Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Developmental ftychology Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1990, Vol. 26, No. 4, 546-551 0O12-1649/9O/$O0.75

Pass the Sugar, Pass the Salt: Experience Dictates Preference


Susan A. Sullivan and Leann L. Birch
Child Development Laboratory
University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign

How children acquire preferences for added sugar and salt was examined by investigating the effects
of repeated exposure to 1 of 3 versions of a novel food (sweetened, salty, or plain tofu) on children's
preference for those and other similar foods. Participants were 39 4- and 5-year-olds assigned to taste
only 1 of the 3 flavored versions 15 times over several weeks. Preferences for all versions were ob-
tained before, during, and after the exposure series. Preference increased for the exposed version
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

only. Experience with 1 flavored version did not produce generalized liking for all 3 versions of the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

food. Experience with 1 version (flavored or plain) actually produced a decline in preference for the
other version. This was true whether children had experience with plain orflavoredversions of the
food. The acquired preference was restricted to the particular food/flavor complex; through expo-
sure, children seemed to learn whether it was appropriate to add salt or sugar to a particular food.

Concern regarding the high levels of consumption of sugar preferences for this food and for similar foods with and without
and salt by Americans has led to recent dietary recommenda- the added sugar or salt.
tions to decrease intake of these two substances. Excessive salt Several studies with adult subjects have revealed that a reduc-
intake is of concern because it is associated with hypertension, tion or an increase in total dietary sodium intake over a period
at least among susceptible individuals (Dahl, 1972; U.S. De- of weeks or months can alter the individual's preferred level of
partment of Health and Human Services [DHHS], 1988). The salt in familiar foods. The consumption of a diet low in sodium
current recommendation is to reduce sodium intake to 1,100- led to increased preference for less salty preparations and to
3,300 mg (National Academy of Science, 1980); on average, decreased preference for salty foods (Blais et al., 1986). When
current intakes are 2,300-6,900 mg (Dahl, 1972). Recent con- a diet with a level of salt higher than baseline is consumed,
cern over the high levels of consumption of refined and pro- adults show an increased preference for higher levels of salt in
cessed sugars has led to a suggested decrease in intake of refined food (Bertino, Beauchamp, & Engleman, 1986). These results
sugars to nearly one half of the current level (U.S. Senate, 1977; are consistent with the hypothesis that the preferred level of
DHHS, 1988). Research on the etiology of preferences for levels salt in a familiar food is directly influenced by prior dietary
of added sugar and salt in foods could aid in implementing these experience. However, this research does not provide informa-
guidelines. tion on how these preferred levels are acquired initially. A re-
Food andflavorpreferences are major determinants of con- lated question that remains unanswered by the existing studies
sumption patterns, particularly during childhood, when factors is whether acquired preferences for particular levels of sugar or
salt are specific to particular familiar foods or will lead to a
such as cost, availability, and ease of preparation are not yet
sweet or a salty tooth (i.e., a generalized liking for sweet or salty
salient. Excessive consumption of sugar and salt is at least in
tastes across many foods).
part a result of preferences for high levels of these tastes in
foods. Individual differences in such preferences are attribut- There is some evidence that there may be a developmental
able in large part to differences in experiential history. A num- shift in the specificity of preferences for sweet and salty tastes
ber of acquisition processes have been implicated in the devel- (see also Beauchamp & Cowart, 1990; Harris, Thomas, &
opment of food preferences: mere exposure (Birch & Marlin, Booth, 1990). However, it is important to note that this does
1982; Pliner, 1982), social learning (Birch, 1980; Michela & not mean that the developmental patterns in the acceptance of
Contento, 1986), and associative conditioning (Birch, Zimmer- sweet and salty tastes are the same (Beauchamp & Cowart,
man, & Hind, 1980; Zellner, Rozin, Aron, & Kulish, 1983). 1985; Desor, Mailer, & Turner, 1973). Although the preference
Previous research has indicated that repeated exposure to ini- for the sweet taste seems to be innate, evidence suggests that
tially novel foods andflavorstends to lead to enhanced prefer- the perceptual system in humans for detecting salt continues
ences. The research reported in the present article deals with to develop postnatally (Beauchamp, Cowart, & Moran, 1986).
how repeated experience with an initially novel food, served Preferences for both sweet and salty tastes are soon modified by
either plain or with added sugar or salt, influenced children's experience, and preferences seem to become increasingly con-
text specific in both cases. It is our view that repeated experi-
ence with a food with the sweet or salty taste added causes that
particular preparation of the food to become familiar, accepted,
and appropriate. For example, although very young infants
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Leann
L. Birch, School of Human Resources and Family Studies, 1105 West
show a preference for sweetened water, this preference was only
Nevada Street, Urbana, Illinois 61801. maintained several months later among infants whose mothers

546
SPECIAL SECTION: EXPERIENCE AND PREFERENCE 547

continued to feed them sweetened water during this time, and exposed to a plain version of the novel food without added sugar or salt.
their preference for sweetened water was not related to their The mean age of these 8 boys and 6 girls was 59.4 months.
preferred level of sweetness in fruit drinks (Beauchamp & Cow-
art, 1985). Materials
Infants are initially indifferent to salty tastes, and they begin
to preferentially ingest salty water at about 4 months. However, Three initially novel foods were chosen as contexts for the added
although infants will prefer salted water to plain water, by the sugar and salt because of their similarity in physical appearance, tex-
time they are between 2 and 3 years of age, they have acquired ture, and neutrality of flavor; they were tofu (Azumaya), ricotta cheese
the adultlike rejection of salt water. At about the same age, chil- (Sargento, 25-mg/oz), and jicama. Food history data obtained from the
parents of the children confirmed that these foods were novel to the
dren prefer salted soup to unsalted soup. Harris and Booth
children.
(1985) reported that for 6-month-old infants, preference for
The amount of salt and sugar (sucrose) added to the foods was deter-
salted cereal was directly related to the child's intake of sodium mined on the basis of the results of pilot work with an adult sample. On
from all food sources during the previous week; the preference the basis of the pilot data, one level of saltiness and one level of sweetness
for salty cereal was not dependent on specific experience with was selected that was palatable for each of the foods yet was distinctly
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

salted cereal. However, the evidence presented in this volume salty or sweet in taste. The levels of salt and sucrose added differed for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

suggests that prior experience may have much more specific the three food stimuli because of differences in the intrinsic sweetness
effects on preference for foods later in infancy and early child- and saltiness of the three foods. Through variation of the ratios of fla-
hood. Taken together, thesefindingssuggest that the preference vorant to food, an attempt was made to match the perceived level of
for the sweet or salty taste is becoming increasingly focused and sweetness and saltiness across the foods. Those levels included salty tofu
context specific. Children are learning through repeated experi- prepared with 2 g NaCl (salt)/100 g tofu, and sweetened tofu with 14 g
sucrose (sugar)/100 g tofu; salty ricotta cheese prepared with 1.6 g
ence which foods provide appropriate contexts for sweet and
NaCl/100 g ricotta cheese, and sweetened ricotta with 12 g sucrose/100
salty tastes. g ricotta cheese; and salty jicama prepared with 1.6 g NaCl/100 g
The purpose of the present experiment was to explore the jicama, and sweetened jicama with 12.5 g sucrose/100 g jicama.
role of experience with a novel food, eaten either sweetened, In an attempt to match the textural characteristics of the tofu and
salty, or plain, on the development of a preference for those the ricotta cheese, the foods were processed to a smooth consistency,
three versions of that food. Cowart and Beauchamp (1986) sug- incorporating the added sugar or salt. The foods in the plain version
gested that what becomes familiar with repeated experience is were also processed to match the consistency of the flavored stimuli.
the context for the sweet or salty taste. We predicted that re- The tofu was first drained, rinsed, and then blotted to remove excess
peated experience with eating one version of the food would moisture. This procedure enabled the consistency of the tofu to match
nearly identically that of the ricotta cheese.
be very specific to that preparation and would not produce a
Jicama was prepared to match the tofu in appearance (color) and
generalized enhanced liking for all three versions of the exposed
neutrality offlavoronly. This food stimulus was shredded in a food
food. We also hypothesized that it is the a X b interactive com- processor then thoroughly blended with the added flavorant while main-
plex of the food and the sweet or salty taste that becomes famil- taining afinelyshredded texture.
iar and liked with repeated exposure, not some simple additive To maintain freshness, all of the foods were purchased the same week
combination of the food and the flavor. We proposed to test this they were to be consumed and were stored at 6.5 C. Preparation was
hypothesis by also looking for generalization of mere exposure done no more than 36 hr before use. The 1 -tsp food samples were always
effects on preference for other versions of the same food and on presented to the children in separate 4-oz clear plastic cups.
preference for other, similar novel foods prepared with the same
flavors added. Only the specific food/taste complex would be-
come familiar and preferred with repeated exposure, and no
Procedure
generalization was expected: Neither another version of the The children had repeated exposure to one of the three versions of
same food nor another food with the same added taste would tofu. Each exposure involved taking a small taste, which was typically
become more preferred as a result of experience with the com- swallowed. To assess the effects of this exposure, preference was mea-
plex. sured at several points before, during, and after the exposure period.
Training period. Prior to the beginning of the exposure period, the
children were familiarized with the tasting procedure. During this time
Method the children were asked to taste small samples of familiar foods. The
procedures used in training were the same as those used throughout
Subjects the study. Children had also been trained in the preference assessment
procedure (discussed later) as part of another study.
Participants in the study were 39 preschool children, 21 boys and 18 Preexposure assessment. To evaluate changes in preference resulting
girls. The children ranged in age from 44 months to 71 months, with a from increased exposure, we assessed preferences for the test foods be-
mean age of 55 months. The children had repeated experience with only fore the exposure series began. First, an assessment was made of the
one of three initially novel food preparations: tofu, either with added initial preferences of six foods, including all three versions of both tofu
salt or sugar or plain, without added sugar or salt. The three exposure and ricotta cheese. In the preference assessment procedure each child
groups were balanced for sex and initial group mean preference for the was interviewed individually by a familiar adult. Each child took one
foods and were randomly assigned to experience one of the three ver- food sample at a time from a tray containing all six foods and tasted it
sions. The 11 children exposed to the salty version of the novel food with a small spoon. The child then placed the cup containing the re-
were 5 boys and 6 girls with a mean age of 54.4 months. The 14 children mainder of the sample in front of one of three faces: a smiling face
exposed to the sweet version of the novel food included 8 boys and 6 signifying like, a neutral face signifying just okay, and the third face,
girls with a mean age of 55.7 months. A third group of 14 children were signifying dislike. Within each category, the child then ranked the foods
548 SUSAN A. SULLIVAN AND LEANN L. BIRCH

from most liked to most disliked. These data formed the basis for the more 1 -
analyses. Such data have been shown to be reliable and valid predictors
of children's consumption in a self-selection setting (Birch, 1979).
Exposure period. Experience with the novel food-flavor combina-
tion was provided during the exposure period. Each child tasted one of
the threeflavoredversions of the tofu twice per week for 9 weeks, for a
total of 15 exposures. The children tasted the foods in the preschool
classroom during their free-play period. A special area, blocked from
direct view of the rest of the room, was reserved for the "tasting game"
on days the tasting was to occur.
Prompts to encourage the child to taste the sample, although usually
not necessary, involved simply telling the child to "take a taste." An
exposure, or tasting, was recorded as touching the food to the tongue or
placing the food in the mouth and either swallowing or spitting out.
Throughout the experiment, the adults refrained from commenting in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

any positive or negative manner about the food or the situation. less 6
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Postassessments. Preferences for the six foods (three versions of tofu 8 8+ 15


and ricotta cheese) were assessed three times during the exposure pe- one week
riod: immediately following the completion of the eighth exposure to Exposure Frequency
the taste stimuli (Postassessment 1), one week after thefirstpostassess-
ment with no further exposures (Postassessment 2), and again immedi- Figure 1. Mean tofu preference for exposed version of tofu,
ately following an additional seven exposures to the taste stimuli (Post- all groups combined (sweet, salt, and plain).
assessment 3). The procedures followed during the administration of
these assessments were identical to the procedures for the initial prefer-
ence assessment. preference for the plain version as exposure to either flavored
Finally, an additional preference ranking assessment was adminis- version increased. This observation was confirmed by the re-
tered to a subset of subjects comparing sweet, salty, and plain tofu with
sults of a preliminary ANOVA, and therefore the sweet and salty
the same three versions of jicama, a completely novel food. The same
procedures as used in previous preference ranking assessments were groups were combined for subsequent analysis: Results of a 4 X
followed in this postassessment. 2 X 2 (Exposure Level X Flavor [sweet and salty] X Group
[sweet or salty exposed]) repeated measures ANOVA revealed
that the main effect of neither group, F(l, 23) = 0.28, p = .60,
Results Group X Exposures, F(3, 69) = 0.40, p = .75, nor Group X
The preference data for the exposed version of the food across Flavor, F( 1,23) = 2.90, p = . 10, interactions were significant.
preference assessments provided evidence regarding the effects As shown in Figure 2, for the children exposed to the sweet
of exposure on preference. Potential generalization of this ac- or saltyflavoredversions, preference for that version increased,
quired preference to other similar preparations was also exam- whereas preference for the unflavored, plain version of tofu ac-
ined. "Similar" preparations included (a) differently flavored tually decreased steadily from the initial preexposure means.
versions of the same food (tofu) and (b) a different novel food A 4 X 2 (Exposure Level X Exposed Flavor vs. Plain Version)
(ricotta cheese and jicama) with the sameflavorantadded. repeated measures ANOVA was performed on these data. The
Exposure X Flavor Version interaction was highly significant,
F{3, 72) = 8.21, p < .0001, reflecting the differing direction of
Effects ofExposure on Preferencefor Exposed Food change in the children's preference for the flavored versus the
The pattern of preference change for the exposed food con- plain versions. Although the magnitude of the interaction
firms results of previous research on mere exposure effects. As makes the interpretation of main effects problematic, only the
shown in Figure 1, with repeated exposure, the preference for main effect offlavorwas significant, F{3, 72) = 9.10, p = .01.
the initially novel food increases, regardless of whether it was After exposure, the flavored versions were preferred over the
sweetened, salty, or plain. When the data for the exposed food plain version, as shown in Figure 2. Results of t tests indicated
were combined across all three exposure groups, a one-way re- that prior to exposure, the initial difference between the means
peated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) indicated that of the plain and experienced versions combined was not sig-
with increasing exposure, preferences for the initially novel nificant, r(24) = 1.14, p < .30, whereas following exposure, the
food increased, F(3,114) = 3.49, p < .05. With respect to gener- differences were significant, f(24) = 3.62, p < .002.
alization, distinctive patterns emerged for the children exposed With increasing exposure to a flavored version, preference
to the flavored or to the plain preparations, and the data for for the exposed version increased. A one-way ANOVA revealed
these groups are presented separately later. that the main effect of exposure was significant, F(3,72) = 4.67,
Exposure to flavored version. As was indicated, preference p < .01. Also, as exposure to the flavored version increased,
for the exposed food increased with exposure across all three preference for the plain version declined, and a one-way re-
groups. With respect to generalization of enhanced preference peated measures ANOVA on the preference data for the plain
to the other versions of the same food for the children who had version confirmed a significant decline with exposure, F(3,
experience with the sweet and salty versions, the pattern of re- 72) = 4.48, p < .01. These data provide strong evidence against
sults was similar: There was no effect on the preference for the generalization of exposure effects to the other version of the
other, unexposedflavoredversion, and an apparent decrease in food.
SPECIAL SECTION: EXPERIENCE AND PREFERENCE 549
1
more l
' more -\
Flavor sweel
Plain 2 " 0 sail
plain
-o
CD 3 -

5 -

less
8 8 + 15
less one week
exposure frequency
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Exposure Frequency Figure 4. Mean ricotta cheese preference for sweetened, salty, and plain
versions of ricotta, data from all subjects combined.
Figure 2. Mean tofu preference for exposed flavor version (sweetened
or salty) versus plain,flavorexposed groups only (sweet and salt).

3.54, p < .05, was significant, whereas that for flavor was not.
The initial difference between the preference means for the
Exposure to plain version. A preliminary examination of the plain and flavored versions was not significant, i(13) = -1.29,
data for this group of children revealed a pattern similar in gen- p = .22, whereas the difference following exposure was signifi-
eral form to that observed for the sweet and salty groups. The cant, i(13) = -3.20, p < .01.
preference rankings of the plain exposure group for both fla- As exposure to the plain tofu increased, preference also in-
vored versions were collapsed, yielding two variables, plain and creased. Although the main effect of exposure in a one-way AN-
flavored (sweetened combined with salty). The preference data OVA did not reach significance, F\7>, 39) = 1.78, p = .17, the
for the children exposed to plain tofu are shown in Figure 3. difference between assessments of preference made after 8 and
Again, although preference for the exposed version increased, 15 exposures was significant, f\\, 13) = 6.79, p < .05. In con-
their preference for the flavored versions declined, providing trast, exposure to the plain version decreased preference for the
strong evidence against generalization. flavored versions of the same novel food. Although the main
A 4 X 2 (Exposure Level X Flavor Version) repeated mea- effect of exposure in the one-way ANOVA only approached sig-
sures ANOVA was performed, comparing the change in prefer- nificance, F{3, 39) = 2.27, p < .10, changes in preference be-
ence of the plain exposed group for the plain version with the tween specific preference assessments were significant. For the
same group's change in preference for theflavoredversions. The flavored versions, the decrease in preference after 15 exposures
interaction between exposure and flavor was again significant, from the assessed preference following 8 exposures was signifi-
Fif>, 78) = 4.18, p<. 001, indicating again that the direction of cant, F( 1,13) = 7.18, p < .05, as was the decrease from 8 expo-
preference change differed for the plain and flavored versions. sures, with a one-week delay, to following 15 exposures, F{1,
Although this interaction makes the interpretation of main 13) = 8.55, p< .01. The overall pattern of results was therefore
effects problematic, the main effect of exposure, F\3, 39) = similar for all exposure groups, whether they were exposed to
the plain or flavored versions: Preference for the exposed ver-
sion increased with exposure, whereas preference for the unfa-
miliarflavoredversions actually declined.
Plain
Effect of exposure to one food on preference for another food.
Flavored
The effect of exposure to one novel food with a particular added
flavor on the preference for another similar novel food was ex-
amined. The results of a 4 X 3 (Exposure Level X Flavor Ver-
sion) repeated measures ANOVA indicated a significant main
effect of flavor for the three versions of ricotta cheese (sweet,
salty, and plain), 7=1(2, 72) = 11.97, p < .0001, for all groups
combined. However, neither the main effect of exposure group,
F(2, 36) = 0.34, p = .71, nor the main effect of exposure, F(3,
108) = 0.48, p = .70, were significant. The results, shown in
less 6 Figure 4, indicated that regardless of how much exposure the
8 8+
one week
children had to tofu or whether the tofu was sweet, salty, or
Exposure Frequency plain, it had no effect on preference for ricotta: Sweetened ri-
cotta cheese was preferred over salted ricotta cheese, F( 1,38) =
Figure 3. Mean tofu preference for plain versus combined 6.33, p < .02, which in turn was preferred over plain ricotta
flavor versions, plain exposed group only. cheese, 7^1, 38) = 6.02, p < .02.
550 SUSAN A. SULLIVAN AND LEANN L. BIRCH

To determine whether the effects of exposure to a flavored preferred at the end of the exposure series than it was when it
version of one novel food would generalize to another com- was completely novel. This unexpected result was noted sepa-
pletely novel food, afinalassessment was made. A single prefer- rately both in the data of the children exposed to sweet or salty
ence assessment including tofu and jicama was administered to tofu and in the data of the children exposed to plain tofu.
22 subjects. No child had any prior experience with jicama. All The absence of generalization of enhanced preference is at
of the children preferred the now-familiar tofu to the corre- least consistent with Kalat and Rozin's (1973) idea that food
sponding version ofjicama, t(21) = 4.15, p < .0005. The sweet neophobia has an adaptive function: It protects the organism
version of jicama was the most preferred of the various flavors from ingesting novel and potentially toxic substances. Exposure
of jicama for all of the subjects combined, and sweetened tofu that is not followed by negative physiological consequences re-
was significantly preferred to sweetened jicama, t{21) = -4.31, sults in "learned safety" and enhanced acceptance. This inter-
p < .0005. However, the flavor of jicama that corresponded to pretation of mere exposure effects implies that such exposure
the child's exposed version of tofu was not preferred to the other effects should be specific to foods actually ingested and should
version ofjicama, /(14) = -0.49, p < .65. There was no evidence not generalize to other similar but untried and potentially dan-
that experience with oneflavoredversion of a novel food pro- gerous substances. However, this interpretation does not ac-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

duced a generalized preference for another, similarly flavored count for the observed decreases in preference we noted for the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

novel food. unexposed, differentlyflavoredversions of the same food.


The pattern of changes in preference revealed that repeated
Discussion experience with one version of the food not only made that ver-
sion seem more acceptable and appropriate but made the other
These results confirm previousfindingsthat mere exposure version even less acceptable than when it was novel, possibly
effects can be obtained with novel foods: Repeated exposure to because the children came to view the unexposed versions as
the initially novel food led to increased acceptance of that food inappropriate food-flavor combinations. Unfortunately, we do
(Birch & Marlin, 1982; Birch, McPhee, Shoba, Pirok, & Stein- not have any direct evidence on how children develop concep-
berg, 1987; Pliner, 1982). This was true regardless of whether tual rules regarding when it is appropriate to add sugar or salt
the novel food was sweetened, salty, or presented without added to foods. However, it has been suggested elsewhere (Birch, in
sugar or salt. press; Rozin, 1990) that much of the acquisition of rules of cui-
In accord with the previous research, these results also indi- sine occurs in the absence of explicit instruction by adults.
cated that 8 to 15 exposures were necessary to see this effect. Rules regarding what food-flavor combinations are appropri-
We also noted a significant enhancement in preference that oc- ate in the culture and about what foods are appropriate at par-
curred following a 1-week delay after the initial 8 exposures. ticular mealtimes are probably inferred by the child from re-
During this delay period the children received no additional peated experience with particular flavor-food context combi-
experience with the food. This finding, consistent with results nations. Children know that spaghetti is not for breakfast not
reported by Zellner et al. (1983), is currently without explana- necessarily because their parents have told them so, but because
tion and requires further investigation. they don't eat it then. Similarly, salt becomes appropriate and
The major focus of this research was on how children acquire accepted in soup but not in water because it is routinely served
preferences for sweetened and salty food. In particular, we in- in one liquid but not the other.
vestigated the patterns of generalization that resulted from ex- The familiar becomes preferred and appropriate. Such infor-
perience with one version of an initially novel food (sweetened, mation is acquired early and does have clear effects on prefer-
salty, or plain). First, was there any generalization across the ence. We have shown that 3- and 4-year-olds know rules of cui-
differentlyflavoredversions of the same food? Did repeated ex- sine regarding the appropriate time of day at which familiar
perience produce an enhanced liking for all versions of the ini- foods are served in their culture and that this knowledge is re-
tially novel food, regardless of whether sugar or salt had been lated to their preferences. When we asked young children and
added? An additional question concerned whether repeated ex- adults to categorize foods (cheerios, macaroni and cheese, and
perience with oneflavoredversion of a novel food would facili- pizza) as for breakfast or for dinner and to give us preference
tate acceptance of other similar novel foods prepared with the data on these foods at breakfast and at dinner time, both groups
sameflavorsadded. Was the repeated experience with one food categorized the foods similarly. Even these young children
sufficient to produce generalized acceptance of the sweet or showed the adultlike pattern of showing a higher preference for
salty taste in other food systems, suggesting a mechanism for a food at the time of day when it is appropriate than at a time
the development of the sweet or salty tooth? when it is inappropriate (Birch, Billman, & Richards, 1984).
There was no evidence for generalization of the exposure Thefindingsindicate that with exposure, children can come
effect to preference for the otherflavoredversions of the same to show increased preference for a food, regardless of whether
food. Not only did we see no evidence of generalization, but it is served with or without added sugar or salt. However, the
repeated experience with either the plain or flavored versions pattern of results implies that adults should serve food to the
was sufficient to produce a significant decrease in preference for child prepared in the way they would ultimately like the child
the other version of the food. For children in both the plain and to accept it. The tactic of adding sugar (or salt) to a food to
flavored exposure conditions, there was a significant interaction encourage the child to eat it initially could actually make it less
of Exposure X Flavor Preparation, with increases in liking for likely that the food would subsequently be accepted without
the exposed food accompanied by declines in acceptance of the added sugar or salt.
other version. The nonexperienced version was actually less We also failed to note any generalization of enhanced accep-
SPECIAL SECTION: EXPERIENCE AND PREFERENCE 551

tance to another novel food with the same flavor added. This R. J., & Laing, B. (1986). Effect of dietary sodium restriction on taste
finding fails to provide any support for the suggestion that a responses to sodium chloride: A longitudinal study. American Jour-
sweet or salty tooth might be the result of a generalized prefer- nal ofClinical Nutrition, 44, lil-lAi.
Cowart, B. J., & Beauchamp, G. K. (1986). Factors affecting acceptance
ence for sweetened or salty food based on extensive experience
of salt by human infants and children. In M. R. Kare & J. G. Brand
with one or few sweetened or salty foods. (Eds.), Interaction of the chemical senses with nutrition (pp. 25-44).
New York: Academic Press.
Dahl, L. K. (1972). Salt and hypertension. American Journal ofClinical
References
Nutrition, 25,231-244.
Beauchamp, G. K., & Cowart, B. J. (1985). Congenital and experiential Desor, J. A., Mallei; Q, & Turner, R. (1973). Taste in acceptance of
factors in the development of human flavor preferences. Appetite, 6, sugars by human infants. Journal of Comparative and Physiological
357-372. Psychology, 84,496-501.
Beauchamp, G. K., & Cowart, B. J. (1990). Preference for high salt Harris, G., & Booth, D. A. (1985). Sodium preference in food and pre-
concentrations among children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 339- vious dietary experience in 6-month-old infants. IRCS Medical Sci-
345. ence Psychology and Psychiatry, 13, 1177-1178.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Beauchamp, G. K., Cowart, B. J., & Moran, M. (1986). Developmental Harris, G., Thomas, A., & Booth, D. A. (1990). Development of salt
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

changes in salt acceptability in human infants. Developmental Psy- taste in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 26, 334-338.
chobiology, 29, 17-25. Kalat, J. W., & Rozin, P. (1973). "Learned safety" as a mechanism in
Bertino, M., Beauchamp, G. K., & Engleman, K. (1986). Increasing long-delay taste aversion learning in rats. Journal ofComparative and
dietary salt alters salt taste preference. Physiology and Behavior, 38, Physiological Psychology, 83, 198-207.
203-213. Michela, J. L., & Contento, I. R. (1986). Cognitive, motivational, social,
and environmental influences on children's food choices. Health Psy-
Birch, L. L. (1979). Children's food preferences and consumption pat-
chology, 5, 209-230.
terns. Journal ofNutrition Education, 11, 189-192.
National Academy of Science. (1980). Recommended daily allowances
Birch, L. L. (1980). Effects of peer models' food choices and eating be-
(9th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
haviors on preschooler's food preferences. Child Development, 51,
Pliner, P. (1982). The effects of mere exposure on liking for edible sub-
489-496.
stances. Appetite, 3, 283-290.
Birch, L. L. (in press). Children's experience with food and eating modi-
Rozin, P. (1990). Development in the food domain. Developmental Psy-
fies food acceptance patterns. In M. Friedman & M. Tordoff (Eds.),
chology, 26, 355-362.
The psychobiology ofappetite.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1988). The Surgeon
Birch, L. L., Billman, J., & Richards, S. (1984). Time of day influences General's report on nutrition and health (DHHS Publication No. PHS
food acceptability. Appetite, 5, 109-112. 88-50210). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Birch, L. L., & Martin, D. W. (1982). I don't like it; I never tried it: U.S. Senate, Staff of the Select Committee on Nutrition and Human
Effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences. Ap- Needs. (1977). Dietary goalsfor the United States (2nd ed.). Washing-
petite, 3, 353-360. ton, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Birch, L. L., McPhee, L., Shoba, B. C, Pirok, E., & Steinberg, L. (1987). Zellner, D. A., Rozin, P., Aron, M., & Kulish, C. (1983). Conditioned
What kind of exposure reduces children's food neophobia? Looking enhancement of human's liking for flavor by pairing with sweetness.
vs. tasting. Appetite, 9, 171-178. Learning and Motivation, 14, 338-350.
Birch, L. L., Zimmerman, S. I., & Hind, H. (1980). The influence of
social-affective context on the formation of children's food prefer- ReceivedMarchl0,1989
ences. Child Development, 51, 856-861. Revision received July 18,1989
Blais, C. A., Pangbora, R. M., Borhani, N. O., Ferrell, M. F, Prineas, Accepted October 5,1989

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen