Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY Vol. 21, No.

4
Copyright 1984 by The Society for Psychophysiological Research, Inc. Printed in U.S.A.

Lucid, Prelucid, and Nonlucid Dreams Related to the


Amount of EEG Alpha Activity during REM Sleep
PAUL D . TYSON, ROBERT D . OGILVIE, AND HARRY T . HUNT
Brock University, St. Catharines, Ontario

ABSTRACT
Alpha activity during REM sleep without signs of awakening can discriminate hetween the hlind
classification of prelucid, lucid, and nonlucid dreams. Ten good dream recallers were aroused after
relatively high or low amplitude REM alpha. The spectral and temporal characteristics of EEG
alpha within each REM period were associated with lucidity and other content dimensions. Each
type of dream had a reasonably distinct pattern of REM alpha. High amplitude alpha was found to
he associated with prelucid dreams and hizarre content, which is consistent with theories of waking
alpha activity and the hypothesis that lucidity sometimes emerges from prelucid experiences. The
data are also consistent with the idea that lucidity is a viahle dream content dimension, and
interpreted in terms of systems theory imply that training which emphasizes dream content control
may constrain the potential information integration function of lucid dreams.
DESCRIPTORS: Lucid dreams, REM sleep, EEG alpha. Sleep, Dream content. Spectral
analysis. Systems theory.

Since the publication of Aserinsky and Kleit- schaffen & Kales, 1968). Studies attempting to de-
man's (1953) suggestion that dreams occur during fine an awakening from sleep have frequently re-
periods of rapid, conjugate eye movements (REM), corded abundant alpha activity during REM with-
many investigators have attempted to find phys- out signs of awakening, although button-pressing
iological correlates of dream content (Arkin, An- and increased EMG levels in close association with
trobus, & EUman, 1978; Foulkes, 1978; Ogilvie, alpha activity may satisfactorily define an awak-
Hunt,Sawacki,&Samahalskyi, 1982a; Hunt, 1982). ening (Goodenough, Shapiro, Holden, & Stein-
Lucidity is one dimension of dream content which schriber, 1959; Williams, Morlock, & Morlock, 1966;
has been distinguished by a reported realization that Anch, Salamy, McCoy, & Somerset, 1982; Moffitt
one is dreaming in the midst of the dream and with- et al., 1982). The amount of alpha activity within
out awakening from sleep (Green, 1968). Confir- REM has been correlated (.75) with waking alpha
mation that lucid reports occur most frequently (Johnson, Lubin, Naitoh, Nute, & Austin, 1969)
within the REM stage and are not artifacts of awak- although REM alpha responds differently to stim-
ening from sleep has been found in several labo- ulation than waking alpha (Johnson, 1970). The
ratory studies (Ogilvie, Hunt, Sawicki, & Me- present study focused on whether the amount of
Gowan, 1978; LaBerge, 1980, 1981; Heame, 1981; alpha activity during REM sleep, without signs of
Ogilvie, Hunt, Tyson, Lucescu, & Jenkins, 1982b). an awakening, was associated with reports of lucid-
Ogilvie et al. (1978,1982b) suggested an association ity within the dream.
between lucid reports and high amplitude EEG al- The relationship between lucidity and REM al-
pha activity occurring during REM sleep, which is pha has not been found by the other investigators
paradoxical because standardized sleep stage scor- who have concentrated on dreams which were un-
ing systems define a minimum of 10 s of alpha ambiguously lucid using a small number of trained,
activity within a 20-s epoch as wakefulness (Recht- frequent lucid dreamers (LaBerge, 1980, 1981;
Heame, 1981). In addition to deliberate attempts
to dream lucidly. Green (1968) describes a number
Supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Re- ^^ Prelucid experiences which frequently precede
search Council of Canada #A6354 to R.D.O. ^^cid dreams. Dreams that are highly emotional.
Address requests for reprints to: Paul D. Tyson, De- irrational, or incongruous with waking life can lead
partment of Psychology, Brock University, St. Catharines, to prelucid dreams in which the person reports de-
Ontario, Canada L2S 3A1. veloping a critical attitude toward the dream by
442
July, 1984 Dream Lucidity Related to Alpha Activity 443
questioning the apparent reality ofthe dream. The and lucid dreams. REM periods in the preliminary
prelucid dream's critical attitude may not develop report were divided into high and low amplitude
into reports of the simultaneous realization and alpha arousals based upon subjective estimates of
continuous participation typical ofthe lucid dream the relative amount of filtered alpha 20 or 30 s pre-
and lucid dreams may not all evolve from prelucid ceding the arousal. The subjective amplitude cri-
experiences. The hypothesized difference between terion for low and high alpha was consistent within
deliberate training to produce lucid dreams and lu- subjects, but varied considerably among subjects as
cidity that emerges from prelucid experiences may the Ogilvie et al. (1982b) EEG records clearly dem-
account for the failure to confirm the Ogilvie et al. onstrate. The significant differences between high
(1978,1982b) relationship between REM alpha and and low alpha arousals could be further refined by
the emergence of lucid dreams. calculating the actual mean alpha amplitude pre-
The content of lucid dreams emerging from pre- ceding each arousal. By using an objective measure
lucid experiences may be different from deliberate of alpha amplitude, differences in the amplitude
training to produce lucid dreams. LaBerge (1980, criterion among subjects can be analyzed and the
1981) describes the most salient features of lucid amplitude relative to the individual's variability can
dream^s with trained dreamers as active control of be analyzed by converting the amplitude criterion
imagery, emotional detachment, and critical ra- into standard deviation units from each individu-
tionalitytypical of left hemispheric dominance, in al's average REM alpha. In the preliminary report
agreement with Hearne's (1981) observations and the spectral analysis of four complete REM periods
Cohen's (1979) theorizing. Lucid dreams emerging for one subject showed distinct temporal differences
from prelucid experiences with untrained dreamers in EEG activity during the REM period which could
would be expected to report less control, more emo- be included in the analysis. The present report ana-
tionality, and considerably more bizarre elements. lyzed the Ogilvie et al. (1982b) raw EEG data using
Ogilvie et al. (1982b), comparing nonlucid, prelu- spectral analysis to examine the degree to which
cid, and lucid dreams using blind scoring of dream REM alpha and content variates discriminate be-
content (Hunt, 1982), found that prelucid dreams tween nonlucid, prelucid, and lucid dreams.
were the most bizarre (clouded and hallucinatory),
nonlucid dreams were mundane and realistic, and Method
lucid dreams closer to the realism of nonlucid
dreams and exhibiting the most control, consistent Subjects
with Green (1968) and Hoffman and McCarley Five males and 5 females, aged 19-31, were selected
(1980). Hunt (1982) describes clouding as reports on the bases of recalling 5 or more dreams per week
of unexplained scene changes and cognitive disor- and having clear alpha activity. Five had previously
ganizations of reasoning and memorya confu- slept in the lab, were good dream recallers following
sional dimension; whereas hallucinosis consists of experimental arousals, and displayed moderate
perceptual content and form anomaliesusually in amounts of alpha during REM periods. The other sub-
the visual modality. In this study all lucid dreams jects were screened for dream recall and displayed
included one or more prelucid episodes, which was moderate amounts of waking alpha during relaxed, eyes
closed EEG baselines. All volunteers had reported hav-
consistent with the Ogilvie et al. (1982b) view that ing lucid dreams at least occasionally; frequencies
lucidity frequently emerged from prelucid experi- ranged from one or two lucid dreams in several sub-
ences in untrained dreamers. jects to near nightly lucid episodes reported by one
There are at least two convergent approaches participant. More typically, subjects reported recalling
which can be used to examine the alpha-lucidity several lucid dreams per year. Two subjects were ex-
hypothesis: one used in the preliminary report made perienced meditators and one of the meditators was
systematic observations of dream content by arous- familiar with biofeedback techniques.
ing subjects when their REM alpha levels were par- Apparatus
ticularly high or low. The other approach reported
in the present paper involves the computer analysis A Nihon-Kohden Model ME-175E EEG machine
of the EEG activity preceding each arousal. Com- was used to record 16 channels of physiological in-
formation; details of the monitoring scheme and sub-
puter analysis ofthe Ogilvie et al. (1982b) experi- ject records have been published (Ogilvie et al., 1982b).
ment will refine the testing ofthe alpha-lucidity hy- EEG recorded from the right central derivation (C4)
pothesis by quantifying the spectral and temporal and right mastoid (Aj) was filtered for alpha (8-13 Hz)
aspects of the EEG, standardizing the individual's by a Narco biofeedback system for the high and low
alpha amplitude criterion for an arousal, and ana- alpha arousals, and the raw EEG channels were re-
lyzing the degree to which REM alpha and content corded on an 8-channel Vetter FM tape recorder with
variables discriminate between nonlucid, prelucid. flutter compensator for the computer analysis.
444 Tyson, Ogilvie, and Hunt Vol. 21, No. 4

Design and Controls quency components by power spectral analysis (Fast


Fourier Transform). The raw EEG was transformed
All subjects were required to spend one adaptation using a modified Bruker TI/II program which allowed
night and one or two arousal nights in the sleep lab- magnitude integration within two alpha frequency
oratory. No arousals were made on the adaptation night, bands (8-10.5 Hz and 10.5-13 Hz) and calculation of
but 5 subjects, randomly selected from the 10 volun- the mean integrated amplitude in microvolts (Tyson,
teers, were given four 5-min alpha feedback trials on 1982a). The absolute spectral power of the raw EEG
the adaptation and arousal nights before going to sleep. was transformed at a sample rate twice the highest
The alpha training was not very effective for two peo- frequency (34 Hz) in consecutive 30-s epochs and was
ple who lapsed into Stage 2 sleep during the sessions. calibrated into microvolts with a 50 ^V, 10 Hz sine
The adaptation night was used to set a within-subject wave recorded at the beginning of each session.
criterion for high and low alpha REM arousals using
the filtered alpha channel. The absolute alpha ampli- The temporal parameters within each REM period
tude criterion varied among subjects, but was rela- were summarized in terms of 4 time intervals: 0-.5
tively consistent within subjects for all arousals. min, .5-2 min, 2-5 min before the arousal, and 5 min
to the beginning of the REM period, for both the higher
On the arousal nights, subjects were told that they frequency alpha (HFA) and lower frequency alpha
would be awakened during REM sleep 4 times and (LFA). The first time interval was the HFA and LFA
were made familiar with the questionnaire, but were mean integrated amplitude for the 30-s epoch before
completely blind to the type of arousal, to control for the arousal and corresponds to the Ogilvie et al. (1982b)
suggestibility and demand characteristics. The order arousal criterion. The 3 remaining time intervals were
of arousals was also randomized by tossing a coin to arbitrarily defined without inspection of the data as
determine whether a given arousal was to be made the average of epochs in progressively longer periods;
during high or low alpha activity. To ensure that lucid for example, the second time interval averaged three
reports were not artifacts of awakening from REM sleep, 30-s epochs between .5-2 min and the third interval
the arousal criterion required a minimum of 5 min in averaged 6 epochs. Since the criterion alpha amplitude
stage REM, a minimum of 20 s above or below the for an arousal in this experiment was relative to the
alpha criterion, and no movement artifacts, elevations within-subject variability, the HFA and LFA ampli-
of EMG, eyeblinks, abrupt changes in heart rate or tudes for each epoch were also converted into standard
respiratory activity, or other discontinuities during deviation units from each individual's HFA and LFA
REM. A careful examination of the sleep records after means and standard deviations using the total number
the experiment was used to verify the arousal criterion of 30-s epochs of all REM periods which ranged from
for each arousal and led to the rejection of two arousals 65 epochs to 160 epochs in different subjects. The 30-
as ambiguous. s epoch before the arousal was described as HFA and
Following each arousal, the experimenter read a list LFA average amplitude and standardized amplitude.
of 8 questions (Ogilvie et al. 1982b) to eliminate the In addition to averaging the standard deviations for
possibility of different demand characteristics biasing the 3 remaining time intervals, two deviation mea-
the interrogation (Ome, 1962). The taped arousal in- sures of above average alpha were calculated since pre-
terviews were coded, transcribed blind, and given to vious research hypothesized a relationship between high
HTH for scoring. HTH was blind to the code and had alpha during REM and lucidity (Ogilvie et al., 1978,
not been involved in the laboratory proceedings. Ini- 1982b). The first measure, positive deviation, averaged
tially, HTH was given only the narrative protocols to the deviations above the mean and the second measure
rate for dream content and bizarreness using the scor- found the one peak deviation for each of the 3 re-
ing methods developed and reliability tested on a sam- maining time intervals. These 3 time intervals were
ple of 800 dreams by Hunt (1982). The lucidity ratings therefore described by four measures: average ampli-
(Ogilvie et al., 1982b) were obtained during a second tude, standardized amplitude, positive deviation, and
examination of the recoded, transcribed answers to peak deviation.
standardized lucidity, prelucidity, and control ques-
tions consistent with Green's (1968) typology. Positive Results
answers to the questions were subdivided in terms of
whether a brief momentary awareness was being de- An examination of the EEG 30 s before the
scribed, an awareness that lasted through a definite arousal in terms of alpha amplitude and standard-
narrative sequence (rated beginning, middle, or end), ized amplitude showed a clear relationship between
or an awareness roughly continuous throughout the high alpha activity and prelucid dreams (Table 1).
entire dream. Details of the lucidity scale and proce- After removing the two ambiguous dreams, the 38
dures have been previously published (Ogilvie et al.,
1982b). dreams classified as lucid, prelucid, or nonlucid were
analyzed using an unequal n analysis of variance
Data Analysis adjusting the error term degrees of freedom for re-
In order to quantify the spectral distribution of the peated measures. The lower frequency alpha (LFA)
EEG, the raw C4-A2 channel recorded on the FM tape and higher frequency alpha (HFA) mean ampli-
recorder was first passed through active bandpass fil- tudes (LFA: F(2/32) - 5.57, p<.OI; HFA: i^(2/32)
ters between 1 and 40 Hz and decomposed into fre- = 4.76, p<.025) and standardized amplitudes (LFA:
July, 1984 Dream Lucidity Related to Alpha Activity 445
Table 1
Mean alpha activity 30 seconds before the arousal and at the beginning ofthe REM period

Average Amplitude Standardized1 Amplitude Positive Deviation Peak Deviation


Dream Type LFA HFA LFA HFA LFA HFA LFA HFA

30 Seconds Before Arousal

Nonlucid 19.52 18,04 ,03 ,01


Prelucid 31,22 26,68 1,68 1,94
Lucid 19,24 16,22 ,38 ,24
(jX.025) (jX.025) (p<.OOl)

Beginning of REM Period

Nonlucid 18,24 17,18 -,11 -,01 ,48 ,58 ,52 ,54


Prelucid 21,03 17,77 ,23 ,24 ,91 ,92 1,90 1,61
Lucid 21,16 17,33 ,51 ,38 ,93 ,84 2,07 1,82
ip=.912) (p<.025) (P<.Q5)

F(2/32) = 4.49, p<.025; HFA: F{2/32) = 8.04, than lucid ratings. The revised lucidity ratings for
p<.005) were significantly different and the means the 38 dreams were correlated, correcting the de-
in Table 1 indicated that prelucid dreams were the grees of freedom for repeated measures {df = 32),
highest in alpha amplitude and most deviant rel- with alpha amplitude 30 s before the arousal (LFA:
ative to each subject's mean REM amplitude. In r = .42, p<.025; HFA: r = .40, p<.025) and stand-
order to analyze the degree to which alpha activity ardized amplitude (LFA: r = .43, p<.025; HFA: r
discriminated between nonlucid, prelucid, and lu- = .51, ;7<.OO5). Adding the dream control dimen-
cid dreams, the 7^(1/32) ratios and significance lev- sion to the revised lucidity scale increased the cor-
els were calculated between pairs of dream types relations only slightly for alpha amplitude (LFA: r
(Table 2). Considering only the last 30-s spectral = .45, p<.Ol, HFA: r = .40, p<.025) and stand-
plot, which Ogilvie et al. (1982b) used as the cri- ardized amplitude (LFA: r = .45, p<.01; HFA: r
terion for an arousal, illustrates (Table 2) that high = .53, p<.005).
amplitude alpha which deviated considerably from The two earlier time periods between .5-2 min
an individual's mean was a good indicator of a pre- and 2-5 min before the arousal were poor discrim-
lucid dream (Figure 1, A & C; Figure 2), but low inators of nonlucid, lucid, and prelucid dreams. No
amplitude alpha preceding the arousal indicated significant differences were found in either time in-
either a nonlucid (Figure 1, D & E) or a lucid dream terval for alpha amplitude, standardized amplitude,
(Figure 1, B & F). positive deviation, or peak deviation. Correlations
Ogilvie et al. (1982b) hypothesized a linear re- of these time intervals with the revised lucidity scales
lationship between the amount of alpha activity and also were nonsignificant with the exception of the
lucidity, such that low amplitude alpha would co- .5-2 min period for HFA positive deviation (r =
exist with nonlucid reports, moderate alpha with .35, p<.05) and peak deviation (r = .35, p<.05),
prelucid or brief lucid episodes, and high alpha with and adding the control dimension to the lucidity
lucidity throughout the dream. The above associ- scale slightly reduced these correlations.
ation of prelucidity with high alpha led to reorder- The alpha activity at the beginning ofthe dream
ing the earlier lucidity scale (Ogilvie et al., 1982b), made it possible to differentiate nonlucid from lucid
so that prelucid ratings were weighted more heavily dreams, both of which ended with relatively low

Table 2
Average REM alpha activity used to discriminate between dream types

F Values (p Values in Parentheses)

Before Arousal HFA REM Beginning LFA Discriminant Scores


Standardized Amplitude Positive Deviation Combined HFA & LFA
(df= 1/32)

Dream Type Nonlucid Prelucid Nonlucid Prelucid Nonlucid Prelucid

Prelucid 13,48 (p<,001) 6,75 (p<.025) 10,97 {p<.0Ol)


Lucid 0,17 (p=,680) 8,31 6,71 (jX.025) 0,01 (p=.9O4) 3.50{p<.05 ) 4,05 (p<.025)
446 Tyson, Ogilvie, and Hunt Vol. 21. No. 4
(A)CR REM 5 PRELUCID (B)NN REM 4 LUCID

12
11

10
9

8
7 ;v^^2-^\^AAA/^v2^^^

13 20 25 30 1 13 20 25

E (C) RR REM 2 PRELUCID (D) JD REM 3 NONLUCID

13 20 25
(E)CR REM3 NONLUCID

12
(F) RR REM5 LUCID
11

10
9
8
7
6

5
4
3
2

13 20 25 30 1 13 20 25 30

Frequoncy (Hz) Frequency (Hz)


Figure 1. Power spectral analysis (FFT) of 6 complete REM periods plotted in consecutive 30-s epochs begiiming
at REM onset and ending 30 s preceding the arousal. Two examples of prelucid (A & C), ludd (B & F), and nonlucid
(D & E) dreams illustrate the different patterns of REM alpha (8-13 Hz) used to discriminate dream types.
July. 1984 Dream Lucidity Related to Alpha Activity 447
(A) RR REM 3 PRELUCID (B)JS REM4 PRELUCID

21

20

19 19

18 18 ^

17 17

16 16

15 15

14 14

f
o 12

I
a

I,
ICD

5 7

13 20 25 30 1 13 20 25 30
Frequency (Hz) Frequency (Hz)

Figure 2. Power spectral analysis (FFT) of 2 complete REM periods plotted in consecutive 30-s epochs beginning
at REM onset and ending 30 s preceding the arousal. Two examples of prelucid dreams illustrate the waxing and
waning of REM alpha (8-13 Hz) during the REM period.

levels of alpha activity 30 s preceding the arousal indicate either a prelucid (Figure 1, A & C; Figure
(Table 1). No significant differences in alpha am- 2) or lucid dream (Figure 1, B & F).
plitude were found at the beginning of the three The classification of the three dream types com-
types of dreams; however, the LFA standardized bining the most significant alpha measures at the
amplitude approached significance (LFA: i^2/32) beginning and end of the REM period called for a
= 3.30, p = .054), which suggested that lower alpha direct discriminant analysis (Tabachnick & Fidell,
deviations at the beginning of the dream might be 1983). The two alpha measures were combined by
associated with nonlucidity at the time of the weighting each score to maximize the separation of
arousal, whereas both lucid and prelucid dreams the groups and the first discriminant function was
had hi^er alpha at the beginning. The significant significant (Wilks' Lambda = .548, x^ = 20.75,
results in Table 1 for LFA positive deviation (LFA: p<.OOl). The differences between pairs of dream
F(2/32) = 4.78, ;7<.O25) and peak deviation (LFA: types by a linear combination of HFA standardized
F{2/32) = 4.55, p<.025; HFA: F{2/32) = 3.35, amplitude 30 s preceding the arousal and LFA pos-
P<.05) reinforced this association. The significant itive deviation at the beginning of the REM period
diiSerences between pairs of dream types in Table are displayed in Table 2. The three types of dreams
2 showed that low average amounts of alpha below were empirically distinct from each other, and as
an individual's mean indicate nonlucidity (Figure expected, the most significant difference was be-
1, D & E), but high deviations at dream onset may tween nonlucid and prelucid dreams. The dreams
448 Tyson, Ogilvie, and Hunt Vol. 21, No. 4

with the highest probability of being classified as lucid dreams (F(l/32) = 5.12, p<.05). Time of night
nonlucid had relatively low levels of alpha activity was significantly correlated with the revised lucidity
at the end and beginning of the REM period, and scale (r = .37, p<.05), tended to be negatively cor-
the dreams classified as prelucid had relatively high related with control (r = .30, p = .07), and not
levels at the end and beginning. As REM alpha lev- significantly correlated with REM alpha (LFA: r =
els became less extreme the probability of correct .17; HFA: r = .24). It is interesting to note that
classification was reduced; therefore lucid dreams self-rated emotional involvement increased signif-
classified by moderately low levels of alpha at the icantly {r = .44, p<.Ol) with the number of arousals
end and moderately high levels at the beginning during the night.
were the most difficult dream type to discriminate. In summary, the major finding was a relatively
Overall the discriminant score for each dream cor- distinct pattern of REM alpha for prelucid, non-
rectly classified 71% of the dreams using the begin- lucid, and lucid dreams which correctly classified
ning and end of the REM period. 71% of the dreams. Prelucid dreams with relatively
Blind scoring of dream content found the three high REM alpha at the beginning of the period and
types of dreams significantly different on both di- preceding the arousal were most distinct from non-
mensions of bizarreness (Clouding: F(2/32) = 4.98, lucid dreams with low REM alpha. Bizarre dream
p<.025; Hallucinosis: Fi2/32) = 3.61, p<.05), but content also discriminated prelucid from nonlucid
not significantly different in control or self-rated dreams and was associated with high REM alpha
emotional involvement in the dream. The amount at the beginning of the period. Although empirically
of clouding in prelucid dreams was significantly distinct, lucid dreams were the most difficult to dis-
greater than both nonlucid (/'(1/32) = 8.85,/x.Ol) criminate with a pattern of relatively high REM
and lucid (F(l/32) = 5.88, p<.025) dreams, but alpha at the beginning and moderately low REM
nonlucid and lucid dreams were not significantly alpha preceding the arousal. The amount of cloud-
different in reported clouding {F(l/32) = 0.58). The ing discriminated prelucid from lucid dreams, but
prelucid hallucinatory reports were significantly outside of the lucidity dimension nonlucid and lu-
greater than nonlucid dreams (F(l/32) = 7.15, cid dreams could not be discriminated with any of
p<.025), but hallucinosis was unable to signifi- the other content dimensions measured in this study.
cantly discriminate between prelucid and lucid iF{l/
32) = 1.23) or lucid and nonlucid (i='(l/32) = 1.83). Discussion
The revised scale which ordered prelucidity greater The occurrence of alpha activity during REM
than lucidity was significantly correlated with sleep without signs of awakening encourages further
clouding (r = .34, p<.05) and hallucinosis (r = research into psychophysiological correlates of REM
.35, p<.05), and these two dimensions of bizarre- alpha, and the relationship with the lucidity di-
ness were significantly (/* = .54, p<.001) correlated mension enhances the face validity of lucid dream
with each other. Bizarre content was significantly reports. Independent assessment of physiological
correlated (Clouding: r = .36, p<.05; Hallucinosis: measures and dream content classification systems
r = .47, p<.005) with alpha activity at the begin- is essential to establishing a psychophysiological as-
ning of the REM period, but not significantly cor- sociation. In this study the amount of REM alpha
related with REM alpha preceding the arousal was found to discriminate between the blind clas-
(Clouding: r = .26; Hallucinosis: r = .14). Bizarre sification of prelucid, lucid, and nonlucid dreams.
dream content seems to be most typical of prelucid Lucidity was scaled independent of any involve-
dreams and high REM alpha at the beginning of ment in the laboratory proceedings and was scaled
the REM period; however REM alpha was a better blind to the arousal condition and other content
discriminator of lucid dream types than any of the dimensions. The demand characteristics and sug-
dimensions of dream content. gestibility of participating in a lucid dream study
Cohen (1979) hypothesized that dream lucidity were equivalent for all dream types and independ-
would gradually increase during the period asleep ent of the arousal manipulation. Arousal order ef-
refiecting a shift from right to left hemispheric dom- fects were randomized by flipping a coin to deter-
inance. There was a tendency for the three dream mine a high or low alpha arousal and the arousal
types to differ in terms of time of night (F(2/32) = interrogation was standardized. The association be-
2.11, p = .08) and not to differ significantly for tween REM alpha and lucidity adds support to the
REM (F(2/32) = 0.74) or arousal number (F(2/32) psychophysiological study of lucidity as a dream
= 0.72). The average time of night in hours and content dimension.
minutes of prelucid (5:38) and lucid (5:02) dreams High amplitude and deviant REM alpha pre-
was later than nonlucid (3:55) dreams, but only pre- ceding the arousal clearly discriminates prelucid
lucid dreams were significantly different from non- dreams from nonlucid and lucid dreams. Current
July, 1984 Dream Lucidity Related to Alpha Activity 449

theories of waking alpha offer one explanation for cid dreams to other dream types, failed to quantify
the association between prelucid dreams and high the spectral and temporal characteristics of REM
REM alpha which has some interesting parallels to alpha, and used a small select sample of lucid dreams
the suggestion that REM sleep reflects endogenous and trained lucid dreamers. The findings in this
stimulation of the oculomotor system (Roffwarg, study show why these investigators observing only
Muzio, & Dement, 1966), more general periodic lucid dreams would not see the relationship be-
cortical activation (Ephron & Carrington, 1966), and tween high REM alpha and prelucidity or a pattem
rate of information processing (de la Pena, Zarcone, of REM alpha associated with lucidity; they con-
& Dement, 1973). The oculomotor hypothesis firmed the difficulty of discriminating lucid and
(MulhoUand, 1968, 1973; Peper, 1971), synthesized nonlucid dreams by the amount of REM alpha pre-
by Wertheim (1974), argues that the blocking of ceding the arousal.
waking alpha is determined not by the presence or Discrimination of lucid dreams from nonlucid
absence of visual stimulation or efferent motor dreams was possible by measuring the positive al-
commands, but by the interlocking of these systems pha deviations from the individual's REM mean
where sensory or imagined information guides the and by measuring the peak deviation within a time
motor adjustments (Tyson & Audette, 1979). One period. This supports the Ogilvie et al. (1978, 1982b)
method for producing alpha by disconnecting these hypothesis that high REM alpha or positive devia-
two systems would be to eliminate the predictions tions may be more important or at least better dis-
necessary for neuromuscular control, attention, and criminators of lucid dreams. Positive and peak de-
habituation (Weiner, 1948; Powers, 1973; Treis- viations refine the testing of the hypothesis by lim-
man, 1969; Pribram, 1971; Tyson & Gavard, 1976). iting the measurement range to the positive or the
The unpredictable, bizarre prelucid dream would single most deviant epoch. The significance of these
be expected to disconnect the sensory motor sys- measures lies in the possibility that a definable, rel-
tems and produce alpha activity. Once released from atively brief, unusual physiological change at par-
sensory motor control, the alpha generators, wheth- ticular points in the REM period may be associated
er corticothalamic (Andersen & Andersson, 1968, with becoming lucid. Absolute alpha amplitude was
1974) or cortical (Lopes da Silva, Van Lierop, Schri- found to be an insensitive measure at the beginning
jer, & Storm van Leeuwen, 1973; Lopes da Silva, of the REM period and did not approach signifi-
vanRotterdam,Barts, van Heusden, & Burr, 1976), cance. The standardization of physiological mea-
could recruit other cortical generators to produce sures occurs infrequently in psychophysiology, al-
the harmonic spectral plots (Figures 1 and 2) found though the REM period, in contrast to NREM, is
in this study and Ogilvie et al. (1982b). The base usually characterized by heightened variability in
frequencies, determined by the harmonic power physiological measures and a relationship has been
distributions (Pavlidis, 1973), in all cases were with- found between the variability of physiological mea-
in the alpha frequency range which supports the sures and dream content (Rechtschaffen, 1973).
suggestion that alpha was the basic generator, al- Lucid dreams were found to have relatively high
though future research may find other harmonics REM alpha at the beginning of the period followed
better discriminators due to alpha power ceiling ef- by lower levels near the arousal. In some lucid
fects. Theoretically there are a number of ways and dreams the change from relatively high to low alpha
levels at which the sensory motoi systems can be was quite abrupt (Figure 1, B & F) and may cycle
disconnected, which may explain the variety of ex- over time during long REM periods (Figure 2). The
periences associated with waking alpha and medi- association between high alpha and prelucid, bi-
tation (Kamiya, 1969; Walsh, 1974; Plotkin, 1969). zarre experiences at the beginning of the REM pe-
Multivariate analysis of waking alpha experiences riod supports Green's (1968) suggestion that lucid-
(Tyson & Audette, 1979) found one distinct pattern ity is sometimes preceded by prelucid experiences,
of experiences which is analogous to the unpre- Finding lucid dreams in the middle in terms of REM
dictable, bizarre prelucid experiences. Depending alpha and between the predictable, realistic non-
on many variables such as set, setting, and stimu- lucid dream content and the unpredictable, bizarre
lation, other distinct experiences have been found prelucid dreams implies that lucidity may be a com-
associated with waking alpha (Tyson, 1982a) and bination of more than one process, coexisting si-
similar variables may affect the dream experiences multaneously. According to systems theory, the si-
associated with REM alpha. multaneous coexistence of subsystems is as com-
There are several reasons other investigators mon as driving a car and carrying on a conversation
(LaBerge, 1980, 1981; Heame, 1981) failed to con- (Tyson, 1982b). Lucidity shifts the hierarchy of at-
firm the Ogilvie et al. (1978, 1982b) alpha-lucidity tention allowing you to drive the car and simul-
hypothesis. These investigators did not compare lu- taneously monitor the driving instead of engaging
450 Tyson, Ogilvie, and Hunt Vol, 21, No, 4

in a variety of other activities such as carrying on vestigating. Lucid dream training which is effective
a conversation. The allocation of attention to mon- should also manipulate REM alpha assuming they
itoring waxes and wanes as you attend to other ac- are causally related. The pattem of association may
tivities. Lucid monitoring of driving wanes during change during training, a person may learn to rec-
extremely predictable and unpredictable driving ognize a dream using more subtle cues, circum-
conditions. Lapses of active monitoring sometimes venting bizarre, prelucid experiences, or may leam
happen for long periods of time on long trips or to remain lucid even during bizarre experiences and
well trod routes, and lapses also happen during slip- abundant REM alpha. Analogous to the de la Pena
pery conditions or stressful emergency situations et al. (1973) theorizing, the functional significance
where total attention capacity is allocated to driving of lucid dreams may be to facilitate the consoli-
the car (Tyson, 1982b). The periodic cycling of lu- dation and integration of dream content into the
cidity is like a balanced scale and similar to the language and behavior ofthe individual. However,
waxing and waning of alpha activity. In terms of attempts to control dream content, according to in-
dream content, as the hierarchy of cognitive orga- formation theory (Gamer, 1962), would constrain
nizations begins to level, attention becomes more channel capacity and information exchange. The
simultaneous, memory more image-like, associa- implication is that lucid dream training which em-
tions become more random and dreams more bi- pasizes dream content control may constrain the
zarre. The more equivalent allocation of attention potential information integration function of lucid-
should involve greater cerebral area with a resulting ity.
increase in EEG power. In conclusion, the anaiysis refined the Ogilvie et
Lucidity occurs in the middle between prelucid al. (1978, 1982b) alpha-lucidity hypothesis by de-
and nonlucid dreams and is associated with mod- fining distinct patterns of alpha activity during the
erate levels of REM alpha. Research is needed to course of the REM period for each type of dream.
test this association by training people to manip- High REM alpha was associated with prelucid
ulate lucidity or alpha activity, but the training may dreams and bizarre dream content. Lucid dreams
change the association. Training using alpha feed- were found to have high alpha early in the REM
back has shown that the duration, amplitude, and period followed by a distinct lowering of REM al-
spectral distribution of waking alpha can be signif- pha, which supports the hypothesis that lucidity
icantly manipulated (Tyson, 1982a). Whether wak- sometimes emerges from prelucid experiences. The
ing alpha training affects REM alpha is uncertain, findings in this study should encourage further re-
but the strong correlation (Johnson et al, 1969) search into the psychophysiological correlates of
between the two makes the possibility worth in- REM alpha and the lucidity content dimension.

REFERENCES
Anch, A.M., Salamy, J.G., McCoy, G.F., & Somerset, J.S. Ephron, H.S., & Carrington, P. (1966). Rapid eye move-
(1982). Behaviorally signalled awakening in relation- ment sleep and cortical homeostasis. Psychological Re-
ship to duration of alpha activity, Psychophysiology. view, 75, 500-526.
19, 528-530. Foulkes, D. (1978). A grammar of dreams. New York:
Andersen, P., & Andersson, S.A. (1968). Physiological ba- Basic Books.
sis ofthe alpha rhythm. New York: Appleton-Century Gamer, W.R. (1962). Uncertainty and structure as psy-
Crofts. chological concepts. New York: Wiley.
Andersen, P., & Andersson, S.A. (1974). Thalamic origin Goodenough, D.R., Shapiro, A., Holden, M., & Stein-
of cortical rhythmic activity. In O. Creutzfeldt (Ed.), schriber, L. (1959). A comparison of dreamers and
Handbook of electroencephalography and clinical neu- nondreamers: Eye movements, electroencephalo-
rophysiology (pp. 90-118). Amsterdam: Elsevier. grams, and the recall of dreams. Journal of Abnormal
Arkin, A., Antrobus, J., & EUman, S. (Eds.) (1978). The and Social Psychology, 59, 295-302.
mind in sleep. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum As- Green, C. (1968). Lucid dreams. London: Hamish Ham-
sociates. ilton.
Aserinsky, E., & Kleitman, N. (1953). Regularly occurring Heame, K. (1981). Control your own dreams. New Sci-
periods of eye motility and concomitant phenomena entist, 24, 783-785.
during sleep. Science, 118, 273-274. Hoffman, E., & McCarley, R. (1980). Bizarreness and lu-
Cohen, D.B. (1979). Sleep and dreaming: Origins, nature cidity in REM sleep dreams [Abstract]. Sleep Research,
and functions. New York: Pergamon Press. 9, 134.
De la Pefta, A., Zarcone, V., & Dement, W.C. (1973). Hunt, H. (1982). Forms of dreaming. Perceptual and Mo-
Correlations between measures ofthe rapid eye move- tor Skills, 54, 559-633.
ments of wakefulness and sleep. Psychophysiology, 10, Johnson, L , Lubin, A., Naitoh, P., Nute, C , & Austin,
488-500. M. (1969). Spectral analysis ofthe EEG of dominant
July. 1984 Dream Lucidity Related to Alpha Activity 451
and non-dominant alpha subjects during waking and Peper, E. (1971). Reduction of efferent motor commands
sleep. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophys- during alpha feedback as a facilitator of EEG alpha
iology, 26, 361-370. and a precondition for changes in consciousness. Ky-
Johnson, L.C. (1970). A psychophysiology for all states. bernetic, 9, 226-231.
Psychophysiology, 6, 501-516. Plotkin, W.B. (1979). The alpha experience revisited: Bio-
Kamiya, J. (1969). Operant control of the EEG alpha feedback in the transformation of psychological state.
rhythm and some of its reported effects on conscious- Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1132-1148.
ness. In C.T. Tart (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness Powers, W.T. (1973). Behaviour: The control of perception.
(pp. 507-517). New York: Wiley. Chicago: Aldine.
LaBerge, S. (1980). Lucid dreaming as a leamable skill: A Pribram, K.H. (1971). Languages of the brain: Experi-
case study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 1039-1042. mental paradoxes and principles in neuropsychology.
LaBerge, S. (1981). Lucid dreaming: Directing the act as Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
it happens. Psychology Today, 15(1), 48-57. Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (Eds.) (1968). A manual of
Lopes da Silva, F.H., Van Lierop, T.H., Schrijer, C.F., & standardized terminology, techniques and scoring sys-
Storm van Leeuwen, W. (1973). Organization of tha- tem for sleep stages of human subjects. Washington,
lamic and cortical alpha rhythms: Spectra and coher- DC: Public Health Services, U.S. Government Print-
ences. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophvs- ing Office.
iology, 35, 627-639. Rechtschaffen, A. (1973). The psychophysiology of mental
Lopes da Silva, F.H., van Rotterdam, A., Barts, P., van activity during sleep. In F.J. McGuigan & R.A.
Heusden, E., & Burr, W. (1976). Models of neuronal Schnoonover (Eds.), The psychology of thinking (pp.
populations: The basic mechanisms of rhythmicity. In 153-200). New York: Academic Press.
M.A. Corner & D.F. Swaab (Eds.), Progress in brain Roffwarg, H.P., Muzio, J., & Dement, W.C. (1966). The
research (Vol. 45, pp. 281-308). Amsterdam: Elsevier. ontogenetic development of the human sleep dream
Moffitt, A., Hoffmann, R., Wells, R., Armitage, R., Pigeau, cycle. Science, 152, 604-618.
R., & Shearer, J. (1982). Individual differences among Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (1983). Using multivar-
pre- and post-awakening EEG correlates of dream re- iate statistics. New York: Harper & Row.
ports following arousals from different stages of sleep. Treisman, A.M. (1969). Strategies and models of selective
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 7, 111- attention. Psychological Review, 76, 282-299.
125. Tyson, P.D., & Gavard, M. (1976). Perceptual compen-
MulhoUand, T. (1968). Feedback electroencephalography. sations for the effects of alcohol. Journal of the Studies
Activitas Nervosa Superior, 10, 410-438. on Alcohol, 37, 1593-1599.
MulhoUand, T. (1973). Objective EEG methods for study- Tyson, P.D., & Audette, R. (1979). A muitivariate ap-
ing covert shifts of visual attention. In F.J. McGuigan proach to the relationship between alpha waves and
& R.A. Schoonover (Eds.), The psychophysiology of experience during feedback. Biofeedback and Self-Reg-
thinking (pp. 109-151). New York: Academic Press. ulation, 4, 63-79.
Ogilvie, R., Hunt, H., Sawicki, C, & McGowan, K. (1978). Tyson, P.D. (1982a). The choice of feedback stimulus can
Searching for lucid dreams [Abstract]. Sleep Research, determine the success of alpha feedback training. Psy-
7, 165. chophysiology, 19, 218-230.
Ogilvie, R.D., Hunt, H.T., Sawicki, C , & Samahalskyi, J. Tyson, P.D. (1982b). A general systems theory approach
(1982a). Psychological correlates of spontaneous mid- to consciousness, attention, and meditation. Psycho-
dle ear "^uscle activity during sleep. Sleep, 5, 11-27, logical Record, 32, 491-500.
Ogilvie, R.D., Hunt, H.T., Tyson, P.D., Lucescu, M.L., Walsh, D.H. (1974). Interactive effects of alpha feedback
& Jenkins, D.B. (1982b). Lucid dreaming and alpha and instructional set on subjective state. Psychophys-
activity: A preliminary report. Perceptual and Motor iology, 11, 428-435.
Skills, 55, 795-808. Weiner, C. (1948). Cybernetics. New York: Wiley.
Ome, M.T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psy- Wertheim, A.H. (1974). Oculomotor control and occipital
chological experiment: With particular reference to de- alpha activity: A review and a hypothesis. Acta Psy-
mand characteristics and their implications. American chologica. 38, 235-256.
Psychologist, 17, 776-783. Williams, H.L., Morlock, H.C., & Morlock, J.V. (1966).
Pavlidis, T. (1973). Biological oscillators: Their mathe- Instrumental behavior during sleep. Psychophysiology,
matical analysis. New York: Academic Press. 2, 208-216.

(Manuscript received October 12, 1983; accepted for publication January 20, 1984)

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen