Sie sind auf Seite 1von 13

Journal of Alzheimers Disease 49 (2016) 3143 31

DOI 10.3233/JAD-150378
IOS Press

Strategies for Improving Memory:


A Randomized Trial of Memory Groups
for Older People, Including those with Mild
Cognitive Impairment
Glynda J. Kinsellaa,b, , David Amesc,d , Elsdon Storeye , Ben Onga , Kerryn E. Pikea , Michael M.
Salingf , Linda Clareg , Elizabeth Mullalyh and Elizabeth Randh
a School of Psychology & Public Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia
b Department of Psychology, Cauleld Hospital, Cauleld, Australia
c National Ageing Research Institute, Parkville, Australia
d University of Melbourne Academic Unit for Psychiatry of Old Age, St Georges Hospital, Kew, Australia
e Department of Neuroscience (Medicine), Monash University, Alfred Hospital Campus, Melbourne, Australia
f University of Melbourne, Parkville, Australia
g Department of Psychology, University of Exeter, Exete, UK
h Cognitive, Dementia and Memory Service, Cauleld Hospital, Cauleld, Australia

Accepted 27 July 2015

Abstract.
Background: Governments are promoting the importance of maintaining cognitive health into older age to minimize risk of
cognitive decline and dementia. Older people with amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) are particularly vulnerable to
memory challenges in daily activities and are seeking ways to maintain independent living.
Objective: To evaluate the effectiveness of memory groups for improving memory strategies and memory ability of older people,
especially those with aMCI.
Methods: 113 healthy older adults (HOA) and 106 adults with aMCI were randomized to a six-week memory group or a waitlist
control condition. Outcome was evaluated through knowledge and use of memory strategies, memory ability (self-report and
neuropsychological tests), and wellbeing. Assessments included a six-month follow-up.
Results: Using intention to treat analyses, there were intervention effects for HOA and aMCI groups in strategy knowledge
(HOA: 2 = 0.20; aMCI: 2 = 0.06), strategy use (HOA: 2 = 0.18; aMCI: 2 = 0.08), and wellbeing (HOA: 2 = 0.11; aMCI:
2 = 0.05). There were also intervention effects in the HOA group, but not the aMCI group, in self-reported memory ability
(2 = 0.06) and prospective memory tests (2 = 0.02). By six-month follow-up, gains were found on most HOA outcomes. In the
aMCI group gains were found in strategy use, and by this stage, gains in prospective memory were also found.
Conclusion: Memory groups can engage older people in techniques for maintaining cognitive health and improve memory
performance, but more modest benefits are seen for older adults with aMCI.

Keywords: Aging, memory, memory training, mild cognitive impairment, prospective memory, randomized controlled trial

INTRODUCTION
Correspondence to: Professor Glynda Kinsella, School of Psy-

chology & Public Health, La Trobe University, Melbourne, Victoria,


In our aging societies, maintaining independent liv-
3086, Australia. Tel.: +61 03 9479 2409; Fax: +61 03 9479 1956; ing is important for sustaining physical and mental
E-mail: g.kinsella@latrobe.edu.au. health, thereby contributing to minimizing pressure on

ISSN 1387-2877/16/$35.00 2016 IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved
32 G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training

limited social services, and governments worldwide important study in memory strategy research is the
are promoting the importance of maintaining cogni- ACTIVE trial [22], involving over 2,800 healthy older
tive health into older age [1]. In this respect, many adults who were randomly allocated to one of three
older people experience troublesome changes in mem- training groups (memory, reasoning, or speed of pro-
ory performance [2, 3] and seek guidance regarding cessing) or a no-contact control group. The memory
how best to manage memory challenges in everyday training module involved practicing various mnemonic
activities. This is especially the case for older peo- techniques, resulting in small gains on training-related
ple who meet criteria for the amnestic form of mild memory tests, up to a five-year follow-up (d = 0.23)
cognitive impairment (aMCI) in which, in addition to [23]. Although there was no effect on naturalistic cog-
subjective memory complaints, memory impairment nitive tests, there was some evidence of transfer at
can be quantified through neuropsychological testing 10-year follow-up in self-ratings of instrumental activ-
[4]. The importance of aMCI is that it carries increased ities of daily living [24]. Whether similar effects can
risk of development of dementia due to Alzheimers be obtained in aMCI is yet to reach a consensus, with
disease (AD), occurring at a rate of 515% per year some research finding positive results [2528] but other
as distinct from 12% in healthy adults [5]; although research reporting no, or only limited, effects [21,
a proportion of this group never progress to develop 2932].
dementia [6, 7]. Nevertheless, it has been estimated Translating newly acquired knowledge about mem-
that up to 21% of older people (aged between 70 and ory strategies to everyday situations requires behavior
90 years) will display features of aMCI [7]. Therefore, change, and it is recognized from health promotion
early cognitive interventions which moderate memory studies that behavior change in older age is difficult to
difficulties and assist in maintaining functional inde- achieve [33]. Readiness for using a memory strategy at
pendence as long as possible would be useful to large the appropriate time firstly requires a knowledge base
numbers of older people. about strategies [34]. Hutchens et al. [35] demonstrated
Studies of memory training for older people that older people with aMCI have less capacity for
through controlled practice on memory tasks (typically accessing strategy knowledge than healthy older peo-
computer-based) have provided compelling results, ple, and that level of strategy knowledge was related to
forcing reconsideration of the potential for neu- actual memory performance. This indicates that mem-
ronal plasticity in older brains [8, 9]. However, the ory interventions, especially for people with aMCI,
mental-exercise approach (or brain training) remains need to include components devoted to increasing
contentious, with much of the criticism relating to lack knowledge about memory strategies.
of generalization [10, 11], especially to everyday func- An additional issue, and consistent with theoret-
tioning [12]. This is important for aMCI, as the memory ical models of behavior change [36, 37], is that
deficit is more problematic than for healthy older peo- personally-relevant skills training and promotion of
ple and a critical issue is to reduce memory disability self-confidence in ability to apply these skills will be
in daily activities rather than focusing exclusively on more likely to encourage older people to use strategies
performance in arcane memory tests. in daily activities [38, 39]. Group-based interventions,
Following from research demonstrating the efficacy or memory groups, provide opportunity for peer-
of cognitive strategies, or mnemonics, in experimen- learning through sharing of experience in strategy-use
tal memory studies [1315], an alternative approach and have been identified as facilitating better out-
is to train older adults in compensatory cognitive- comes than individual-based training [18, 20]. Using
behavioral memory strategies to be used in real-world this information, we undertook a preliminary study of
situations. These approaches can be varied; for exam- a memory group for older adults with aMCI using a
ple, using mnemonic techniques such as associative multi-strategy approach and incorporating education
strategies [16] to aid retrieval of a persons name; about health behaviors that facilitate effective mem-
or, using an external aid, such as a mobile phone ory performance [27]. We were able to demonstrate
reminder, to remember to pick up the grandchildren gains in knowledge and use of strategies, and also a
after supermarket shopping. Several meta-analyses small effect on everyday memory, demonstrable on
have identified pre to post gains on memory tests prospective memory tests. Many daily activities rely
in trained groups of healthy older adults [1720], on prospective memory, or remembering to perform
although all have cited caution in over-interpretation intended actions in the future (such as passing on a
of findings due to a highly variable and methodolog- message when you next meet a friend), and prospective
ically limited literature base [21]. Nevertheless, an memory performance can provide a useful approach to
G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training 33

measurement of everyday memory competence [12]. investigation due to concern about memory perfor-
However, the sample size was small (n = 20 per group) mance); (b) objective memory impairment 1.5 SD
and we did not include a comparison healthy older or more below age norms for delayed recall on at least
adult group to evaluate the extent of gain from inter- one of four screening measures of episodic memory:
vention by the aMCI group. This indicates the need for Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised (HVLT-R)
further study, especially as Reijnders et al. [29] con- [40], Logical Memory subtest from the Wechsler Mem-
cluded from their systematic review of interventions ory Scale Third Edition [41], Verbal Paired Associates
for older people that whether the effects of cognitive subtest from the Wechsler Memory Scale Fourth Edi-
interventions generalize to improvement in everyday tion [42], Complex Figure of Rey [43]; (c) absence of
activities remains unresolved. significant impairment in basic activities of daily life
Therefore, our objective was to evaluate older peo- (Clinical Dementia Rating) [44] score <1, and indepen-
ples responses to a cognitive-behavioral intervention, dent or only occasional assistance on the Alzheimers
i.e., memory group training, for improving everyday Disease Functional Assessment and Change Scale-
memory performance. We focused on people with personal sub-scale [45]; and (d) absence of dementia
aMCI as this group is particularly vulnerable to mem- using NINCDS-ADRDA criteria [46] and a Mini-
ory challenges in daily activities and are seeking Mental Status Examination (MMSE) score of 24
ways to maintain independent living. Our aim was [47]. HOA participants were volunteers who responded
to improve memory ability through increased knowl- to advertising in community centers and groups to find
edge of effective memory strategies, and by training out more about memory and memory strategies. In
and modeling the use of several strategies that can be addition to the initial inclusion criteria listed above,
applied in daily activities. We expected that following HOA criteria were: (a) had not sought professional
intervention, and at six-month follow-up, older partic- assessment due to concern about memory; (b) perfor-
ipants, including those with aMCI, would demonstrate mance above - 1.5 SD of age norms on all of the four
greater knowledge and use of memory strategies, screening measures of delayed memory; (c) absence
and improved memory ability in daily life. By con- of impairment in activities of daily life (see above for
trast, there was limited expectation that performance assessment).
would improve on standard neuropsychological tests Exclusion criteria for both groups were: (a) pres-
of memory as many strategies are difficult to use on ence of significant co-morbidities liable to impact on
these researcher-controlled performance measures. We cognition; and (b) presence of significant visual and/or
did, however, expect that participants would report auditory impairment uncorrected by aids. All potential
improved wellbeing and self-confidence in everyday participants were reviewed by a case selection panel
memory performance. including a neurologist (ES), a psychiatrist (DA), a
neuropsychologist (GJK), and the research coordinator
MATERIALS AND METHODS (KEP), who formed a consensus decision on eligibility.

Participants Study design and expectations

Participants were recruited to form two cohorts: The study was a randomized controlled cross-over
healthy older adults (HOA) and older adults diagnosed trial of a memory group intervention (see Fig. 1
with aMCI. Initial inclusion criteria for all participants which illustrates the study design). The trial was
were: (a) age >60 years; (b) community-dwelling; approved by ethics committees at La Trobe University
and (c) English language sufficient for neuropsycho- and participating health services, and registered with
logical assessment. Additional specific criteria were the Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trials Reg-
used in the selection of participants for each cohort. istry, N012605000711617. At recruitment, participants
Participants with aMCI were diagnosed and referred (HOA and aMCI) were offered a training program in
from memory clinics or experienced aged care special- strategies for everyday memory challenges. If willing
ists, following multidisciplinary diagnostic consensus to participate, informed consent procedures, a neu-
review (i.e., neurological, psychiatric, radiological, ropsychological assessment, and screening assessment
neuropsychological, and functional assessment). Eli- for eligibility were undertaken (see Table 1 for per-
gible participants were further screened to satisfy formances on selected neuropsychological tests for
clinical criteria [4, 5]: (a) subjective memory complaint screening). This was followed by baseline pre-test
(participant or informant had sought professional assessment on the outcome measures (listed below),
34 G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training

Assessed for eligibility


n = 378

Excluded n = 159
Did not meet study criteria n = 138
Declined/unavailable n = 21

Pre-test
113 HOA 106 aMCI

Early Intervention Late Intervention


Randomizaon

Allocated Early Intervenon 59 HOA 51 aMCI Allocated Late Intervenon 54 HOA 55 aMCI

Early Intervenon

Early Post-test

Completed Post-test Early 56 HOA 43 aMCI Completed Post-test Early 53 HOA 48 aMCI
Did not complete Post-test Early n = 11 Did not complete Post-test Early n=8
Withdrew 2 HOA 6 aMCI Withdrew 1 HOA 1 aMCI
Illness/Unavailable 1 HOA 2 aMCI Illness/Unavailable 6 aMCI

Late Intervenon
Late Post-test

Completed Post-test Late 55 HOA 39 aMCI Completed Post-test Late 51 HOA 43 aMCI
Did not complete Post-test Late n=5 Did not complete Post-test Late n=7
Withdrew 1 HOA Withdrew 4 aMCI
Illness/Unavailable 4 aMCI Illness/Unavailable 2 HOA 1 aMCI

Six-month Follow-up

Completed 6-mo Follow-up 53 HOA 37 aMCI Completed 6-mo Follow-up 48 HOA 36 aMCI
Did not complete 6-mo Follow-up n=4 Did not complete 6-mo Follow-up n = 10
Withdrew 1 HOA Withdrew 1 HOA 5 aMCI
Illness/Unavailable 2 aMCI Illness/Unavailable 1 aMCI
Protocol violaon 1 HOA Protocol violaon 2 HOA 1 aMCI

Fig. 1. Diagram to show flow of participants through the study design.

and subsequent block randomization by diagnostic ing the six-week memory group intervention there was a
group to early- or late-intervention by a member of the two-week delay interval before both groups underwent
research team (BO) who had no contact with partici- post-assessment (early post-test). This was followed
pants, using a computer algorithm. Participants in the by a cross-over phase in procedure such that the par-
early-intervention condition were invited to the next ticipants in the late-intervention groups were provided
memory group scheduled to start within two weeks and with the intervention while the participants in the early-
participants in the late-intervention (a wait-list control intervention groups crossed to a maintenance phase. On
group at this stage) were informed that there was a delay completion of the cross-over phase and a further two-
until they could join the next memory group. Follow- week delay interval, both groups underwent a further
G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training 35

Table 1
Sample characteristics of HOA and aMCI groups
HOA (n = 113) aMCI (n = 106) 2
M (SD) M (SD)
Demographic variables
Age: years 72.27 (6.95) 76.08 (7.10) 0.07
Gender: female n (%) 82 (74%) 61 (58%) 0.02
Education: more than 12 y n (%) 73 (65%) 55 (52%) 0.01
WTAR: predicted IQ 109.56 (7.84) 108.83 (7.37) <0.01
MMSE 28.93 (1.01) 26.95 (1.87) 0.31
Screening memory measures
HVLT-R delayed recall 9.09 (2.12) 0.94 (1.63) 0.82
Logical Memory delayed recall 23.27 (7.56) 7.67 (6.73) 0.55
Verbal Paired Assoc delayed recall 7.50 (1.88) 3.85 (2.27) 0.44
Rey Figure delayed recall 16.37 (5.68) 7.58 (5.18) 0.40
Outcome measures (baseline pre-test)
Strategy Repertoire 20.42 (8.21) 16.13 (6.90) 0.08
MMQ Strategy internal 12.72 (5.79) 10.87 (5.76) 0.03
MMQ Strategy external 21.36 (5.93) 21.80 (5.56) <0.01
MMQ Ability 49.26 (9.03) 43.73 (11.79) 0.07
CAMPROMPT total 23.16 (6.59) 13.16 (8.06) 0.32
CVLT II delayed recall 11.57 (2.95) 3.17 (3.26) 0.65
MMQ Contentment 45.59 (11.72) 32.54 (11.01) 0.25
WTAR, Wechsler Test of Adult Reading; Wechsler, 2001. p < 0.05, p < 0.001.

post-assessment (late post-test), followed at six months memory groups [27] and built upon an evidence-based
after training completion with a final assessment (six- literature relating to memory, aging, and memory strate-
month follow-up). gies that can be implemented by persons with aMCI
By comparing the randomized intervention groups at as well as older adults [25, 27, 31, 48]. The LaTCH
early post-test (for both HOA and aMCI participants), program was manualized, facilitators were trained in
the effect of intervention on the outcome measures the program, and there were regular training meetings
was evaluated, with the expectation that the early- throughout the study to ensure protocol adherence. The
intervention groups would perform better than the interactive sessions provided information about mem-
late-intervention groups (waitlist controls). At late post- ory and changes in memory as a result of health and
test, when all participants had received intervention, lifestyle issues, aging, or neurological disorder. We
we expected that all participants would have improved used a problem-solving approach to illustrate com-
as compared to pre-test performances. By six-month mon everyday memory problems, and provided practice
follow-up, we anticipated some decline in perfor- in cognitive strategies (e.g., task organization, seman-
mance, but expected that all participants would have tic association, visual imagery, retrieval practice, goal
made gains in outcome performances as compared to planning). As a frequently used approach to compen-
pre-test. sation of memory impairment is the use of external
aids [49], the sessions explored a variety of aids (e.g.,
Intervention diaries/smartphones/manual timers) for various tasks
and situations. We also addressed coping strategies to
The La Trobe and Caulfield Hospital (LaTCH) Mem- develop self-efficacy and emotional balance in respond-
ory Group program consisted of six 2-hour weekly ing to everyday memory challenges [39]. At the end of
sessions conducted by neuropsychologists or an experi- each session, participants received handouts covering
enced occupational therapist assisted by co-facilitators, the session material, and an assignment to facili-
and held in health service facilities throughout Mel- tate application and practice of the skills in everyday
bourne and regional Victoria, Australia. Group size activities.
ranged from six to twelve, and consisted of a mix of Participants assigned to the late-intervention groups
HOA and aMCI participants who could bring a family (the waitlist controls) did not receive experimental
member or friend if they wished. The LaTCH pro- intervention until the cross-over stage; but the aMCI
gram was developed from our earlier experience of participants did receive usual care from their referring
36 G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training

clinicians in the memory clinics and all the participants linked to memory performance and self-belief in the
could access community-based support services. controllability of memory (Cronbachs alpha 0.95;
test-retest r = 0.93).
Outcome assessments

Assessors, who were graduate psychologists, were Statistical method and sample size estimate
masked to the randomized condition and also to the
diagnostic group of the participant (HOA, aMCI). All statistical analyses were undertaken with IBM
Reflecting the multidimensional structure of the SPSS v. 21. Socio-demographic and clinical charac-
intervention, the primary outcomes covered several teristics between diagnostic groups (HOA, aMCI) at
domains of interest: baseline were compared by ANOVAs for interval-
Strategy knowledge: The Strategy Repertoire Test scaled variables, with eta-squared (2) given as the
[31] measured the number and quality of memory effect size, and chi-square (2) analyses were used for
strategies that were familiar to the participant by pro- categorical variables, with phi-squared (2) given as the
viding eight hypothetical everyday memory scenarios effect size.
(e.g., remembering to pass on a message), and required Missing data analysis with SPSS (EM technique)
participants to list as many strategies as possible that suggested that data could be assumed to be missing at
would be useful for each situation (internal consistency random, Little MCAR test, 2 (1830, n = 219) = 1923,
Cronbachs alpha 0.78 [35]). Responses were scored p = 0.06. Therefore, adopting an intention-to-treat
by effectiveness and specificity: 2 points for each spe- approach, missing data was imputed with SPSS in
cific strategy; 1 point for a non-specific strategy; and, which demographic and screening data was used to
0 points for an ineffective or non-strategy (inter-rater predict missing pre-test scores and then, together, to
reliability r = 0.94 [35]). predict missing scores at early post-test, late post-test,
Strategy use: The Multifactorial Memory Question- and follow-up. Statistical results arising from ANOVAs
naire, the 19-item Strategy subscale (MMQ-Strategy) and ANCOVAs were pooled from the 20 imputed
[50] was used to assess self-reported frequency of strat- datasets; namely, means were pooled by averaging, F
egy use in everyday memory tasks (Cronbachs alpha and 2 by median, SD by formula (see [54]), and p by
0.83; test-retest r = 0.88). Internal strategies are reliant re-calculation from F and df.
on cognitive manipulation to improve memory (e.g., As significant differences were expected between
forming a semantic association of a name with a face), diagnostic groups at pre-test on the outcome mea-
whereas external strategies recruit external aids or envi- sures from four domains (strategy knowledge, strategy
ronmental cues (e.g., smartphone reminders). To reflect use, memory ability, and wellbeing), the effect of
this distinction, we used factor scores for internal and randomized memory intervention (early versus late)
external strategies [51]. was analyzed separately for each diagnostic group
Memory ability: The Multifactorial Memory (HOA, aMCI), and assessed by comparing randomized
QuestionnaireAbility subscale (MMQ-Ability) [50] subgroups(early-versuslate-intervention)atearlypost-
provided a 20-item self-rating of memory ability test,usingANCOVAs,withpre-testscoresascovariates.
in everyday life (Cronbachs alpha 0.93; test-retest Gains from any intervention effects on outcomes mea-
r = 0.86). In addition, observed memory performance sures in HOA and aMCI groups were assessed by
was assessed through two standard neuropsycholog- ANOVAs at late post-test and at six-month follow-up,
ical tests: the Cambridge Assessment of Prospective using gain (difference) scores from pre-test.
Memory (CAMPROMPT) [52] total score assessed The differential impact of memory intervention on
prospective memory or remembering to perform diagnostic groups was compared at late post-test using
intended activities; and the California Verbal Learning ANOVAongainscores(frompre-test),andatsix-month
TestSecond Edition (CVLT-II) [53] long delay recall follow-up using ANOVA on gain scores (from pre-test).
score assessed episodic memory and new learning. For The sample size required to detect a medium effect
both of these measures, alternate forms were used and of intervention between randomized groups (early-
counterbalanced across the assessment phases. versus late-intervention) was estimated to be 45 per
Wellbeing: The Multifactorial Memory randomized group for a power of 0.80 at = 0.05 [55].
QuestionnaireContentment subscale (MMQ- Assuming a 15% drop-out rate, we planned for a min-
Contentment) [50] was used to assess wellbeing. imum of 104 HOA and 104 aMCI participants (52 per
Eighteen items described emotions and perceptions randomized group).
G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training 37

RESULTS icantly different in general cognition (MMSE) and the


screening measures of memory. At baseline (pre-test)
Characteristics of the groups (HOA and aMCI) the aMCI group provided significantly lower scores
than the HOA group on all of the outcome measures
From a referral pool of 378 potential participants, apart from reporting equivalent use of external strate-
138 did not meet study criteria but only 21 potential gies (MMQ Strategy - external).
participants declined or were unavailable during the
study phase. Therefore, we recruited 219 (113 HOA, Effect of randomized intervention
106 aMCI). A flow diagram of the progress of the sam-
ple through the phases of the study and reasons for The effect of randomized memory intervention
exclusions is provided in Fig. 1. By the final six-month (early versus late) was assessed separately for HOA
follow-up, there were 174 participants (i.e., 80%) and aMCI groups by ANCOVAs at early post-test,
remaining from the initial sample. It should be noted using the pre-test scores of the outcome measures as
that attrition by six-month follow-up (which included covariates (see Table 2).
non-adherence to the intervention, i.e., attending <4 Significant improvements in strategy knowledge
out of the 6 sessions) was considerably lower from the (Strategy Repertoire) were demonstrated with a large
HOA group than from the aMCI group (11% versus intervention effect for the HOA group (2 = 0.20) and a
31%); however, within each diagnostic group, there moderate effect for the aMCI group (2 = 0.06). Signif-
was no differential attrition between the randomized icant improvements in self-reported strategy use were
intervention groups (HOA: 10% early versus 11% late; foundthroughlargeeffectsintheHOAgroupforinternal
aMCI: 28% early versus 34% late). There was no strategies (2 = 0.18) and a moderate effect for exter-
significant difference in age (HOA p = 0.116; aMCI nal strategies (2 = 0.07). There was also a moderate
p = 0.225), gender (HOA p = 0.506; aMCI p = 0.289), effect reported by the aMCI group for internal strate-
education (HOA p = 0.079; aMCI p = 0.463), or screen- gies (2 = 0.08); however, not for external strategies
ing memory performance (HVLT R delayed recall, (2 = <0.01).
HOA p = 0.137; aMCI p = 0.814) between the ini- With respect to self-rated memory ability in every-
tial sample and the participants retained at six-month day tasks (MMQ ability), improvement in response
follow-up, in either diagnostic group. to intervention was reported in the HOA group with
Table 1 shows that the randomized HOA and aMCI a moderate effect (2 = 0.06), but not in the aMCI
groups were similar in distribution of years of educa- group (2 < 0.01). Correspondingly, observed mem-
tion and predicted IQ. However, the mean age of the ory performance on the neuropsychological measure
aMCI group (76.08 y) was several years older than the of everyday prospective memory (CAMPROMPT total
HOA group (72.27 y) and the percentage of females score) showed a small but non-significant effect in the
was slightly lower in the aMCI group than the HOA HOA group (2 = 0.02), but not in the aMCI group
group. As expected, the diagnostic groups were signif- (2 < 0.01). On the neuropsychological measure of

Table 2
Comparing randomized (early versus late) intervention subgroups, separately for HOA and aMCI, at early post-test using ANCOVA with pre-test
as covariate
HOA intervention subgroups aMCI intervention subgroups
Early (n = 59) Late (n = 54) 2 Early (n = 51) Late (n = 55) 2
Outcome measure M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Strategy Knowledge
Strategy Repertoire 28.52 (9.93) 20.42 (8.41) 0.20 19.47 (8.14) 16.19 (6.88) 0.06
Strategy Use
MMQ Strategy- Internal 17.85 (5.99) 13.47 (5.53) 0.18 14.24 (6.84) 9.90 (6.47) 0.08
MMQ Strategy- External 23.99 (5.73) 21.68 (4.69) 0.07 21.73 (7.08) 22.11 (6.92) <0.01
Memory Ability
MMQ Ability 51.50 (9.17) 47.87 (8.69) 0.06 46.90 (12.15) 44.75 (9.39) <0.01
CAMPROMPT total 27.35 (6.77) 24.88 (6.94) 0.02 15.79 (9.25) 15.75 (8.94) <0.01
CVLT II long delay 11.64 (3.35) 12.14 (2.93) <0.01 3.88 (3.83) 2.93 (3.57) <0.01
Wellbeing
MMQ Contentment 50.14 (10.83) 44.06 (12.22) 0.11 34.74 (11.50) 32.04 (12.81) 0.05
p < 0.05, p < 0.01, p < 0.001.
38 G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training

Table 3
Long-term gains (from pre-test) within diagnostic group (HOA, aMCI) at late post-test and six-month follow-up,
as well as a comparison of these gains between diagnostic groups
Assessment Stage Combined HOA Combined aMCI HOA versus aMCI
group (n = 113) group (n = 106) Differential gain
Gain (from Pre-test) Gain (from Pre-test)
M 95% CI M 95% CI 2
Strategy Repertoire
Late Post-test 6.73 [5.02, 8.44] 1.93 [0.46, 3.39] 0.07
six-month follow-up 7.35 [5.67, 9.04] 1.58 [0.28, 3.45] 0.09
Strategy Use Internal
Late Post-test 4.92 [3.92, 5.92] 3.19 [2.00, 4.38] 0.02
six-month follow-up 3.86 [2.91, 4.81] 2.29 [1.32, 3.26] 0.03
Strategy Use - External
Late Post-test 3.02 [2.18, 3.86] 1.38 [0.38, 2.38] 0.03
six-month follow-up 2.27 [1.34, 3.20] 0.95 [0.01, 1.88] 0.02
Memory Ability
Late Post-test 2.14 [0.71, 3.57] 1.66 [0.08, 3.39] <0.01
six-month follow-up 1.05 [0.37, 2.47] 0.79 [0.94, 2.53] <0.01
CAMPROMPT total
Late Post-test 5.38 [4.14, 6.61] 4.77 [3.35, 6.19] <0.01
six-month follow-up 5.05 [3.78, 6.31] 4.09 [2.56, 5.62] <0.01
CVLT II long delay
Late Post-test 0.76 [0.22, 1.28] 0.64 [0.05, 1.22] <0.01
six-month follow-up 0.59 [0.14, 1.04] 0.02 [0.53, 0.57] 0.01
MMQ Contentment
Late Post-test 4.24 [2.67, 0.81] 3.33 [1.46, 5.20] <0.01
six-month follow-up 2.43 [0.66, 4.19] 1.55 [0.27, 3.37] <0.01
p < 0.05, p < 0.001.

new learning (CVLT II delayed recall), neither the maintained at six-month follow-up (HOA M = 2.27,
HOA group nor the aMCI group showed a significant 95% CI [1.34, 3.20]; aMCI M = 0.95, 95% CI [0.01,
improvement following intervention. 1.88]).
As can be seen in Table 2, estimate of wellbeing There were gains in self-reported memory perfor-
and self-efficacy as measured through the MMQ- mance (Memory Ability) for the HOA group at late
Contentment scale, showed improvement following post-test (HOA M = 2.14, 95% CI [0.71, 3.57]) but not
intervention for both HOA and aMCI groups with a for the aMCI group (M = 1.66, 95% CI [0.81, 3.39]).
moderate effect for the HOA group (2 = 0.11), and a However, neither group maintained gains by six-month
small effect for the aMCI group (2 = 0.05). follow-up (HOA M = 1.05, 95% CI [0.37, 2.47];
aMCI M = 0.79, 95% CI [0.94, 2.53]). In relation to
Gains in outcome measures at late post-late neuropsychological test performances, both diagnostic
and six-month follow-up groups showed significant gains on the CAMPROMPT
at late post-test (HOA M = 5.38, 95% CI [4.14, 6.61];
By late post-test (see Table 3) when all partici- aMCI M = 4.77, 95% CI [3.35, 6.19]) and maintained
pants had received intervention, both diagnostic groups at six-month follow-up (HOA M = 5.05, 95% CI [3.78,
(HOA, aMCI) showed significant gains in strategy 6.31]; aMCI M = 4.09, 95% CI [2.56, 5.62]). On the
knowledge; but only the HOA group maintained an CVLT long delay, the HOA group showed signifi-
effect at six-month follow-up (M = 7.35, 95%CI [5.67, cant gains at late post-test and at six-month follow-up
9.04]). Both diagnostic groups achieved significant (late post-test - HOA M = 0.76, 95% CI [0.22, 1.28]
gains in internal strategies through memory interven- and follow-up HOA M = 0.59, 95% CI [0.14, 1.04]);
tion at post-late (HOA M = 4.92, 95% CI [3.92, 5.92]; whereas aMCI only showed significant gain at late
aMCI M = 3.19, 95% CI [2.00, 4.38]) and also at six- post-test (M = 0.64, 95% CI [0.05, 1.22]) but not at
month follow-up (HOA M = 3.86, 95% CI [2.91, 4.81]; six-month follow-up (M = 0.02, 95% CI [0.53, 0.57]).
aMCI M = 2.29, 95% CI [1.32, 3.26]). Similarly, there Finally, in relation to wellbeing and self-efficacy,
were significant gains for both diagnostic groups in both diagnostic groups (HOA, aMCI) showed signifi-
external strategies at post-late (HOA M = 3.02, 95% CI cant gains at late post-test on the MMQ-Contentment
[2.18, 3.86]; aMCI M = 1.38, 95% CI [0.38, 2.38]) and scale(HOA M = 4.24, 95% CI [2.67, 5.81]; aMCI
G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training 39

M = 3.33, 95% CI [1.46, 5.20]), but only the HOA activities and prospective memory test performance) in
group maintained an effect at six-month follow-up the HOA group, but not for the aMCI group. By six-
(M = 2.43, 95% CI [0.66, 4.19]). month follow-up, gains persisted on most outcomes
for the HOA group; however, for the aMCI group gains
Differences in extent of gains on outcome were only maintained in strategy use in daily activities,
measures between diagnostic groups although it was also noted that by this stage there were
gains in prospective memory test performance. The
Pre-test differences between diagnostic groups trial findings are encouraging as they indicate benefits
(HOA, aMCI) were found, as expected, on most out- from memory groups for older age participants, albeit
come measures, using ANCOVAs at pre-test with with generally larger and more varied effects for the
age as the covariate (see Table 1). Of greater inter- HOA group than the aMCI group.
est was whether age-adjusted gains were different In relation to the first question, strategy knowledge
between diagnostic groups at late post-test and six- is a necessary prelude to using an appropriate strategy
month follow-up (see HOA versus aMCI column, in real life activities [35]; by increasing an understand-
Table 3). ing of the range of strategies that can be considered for
A differential gain between HOA and aMCI groups specific tasks, an older person will be more prepared
in Strategy Repertoire was significant at late post- for unexpected situations when a go to strategy fails
test (2 = 0.07) and at six-month follow-up (2 = 0.09), [17]. For example, following training one of the par-
favoring the HOA group. For internal strategies, ticipants reported being able to successfully use goal
gains were significantly different between HOA and planning (implementation intention) [56] to remem-
aMCI groups at late post-test (2 = 0.02) and six- ber an appointment later in the day after he mislaid
month follow-up (2 = 0.03), favoring the HOA group. his mobile phone (and reminder function). Neverthe-
In addition, the HOA group gained more in exter- less, despite immediate gains following intervention,
nal strategies use at post-test than the aMCI group and in contrast to the HOA group, the aMCI group
(2 = 0.03), and at six-month follow-up (2 = 0.02). was unable to demonstrate long-term gains in strat-
There were no significant differences in gains in self- egy knowledge. In earlier small sample studies with
reported Memory Ability between diagnostic groups at aMCI participants [27, 31], it was reported that strat-
post-test and follow-up. There were also no diagnostic egy knowledge gains could be maintained up to three
group differences in gains at post-test and follow-up or four months post-training, but there was no fur-
in relation to neuropsychological test performances ther follow-up assessment. In contrast, our six-month
(CAMPROMPT and CVLT-II). follow-up provides a more challenging test of sustain-
Finally, in relation to wellbeing estimates, there were able change. It was also apparent that even before the
no diagnostic group differences in gains at post-test and intervention our aMCI group was less able than the
follow-up on the MMQ-Contentment. HOA group in identifying appropriate strategies for
everyday memory scenarios, suggesting deterioration
DISCUSSION in pre-existing semantic knowledge [57]. The loss of
semantic memory in aMCI adds a further constraint in
We explored two main questions: (i) does a six-week learning and maintaining new skills, and highlights the
memory group improve knowledge and use of strate- potential benefit of extending the number of sessions in
gies in older people, especially in those with aMCI? the primary memory group or adding booster sessions
and (ii) do memory ability and wellbeing improve for consolidating newly acquired information in peo-
following a memory group? We used a cognitive- ple confronted by significant memory difficulties [58].
behavioral intervention (the LaTCH Memory Group However, our decision to constrain the number of ini-
program), which allowed the training and modeling of tial sessions in the memory group was determined by
compensatory strategies that can be readily applied in feedback about the feasibility of group length from par-
daily activities. We found that in older age participants, ticipants in our earlier pilot study [27]. Furthermore,
including those with aMCI, knowledge about mem- in their systematic review of intervention studies, Rei-
ory strategies improved following intervention, and jnders et al. [29] found no dose-relationship between
that there was a behavioral change with greater use of hours of intervention and effectiveness. Alternatively,
strategies in daily activities, while wellbeing and self- some test specific effects were found in the ACTIVE
confidence in memory also improved. These effects study when booster training was provided [23] sug-
were translated into improved memory ability (in daily gesting that this approach may provide the greatest
40 G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training

advantage for consolidating skill development in use ties in the real world rather than to effect a shift in
of strategies. performance on traditional clinical tests where there is
Of course, increasing knowledge alone will not guar- often little opportunity to put into practice strategies
antee changing behavior; therefore, providing skills that are relevant for functional problems. A counter-
in the actual techniques and application of memory issue is that caution is also needed in interpreting the
strategies is necessary to persuade older adults that apparent long-term gains on neuropsychological tests
they can manage their everyday memory more effec- as although alternate versions of tests were used and
tively [39]. Furthermore, Dixon and de Frias [59] counterbalanced across assessment phases, the contri-
reported that in a six-year observational study of older bution of practice effects from familiarity with test
adults, participants who were found to have a mild protocols must be considered. While neither group
memory deficit were increasingly less likely to use reported improved memory ability in daily activities by
memory strategies than older adults with normal mem- six-month follow-up, the HOA group did report benefit
ory function. In this context, it was notable that our immediately after intervention. Troyer et al. [31] pro-
participants with aMCI, as well as the HOA group, posed a staged model of intervention effects, whereby
reported that their strategy use increased following an increase in knowledge of what to do, is followed by
intervention, and long-term gains were found in both a behavioral change in increased use of strategies, and
internal and external strategies. The intervention, fol- finally by translation of behavioral change into task
lowing the principles of promoting behavior change performance change. In our study, there is support for
[37], focused on modeling of strategies to real-world the translation of behavioral change (knowledge and
scenarios and provided opportunity for mastery of skill use of strategies) into task performance change for the
through practice and weekly assignments. Its multi- HOA group immediately following intervention and
strategy approach endorsed the position that different qualified support for long-term task gains. However,
people can use different strategies to assist perfor- although some task gains were observed by six-month
mance of everyday tasks [60]. Therefore, participants follow-up, the aMCI group may need more extensive
were encouraged to develop skills in multiple strategies intervention to be clipped onto a standard memory
rather than focus on one approach; thereby, providing group program to reinforce translation of strategy use
more secure scaffolding for real world demands [24]. into functional behavior. This may take the form of
Indeed, the positive shift in reported increased strat- combining individualized goal-oriented sessions [62]
egy use was reflected in evidence of improvement in with the group sessions in order to reinforce the prac-
objective memory performance. There were long-term tical application of strategy use for personalized goal
gains at six-month follow-up on prospective mem- attainment in everyday activities.
ory test performance for both groups, and the HOA Finally, a component of the positive effect of the
group showed a small effect in prospective mem- memory groups is captured by the gains in wellbeing,
ory immediately after intervention. The relevance of including increased self-efficacy or self-confidence in
prospective memory, or remembering to perform an managing memory performance (also see [39]) and
intended action at some point in the future, is that indexed through increased contentment with mem-
memory complaints of older people are frequently ory performance (MMQ Contentment). Although the
about prospective memory errors [12] and prospective aMCI group could not maintain their early gains by
memory has been associated with capacity to main- six-month follow-up, the HOA group was able to show
tain independent living [61]. The prospective memory sustainable wellbeing effects from participation in the
test used in the study (CAMPROMPT) allowed the memory group. Following intervention, most partici-
use of memory strategies to provide a more naturalis- pants reported being reassured that they had been able
tic approach to memory testing, which may account for to meet with others who experienced similar memory
why early intervention effects were noted on this test challenges in daily activities, and many participants
but not on the word-list memory test (CVLT-II). Fur- commented that as a result of attending the group, they
thermore, when questioned about how they approached regained self-confidence and were able to re-engage
memory testing, many participants reported that they with former activities. For example, one participant
would not use strategies on these tests as that would described returning to volunteer driving for meals on
be cheating. This illustrates the dilemma of a dis- wheels after he had mastered a technique for remem-
connection between performance in real life activities bering the routes to be taken for dropping off the meals.
and performance on clinical tests. The objective of It is the re-engagement with previous life activities as
the memory group was to manage memory difficul- much as engagement with new activities that represents
G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training 41

an important contribution in the pathway to better men- contributions to maintaining independence and social
tal health, as social re-engagement is potentially more engagement in situations of memory difficulty. Future
sustainable. research is needed to evaluate how these early train-
In relation to methodology, the major strengths of ing effects can be sustained over extended periods of
our study are that the sample includes a large, well- time by extending the sessions of the primary mem-
defined group of older people with aMCI, and that ory group; or, more specifically, to evaluate whether
the multi-component intervention was developed on the beneficial effects of the intervention for people
evidence-based techniques for improving memory per- with aMCI can be enhanced by incorporating periodic
formance [17] and combined with current models of goal-oriented booster sessions to consolidate new skills
effecting behavior change [37]. In addition, the study acquisition and also to improve self-confidence in
methodology carefully adhered to CONSORT recom- applying skills in daily activities. From a cost perspec-
mendations and included a six-month follow-up to tive, the memory group intervention has relatively low
evaluate gains in outcome measures after the interven- resource requirements and can be readily incorporated
tion had ceased. However, it should be noted that in into existing health and community services. Such sus-
order to address our research questions, we elected to tainable practice is particularly important within the
use a randomized waitlist control group rather than context of meeting the concerns of our aging commu-
an active control. We used this approach as recruit- nities.
ment of the aMCI group was through memory clinics,
where there was a reluctance to commit to an extended ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
delay (including the follow-up period) in offering the
memory groups for patients struggling with memory The authors wish to acknowledge the study par-
difficulties. As a result the beneficial effects of the ticipants who participated in the memory groups.
intervention may have been moderated by the positive We also acknowledge the support of the Cognitive,
expectances of the waitlist group, who were anticipat- Dementia and Memory Services at Austin Health,
ing involvement in the memory group. Nevertheless, Barwon Health, Bendigo Health, Bundoora Extended
we were still able to detect small to medium effects Care Centre, Caulfield Hospital, Melbourne Health,
following the randomized intervention in most of the St. Georges Hospital and Wantirna Hospital, as well
outcome measures. A further constraint of the wait- as Associate Professor Michael Woodward and Dr.
list control group is that we are unable to specify Alastair Mander for their referral of aMCI participants
which component of the intervention was more potent to the study. We would also like to thank Dr. Sarah
in effecting behavior change, including the impact Price, Dr. Nadia Petruccelli, Sam Parsons and Fenny
of therapeutic attention alone. However, by selecting Muliadi for their contribution to the organization of
discrete assessments of memory strategy knowledge, the study, and the research assistants who aided in data
strategy use, and memory ability as well as the more collection. This work was supported by a NHMRC,
general outcome measure of wellbeing, we minimized Australia grant (487318) to GJK, DA, ES, BO, MMS,
the risk of over-interpreting the potential for simply LC, EM, and ER. KEP is supported by a NHMRC
generalized effects from therapeutic attention. Clinical Research Training Fellowship (602543).
In conclusion, our study findings provide support Authors disclosures available online (http://j-alz.
for the effectiveness of memory groups as a feasible com/manuscript-disclosures/15-0378r1).
intervention in an older age population to reduce and
limit cognitive difficulties in daily life. The value of
REFERENCES
memory groups is that they provide a low cost method
for focusing on the engagement of older adults into [1] Editorial (2015) WHO takes up the baton for dementia. Lancet
techniques for maintaining cognitive health. However, Neurol 14, 455.
our study findings suggest that a standard six-week [2] Deary IJ, Corley J, Gow AJ, Harris SE, Houlihan LM, Marioni
model of intervention needs adaptation to maximize RE, Penke L, Rafnsson SB, Starr JM (2009) Age-associated
cognitive decline. Br Med Bull 92, 135-152.
the benefits for people with aMCI. Although there was [3] Grady C (2012) The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat
no clear evidence of translation of intervention effects Rev Neurosci 13, 491-505.
into improved memory ability for the aMCI group, [4] Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH,
Fox NC, Gamst A, Holtzman DM, Jagust WJ, Petersen RC,
there were intervention benefits (increased knowl-
Snyder PJ, Carrillo MC, Their B, Phelps CH (2011) The diag-
edge and use of strategies, improved wellbeing and nosis of mild cognitive impairment due to alzheimers disease:
self-confidence in memory) that provide important Recommendations from the national institute on aging-
42 G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training

alzheimers association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines Unverzagt FQ, Willis SH (2002) Effects of cognitive training
for alzheimers disease. Alzheimers Dement 7, 270-279. interventions with older adult: A randomized control trial.
[5] Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jellic V, JAMA 288, 2271-2281.
Fratiglioni L (2014) Mild cognitive impairment: A concept in [23] Willis SL, Tennstedt SL, Marsiske M, Ball K, Elias J, Koepke
evolution. J Intern Med 275, 214-227. KM, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Unverzagt FW, Stoddard AM,
[6] Le Couteur DG, Doust J, Creasey H, Brayne C (2013) Politi- Wright E (2006) Long-term effects of cognitive training on
cal drive to screen for pre-dementia: Not evidence based and everyday functional outcomes in older adults. JAMA 296,
ignores the harms of diagnosis. BMJ 347, 1-6. 2805-2814.
[7] Brodaty H, Heffernan M, Kochan NA, Draper B, Trollor JN, [24] Rebok GW, Ball K, Guey LT, Jones RN, Kim HY, King JW,
Reppermund S, Slavin MJ, Sachdev PS (2013) Mild cognitive Marsiska M, Morris JN, Tennstedt SL, Unverzagt FW, Willis
impairment in a community sample: The Sydney memory and SL (2014) Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive train-
ageing study. Alzheimers Dement 9, 310-317. ing for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial
[8] Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul J, Al-Hashimi O, Faraji on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J Am
F, Janowich J, Kong E, Larraburo Y, Rolle C, Johnston E, Geriatr Soc 62, 16-24.
Gazzaley A (2013) Video game training enhances cognitive [25] Belleville S, Gilbert B, Fontaine F, Gagnon L, Menard E, Gau-
control in older adults. Nature 501, 97-101. thier S (2006) Improvement of episodic memory in persons
[9] Lampit A, Hallock H, Valenzuela M (2014) Computerized with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Evi-
cognitive training in cognitively healthy older adults: A sys- dence from a cognitive intervention program. Dement Geriatr
tematic review and meta-analysis of effect modifiers. PloS Cogn Disord 22, 486-499.
Med 11, 1-18. [26] Jean L, Bergeron ME, Thivierge S, Simard M (2010) Cog-
[10] Owen AM, Hampshire A, Grahn JA, Stenton R, Dajani S, nitive intervention programs for individuals with might
Burns AS, Howard RJ, Ballard CG (2010) Putting brain train- cognitive impairment: Systematic review of the literature. Am
ing to the test. Nature 465, 775-778. J Geriatr Psychiatry 18, 281-296.
[11] Shipstead Z, Hicks KL, Engle RW (2012) Cogmed working [27] Kinsella GJ, Mullaly E, Rand E, Ong B, Burton C, Price S,
memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? J Phillips M, Storey E (2009) Early intervention for mild cog-
Appl Res Mem Cogn 1, 185-193. nitive impairment: A randomised controlled trial. J Neurol
[12] McDaniel MA, Bugg JM (2012) Memory training interven- Neurosurg Psychiatry 80, 730-736.
tions: What has been forgotten? J Appl Res Mem Cogn 1, [28] Li H, Li J, Li N, Li B, Wang P, Zhou T (2011) Cognitive
45-50. intervention for persons with mild cognitive impairment: A
[13] Hampstead BM, Sathian K, Phillips PA, Amaraneni A, meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev 10, 285-296.
Delaune WR, Stringer AY (2012) Mnemonic strategy train- [29] Rejinders J, Van Heugten C, Van Boxtel M (2013) Cognitive
ing improved memory for object location associations in both interventions in healthy older adults and people with mild
healthy elderly and patients with amnestic mild cognitive cognitive impairment: A systematic review. Ageing Res Rev
impairment: A randomized, single-blind study. Neuropsy- 12, 263-275.
chology 26, 385-399. [30] Stott J, Spector A (2011) A review of the effectiveness of
[14] Kirchhoff BA, Anderson BA, Barch DM, Jacoby LL (2012) memory intervention in mild cognitive impairment (MCI).
Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy Int Psychogeriatr 23, 526-538.
training in older adults. Cereb Cortex 22, 788-799. [31] Troyer AK, Murhpy KJ, Anderson ND, Moscovitch M, Craik
[15] Roediger HL, Butler AC (2011) The critical role of retrieval FIM (2008) Changing everyday memory behavior in amnes-
practice in long-term retention. Trends Cogn Sci 15, 20-27. tic mild cognitive impairment: A randomised controlled trial.
[16] Naveh-Benjamin M, Brav TK, Levy O (2007) The associative Neuropsychol Rehabil 18, 65-88.
memory deficit of older adults: The role of strategy utilization. [32] Unvezagt FW, Kaston L, Johnson KE, Rebok GW, Mar-
Psychol Aging 22, 202-208. siska M, Koepke KM, Elias JW, Morris JN, Willis SL, Ball
[17] Gross AL, Parisi JM, Spira AP, Kueider AM, Ko JY, Sacynski K, Rexroth DF, Smith DM, Wolinsky FD, Tennstedt SL
JS, Samus QM, Rebok GW (2012) Memory training interven- (2007) Effect of memory impairment on training outcomes
tions for older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging Ment Health 16, in ACTIVE. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 13, 953-960.
722-734. [33] Brawley LR, Rejeski WJ, King AC (2003) Promoting phys-
[18] Kelly ME, Loughrey D, Lawlor BA, Robertson IH, Walsh C, ical activity for older adults: The challenges for changing
Brennan S (2014) The impact of cognitive training and mental behavior. Am J Prev Med 25, 172-183.
stimulation on cognitive and everyday functioning of healthy [34] Borkowski JG, Carr M, Pressley M (1987) Spontaneous
older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing strategy use: Perspective from metacognitive theory. Intelli-
Res Rev 15, 28-43. gence 11, 61-75.
[19] Papp KV, Walsh SJ, Snyder PJ (2009) Immediate and delayed [35] Hutchens RL, Kinsella GJ, Ong B, Pike KE, Parsons S, Storey
effects of cognitive interventions in healthy elderly: A review E, Ames D, Saling MM, Mullaly E, Rand E, Clare L (2012)
of current literature and future directions. Alzheimers Dement Knowledge and use of memory strategies in amnestic mild
5, 50-60. cognitive impairment. Psychol Aging 27, 768-777.
[20] Verhaeghen P, Marceon A, Goossens L (1992) Improving [36] Bandura A (1997) Self-efcacy: The exercise of control, WH
memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: Freeman, New York.
A meta-analytic study. Psychol Aging 7, 242-251. [37] Rothman AJ (2006) Initiative to Motivate Change: A review
[21] Martin M, Clare L, Altgassen AM, Cameron MH, Zehn- of theory and practice and their implications for older adults.
der F (2011) Cognition-based interventions for healthy older In When Im 64, Carstensen LL, Hartel CR, eds. The National
adults and people with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Academic Press, Washington D.C., pp. 121-144.
Database Syst Rev 1, 1-51. [38] Stine-Morrow EAL, Basak C (2011) Cognitive interventions.
[22] Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Mar- In Handbook of the psychology of aging, Schaie KW, Willis
siske M, Morris JN, Rebok GW, Smith DM, Tennstedt SL, SL, eds. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 153-171.
G.J. Kinsella et al. / MCI and Memory Training 43

[39] West RL, Bagwell DK, Dark-Freudeman A (2008) Self- [51] Fort I, Adoul L, Holl D, Kaddour J, Gana K (2004) Psycho-
efficacy and memory aging: The impact of a memory metric properties of the french version of the multifactorial
intervention based on self-efficacy. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn memory questionnaire for adults and the elderly. Can J Aging
B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 15, 302-329. 23, 347-357.
[40] Brandt J, Benedict RH (2001) The Hopkins Verbal Learn- [52] Wilson BA, Emslie H, Foley J, Shiel A, Watson P, Hawkins K,
ing Test Revised. Psychological Assessment Resources Inc., Groot Y, Evans JJ (2005) The Cambridge Prospective Memory
Lutz, Fl. Test, Harcourt Assessment, London.
[41] Wechsler D (1997) Wechsler Memory Scale Third Edition [53] Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA (2000) CVLT-II:
(WMS-III). The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. California verbal learning test second edition adult version,
[42] Wechsler D (2009) Wechsler Memory Scale Fourth Edition Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX.
(WMS-IV) Administrative and Scoring Manual. Pearson, San [54] Baraldi AN, Enders CK (2010) An introduction to modern
Antonio, TX. missing data analyses. J Sch Psychol 48, 5-37.
[43] Meyers JE, Meyers KR (1995) Rey complex figure test under [55] Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A (2007) G*Power
four different administration procedures. Clin Neuropsychol 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social,
9, 63-67. behavioural, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods 39,
[44] Morris JC (1993) The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Cur- 175-191.
rent version and scoring rules. Neurology 43, 2412-2414. [56] Gollwitzer PM (1999) Implementation intentions: Strong
[45] Mohs RC, Doody Rs, Morris JC, Ieni JR, Rogers SL, Perdomo effects of simple plans. Am Psychol 54, 493-503.
CA, Pratt RD (2001) A 1-year, placebo-controlled preserva- [57] Joubert S, Brambati SM, Ansado J, Barbeau EJ, Felician
tion of function survival study of donepezil in AD patients. O, Didic M, Lacombe J, Goldstein R, Chayer C, Kergoat
Neurology 57, 481-488. MJ (2010) The cognitive and neural expression of semantic
[46] McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, memory impairment in mild cognitive impairment and early
Jack CR Jr, Kawas CH, Klunk WE, Koroshetz WJ, Manly alzheimers disease. Neuropsychologia 48, 978-988.
JJ, Mayeux R, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rossor MN, Schel- [58] Fairchild JK, Friedman L, Rosen AC, Yesavage JA (2013)
tens P, Carrillo MC, Thies B, Weintraub S, Phelps CH Which older adults maintain benefit from cognitive train-
(2011) The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimers disease: ing? Use of signal detection methods to identify long-term
Recommendations from the national institute of aging- treatment gains. Int Psychogeriatr 25, 607-616.
alzheimers association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines [59] Dixon RA, de Frias CM (2007) Mild memory deficits differ-
for alzheimers disease. Alzheimers Dement 7, 263-269. entially affect 6-year changes in compensatory strategy use.
[47] Anstey KJ, Burns RA, Birrell CL, Steel D, Kiely KM, Luszcz Psychol Aging 22, 632-638.
MA (2010) Estimates of probable dementia prevalence from [60] Hertzog C, Price J, Dunlosky J (2008) How is knowledge gen-
population-based surveys compared with dementia preva- erated about memory encoding strategy effectiveness? Learn
lence estimates based on meta-analyses. BMC Neurol 10, Individ Diff 18, 430-445.
1-12. [61] Schmitter-Edgecombe M, Woo E, Greeley DR (2009) Char-
[48] Craik FIM, Winocur G, Palmer H, Binns MA, Edwards M, acterizing multiple memory deficits and their relation to
Bridges K, Glazer P, Chavannes R, Stuss DT (2007) Cog- everyday functioning in individuals with mild cognitive
nitive rehabilitation in the elderly: Effects on memory. J Int impairment. Neuropsychology 23, 168-177.
Neuropsychol Soc 13, 132-142. [62] Clare L, Bayer A, Burns A, Corbett A, Jones R, Knapp M,
[49] Evans JJ, Wilson BA, Needham P, Brentnall S (2003) Who Kopelman M, Kudlicka A, Leroi I, Oyebode J, Pool J, Woods
makes good use of memory aids? Results of a survey of people B, Whitaker R (2013) Goal-oriented cognitive rehabilitation
with acquired brain injury. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 9, 925-935. in early-stage dementia: Study protocol for a multi-centre
[50] Troyer AK, Rich JB (2002) Psychometric properties of a new single-blind randomised controlled trial (GREAT). Trials 14,
metamemory questionnaire for older adults. J Gerontol B 1-15.
Psychol Sci Soc Sci 57, 19-27.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen