 0.13 107341.3
4 0.08107 0.24 55201.4 0.03433 0.38 22988.3
5 0.27252 0.05** 176698.1 0.22038 0.09* 140315.3
6 0.46397 0.03** 287388.1 0.40643 0.05** 246599.2
a
The regressions use the LN(prot) as the dependent and Models 1 and 2 use similar control variables as reported in Table 2.
Consistent with Aiken and West (1991), knowledge was mean centered (Mean: 3.8364). Predicted values were calculated for each
knowledge score by subtracting the mean from the score. Predicted values of LN(prot) are transformed to actual deviations.
b
Following Aiken and West (1991, p. 132133), I performed ve regressions of the following structure:
ln(prot) = b1 PF + b2 K + b3 KP + (covariates of either Model 1 or Model 2), whereby k was equal to knowledge-2, knowl-
edge-3, . . ., knowledge-6 to evaluate the factor presentation format at each level of the knowledge score. Panel B displays the parameter
estimate of presentation format, the signicance level (one-tailed, see Pedhazur, 1982) and predicted dierences in prot deviation
between graphs and tables (eect size). A negative (positive) signs denote that graphical formats lead to lower (higher) prot deviations
in comparison to tables. **, * indicates signicance at the 5 or 10% level.

show that graphs as opposed to tables are bene- mats are benecial to the low-knowledge decision
cial for people with a low-knowledge score (score maker, whereas tables result in better performance
of 2) and disadvantageous for people with scores for a knowledgeable decision maker.
higher than or equal to 5. People with scores near I rst study the eects of presentation format
the points of intersection (scores of 3 or 4; see and knowledge (KNOW) on self-perceived com-
Panel A of Table 3; 3.576 in Model 1; 3.816 in plexity, as indicated by participants on a ve-point
Model 2) are not aected by presentation format. Likert scale in the debrieng questionnaire. The
ndings in Panel A of Table 4 show that users with
Supplementary analysis a low level of cost accounting knowledge perceive
the task to be more complex under tabular formats
The supplementary analyses in Table 4 explore than under graphical formats (p = 0.02). The pre-
some reasons for the nding that graphical for- sentation format does not aect the cognitive
596 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

Table 4
Supplementary analyses of the level of perceived complexity and information search
Panel A: Self-perceived complexity (ve-point Likert scale) and the eect of format (tables versus graphs) per knowledge categorya
Mean Median SD
Low-Knowledge subgroup (N = 23)
Table (N = 15) 3.20 3.00 0.86 Test of dierence between Table-Graph
Graph (N = 8) 2.25 2.00 0.71 t-value: 2.67; p < .02** (v2: 5.598, p < .02**)
High-Knowledge subgroup (N = 32)
Table (N = 13) 2.31 2.00 0.95 Test of dierence between Table-Graph
Graph (N = 19) 2.53 3.00 0.70 t-value: .075; p > .45 (v2: 0.437, p > .51)
Panel B: ANCOVA analysis on information search (SEARCH) prior to the decision trialsb
Source of variation Mean square F-stat p-value
Factors
800
Presentation (P) 728 0.03 0.863 749.3
Know (K) 260313 10.69 0.002*** 750 Table
PK 80141 3.29 0.076*
.062*
700
Covariates
.001*** 661.3
Reading speed 1010714 41.52 0.001*** 650
Graphic
600 595.8 .164
2
R = .555; N = 55 550 .147

500 523.2

450

400
LOW HIGH
a
Analysis of the item I considered the task as extremely complex. Due to the word extremely, the means are not that high. The
tests employ a t-test assuming equal variances (a non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis test is reported between brackets). Note that for the
low-knowledge subgroup the number of users considering the task as complex (scores P 3) dropped from 80% for tabular ABC-
reports to 37.5% for graphical ABC-reports. For the high-knowledge subgroup, this number increased (from 46% to 53%). A further
ANOVA-test also reveals a signicant P K interaction: F = 6.49; p < 0.02 (main eects: p > 0.11).
b
The dependent variable of this ANCOVA is the time (recorded in seconds) that participants spent on the problem description that
contained the initial ABC report either in a graphical or tabular format (see Fig. 1). Subjects were instructed to thoroughly review this
information. Similar to Cloyd (1997), the test controls for reading speed, which is measured by the participants total time in the full
experiment. The gure on the right gives a detailed analysis of the P K interaction of the model (i.e. similar test as reported in Panel B
of Table 2). A higher score represents more search time prior to the problem (scores evaluated at covariate reading speed = 2263.8);
***, **, * indicates signicance at the 1, 5 or 10% level.

burden for the high-knowledge subgroup (p > format and hence nd the task to be more complex
0.45). An ANOVA-test of the measure of complex- than their counterparts who receive a graphical
ity (not reported in Panel A of Table 4) suggests format.
that the eect of presentation format depends on A second test analyzes the amount of time that
knowledge (P K interaction: F = 6.49 and p = participants spend reviewing the task description
0.02; main eects, p > 0.11). Consistent with the and the initial cost report (information search)
theory on representational congruence (see Section prior to making their decisions (Panel B of Table
Theory and hypothesis development), I presume 4). Similar to Cloyd (1997), I control for a partic-
that participants with low knowledge lack the ipants reading ability (reading speed). Results are
appropriate schemata to make a t with a tabular analyzed via an ANCOVA in a similar way as
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 597

reported in Table 2. Panel B of Table 4 shows that Conclusion


knowledgeable decision makers spent more time
on information search (KNOW; p = 0.002) and In many rms, managers with various levels of
there is also weak evidence that the eect of pre- cost accounting knowledge make cost-based deci-
sentation format on information search depends sions on the basis of accounting information sys-
on knowledge (P K interaction is marginally sig- tems that provide easy-to-understand graphical
nicant; p = 0.076). As shown in the gure in rather than tabular cost reports. Yet, the perfor-
Panel B of Table 4, the dierence between tables mance eects of dierent types of presentation for-
and graphical formats is marginally signicant mats in relation to varied levels of cost accounting
(p = 0.062) for high-knowledge participants; tables knowledge have received scant attention in the lit-
tend to increase their information search. The erature. This study experimentally investigates
eect of format on search time is not signicant these joint eects in the context of a relatively com-
for low-knowledge decision makers (p = 0.147). plex task that requires decision makers to make
Consistent with theory, this nding could imply customer-specic price and resource allocation
that due to an analytical focus of high-knowledge decisions that aect protability.
decision makers, tables that provide an analytical This study advances our understanding of man-
view of the data better assist their information agerial decision-making in several ways. First, I
search. Graphics display the same data at a glance nd that dierent formats inuence protability
and are less appropriate for knowledgeable deci- dierently, with the direction of impact contingent
sion makers who approach the problem in an ana- upon the users level of accounting sophistication.
lytical way (and therefore search time is reduced). After controlling for dierences in ability and
The gure in Panel B of Table 4 also reveals that experience, I show that cost report users with a
low- and high-knowledge participants have the low level of cost accounting knowledge achieve
same search time under graphics (p = 0.164); higher prots when ABC data are displayed in a
knowledge level only makes a dierence with graphical format rather than in a tabular format.
tables (p = 0.001). More surprisingly, the opposite is true for sophis-
Although other explanations are possible ticated users; their prots are higher given a tabu-
besides the ones explored here, these metrics indi- lar format.
cate that low-knowledge decision makers seem to Second, this nding has important practical
benet from graphics because of a reduction in implications. There is no unique way to present
complexity, whereas sophisticated users likely ben- cost data to managers. To extract maximum
et from tables because tables enhance their infor- potential for improved decision-making, account-
mation search.12 ing information systems may need to adjust a cost
reports format to the users level of accounting
sophistication. In most rms, many non-accoun-
12
I further explore these assertions by testing how these
tants make use of accounting information systems
metrics relate to prot. For each knowledge category, I regress (Birnberg, 2000; Shields, 1995; Mauldin & Ruc-
the measures for search and complexity together with the hala, 1999). These non-accountants are better
eects of ability and work experience (control variables of Table served by graphical presentations of accounting
2) on the prot deviation. While overall prot is not aected, information. In contrast, accountants are likely
analysis of the rst-trial prot deviation produces interesting
ndings (it is logical to assume that only rst-trial prots are
to receive appropriate access to the data by means
aected by information search; subsequent decisions are of traditional tables. In a broader context, with
anchored on and adjusted from prior decisions). For the respect to the debate on how non-professional ver-
high-knowledge subgroup, information search tends to reduce sus professional investors acquire rm-specic
the rst-trial prot deviation (t = 1.75, p = 0.092); the eect information (Maines & McDaniel, 2000), rms
of complexity is not signicant (p > 0.19). For less knowl-
edgeable decision makers, more complexity tends to increase
can capitalize on my ndings and adjust the for-
the rst-trial prot deviation (t = 1.76, p = 0.096), while mat of their disclosures to the level of accounting
information search is not signicant (p > 0.68). knowledge of their investors.
598 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

Third, from a theoretical viewpoint, this study makers expertise (Sprinkle, 2003) may provide
shows that knowledge is an important attribute that guidance for how rms can optimize the many
explains when particular presentation formats are ows of accounting information to internal and
likely to provide prot improvement. While most external users of that information.
research on presentation formats has produced con-
icting evidence on several aspects of the decision
task (Vessey, 1991), my ndings indicate that a Acknowledgements
focus on a users characteristics may explain part
of these mixed results. I show that researchers can I want to thank Jan Bouwens, Lynn Hannan,
expect adverse performance eects when the exter- Laurence van Lent, Paula van Veen-Dirks, Luk
nal presentation format is not well aligned with a Warlop, and two anonymous reviewers for their
decision makers mental model or decision style. insightful comments. This study also benets from
To focus on the joint eects of knowledge and comments of participants at the Management
presentation format, I hold the level of task com- Accounting Sections midyear meeting in Phoenix
plexity constant. Future research could vary task (2005), the 4th conference on new directions in
complexity (e.g. Tan & Kao, 1999; Tan et al., management accounting of the European Institute
2002) and explore how the joint eects of knowl- for Advanced Management Studies in Brussels
edge and presentation format extend to dierent (2004), and the accounting seminar at Tilburg Uni-
levels of task complexity. It might also be interest- versity (2004).
ing to allow decision makers to choose their for-
mat rather than simply assigning them graphical
or tabular formats, in order to study the types of Appendix
decision makers who are more likely to access
graphical rather than tabular representations. This appendix describes the underlying func-
Other dimensions of managerial expertise (junior- tions, parameters, and cost allocation assumptions
versus senior-level managers) or job functions in my experiment. Customer demand (Equations 1
(top- versus lower-level managers) could lead to and 2 in Panel A of Table A1) is based on a com-
preferences for a specic type of format. mon sales response function (Mantrala, Prabhak-
Finally, a potential limitation of the study is ant, & Zoltners, 1992): the price (pi) inuences
that graphical formats did not contain all the items potential demand, whereas the amount allocated
of the table. Nevertheless, this probably did not to sales visits (xi) determines how much demand
aect my ndings because the extra items con- is realized. The gross contribution for a customer
tained little extra information content for the is equal to revenues minus a xed portion of reve-
objective of maximizing prots. Yet, some deci- nues: di represents the products purchase cost
sions, like the evaluation of division managers, (Equation 3). Given my focus on cost-based deci-
involve a more subjective judgment and further sion-making, I introduce a complex customer cost
studies could explore whether extra (redundant) function (Equation 4) with the following elements:
cues do matter here. Evaluations may dier when a xed cost (FCi) per customer (e.g. storage space);
managers have access to only a few or many met- a nonlinear distribution function (uiqei + viqe1:5
i
rics that are all equally informative about perfor- with costs increasing more than proportionally
mance. Again, one could explore whether with volume (Klincewicz, 1990); and a cost func-
expertise of the evaluator matters herein. Also, tion for sales visits (xiwi), with sales visits (xi)
the practice of presenting these metrics to evalua- acquired at a specic rate (wi). Consistent with
tors strongly varies across rms. Sometimes bal- practice, the set of customers is heterogeneous
anced scorecards use red, yellow, and green (Kaplan & Narayanan, 2001). Compared to Cus-
ratings for their performance metrics (Malina & tomers A and C, Customer B is a high-cost-to-
Selto, 2001). In any event, further research that serve customer, consuming more xed costs
links such variations in practice to a decision (FC), more resources in distribution (ui and vi),
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 599

Table A1
Underlying functions of the experimental task and ABC cost allocation assumptionsa
Panel A: Underlying functions of the experimental task (by substituting all are f(xi, pi))

Panel B: ABC-customer cost pools (CCP) and cost driver assumptions (i = Customer A, B, C)

a
Optimal solutions can be found by solving rst order conditions for pi and xi based on the parameter data for customers i = A,B, C
(cost parameters in bold). Optima are Pa = 115.5; Xa = 62.0; Pb = 124.9; Xb = 49.4; Pc = 97.6; Xc = 83.1; rm-wide prot: 3,036,145.
The ABC-reports (in Fig. 1) use these functions of Panel A. Save for customer costs, I introduced some small errors following common
assumptions of Datar and Gupta (1994) and Christensen and Demski (1995) that ABC seldom reects true costs.

and more in sales visits (wi). Customer protability Given the focus on cost-based decisions, optima
(Equation 5) equals gross margin minus customer- are determined in large part by the cost variations
specic costs. Via substitution, one can rewrite across customers. Firm-wide prot is equal to the
Equation 5 entirely in terms of the participants sum of customer prots (Equation 6).
decision choices (pi and xi). There is a unique opti- Both the graphical format as well as the tabu-
mum for each customer (see the note to Table A1). lar format present ABC-driven customer data in
600 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

addition to total rm-wide prot (actual outcome). Bonner, S. E., Davis, J. S., & Jackson, B. R. (1992). Expertise in
To generate the reports, I use the functions of corporate tax planning: The issue identication stage.
Journal of Accounting Research, 30, 128.
Panel A, except for the total customer costs, which Bonner, S. E., Libby, R., & Nelson, W. (1997). Audit
I allocate along the assumptions of Panel B of category knowledge as a precondition to learning from
Table A1. In the rst stage, I assign customer costs experience. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 22(5),
to cost pools, and in the second stage, I assign 387410.
them to customers on the basis of cost drivers. I Briers, M., Chow, C., Hwang, N., & Luckett, P. (1999). The
eects of alternative types of feedback on product-related
introduce small errors by specifying the driver decision performance: A research note. Journal of Manage-
for sales visits slightly dierently from the actual ment Accounting Research, 11, 7592.
driver and by introducing linear cost drivers for Bucheit, S. (2003). Reporting the cost of capacity. Accounting,
a nonlinear distribution function. Consistent with Organizations and Society, 28, 549565.
Datar and Gupta (1994) and Christensen and Chandra, A., & Krovi, R. (1999). Representational congruence
and information retrieval: Towards an extended model of
Demski (1995), the reports never reect true cognitive t. Decision Support Systems, 25, 271288.
costs. Because Customer B requires more of each Chi, M., Feltovich, P., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and
cost driver, customer cost and protability gures representation of physics problems by experts and novices.
remain quite accurate. Cognitive Science, 5(2), 121152.
Christensen, J., & Demski, S. J. (1995). The classical founda-
tions of modern costing. Management Accounting Research,
6(2), 1332.
References Cloyd, B. C. (1997). Performance in tax research tasks: The
joint eects of knowledge and accountability. Accounting
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing Review, 72(1), 111131.
and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park: Sage. Datar, S. M., & Gupta, M. (1994). Aggregation, specication
Alba, J. W., & Hutchinson, J. W. (1987). Dimensions of and measurement error errors in product costing. Account-
consumer expertise. Journal of Consumer Research, 13(4), ing Review, 69(3), 567591.
411454. Davis, L. R. (1989). Report format and the decision makers
Amer, T. (1991). An experimental investigation of multi-cue task: An experimental investigation. Accounting, Organiza-
nancial information display and decision-making. Journal tions and Society, 14(5/6), 495508.
of Information Systems, 5(2), 1834. Dearman, D. T., & Shields, M. D. (2001). Cost knowledge and
Anderson, J. R. (1990). Cognitive Psychology and its implica- cost-based judgment performance. Journal of Management
tions (third ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman & Co Ltd. Accounting Research, 13, 118.
Arnold, V., Collier, P. A., Leech, S. A., & Sutton, S. G. (2004). Dearman, D. T., & Shields, M. D. (2005). Reducing accounting
Impact of intelligent decision aids on expert and novice xation: Determinants of cognitive adaptation to variation
decision-makers judgments. Accounting and Finance, 44, 126. in accounting method. Contemporary Accounting Research,
Benbasat, I., & Schroeder, R. G. (1977). An experimental 22(2), 351384.
investigation of some MIS Design Variables. MIS quarterly, Desanctis, G., & Jarvenpaa, S. L. (1989). Graphical presenta-
1(1), 3749. tion of accounting data for nancial forecasting: An
Bierstaker, J. L. (2003). Auditor recall and evaluation of experimental investigation. Accounting, Organizations and
internal control information: does task-specic knowledge Society, 14(5/6), 509525.
mitigate part-list interference?. Managerial Auditing Journal Drake, A. R., Haka, S. F., & Ravenscroft, S. P. (1999). Cost
18(1/2), 9099. system and incentive structure eects on innovation,
Birnberg, J. G. (2000). The role of behavioral research in eciency and protability in teams. Accounting Review,
management accounting education in the 21st century. 74(2), 323345.
Issues in Accounting Education, 15(4), 713728. Frownfelter-Lohrke, C. (1998). The eects of diering infor-
Bonner, S. E. (1990). Experience eects in auditing: the role of mation presentations of general purpose nancial state-
task specic knowledge. Accounting Review, 65(1), 7292. ments on users decisions. Journal of Information Systems,
Bonner, S. E. (1994). A model of the eects of audit task 12(2), 99107.
complexity. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 19(3), Ganzach, Y. (1993). Predictor representation and prediction
213234. strategies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Bonner, S. E., & Lewis, B. L. (1990). Determinants of auditor Processes, 56, 190212.
expertise. Journal of Accounting Research, 28, 119. Gupta, M., & King, R. R. (1997). An experimental investiga-
Bonner, S. E., & Walker, P. L. (1994). The eects of tion of the eect of cost information and feedback on
instructions and experience on the acquisition of audit product cost decisions. Contemporary Accounting Research,
knowledge. Accounting Review, 69(1), 157178. 14, 99127.
E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602 601

Harvey, N., & Bolger, F. (1996). Graphs versus tables: Eects Moriarity, S. (1979). Communicating nancial information
of data presentation format on judgmental forecasting. through multidimensional graphics. Journal of Accounting
International Journal of Forecasting, 12, 119137. Research, 17(1), 205224.
Haynes, C. M., & Kachelmeier, S. J. (1998). The eects of ODonnell, E., Koch, B., & Boone, J. (2005). The inuence of
accounting contexts on accounting decisions: A synthesis domain knowledge and task complexity on tax profession-
of cognitive and economic perspectives in accounting als compliance recommendations. Accounting, Organiza-
experimentation. Journal of Accounting Literature, 17, tions and Society, 30(2), 145165.
97136. Painton, S., & Gentry, J. W. (1985). Another look at the impact
Hwang, M. I. (1995). The eectiveness of graphic and tabular of information presentation format. Journal of Consumer
presentation under time pressure and task complexity. Research, 12(2), 240244.
Information Resource Management Journal, 8(3), 2531. Pedhazur, E. J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research
Jarvenpaa, S. L. (1989). The eects of task demands and (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
graphical format on information processing strategies. Remus, W. (1987). A study of graphical and tabular displays
Management Science, 35(3), 285303. and their interaction with environmental complexity. Man-
Kaplan, R. S., & Narayanan, V. G. (2001). Measuring and agement Science, 33(9), 12001204.
managing customer protability. Journal of Cost Manage- Rose, J. M., & Wolfe, C. J. (2000). The eects of system design
ment(September/October), 515. alternatives on the acquisition of tax knowledge from a
Klincewicz, J. G. (1990). Solving a freight transport problem computerized tax decision aid. Accounting, Organizations
using facility location techniques. Operations Research, and Society, 25, 285306.
38(2), 99109. Rose, J. M., Douglas, R. F., & Rose, A. M. (2004). Aective
Libby, R. (1981). Accounting and Human Information Process- responses to nancial data and multimedia: The eects of
ing. Englewood Clis, NJ: Prentice-Hall. information load and cognitive load. International Journal
Libby, R. (1995). The role of knowledge and memory in audit of Accounting Information Systems, 5, 524.
judgment. In R. Ashton & A. Ashton (Eds.), Judgment and Shields, M. D. (1995). An empirical analysis of rms imple-
Decision-Making Research in Accounting and Auditing. mentation experiences with activity-based costing. Journal
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. of Management Accounting Research, 7, 148166.
Libby, R., & Luft, J. (1993). Determinants of judgment So, S., & Smith, M. (2002). Colour graphics and task
performance in accounting settings: Ability, knowledge, complexity in multivariate decision-making. Accounting,
motivation, and environment. Accounting, Organizations Auditing and Accountability Journal, 15(4), 565592.
and Society, 18(5), 425450. So, S., & Smith, M. (2004). Multivariate decision accuracy and
Lucas, H. C. (1981). An experimental investigation of the use of the presentation of accounting information. Accounting
computer-based graphics in decision-making. Management Forum, 283305.
Science, 27(7), 757768. Spilker, B. C. (1995). The eects of time pressure and
Luft, J. (1994). Bonus and penalty incentives: Contract choice knowledge on key word selection behavior in tax research.
by employees. Journal of Accounting and Economics, 18, Accounting Review, 70, 514528.
181206. Sprinkle, G. B. (2003). Perspectives on experimental research in
Mackay, D. B., & Villarreal, A. (1987). Performance dierences managerial accounting. Accounting, Organizations and Soci-
in the use of graphic and tabular displays of multivariate ety, 28(2-3), 287318.
data. Decision Sciences, 18, 535546. Stock, D., & Watson, C. J. (1984). Human judgment accuracy,
Maines, L. A., & McDaniel, L. S. (2000). Eects of compre- multidimensional graphics, and human versus models.
hensive-income characteristics on non-professional inves- Journal of Accounting Research, 22, 192206.
tors judgments: The role of nancial-statement Stone, D. N., Hunton, J. E., & Wier, B. (2000). Succeeding in
presentation format. Accounting Review, 75(2), 179207. managerial accounting. Part 1: Knowledge, ability and
Malina, M. A., & Selto, F. H. (2001). Communicating and rank. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 25, 697
controlling strategy: An empirical study of the eectiveness 715.
of the balanced scorecard. Journal of Management Account- Sullivan, J. J. (1988). Financial presentation format and
ing Research, 13, 4790. managerial decision-making: Tables versus graphics. Man-
Mantrala, M. K., Prabhakant, S., & Zoltners, A. (1992). Impact agement, 2(2), 194216.
of resource allocation rules on marketing investment-level Tan, H. T., & Libby, R. (1997). Tacit managerial versus
decisions and protability. Journal of Marketing Research, technical knowledge as determinants of audit expertise in
29(May), 162175. the eld. Journal of Accounting Research, 35(1), 97113.
Mauldin, E. G., & Ruchala, L. V. (1999). Towards a meta- Tan, H. T., & Kao, A. (1999). Accountability eects on
theory of accounting information systems. Accounting, auditors performance: the inuence of knowledge, problem
Organizations and Society, 24, 317331. solving ability, and task complexity. Journal of Accounting
Mooney, J. L., Rogers, C., & Wright, H. (2000). A guide to Research, 37(1), 209223.
selecting cost management software. Journal of Corporate Tan, H. T., Ng, B. P., & Mak, W. Y. (2002). The eects of task
Accounting & Finance, 11(3), 1927. complexity on auditors performance: The impact of
602 E. Cardinaels / Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 (2008) 582602

accountability and knowledge. Auditing: A Journal of Vessey, I. (1991). Cognitive t: A theory-based analysis of the
Practice and Theory, 21(2), 8195. graphs versus tables literature. Decision Sciences, 22(2),
Tufte, E. (2001). The visual display of quantitative information 219240.
(second ed.). Cheshire, CT: Graphics Press. Waller, W., Shapiro, B., & Sevcik, G. (1999). Do cost-based
Umanath, N. S., & Vessey, I. (1994). Multiattribute data pricing biases persist in laboratory markets? Accounting,
presentation and human judgment: A cognitive t perspec- Organizations and Society, 24(8), 717739.
tive. Decision Sciences, 25, 795823. Wood, S. L., & Lynch, J. G. (2002). Prior knowledge and
Vera-Munoz, S. C. (1998). The eects of accounting knowledge complacency in new product learning. Journal of Consumer
and context on the omission of opportunity costs in Research, 29(3), 416426.
resource allocation decisions. Accounting Review, 73(1), Wright, W. F. (1995). Superior loan collectibility judgments
4772. given graphical displays. Auditing: A Journal of Theory and
Vera-Munoz, S. C., Kinney, W. R., & Bonner, S. E. (2001). The Practice, 144154.
eects of domain experience and task presentation format Yates, J. (1985). Graphics as a managerial tool. A case study of
on accountants information relevance assurance. Account- DuPonts use of graphs in the early twentieth century.
ing Review, 76(3), 405429. Journal of Business Communications, 22, 533.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen