Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

J Neurophysiol 113: 883 889, 2015.

First published November 12, 2014; doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014.

What does a neuron learn from multisensory experience?

Jinghong Xu,1* Liping Yu,1* Terrence R. Stanford,2 Benjamin A. Rowland,2 and Barry E. Stein2
1
School of Life Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China; and 2Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy,
Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston-Salem, North Carolina
Submitted 11 April 2014; accepted in final form 11 November 2014

Xu J, Yu L, Stanford TR, Rowland BA, Stein BE. What does tion taking place among other modalities (Hotting et al. 2004;
a neuron learn from multisensory experience? J Neurophysiol Occelli et al. 2012; Wallace et al. 2004).
113: 883 889, 2015. First published November 12, 2014; Nevertheless, explicit proof for this hypothesis has not yet
doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014.The brains ability to integrate infor- been provided. The alternative possibility, that neurons do not
mation from different senses is acquired only after extensive sensory develop a general integrative capability, but one specific to
experience. However, whether early life experience instantiates a

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


each cross-modal pairing, cannot yet be excluded. Although
general integrative capacity in multisensory neurons or one limited to this alternative would involve a more nuanced experiential
the particular cross-modal stimulus combinations to which one has process, and greater sophistication in the learning rules oper-
been exposed is not known. By selectively restricting either visual-
ating at the level of the individual multisensory neuron, it is no
nonvisual or auditory-nonauditory experience during the first few
less possible. Distinguishing between these alternatives would
months of life, the present study found that trisensory neurons in cat
superior colliculus (as well as their bisensory counterparts) became
be straightforward if, for example, obtaining the cross-modal
adapted to the cross-modal stimulus combinations specific to each experience with visual and auditory cues necessary for a
rearing environment. Thus, even at maturity, trisensory neurons did trisensory (visual-auditory-somatosensory) SC neuron to inte-
not integrate all cross-modal stimulus combinations to which they grate visual and auditory inputs could be shown to be sufficient
were capable of responding, but only those that had been linked via for it to integrate auditory and somatosensory cues and vice
experience to constitute a coherent spatiotemporal event. This selec- versa. The present experiments directly examined this issue by
tive maturational process determines which environmental events will rearing animals in environments that compromised either vi-
become the most effective targets for superior colliculus-mediated sual or auditory experience and then examining the ability of
shifts of attention and orientation. trisensory (as well as bisensory) SC neurons to integrate the
different cross-modal stimulus pairs to which they were re-
visual; auditory; somatosensory; superior colliculus; cat
sponsive. The results indicate that SC neurons do not incorpo-
rate a general integrative rule, but instead must learn each
THE BRAIN USES DIFFERENT SENSORY inputs synergistically to pairwise cross-modal association independently.
maximize the detection and identification of environmental
events (Stein and Meredith 1993). This fundamental capacity MATERIALS AND METHODS
for multisensory integration is critical for survival, but is
neither inborn nor predetermined and, as shown by studies of All experimental protocols complied with the National Institutes of
multisensory neurons in the superior colliculus (SC), is ac- Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th Ed.
quired only after extensive postnatal experience with cross- (NRC 2011), and were approved by the Animal Care and Use
modal cues. Only then can cross-modal inputs be integrated to Committee of Wake Forest School of Medicine, an Association for the
Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care Interna-
enhance the responses of SC neurons and the detection and tional-accredited institution.
orientation behaviors that depend on them (Stein 2012).
However, the nature of the maturational change that takes
Experimental Groups
place in these neurons is not yet clear. One assumption is that
once a neuron has had sufficient experience to develop the A total of nine cats (six males and three females) were used: three
underlying neural mechanisms required for multisensory inte- were reared in standard housing conditions, three were reared in a
gration, it is able to integrate any of the cross-modal inputs to dark room from birth, and three were raised from birth in a room with
which it is responsive (Wallace et al. 2004). In short, its continuous 80-dB omnidirectional broadband noise provided by
general integrative capability has matured. Two complemen- speakers (n 5) located on all sides of their pen. Daily care
tary observations support the view. First, that SC neurons observations and routine veterinary procedures for dark-reared ani-
mals were conducted using infrared goggles and an infrared viewing
trained to integrate a single pair of cross-modal inputs (i.e.,
system that was activated periodically to allow the animals to be
visual-auditory) are capable of integrating them even when the monitored from an adjacent room. In all other relevant respects, the
physical features of the visual and auditory stimuli are sub- housing and care provided was the same for all animals.
stantially altered (Xu et al. 2012), and second, that early Efforts were made to preclude any confounding visual and auditory
deprivation in one sensory modality not only affects multisen- experiences in the dark-reared and noise-reared groups by anesthetiz-
sory integration involving that modality, but also the integra- ing animals in their home cages and maintaining anesthesia through-
out experimentation. Thus, care was taken to minimize the exposure
* J. Xu and L. Yu contributed equally to this work. of animals to restricted modalities. Despite these efforts, however, no
Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: B. E. Stein, Dept. of sensory restriction paradigm can be absolute and simultaneously
Neurobiology and Anatomy, Wake Forest School of Medicine, Winston- allow evaluation of those senses. Minimal cross-modal experience can
Salem, North Carolina 27157 (e-mail: bestein@wakehealth.edu). be acquired by animals during electrophysiological testing, albeit no

www.jn.org 0022-3077/15 Copyright 2015 the American Physiological Society 883


884 WHAT DOES A NEURON LEARN FROM MULTISENSORY EXPERIENCE?

experimentally-induced effects were noted over the comparatively three times the elevation of the action potential amplitude above
short series of recording experiments. In the case of noise-reared background noise. The neural activity was recorded and amplified and
animals, some patterned auditory stimuli were expected to evade routed to an oscilloscope, audio monitor, and computer for online and
masking by the omnidirectional noise stimulus. Noise-reared and offline analyses. At the end of the recording session, saline (40 50
dark-reared cats began recording sessions at 1214 mo of age, and had ml) was given subcutaneously as needed, and anesthetic and paralytic
9 12 and 8 15 recording experiments respectively in both SCs. agents were discontinued. Once stable respiration and locomotion
returned, the animal was returned to its home cage.
Implantation Surgery and Electrophysiological Recording Sensory-responsive neurons were identified using a variety of
visual, auditory and somatosensory search stimuli. Visual search
Animals were anesthetized with ketamine hydrochloride (20 mg/kg stimuli consisted of moving or flashed bars of light back-projected
im) and acepromazine maleate (0.1 mg/kg im), intubated through the from an LC 4445 Philips projector onto a tangent screen placed in
mouth, and then placed in a stereotaxic apparatus. Isoflurance anes- front of the animal. Auditory search stimuli consisted of broadband
thesia (2 4%) was then induced and maintained (1.52%) during (20 20,000 Hz) noise bursts and tones delivered from speakers placed
surgery. Expiratory CO2, blood pressure, and heart rate were contin- around the animal and manually produced sounds. Somatosensory
uously monitored using a digital vital signs monitor (VetSpecs search stimuli consisted of taps, brushes, and manual manipulation of
VSM7), and body temperature was maintained at 3738C with a deep tissue and movement of joints. Whenever a multisensory neuron
heating pad. A recording chamber that provided access to the SC was was identified, its receptive fields were mapped using conventional

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


placed over a craniotomy and attached to the skull with screws and methods (Xu et al. 2012). Visual, auditory and somatosensory recep-
dental acrylic. Dexamethasone sodium phosphate was administered tive fields were transferred to standardized representations of visual-
prior to surgery (1 mg/kg im, once daily, 1 day prior to surgery; once auditory space and a diagram of the body surface (Stein and Meredith
daily perisurgery) and after surgery (0.5 mg/kg im, twice daily for 3 1993).
days postsurgery; 0.25 mg/kg im, twice daily on postsurgery day 4; To maximize the sample of trisensory neurons, each neuron en-
once daily on postsurgery day 5) to reduce the likelihood of brain countered was subjected to an extensive battery of tests with the three
edema. After surgery, analgesic (buprenorphine 0.005 0.01 mg/kg types of stimuli interleaved to determine its modality-convergence
im) was administered twice daily for 3 days. In addition, antibiotic pattern (i.e., trisensory, bisensory, unisensory). Once this pattern was
(ceftriaxone, 20 mg/kg im) was administered twice daily for 7 days. determined, each multisensory neurons unisensory and multisensory
After a postsurgical recovery period of at least 7 days, weekly responses were quantified using randomly interleaved modality-spe-
recording began. The animal was anesthetized with ketamine hydro- cific and cross-modal stimulus pairs at intertrial intervals of 57 s.
chloride (20 mg/kg im) and acepromazine maleate (0.1 mg/kg im), Generally, this involved interleaving stimuli from each cross-modal
intubated, and artificially respired. Paralysis was induced with an stimulus pair and their component stimuli (3 sequences of 3 condi-
injection of pancuronium bromide (0.1 mg/kg iv) to prevent ocular tions). However, in some cases, all combinations involving a visual
drift. Anesthesia, paralysis, and hydration were maintained by con- stimulus were combined into a single interleaved sequence (5 condi-
tinuous intravenous infusion of ketamine hydrochloride (510 tions: visual, auditory, somatosensory, visual-auditory, visual-somato-
mgkg1h1), pancuronium bromide (0.05 mgkg1h1), and 5% sensory) before running the auditory-somatosensory sequence (3 con-
dextrose in sterile saline (2.4 3.6 ml/h). Body temperature was kept ditions: auditory, somatosensory, auditory-somatosensory). Visual
at 37 38C. Respiratory rate and volume were adjusted to keep the stimuli (100- to 200-ms duration) were rectangular bars of light (6
end-tidal CO2 at 4.0%. Expiratory CO2, heart rate, and blood 2) of varying intensity (1.113.5 cd/m2 against a 0.86 cd/m2 back-
pressure were monitored continuously, and drug administration ad- ground) and moved in the optimal direction and speed. The auditory
justments were made as necessary to maintain anesthesia. stimuli consisted of brief (100 200 ms) broadband noise bursts
Conventional methods were used for single-neuron electrophysio- (20 20,000 Hz) of varying intensity (5570 dB sound pressure level
logical recording as reported in the past (Xu et al. 2012). A glass- against an ambient background of 51.452.7 dB sound pressure
coated tungsten electrode (tip diameter: 13 m, impedance: 13 M level), originating from any of 15 hoop-mounted speakers placed 15
at 1 kHz) was advanced through the multisensory (i.e., deeper) layers apart and 15 cm from the animals head on a rotating hoop. Somato-
of the SC using a hydraulic microdrive to search for single neurons. sensory stimuli consisted of vertical displacements of the hair and skin
An isolated single neuron was identified using a threshold criterion of with a computer-controlled tactile probe. The amplitude and velocity

Table 1. Trisensory neurons: incidence and magnitude of multisensory integration


VA VS AS
Integration Nonintegration Integration Nonintegration Integration Nonintegration

Control
Incidence 84 (47/56) 16 (9/56) 77 (43/56) 23 (13/56) 82 (46/56) 18 (10/56)
ME 104 7 24 7 88 6 24 6 106 7 16 6
Mean ME 91 7 73 6 90 7
Dark-reared
Incidence 17 (8/47) 83 (39/47) 11 (5/47) 89 (42/47) 77 (36/47) 23 (11/47)
ME 87 11 64 90 16 84 104 8 14 9
Mean ME 20 6 17 5 83 8
Noise-reared
Incidence 22 (11/51) 78 (40/51) 75 (38/51) 25 (13/51) 25 (13/51) 75 (38/51)
ME 85 8 11 5 117 9 15 6 94 9 34
Mean ME 27 6 91 9 26 7

Incidence: percentage of neurons showing multisensory enhancement, which is defined as a significant increase in the number of impulses to the
combined cross-modal stimuli compared with the most effective single-modality stimulus (t-test, P 0.05). ME, the magnitude of multisensory
enhancement. ME [(CM SMmax)/SMmax] 100, where CM represents the mean magnitude of the multisensory response and SMmax represents the
magnitude of the response evoked by the more effective modality-specific stimulus. VA, visual-auditory; VS, visual-somatosensory; AS, auditory-somatosensory.

J Neurophysiol doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014 www.jn.org


WHAT DOES A NEURON LEARN FROM MULTISENSORY EXPERIENCE? 885

Table 2. Bisensory neurons: incidence and magnitude of multisensory integration


VA VS AS
Integration Nonintegration Integration Nonintegration Integration Nonintegration

Control
Incidence 77 (58/75) 23 (17/75) 71 (29/41) 29 (12/41) 87 (26/30) 13 (4/30)
ME 98 6 11 7 91 9 26 7 90 7 53 4
Mean ME 78 7 72 8 86 8
Dark-reared
Incidence 10 (7/73) 90 (66/73) 14 (4/28) 86 (24/28) 72 (18/25) 28 (7/25)
ME 58 10 73 60 12 1 4 100 12 31 3
Mean ME 12 3 86 81 11
Noise-reared
Incidence 19 (14/74) 81 (60/74) 74 (29/39) 26 (10/39) 22 (5/23) 78 (18/23)
ME 84 9 43 117 11 17 5 76 9 10 6
Mean ME 19 5 91 11 24 8

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


of the probe could be varied independently. Stimulus intensities were reared group reared with omnidirectional sound (see MATERIALS
adjusted to evoke weak unisensory responses (consisting of few im- AND METHODS). A total of 562 SC neurons (154 trisensory and
pulses) in each neuron independently to maximize the amplitude of 408 bisensory) were recorded in these animals (see Tables 1
multisensory enhancement (ME) according to the principle of inverse and 2), with all experimental groups exhibiting all possible
effectiveness (Meredith and Stein 1986) in each neuron of each group.
Impulse times (1-ms resolution) were recorded for each trial and
modality convergence patterns (Fig. 1). However, because
analyzed offline. The response window was defined for each neuron in trisensory neurons were expressly sought in each animal and in
the same way by using a geometric algorithm based on the cumulative each condition by testing each neuron encountered in each
impulse count described in an earlier study (Rowland et al. 2007). The electrode penetration with stimuli in all three modalities indi-
magnitude of each response was identified as the mean number of vidually and in combination, their incidence, shown in Table 1,
impulses occurring in the response window minus the expected proved to be higher than previously reported (Kadunce et al.
number given the spontaneous firing rate (mean spontaneous firing 2001; Meredith and Stein 1986). A neurons integration of
rate for each condition was calculated in the 500-ms window preced- concordant cross-modal signals was indicated by a signifi-
ing the stimulus). The response to the combined cross-modal stimuli cantly more robust multisensory response than the response to
was statistically compared with the response evoked by the more either component stimulus (Stein et al. 2009). The incidence of
effective modality-specific stimulus (t-test, P 0.05). Multisensory
enhancement was defined as a significant increase in the number of
neurons showing elevated multisensory responses for each
impulses to the combined cross-modal stimuli compared with the most stimulus combination, and the mean proportional multisensory
effective single-modality stimulus. The magnitude of this multisensory response elevation (or ME, a measure of the effectiveness of
response enhancement was evaluated with ME: ME [(CM SMmax)/ multisensory integration), were calculated for each population
SMmax] 100, where CM represents the mean magnitude of the and cross-modal pairing. There were no reliable differences in
multisensory response, and SMmax represents the magnitude of the the observed pattern of results across animals within experi-
response evoked by the more effective modality-specific stimulus mental groups or across experimental sessions. There were no
(Meredith and Stein 1983). significant differences in the visual and auditory stimulus
Data were compared statistically to determine significant differ- intensities used for testing neurons ME capabilities in the
ences. Incidence values were compared across groups using 2 tests; three groups (Kruskal Wallis test, auditory stimulus: P 0.25;
ME values were compared using t-tests, Mann-Whitney rank sum
tests; and ANOVA was used to evaluate trends where more than three
visual stimulus: P 0.718).
groups were involved.
Trisensory Neurons
RESULTS Trisensory neurons in the control group displayed an equal
Three groups of cats (n 3 in each) were used in the likelihood (P 0.605, 2 test) of integrating each of the different
present experiments: a normal control group, a dark- cross-modal stimulus pairs (Table 1). Furthermore, ME (see
reared group reared without light exposure, and a noise- MATERIALS AND METHODS) showed no significant variation across

Control Dark-reared Noise-reared


VAS Visual VAS Visual VAS
17% 20% 13% 16% 14% Visual
(56)
AS (47) (58) AS (51) 28%
(65) (102)
6% (23)
AS 9% 10%
7% (25)
18% Auditory
(30) Auditory VS 8% (28) VS 11% (39)
(31) (63)
13% 8% 7% (25)
(41) 21% 17% Auditory
VS 23% (25) Somato- 21% 13%
(73) (61) (74)
(75) sensory (47)
VA Somato- VA Somato-
VA sensory sensory

Fig. 1. The convergence patterns of superior colliculus (SC) neurons studied in each rearing condition. The neuronal population in control, dark-reared and
noise-reared animals had all possible convergence patterns among the three senses represented in the SC. However, there was a small but statistically significant
decrease in the incidence of visually-responsive neurons in dark-reared animals, and a corresponding change in the incidence of auditory-responsive neurons in
noise-reared animals. VA, visual-auditory, VS, visual-somatosensory; AS, auditory-somatosensory; VAS, visual-auditory-somatosensory.

J Neurophysiol doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014 www.jn.org


886 WHAT DOES A NEURON LEARN FROM MULTISENSORY EXPERIENCE?

the pairs (P 0.138, one-way analysis of variance, range: than normal (Wallace et al. 2004; Yu et al. 2010), many of
7391%). A typical trisensory neuron is illustrated in Fig. 2A. them integrated auditory-somatosensory cues (77%, 36/47;
Dark-reared group. Although the visual receptive fields of P 0.652) with normal ME levels (P 0.54, t-tests see Table
dark-reared neurons were larger and had poorer spatial fidelity 1 and Fig. 2B). In short, there was no apparent rearing effect on

X impulses / Trial
A 10 230% 250
V A V 8
A Normal 6
** 200
150

(%)
4 sum 100
Visual 150 ms 2 50
0 0
V A VA ME
S 10 250
V S V 8 **187% 200
6 150
sum
4 100
2 50
Auditory 0 0
V S VS ME

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


Somatosensory S 10 250
A S A 8 **188% 200
6 sum 150
4 100
2 50
0 0
A SAS ME

A 10 200
B Dark-reared V A V 8
sum
150
6
4 100
Visual 2 50
13%
0 0
S 10 V A VA ME
200
V S V 8
150
6 sum
4 100
2 36% 50
Auditory 0 0
V S VS ME
S 10 200
Somatosensory A S A 8 152%
150
6
sum
** 100
4
2 50
0 0
A S AS ME

A 25 150
C Noise-reared V A V 20
15 100
sum
10 50
Visual 5 19%
0 0
V A VA ME
S 25 150
V S V 20 sum
**104%
15 100
10 50
5
Auditory 0 0
V S VS ME
S 25 150
Somatosensory A S A 20
15 sum 100
10 50
5 17%
0 0
A S AS ME
Fig. 2. The development of multisensory integration capabilities in trisensory SC neurons following different rearing conditions. A: multisensory enhancement in a
normal trisensory neuron was produced by any cross-modal stimulus combination. Left: the sensory convergence pattern. Middle: rasters (ordered bottom to top) illustrate
responses to the 3 modality-specific and 3 cross-modal stimuli. Right: the summary bar graph illustrates the mean responses elicited by each stimulus condition, and the
resultant ME, a measure of multisensory enhancement. Note that each cross-modal stimulus combination produced responses exceeding the predicted sum of the
unisensory responses, a frequent consequence of combining weakly effective stimuli. B: a typical neuron in a dark-reared animal. It was incapable of integrating visual
and nonvisual stimuli, but integrated auditory-somatosensory stimuli to produce the characteristic multisensory response. C: a typical neuron from a noise-reared animal.
Despite failure to integrate auditory and nonauditory information, visual-somatosensory integration is intact. **P 0.001.

J Neurophysiol doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014 www.jn.org


WHAT DOES A NEURON LEARN FROM MULTISENSORY EXPERIENCE? 887

their ability to integrate cues with which they had extensive mean ME 91%). However, they were far less likely than
experience. normal to integrate auditory-nonauditory stimulus combina-
However, significantly fewer of them were capable of visual- tions (see Table 1 and Fig. 2C), despite the fact that the
nonvisual integration than normal (P 0.001 in both cases). noise-rearing paradigm provides an imperfect masking of au-
Visual-auditory integration took place in only 17% (8/47) of ditory stimuli in the environment.
them, and visual-somatosensory integration in only 11% (5/47) Only 22% (11/51) of trisensory neurons became capable of
(Table 1). Furthermore, their respective MEs (20 and 17%) integrating a visual-auditory stimulus combination, and only
were also significantly lower than normal (P 0.001 in both 25% (13/51) became capable of integrating an auditory-so-
cases) and lower than their ME for auditory-somatosensory matosensory stimulus combination: much lower (P 0.001 in
integration (P 0.001 in both cases). The inability to integrate both cases) incidences than in the control cohort, and lower
visual-nonvisual cues occurred despite the fact that the unisen- (P 0.001 in both cases) than their own probability of
sory comparator response magnitudes used to determine mul- visual-somatosensory integration. Similarly, ME levels for
tisensory integration were statistically indistinguishable from each auditory-nonauditory combination were significantly
those used in auditory-somatosensory integration (P 0.169) (P 0.001 in both cases) lower (visual-auditory 27%, and
and from those used in the control cohort (P 0.073). Thus the auditory-somatosensory 26%) than in the control cohort and
absence of visual experience selectively compromised the significantly (P 0.001 in both cases) lower than for their own

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


development of visual-nonvisual integration in trisensory neu- visual-somatosensory pairings. The incidence and mean ME of
rons (see Fig. 3). auditory-somatosensory integration were also lower than those
Noise-reared group. Functional homologies with dark- for auditory-somatosensory in the dark-reared group (inci-
reared neurons were apparent. These neurons had larger than dence: P 0.001, ME: P 0.001).
normal auditory receptive fields (Efrati and Gutfreund 2011), As in the case of dark-reared neurons, these differences
evidencing poor overlap with their nonauditory counterparts could not be attributed to differences in the unisensory com-
receptive fields. Similarly, extensive visual-somatosensory ex- parator responses (P 0.882). Thus this rearing condition
perience in the rearing environment was reflected in a normal selectively compromised the development of trisensory neu-
(P 0.961) proportion (75%, 38/51) of neurons that had rons ability to synthesize auditory-nonauditory stimuli (see
developed the ability to integrate this cross-modal stimulus Fig. 3). Interestingly, the incidence and effectiveness of visual-
combination. They also did so at normal levels (P 0.207; auditory integration were similar in both noise-reared and

A All Neurons B All Neurons C Integrating Neurons Only


Normal
% Neurons with Multi. Integ.

VA VS AS Dark-reared
100 Normal
100 Noise-reared
Probability A Neuron

Dark
80 Noise 100
Would Integrate

80
80
ME(%)

60
60 60
40 40 40
20 20 20
0 0 0
VAVSAS VAVSAS VAVSAS With cross-modal Without cross-modal VS VA AS
Normal Dark-reared Noise-reared experience experience

D Normal E Dark-reared F Noise-reared


300 300 300
VA
200 VS 200 200
AS
ME (%)
ME (%)

ME (%)

100 100 100

0 0 0

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18
Best Unisensory Response Best Unisensory Response Best Unisensory Response
Mean Impulses / Trial Mean Impulses / Trial Mean Impulses / Trial
Fig. 3. Summary of multisensory integration for trisensory neurons in each rearing condition. A: as shown here, similar percentages of neurons in normally-reared
animals are able to integrate the 3 different combinations of cross-modal sensory inputs. However, integration was compromised in the cross-modal pairs
involving the deprived modality. B: the averaged ME was high (indistinguishable from normal), for those cross-modal combinations with which neurons had
experience, and very low for those with which they did not. C: in normally-reared animals, neurons capable of integrating one cross-modal stimulus combination
(integrating neurons) also had a high probability of integrating others, but this was not true in dark-reared and noise-reared animals. Their integration
capabilities were largely restricted to those combinations with which they had experience. DF: MEs are plotted for each neuron in each group. Note that neurons
with ME values greater than 100% reflect a superadditive comutation. The stars in D, E, and F represent the example neurons shown in Fig. 2.

J Neurophysiol doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014 www.jn.org


888 WHAT DOES A NEURON LEARN FROM MULTISENSORY EXPERIENCE?

dark-reared populations (incidence: P 0.754; ME: P 0.344), through which it is then able to synthesize all combinations of
presumably because each rearing condition compromised one of senses to which it is responsive. The independence with which
the modalities involved in that same cross-modal pair. different cross-modal associations are learned reveals a previ-
ously unappreciated complexity in the multisensory SC circuit,
Bisensory Neurons
one that affords it the capacity to deal very differently with
The population of bisensory neurons showed developmental different cross-modal stimulus combinations, even in the same
profiles that paralleled those of trisensory neurons (Table 2 and neurons. The biological basis of this is not yet understood, but
Fig. 4). Dark-rearing yielded a significantly lower (P 0.001 its key feature is a computational segregation of sensory inputs
in both cases) incidence of visual-nonvisual neurons capable of by their pairwise affiliation. This segregation may be accom-
multisensory integration (visual-auditory: 10%, 7/73; visual- plished in a variety of ways at the circuit (Cuppini et al. 2012;
somatosensory: 14%, 4/28). However, it had no significant Fetsch et al. 2013) or single-neuron level (Hausser and Mel
impact on the incidence (72%, 18/25, P 0.31) or the 2003), or some combination of factors involving both levels.
effectiveness (81%, P 0.722) of multisensory integration How this developmental event occurs, whether via Hebbian,
in auditory-somatosensory neurons. Similarly, noise-rearing homeostatic, spike timing plasticity, or some other mechanism
yielded a significantly lower (P 0.001 in both cases) inci- requires further research.

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


dence of auditory-nonauditory neurons capable of multisensory These observations, coupled with prior studies (Xu et al.
integration (auditory-visual: 19%, 14/74; auditory-somatosen- 2012; Yu et al. 2010), suggest that the development of these
sory: 22%, 5/23). However, it had little impact on the incidence combinatorial operations is based on cross-modal signal con-
(74%, 29/39, P 0.91) or effectiveness (91%, P 0.18) of
cordance in space and time, not the specific features of the
visual-somatosensory integration. As in the trisensory neurons,
modality-specific stimuli being combined (Xu et al. 2012).
there were no significant differences (P 0.05 in each case) in
Furthermore, they can develop even if their associated unisen-
the vigor of comparator unisensory responses among these
sory response properties are immature. Mature unisensory
different cross-modal combinations.
information processing does not automatically confer the ca-
pacity to integrate across modalities, and mature multisensory
DISCUSSION processing does not require unisensory maturity. Thus unisen-
The present results reveal that the SC multisensory circuit sory and multisensory development are not inextricably linked.
learns to integrate each combination of the senses individually Indeed, sensory deprivation will significantly impact unisen-
and reflects this in the behavior of its individual neurons. Thus, sory development in both the visual and auditory modalities
although some intuitions and observations may have led to (Chang and Merzenich 2003; Efrati and Gutfreund 2011;
predictions to the contrary because, for example, of the signif- Rauschecker and Korte 1993; Wiesel and Hubel 1965). How-
icance of vision for the functional development of other senses ever, as shown here, it will not compromise the development of
(King et al. 1996; Putzar et al. 2007; Wallace et al. 2004; Xu all multisensory integration capabilities, only those involving
et al. 2012), a neuron does not develop a general operation the restricted modality. Restricting visual experience had no

A B
% Neurons with Multi. Integ.

100 VA VS AS
100 Normal
Dark
80 Noise
80
ME (%)

60
60
40 40
20 20
0 0
VA VSAS VA VSAS VA VSAS With cross-modal Without cross-modal
Normal Dark-reared Noise-reared experience experience

C Normal D Dark-reared E Noise-reared


300 300 300
VA
200 VS 200 200
AS
ME (%)

ME (%)
ME (%)

100 100 100

0 0 0

0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25 0 5 10 15 20 25
Best Unisensory Response Best Unisensory Response Best Unisensory Response
Mean Impulses / Trial Mean Impulses / Trial Mean Impulses / Trial
Fig. 4. Summary of multisensory integration for bisensory neurons in each rearing condition. Conventions are the same as in Fig. 3.

J Neurophysiol doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014 www.jn.org


WHAT DOES A NEURON LEARN FROM MULTISENSORY EXPERIENCE? 889

greater effect in this regard than did restricting auditory expe- figures; J.X. and L.Y. drafted manuscript; J.X., L.Y., T.R.S., B.A.R., and
rience, despite the fact that the nature of this restriction was B.E.S. edited and revised manuscript; J.X., L.Y., T.R.S., B.A.R., and B.E.S.
approved final version of manuscript.
quite different. In the former case, visual input was eliminated,
whereas in the latter case auditory experience was common due REFERENCES
to the constant noise, but most transient auditory events were
Chang EF, Merzenich MM. Environmental noise retards auditory cortical
masked. The equivalence of the rearing conditions in this development. Science 300: 498 502, 2003.
regard is, perhaps, surprising given the common view of the SC Cuppini C, Magosso E, Rowland B, Stein B, Ursino M. Hebbian mecha-
as a visually dominant structure. nisms help explain development of multisensory integration in the superior
Furthermore, their equal effectiveness in both dark-reared colliculus: a neural network model. Biol Cybern 106: 691713, 2012.
and noise-reared animals is interesting, given that the cross- Efrati A, Gutfreund Y. Early life exposure to noise alters the representation
of auditory localization cues in the auditory space map of the barn owl. J
modal stimuli that they were exposed to during early life did Neurophysiol 105: 25222535, 2011.
not produce a greater than normal incidence or magnitude of Fetsch CR, DeAngelis GC, Angelaki DE. Bridging the gap between theories
multisensory integration. This suggests that, beyond a certain of sensory cue integration and the physiology of multisensory neurons. Nat
point, more experience does not have an impact on this pro- Rev Neurosci 14: 429 442, 2013.
Hausser M, Mel B. Dendrites: bug or feature? Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:
cess. Thus multisensory integration appears to be optimized by 372383, 2003.
factors other than total experience; most likely, it is calibrated

Downloaded from http://jn.physiology.org/ by 10.220.33.5 on March 20, 2017


Hotting K, Rosler F, Roder B. Altered auditory-tactile interactions in con-
by the informational benefits conferred by the joint consider- genitally blind humans: an event-related potential study. Exp Brain Res 159:
ation of two conditionally independent sensory channels (Pat- 370 381, 2004.
ton and Anastasio 2003). Thus, while theoretically the system Kadunce DC, Vaughan JW, Wallace MT, Stein BE. The influence of visual
and auditory receptive field organization on multisensory integration in the
could generate more and/or larger multisensory responses, it superior colliculus. Exp Brain Res 139: 303310, 2001.
does not do so, presumably because such responses would King AJ, Schnupp JW, Carlile S, Smith AL, Thompson ID. The develop-
confer increases in salience not warranted by the information ment of topographically-aligned maps of visual and auditory space in the
gained. superior colliculus. Prog Brain Res 112: 335350, 1996.
Meredith MA, Stein BE. Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the
In summary, it appears that to develop the ability to integrate superior colliculus. Science 221: 389 391, 1983.
signals from different senses, neurons (circuits) must have Meredith MA, Stein BE. Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence
experience with them in combination. Whether this merely on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J Neuro-
reflects that some degree of observed covariance is necessary physiol 56: 640 662, 1986.
to form the requisite cross-modal associations, or reflects the Occelli V, Bruns P, Zampini M, Roder B. Audiotactile integration is reduced
in congenital blindness in a spatial ventriloquism task. Neuropsychologia
direct encoding of the probabilities of association, is currently 50: 36 43, 2012.
unknown. However, by being specific to cross-modal pairings, Patton PE, Anastasio TJ. Modeling cross-modal enhancement and modality-
the developmental algorithm selectively enhances signals that specific suppression in multisensory neurons. Neural Comput 15: 783 810,
are most likely to be linked to a common event. It thereby 2003.
Pluta SR, Rowland BA, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Alterations to multisensory
configures the system to be of particular benefit in complex and unisensory integration by stimulus competition. J Neurophysiol 106:
environments in which the selection of targets for overt behav- 30913101, 2011.
ior is complicated by the many signals from each modality Putzar L, Goerendt I, Lange K, Rosler F, Roder B. Early visual deprivation
(Pluta et al. 2011). impairs multisensory interactions in humans. Nat Neurosci 10: 12431245,
2007.
Rauschecker JP, Korte M. Auditory compensation for early blindness in cat
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 13: 4538 4548, 1993.
We thank Nancy London for technical assistance. Rowland BA, Quessy S, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Multisensory integration
shortens physiological response latencies. J Neurosci 27: 5879 5884, 2007.
Stein BE. The New Handbook of Multisensory Processing. Cambridge, MA:
GRANTS MIT, 2012.
Stein BE, Meredith MA. The Merging of the Senses. Cambridge, MA: MIT,
This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants 1993.
EY-016716 and NS-036916 and grants from the Wallace Foundation, the Stein BE, Stanford TR, Ramachandran R, Perrault TJ Jr, Rowland BA.
National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grants 31400944 and Challenges in quantifying multisensory integration: alternative criteria,
31300910), and the Shanghai Natural Science Foundation (Grant models, and inverse effectiveness. Exp Brain Res 198: 113126, 2009.
14ZR1411500). Wallace MT, Perrault TJ Jr, Hairston WD, Stein BE. Visual experience is
necessary for the development of multisensory integration. J Neurosci 24:
DISCLOSURES 9580 9584, 2004.
Wiesel TN, Hubel DH. Extent of recovery from the effects of visual depri-
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s). vation in kittens. J Neurophysiol 28: 1060 1072, 1965.
Xu J, Yu L, Rowland BA, Stanford TR, Stein BE. Incorporating cross-
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS modal statistics in the development and maintenance of multisensory inte-
gration. J Neurosci 32: 22872298, 2012.
Author contributions: J.X., L.Y., and B.E.S. conception and design of Yu L, Rowland BA, Stein BE. Initiating the development of multisensory
research; J.X. and L.Y. performed experiments; J.X. and L.Y. analyzed data; integration by manipulating sensory experience. J Neurosci 30: 4904 4913,
J.X. and L.Y. interpreted results of experiments; J.X. and L.Y. prepared 2010.

J Neurophysiol doi:10.1152/jn.00284.2014 www.jn.org

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen