Sie sind auf Seite 1von 11

FOCUSED REVIEW

published: 04 February 2013


doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00008

Neural mechanisms of attentional control in


mindfulness meditation
Peter Malinowski*
School of Natural Sciences and Psychology, Liverpool John Moores University, Liverpool, UK

The scientific interest in meditation and mindfulness practice has recently seen an
unprecedented surge. After an initial phase of presenting beneficial effects of mindfulness
practice in various domains, research is now seeking to unravel the underlying
psychological and neurophysiological mechanisms. Advances in understanding these
processes are required for improving and fine-tuning mindfulness-based interventions
that target specific conditions such as eating disorders or attention deficit hyperactivity
disorders. This review presents a theoretical framework that emphasizes the central role
of attentional control mechanisms in the development of mindfulness skills. It discusses
the phenomenological level of experience during meditation, the different attentional
functions that are involved, and relates these to the brain networks that subserve these
Edited by: functions. On the basis of currently available empirical evidence specific processes
Amishi P. Jha, University of Miami, USA
as to how attention exerts its positive influence are considered and it is concluded
Reviewed by:
Tonya L. Jacobs, University of California, that meditation practice appears to positively impact attentional functions by improving
Davis, USA resource allocation processes. As a result, attentional resources are allocated more fully
Katherine MacLean, Johns Hopkins
University School of Medicine, USA
during early processing phases which subsequently enhance further processing. Neural
*Correspondence: changes resulting from a pure form of mindfulness practice that is central to most
mindfulness programs are considered from the perspective that they constitute a useful
reference point for future research. Furthermore, possible interrelations between the
improvement of attentional control and emotion regulation skills are discussed.

Keywords: meditation, mindfulness, attentional control, Stroop, attention


Peter Malinowski is Senior Lecturer in
Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience INTRODUCTION that account for specific psychological or
at Liverpool John Moores University and
founding director of their Meditation Recent years have seen a burgeoning interest physiological conditions and cater for individual
and Mindfulness Research Group. He in mindfulness-based approaches, primarily circumstances and predispositions. Several the-
graduated in Psychology at the Technical driven by growing evidence of their benefi- oretical propositions have already been made.
University Braunschweig, Germany, and cial effects on physical and mental well-being. For example, neurobiological processes of want-
completed his postgraduate training in
In parallel to research evaluating the effec- ing and liking may be of great importance when
Cognitive Psychology at the University of
Konstanz. Building on his expertise in tiveness of these approaches, a second line of supporting people with addictions or binge
cognitive neuroscience of selective investigation concentrates on unraveling the eating disorders (Kristeller and Wolever, 2011),
attention, he employs multi-method psychological and neurophysiological processes while the monitoring and self-regulation of cog-
approaches for unraveling the involved. A more precise understanding of nitive and emotional states may be emphasized
psychological and neurophysiological
effects and underlying processes of
these processes will facilitate the refinement of in programs tailored to the needs of individuals
meditation and mindfulness practice. mindfulness-based interventions and will allow with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
p.malinowski@ljmu.ac.uk the development and fine-tuning of programs (Zylowska et al., 2008). Programs addressing

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 1


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

recurrent depression may focus on recogniz- THE LIVERPOOL MINDFULNESS MODEL


ing and stepping out of automatic modes of The Liverpool Mindfulness Model presented
thinking and feeling (Kuyken et al., 2008) in Figure 1 aims to capture and integrate the
and the development of self-determination and core components that are involved in mindful-
resilience has been suggested for the treatment ness practice and to provide a framework for
of severe mental illness (Davis and Kurzban, directing future research (Malinowski, 2012).
2012). Consistent with other conceptualizations of
To consolidate these largely theoretical mindfulness meditation practice, the model
propositions, it will be crucial to advance our gives the development of attentional skills a cen-
understanding of the underlying cognitive, tral role in this process (Wallace and Shapiro,
emotional, and neural processes. The refine- 2006; Lutz et al., 2008; Tang and Posner, 2009;
ment of attention regulation skills features Hlzel et al., 2011; Slagter et al., 2011).
centrally in all conceptualizations of mindful- The model structures the process into five
ness training and recent neurophysiological main tiers: the driving motivational factors
evidence shows that regular, brief engagement (tier 1) determine whether and how an indi-
in a simple mindfulness meditation significantly vidual engages in the mind training (tier 2).
improves attentional control processes (Moore Regular engagement in mindfulness practice
et al., 2012). These results provide important develops and refines the mental core processes
insights into the development of core processes (tier 3), primarily based on the refinement
of mindfulness and establish a useful reference of attentional functions that interact with and
point when investigating the effects of more facilitate regulatory processes of emotions and
elaborate or expanded practices, or when cognitions. Improvements in these core pro-
Mindfulness
considering the interactions between attention cesses result in a changed and more bal-
Within the western psychological
context, mindfulness is usually and emotion regulation skills. anced mental stance or attitude (tier 4), that
described as non-judgmental awareness will result in a positive outcome (tier 5) in
of the present moment and is thought MINDFULNESS terms of physical and mental well-being, and
to entail paying attention with a certain the quality of behavior. Each tier and com-
attitude. It is commonly assumed that
The majority of psychological and neuroscien-
levels of mindfulness can be increased tific studies into mindfulness adopt a defini- ponent of this model, as well as the interac-
through meditation practice. tion put forward by Jon Kabat-Zinn, who was tions and assumed causal relationships between
Meditation pivotal in translating Buddhist approaches of them, warrant further detailed research and
Here describes mental practices carried mind training into the secular context of health render the model a suitable roadmap in this
out repeatedly to achieve specific care programs and psychological interventions endeavor.
positive outcomes. Mindfulness
training and/or Buddhist practices
(e.g., Kabat-Zinn et al., 1985, 1992; Kabat-
usually entail aspects of calming and Zinn, 2011). He describes mindfulness as the MINDFULNESS, MEDITATION, AND ATTENTION
stabilizing the mind by training focused awareness that emerges through paying atten- Training and refining attention skills are cen-
attention and of gaining a refined tion on purpose, in the present moment, and tral to most psychological and Buddhist con-
understanding of ones mental states by non-judgmentally to the unfolding of experi- ceptualizations of mindfulness practices (Lutz
cultivating a non-elaborating, open,
and observing attitude toward all
ence moment by moment(Kabat-Zinn, 2003). et al., 2008) and are the main concern of this
arising mental events. This general understanding is echoed by other review. As outlined in Figure 1, the training of
Focused attention authors who explain mindfulness as being attention skills is thought to underpin emo-
A form of meditation that involves the characterized by dispassionate, non-evaluative, tional and cognitive flexibility, bringing about
practice of sustaining the attentional and sustained moment-to-moment awareness the ability to maintain non-judging awareness of
focus on a chosen object, such as the of perceptible mental states and processes. This ones own thoughts, feelings, and experiences in
sensation of ones breathing, and to
return to the object as soon as mind
includes continuous, immediate awareness of more general terms. This, in turn, will change
wandering is detected. Importantly, the physical sensations, perceptions, affective states, the quality of ones behavior and lead to pos-
meditation object only serves as a thoughts, and imagery (Grossman et al., 2004) itive health outcomes and well-being (Wallace
neutral anchor or reference point or as a receptive attention to and awareness of and Shapiro, 2006; Chiesa and Malinowski,
which is not contemplated or evaluated present events and experience (Brown et al., 2011; Malinowski, 2013). Meditations that calm
during the process.
2007). and stabilize the mind are of central impor-
Open monitoring
While significant differences exist between tance in this process and are prerequisite for
This second form of meditation
practice is firmly rooted in Buddhist Buddhist views of mindfulness and modern a second, more advanced class of meditations
forms of mind training. Building on psychological adaptations, there is broad agree- (Wallace, 1999; Lutz et al., 2008; Malinowski,
attentional stability and clarity ment that a clearly formulated mental train- 2008, 2013). These two forms of training have
achieved with focused attention ing, usually referred to as meditation, is been explained as Focused Attention (FA) and
meditation, the aim here is to maintain
an open, curious non-discriminating
required for developing and improving lev- Open Monitoring (OM) meditation practices
awareness of all arising sensations and els of mindfulness (Chiesa and Malinowski, (Lutz et al., 2008), respectively. Although con-
mental events. 2011). ceptually FA and OM can be separated, even

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 2


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

FIGURE 1 | The Liverpool Mindfulness Model.

simple forms of mindfulness training will entail functions, the alerting, orienting, and executive
both components. Initially a practitioner will control networks, respectively (Posner and
engage more with the FA component to develop Petersen, 1990; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002;
attentional stability, clarity, and awareness of the Fan et al., 2005; Raz and Buhle, 2006; Posner
current mental state. Only then will it be pos- and Rothbart, 2007). Figure 2B provides a
sible to engage in a meaningful way in OM schematic presentation of the brain areas asso-
practice, which entails a moment by moment ciated with these networks. The right frontal
attentiveness to anything that occurs in expe- and right parietal cortex and the thalamus
rience. With increasing experience, OM prac- are involved in alerting functions. The supe-
tice will become less reliant on FA and can rior parietal cortex, temporal parietal junction,
eventually be maintained without focusing on frontal eye fields, and superior colliculus are
any explicit object. These fundamental prin- involved in orienting. The anterior cingulate
ciples are captured by common psychologi- cortex (ACC), lateral ventral cortex, prefrontal
cal definitions of mindfulness that emphasize cortex, and basal ganglia contribute to execu-
the development of attentional abilities com- tive control processes (Fan et al., 2005; Posner
bined with a specific, non-evaluative attitude and Rothbart, 2007). Recent neuroimaging
toward the different mental experiences that evidence further subdivides the function of
may arise (e.g., Bishop et al., 2004; Shapiro the latter network, suggesting that the dor-
et al., 2006; Malinowski, 2008, 2013; Chiesa and sal ACC, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex,
Attentional networks Malinowski, 2011). and the neighboring anterior insula constitute
Neuroscientific studies suggest that Of particular interest to this review are the a salience network. This network is involved
specific attentional functions are attentional processes that constitute the back- in the attentional control function of detect-
carried out by several interconnected
brain networks. The attentional
bone of these practices. Within cognitive neu- ing subjectively relevant or salient events across
functions and related networks go roscience attention is commonly thought of modalities (cognitive, homeostatic, or emo-
under different names, but a in terms of three main functions: (1) the tional) and provides signals to the executive net-
classification into the three networks of modulation of arousal, alertness, and atten- work to act upon in accordance with the current
alerting, orienting, and executive tional engagement, (2) the function of stimulus goal set (Dosenbach et al., 2006, 2007; Seeley
control is common. It is now
understood that under many
selection, and (3) the function of attentional et al., 2007; Sridharan et al., 2008). Finally,
circumstances these networks interact control processes. Three different, though inter- whenever attention involuntarily drifts away
and influence each other. related, attentional networks subserve these from the object during meditation, a further

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 3


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

FIGURE 2 | Effortful attention regulation during meditation. Panel (A) provides a schematic representation of the
meditation process. The inner circle outlines the phenomenological layer, presenting the typical sequence
(clockwise) a meditator will go through. The middle circle relates the attentional processes that lie underneath, while
the outer circle represents the different brain networks that are involved in carrying out these functions. The different
attentional processes and the brain networks are represented as partially overlapping to indicate that in many
instances more than one process/network is involved. Panel (B) outlines the main brain areas involved in each of the
five networks. Anatomical details are discussed in the main text.

network will become involved, the default mode of the distracting train of thought or expe-
network, which entails the posterior cingulate rience by means of attentional disengagement
cortex, the medial prefrontal cortex, the poste- and the involvement of the executive network.
rior lateral parietal/temporal cortices, and the The subsequent return to the meditation object
parahippocampal gyrus (Mason et al., 2007; is achieved by shifting the focus back to the
Buckner et al., 2008; Hasenkamp et al., 2012). object, a function of attention involving the
This network has been shown to be acti- executive and the orienting network. This pro-
vated as soon as participants involuntarily cess can unfold within a few brief moments
engage in task-unrelated cognitions or mind or can extend over longer periods of time.
wandering (e.g., Mason et al., 2007; Buckner With increasing levels of expertise, periods of
et al., 2008; Schooler et al., 2011). Tang et al. sustained focus and attentional stability may
(2012) recently presented a similar view on this become more and more extensive (Wallace,
topic. 2006), whereas for a beginner, even longer peri-
Figure 2A summarizes the assumed process ods of mind wandering may pass unnoticed.
of focused meditation by considering three lay- Although described as separate, these processes
ers: the phenomenological experience of the and brain network activations may indeed over-
meditator, the underlying attentional processes, lap and occur in parallel, expressed in Figure 2A
and the brain networks subserving these pro- by rendering the components of the middle
cesses. On the phenomenological level the and outer circle as partially overlapping. For
meditator will engage with the practice by instance, evidence from functional Magnetic
focusing on the relevant meditation object, for Resonance Imaging (fMRI) indicates sustained
instance, the somatosensory sensation accom- activity in the salience network during medi-
functional Magnetic Resonance panying ones breathing. During this phase of tation (Baron Short et al., 2010). Furthermore,
Imaging sustaining attention, the alerting network will and in line with the process model presented
This method highlights differences in
brain activity by measuring related
be involved. In the moment the mind loses the here, Hasenkamp et al. (2012) used fMRI to
blood oxygenation levels. It yields focus on the object and mind wandering occurs, study brain network activity when meditators
information regarding relative the default mode network will become more shifted between periods of mind wandering and
differences in brain activity when active. Sooner or later the meditator will rec- of sustained focus, concluding that the salience
comparing two or more experimental ognize the mind wandering by means of the network signals the detection of mind wan-
conditions and thus offers useful
insight as to which brain areas are
attention monitoring function and the involve- dering to the executive network. This, in turn,
selectively active during certain mental ment of the salience network. When mind would initiate a re-orienting of attention to the
processes. wandering is detected, the meditator lets go object of meditation.

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 4


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

CONVERGING EVIDENCE: THE ROLE OF blink size (Slagter et al., 2007). Because the P3b
ATTENTION component is considered to index the allocation
Evidence gained with a variety of method- of attentional resources, these results suggest
ological approaches clearly indicates that that the meditation training improved the med-
mindfulness meditation increases the efficiency itators ability to sustain attentional engagement
of attentional functions, reflected in perfor- in a more balanced and continuous fashion.
mance increases as well as changes in neural This was expressed as enhanced allocation of
activity and underlying neural architecture. neural resources (Wickens et al., 1983; Marois
and Ivanoff, 2005), which facilitated the detec-
SUSTAINED ATTENTION tion of the second target. An additional analysis
Robertson et al. (1997) defined sustained atten- of the phase of oscillatory theta activity follow-
tion as the ability to self-sustain mindful, ing successfully detected second targets showed
conscious processing of stimuli whose repet- a reduced variability across trials, a signature
itive, non-arousing qualities would otherwise of more consistent deployment of attention in
lead to habituation and distraction to other meditators (Slagter et al., 2009). Taken together
stimuli (p. 747). This definition captures the these findings indicate improved efficiency in
central features of mindfulness practice and engaging and disengaging from relevant tar-
indicates the relevance of sustained atten- get stimuli (Lutz et al., 2008), i.e., flexibility of
tion in this process. Studying a student sam- allocating attentional resources.
ple without significant meditation experience,
Schmertz et al. (2008) found that higher self- ATTENTIONAL CONTROL
reported mindfulness was related to measures As Figure 2 schematically outlines, sustaining
of more stable attention in the Continuous focused attention over extended periods of time
Performance Test (Conners, 2000), a test fre- requires the interplay of several attentional pro-
quently used for assessing sustained atten- cesses. Of particular importance is the ability
tion. Similarly, Moore and Malinowski (2009) to monitor and regulate ones attentional state
reported a positive correlation between self- orduring task performanceones responses.
reported mindfulness and performance on the The majority of the employed paradigms dis-
d2-test of attention (Brickenkamp and Zilmer, cussed so far were not geared toward separating
1998). Furthermore, mindfulness practitioners out the involvement of the different attentional
performed significantly better on this test than functions or networks. Importantly, most tasks
their matched non-meditating controls (Moore tapping sustained attention will also recruit
and Malinowski, 2009). Similarly, Valentine and attentional control functions, such as the mon-
Sweet (1999) and Pagnoni and Cekic (2007) itoring and updating of information, mental set
reported better performance of meditators in shifting, and the inhibition of proponent, but
sustained attention tasks. In other studies the non-relevant responses (Miyake et al., 2000).
attentional blink paradigm was employed to Similarly, because the mental practice of medi-
investigate how a three-month intensive med- tation requires the monitoring and adjustment
itation retreat improves meditators ability to of ones attentional focus, control processes will
sustain the focus of attention, as compared be crucially involved, at least until a level of
to a non-meditating matched control group expertise is achieved where attentional stabil-
Event-related potential (Slagter et al., 2007, 2009). The attentional blink ity can be maintained with little or no effort,
The electrophysiological neural
task requires participants to attend to a rapidly possibly well beyond 19,000 h of accumulated
response measured on the scalp and
directly related to a specific sensory, changing stream of stimuli (e.g., letters) and to meditation practice (Brefczynski-Lewis et al.,
cognitive, or motor event. Event-related report the identity of two embedded target stim- 2007; Tang et al., 2012). Given the central role of
potentials (ERPs) are analyzed after the uli (e.g., digits) after each trial. Performance to these monitoring and control processes in devel-
time-locked averaging of the neural the second target in the stream typically suffers oping such stability, several studies into atten-
response to several repetitions of the
if it appears within 500 ms after the first target, tional functions and meditation focus on these
same event. Their positive and negative
voltage deflections give an indication of the so-called attentional blink effect (Shapiro processes, frequently by employing the Stroop
specific neural processes related to the et al., 1997). This performance detriment was Word-Color Task (Stroop, 1935)a canoni-
analyzed event. significantly reduced after the meditators had cal measure of response inhibition (Macleod,
Attentional resources completed their meditation retreat. In parallel, 1991; Miyake et al., 2000). The task requires
Neuronal processing capabilities which the amplitude of the P3b event-related poten- participants to rapidly name or indicate the
can be allocated to specific cognitive
tial (ERP) elicited by the first target stimulus, color of the font a word is presented in (see
processes. Attentional resource theories
often include capacity limits, although was decreased in meditators. The participants Figure 3B). The highly automatized function
these do not need to be completely with the greatest decrease of the P3b amplitude of reading leads to performance decrements
fixed. also showed the largest decrease in attentional (slower responses and/or higher error rates) in

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 5


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

FIGURE 3 | Behavioral and ERP results from the Stroop task as a components. The glass brain slices show activation differences between T1
function of meditation practice. (A) Performance differences between and T3 for each group and congruency. Salmon-colored areas indicate a
meditators and non-meditators in a cross-sectional comparison (Moore and decrease in activation and green areas indicate activation increase. On the
Malinowski, 2009). (B) Outline of the Stroop task. (C) and (D) Results from right hand side ERPs (line graphs) and ERP-component amplitudes (bar
a longitudinal study (Moore et al., 2012) showing effects of meditation graphs) are depicted for left and right posterior sites (C) and for posterior
training on ERPs during the Stroop task for the N2 (C) and P3 (D) ERP central sites (D).

the incongruent condition, i.e., when the mean- failed to find improvements in Stroop perfor-
ing of a color word conflicts with its font color mance after an 8-week mindfulness-based stress
(e.g., GREEN presented in red). High profi- reduction (MBSR, Kabat-Zinn, 2003) program.
ciency in this task is thus thought to indicate Because Anderson et al. used an atypical Stroop
good attentional control and relatively low auto- task and the MBSR program consists of a broad
maticity or impulsivity of ones responses. range of components, some of which are not
Employing cross-sectional comparisons, sev- directly related to meditation, drawing general
eral studies reported significantly better per- conclusions is difficult. Nevertheless the study
formance for meditators than non-meditators highlights the need for purer designs that do
on this task (see Figure 3A) and found that not conflate too many components. Employing
task performance was also related to lifetime the Attention Network Test (ANT, Fan et al.,
meditation experience (Chan and Woollacott, 2002) Jha et al. (2007) reported better executive
2007; Teper and Inzlicht, 2013) and lev- control performance of meditators compared to
els of self-reported mindfulness (Moore and controls in line with Tang et al. (2007) who
Malinowski, 2009). Similarly, compared to an found meditation-specific improvements in this
active control condition, significant improve- measure after five 20-minute sessions of mind-
ments in Stroop performance were observed fulness training.
when mindfulness was induced by means of However, to gain an understanding of the
three 20-minute mindfulness sessions, deliv- cognitive and neurophysiological processes that
ered via audio-recording (Wenk-Sormaz, 2005). are reflected in performance changes on atten-
However, a study by Anderson et al. (2007) tional control tasks, it is important to study

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 6


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

the underlying mechanisms in a purer and An alternative and possibly related explana-
more detailed fashion. By conducting an exten- tion concerns the involvement of the ACC. The
sive longitudinal, wait-list controlled study, ACC is known to be the generator of the late
Moore et al. (2012) contributed to this in impor- negative ERP (Liotti et al., 2000; Hanslmayr
tant ways. Participants new to meditation prac- et al., 2008) that usually correlates with Stroop
tice engaged in daily 10-minute sessions of performance but was not differentially influ-
mindful breathing meditation over a period of enced by meditation experience. fMRI evidence
16 weeks and performed the Stroop task before suggests that the ACC is more involved in the
(T1), half-way through (T2), and after com- anticipatory regulation of attention rather than
pletion of the 16-week meditation period (T3). the specific selection of responses itself (Roelofs
ERPs were recorded concurrently to study the et al., 2006; Aarts et al., 2008). It is conceivable
neuronal changes of attentional control pro- that with extended task exposure this anticipa-
cesses. The results showed that meditation prac- tory regulation was perfected in both groups,
tice influenced the neuronal responses to the possibly resulting in the observed ceiling effect.
Stroop stimuli in two important ways. Firstly, A recently published study offers a fur-
it led to a relative increase of lateral posterior ther explanation for the lack of meditation-
N2 amplitudes (160240 ms) over both hemi- specific behavioral effects. Teper and Inzlicht
spheres, irrespective of stimulus congruency (2013) investigated attentional control mecha-
(Figure 3C). These changes in the meditation nisms in the Stroop task by focusing on the
group were primarily driven by increased activ- neural processes involved during the response
ity in the left medial and lateral occipitotem- phase, rather than on the stimulus processing
poral areas for congruent stimuli, which was stage discussed so far. The error-related nega-
contrasted by decreased activity in similar brain tivity (ERN), a neurophysiological response that
areas in the control group. The second dif- occurs within 100 ms after participants com-
ference between meditators and controls was mit an incorrect response, is considered to
observed in the P3 component, peaking between be a useful marker of performance monitor-
310 and 380 ms, primarily for incongruent stim- ing processes (Falkenstein et al., 2000; Yeung
uli. While the participants in the control group et al., 2004) and has also been linked to
exhibited an increase of the P3 amplitude for affect and motivation (Ganushchak and Schiller,
incongruent stimuli, a decrease was observed 2008; Weinberg et al., 2012). The authors
for the meditation group, attributed to reduced found enhanced ERN amplitudes in medi-
activity in lateral occipitotemporal and infe- tators compared to controls after a Stroop
rior temporal regions of the right hemisphere error was committed. Further analysis revealed
(Figure 3D). that meditation experience improved atten-
However, a third finding was at odds with tional control primarily in an indirect way, by
what the majority of studies found before. fostering the acceptance of emotional states, an
Improvements in Stroop performance from T1 aspect of mindful emotion regulation abilities
to T3 were as large in the control group as that was assessed by self-report (Philadelphia
they were in the meditators. Reflecting these Mindfulness Scale, Cardaciotto et al., 2008).
behavioral outcomes, meditators and controls In line with this, comparing participants who
did not differ regarding a typical neural sig- scored high vs. low on emotional acceptance
nature of response conflict, a negative ERP a trend toward enhanced ERN amplitudes for
deflection peaking between 400 and 600 ms high emotional acceptance was found (Teper
post stimulus, which is usually correlated with and Inzlicht, 2013). The finding that perfor-
task performance (Liotti et al., 2000). Thus, mance on executive control tasks was affected by
although clear evidence for better Stroop perfor- emotion regulation abilities might explain why
mance of meditators than non-meditators has cross-sectional studies tend to find performance
been found in cross-sectional comparisons, it differences, whereas the longitudinal study did
did not emerge in the same way in a longi- not. It seems plausible that emotional influ-
tudinal study. A possible explanation might be ences are more prevalent during the first contact
that the repeated administration of the same with the task, whereas the emotional component
task mandated by the longitudinal design lead wears off following repeated exposure within
to a performance ceiling. This suggestion is a longitudinal design and related performance
supported by the fact that performance did differences decrease.
not improve in either group after T2 and If this interpretation holds true, better
that accuracy was above 95% for incongruent Stroop performance in meditators, commonly
trials. attributed to de-automatization, mayat least

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 7


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

partiallybe due to less emotional reactivity by the selective involvement of the salience net-
and may thus reflect improved emotion reg- work. However, it should be noted that the
ulation strategies rather than attentional con- participants in that study progressively engaged
trol processes. This perspective highlights the in four mindfulness practices (from focused
close link between attention regulation and breath awareness, to body-scanning, to com-
emotion regulation skills (also see Figure 1) passion and to open monitoring) that progres-
and raises a question concerning their refine- sively require increasing emotional awareness.
ment: do improvements in emotional regula- Presumably, the most dedicated participants will
tion skills precede those in cognitive processing also have engaged more with those emotional
or vice versa and are executive control pro- awareness practices and would thus exhibit
cesses the basis for improved emotion regula- more emotion related changes. Thus, while the
tion skills? The latter relationship is certainly data of these studies are in line with the assump-
what phenomenological accounts of mindful- tion that with growing expertise the meditator
ness practice would suggest (e.g., Wallace and progresses from attention regulation to emotion
Shapiro, 2006; Lutz et al., 2008) and is in regulation, the results are not yet conclusive and
line with evidence from two recent studies. studies that focus specifically on this question
Sahdra et al. (2011) reported that participation are required.
in a three-month intensive meditation retreat Against the backdrop of these studies, the
concurrently resulted in enhanced response main findings by Moore et al. (2012) are of
inhibition performance and improved socio- high significance as they clearly outline the
emotional functioning as measured by a broadly specific neural processes related to attentional
conceived composite measure of adaptive socio- control processes that result from one simple
emotional functioning (consisting of 14 self- form of mindfulness practice. The enhance-
report measures such as emotion regulation, ment of the N2 component and the associated
depression, anxiety, well-being, ego resilience, increase of activity in left-hemispheric areas of
empathy, etc.). Further analysis revealed that the ventral processing stream (medial and lat-
the socio-emotional functioning was influenced eral occipitotemporal areas) likely reflect more
by enhancement of response inhibition skills, successful or consistent attentional amplifica-
lending support to the hypothesis that atten- tion, specific to the features of the color words
tional control skills may underpin the develop- used in the task, contrasting with decreased
ment of emotion regulation skills. Allen et al. activation due to habituation in the control
(2012) used fMRI to investigate neural changes group. This interpretation seems plausible as
in cognitive and emotional processing resulting these brain areas are typically involved in lex-
from six weeks of meditation training. Using ical tasks (Cohen et al., 2002; Cohen and
an emotional Stroop task which included the Dehaene, 2004; Shaywitz et al., 2004) with a
presentation of affective stimuli with positive similar posterior N2 component (Adorni and
or negative valence, the study found that the Proverbio, 2009) and the time course fits to the
conflict scores only diminished in the med- observed attentional enhancement of color as
itation group but not in the active control compared to form stimuli (Eimer, 1997). The
group. This was accompanied by a meditation- evidence thus shows that engaging in a simple
related increase in activation of the dorso- mindful-breathing practice improves the abil-
lateral prefrontal cortex during the task. As ity to selectively allocate attentional resources to
this area is involved in the executive control task-relevant featuresin this case the color of
network (Raz and Buhle, 2006; Seeley et al., a lexical stimulus.
2007; also see Figure 2) this finding may be The reduction of the P3 component dur-
interpreted as an improvement in attentional ing the processing of incongruent color words,
control. Interestingly, the total time partici- attributed to the decrease of activity in lat-
pants had invested in the meditation practice eral occipitotemporal and inferior temporal
was positively related to increased activity in regions of the right hemisphere, appears to
areas implicated in the salience network, such reflect more efficient attentional resource allo-
as the anterior insula and the cingulate cortex cation during perceptual stimulus discrimina-
(Seeley et al., 2007; Buckner et al., 2008; also tion and inhibition processes that are required
see Figure 2). These findings are in line with for resolving the conflicting stimulus infor-
the hypothesized progression from improve- mation (Polich, 2007). A recent fMRI study
ments of attentional control, indexed by the comparing meditators and matched controls
involvement of the executive control network, on the Stroop task reports reduced activity
to improved emotion regulation skills, indexed in various brain areas subserving attention

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 8


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

(Kozasa et al., 2012), lending further sup- When considering these two findings together,
port to the idea of enhanced neuronal effi- an interesting interpretation emerges: the more
ciency resulting from meditation practice. But successful attentional amplification of the color
more precisely, Moore et al. (2012) show that word stimuli may have influenced the subse-
this more efficient resource allocation relates quent object recognition processes in positive
to those perceptual discrimination processes ways, so that less attentional resources needed to
that require a higher degree of attentional be invested.
control. Specific conclusions can be drawn because
Before concluding that the observed effects the study was confined to one specific, simple
are specific to the meditation practice it is worth meditation practice, rather than the more com-
considering the suggestion that the observed plex or varied forms of meditation that were
changes to stimulus processing occurred as a the focus of the majority of previous longi-
by-product of the meditation process. As med- tudinal studies into meditation. It seems that
itators tend to practice either with closed or mindfully focusing on the somatosensory expe-
half-open eyes the resulting reduction of sen- riences of breathing leads to specific improve-
sory load may have increased the excitability ment to core processes of attentional control
of the visual cortex, a phenomenon that has that are considered to be central to all forms
been observed also after shorter periods of sen- of mindfulness practice. As this form of prac-
sory deprivation (Suedfeld, 1975; Boroojerdi tice is the starting point for the majority of
et al., 2000; Pitskel et al., 2007). In turn, mindfulness meditation programs the findings
the reduced threshold for sensory stimuli may are an important reference point for future
have enhanced the related ERP components. research that aims to investigate more complex,
While such an effect of meditation practice advanced, or prolonged and extended mindful-
needs to be considered it seems unlikely that ness programs.
it plays an important role here. The fact that The reported improvements seem to gener-
the laboratory-based Stroop task was com- alize from the specific situation of a meditation
pleted independently of meditation practice, exercise (i.e., focusing on breathing related sen-
that the participants only practiced for about sations and maintaining a non-responsive atti-
10 min/day, and that no differences between tude to all arising experiences) to a different
meditators and controls were observed in the sensory modality, in this case vision. Thus, a
early visual ERP components P1 or N1, speak growing body of evidence provides strong sup-
against such interpretation. Nevertheless, it will port for the idea that improvements in atten-
be useful to consider (and control) such influ- tional core processes of selection and control,
ences in future research, in particular when and their related beneficial effects, may prop-
studying the influence of more prolonged med- agate into modalities different from the medi-
itation regimes, or when recording the ERPs tation practice itself, exerting positive effects in
soon after (or while) participants engaged in various situations, and for various conditions.
formal practice. Although evidence regarding the role of
attention is mounting, these are still early days
CONCLUSION and there are certainly more questions unan-
Longitudinal studies indicate that meditation swered than answered. Due to a paucity of
practice results in significant changes to earlier well controlled longitudinal studies, much of
stimulus processing in terms of enhanced/more the available evidence is gained from cross-
consistent, dynamic, and flexible attentional sectional comparisons, which are of limited
functions. Improvements in attentional selec- use in unraveling the causal contribution of
tion and control appear to be primarily medi- mind training to improvements in attentional
ated by more flexible attentional resource functions. Therefore, more longitudinal studies
allocation that modulates early stimulus pro- that focus on specific psychological and neu-
cessing, possibly in a modality independent ronal mechanisms are required. Furthermore,
fashion. Rather than enhancing response inhi- such studies will need to consider the interplay
bition processes per se, the study by Moore et al. of emotional and attentional factors in more
(2012) revealed meditation-related improve- detail to determine whether emotional flexibil-
ments to earlier stages of stimulus processing ity improves attentional functions or vice versa.
in terms of more focused attentional resources The fact that a simple form of mind
(indexed by the enhanced N2) and more effi- training exerts a clear influence on modality-
cient perceptual discrimination and conflict res- independent attentional processes may indicate
olution processes (indexed by the reduced P3). why mindfulness-based interventions prove to

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 9


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

be beneficial in various situations. It appears ACKNOWLEDGMENTS


that by refining the process of relating to expe- The financial support by the BIAL Foundation
riences, rather than engaging with the content (Research Bursary No. 30/08) for some
of experience, generic skills that can be applied of the data presented here is gratefully
across domains and modalities are enhanced. acknowledged.

REFERENCES theoretical foundations and K. K., Kang, H. C., et al. (2006). Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7,
Aarts, E., Roelofs, A., and Van evidence for its salutary effects. A core system for the implemen- 109119.
Turennout, M. (2008). Anticipatory Psychol. Inq. 18, 211237. tation of task sets. Neuron 50, Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-
activity in anterior cingulate cortex Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., 799812. based interventions in context: past,
can be independent of conflict and and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Eimer, M. (1997). An event-related present, and future. Clin. Psychol.
error likelihood. J. Neurosci. 28, brains default network: anatomy, potential (ERP) study of transient Sci. Pract. 10, 144156.
46714678. function, and relevance to disease. and sustained visual attention to Kabat-Zinn, J. (2011). Some reflections
Adorni, R., and Proverbio, A. M. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 138. color and form. Biol. Psychol. 44, on the origins of MBSR, skillful
(2009). New insights into name Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, 143160. means, and the trouble with maps.
category-related effects: is the age E. M., Moitra, E., and Farrow, Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, Contemp. Buddh. Interdiscipl. J. 12,
of acquisition a possible factor? V. (2008). The assessment of S., and Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP 281306.
Behav. Brain Funct. 5:33. doi: present-moment awareness and components on reaction errors and Kabat-Zinn, J., Lipworth, L., and
10.1186/1744-9081-5-33 acceptance: the Philadelphia their functional significance: a tuto- Burney, R. (1985). The clinical use
Allen, M., Dietz, M., Blair, K. S., Van Mindfulness Scale. Assessment 15, rial. Biol. Psychol. 51, 87107. of mindfulness meditation for the
Beek, M., Rees, G., Vestergaard- 204223. Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Fossella, J., self-regulation of chronic pain.
Poulsen, P., et al. (2012). Chan, D., and Woollacott, M. (2007). Flombaum, J. I., and Posner, M. J. Behav. Med. 8, 163190.
Cognitive-affective neural plasticity Effects of level of meditation I. (2005). The activation of atten- Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A. O.,
following active-controlled mind- experience on attentional focus: tional networks. Neuroimage 26, Kristeller, J. L., Peterson, L.
fulness intervention. J. Neurosci. 32, is the efficiency of executive or 471479. G., Fletcher, K. E., Pbert, L.,
1560115610. orientation networks improved? Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., et al. (1992). Effectiveness of a
Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, J. Altern. Complement. Med. 13, Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). meditation-based stress reduction
Z. V., and Bishop, S. R. (2007). 651657. Testing the efficiency and inde- program in the treatment of anxiety
Mindfulness-based stress reduc- Chiesa, A., and Malinowski, P. (2011). pendence of attentional networks. disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 149,
tion and attentional control. Mindfulness based interventions: J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340347. 936943.
Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 14, are they all the same? J. Clin. Psychol. Ganushchak, L. Y., and Schiller, Kozasa, E. H., Sato, J. R., Lacerda,
449463. 67, 404424. N. O. (2008). Motivation and S. S., Barreiros, M. A., Radvany,
Baron Short, E., Kose, S., Mu, Q., Cohen, L., and Dehaene, S. (2004). semantic context affect brain J., Russell, T. A., et al. (2012).
Borckardt, J., Newberg, A., George, Specialization within the ventral error-monitoring activity: an Meditation training increases brain
M. S., et al. (2010). Regional stream: the case for the visual event-related brain potentials study. efficiency in an attention task.
brain activation during meditation word form area. Neuroimage 22, Neuroimage 39, 395405. Neuroimage 59, 745749.
shows time and practice effects: 466476. Grossman, P., Niemann, L., Schmidt, Kristeller, J. L., and Wolever, R. Q.
an exploratory FMRI study. Evid. Cohen, L., Lehericy, S., Chochon, F., S., and Walach, H. (2004). (2011). Mindfulness-based eating
Based Complement. Alternat. Med. Lemer, C., Rivaud, S., and Dehaene, Mindfulness-based stress reduction awareness training for treating
7, 121127. S. (2002). Language-specific tuning and health benefits A meta-analysis. binge eating disorder: the concep-
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M. A., Shapiro, of visual cortex? Functional prop- J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 3543. tual foundation. Eat. Disord. 19,
S. L., Carlson, L. E., Anderson, erties of the visual word form area. Hanslmayr, S., Pastotter, B., Bauml, 4961.
N. D., Carmody, J., et al. Brain 125, 10541069. K. H., Gruber, S., Wimber, M., Kuyken, W., Byford, S., Taylor, R. S.,
(2004). Mindfulness: a proposed Conners, C. K. (2000). Continuous and Klimesch, W. (2008). The Watkins, E., Holden, E., White,
operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Performance Test II: Technical Guide electrophysiological dynamics of K., et al. (2008). Mindfulness-
Sci. Pract. 11, 230242. and Software Manual. Toronto, ON: interference during the Stroop task. based cognitive therapy to prevent
Boroojerdi, B., Bushara, K. O., Corwell, Multi-Health Systems. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 215225. relapse in recurrent depression.
B., Immisch, I., Battaglia, F., Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 76,
Muellbacher, W., et al. (2000). (2002). Control of goal-directed C. D., Duncan, E., and Barsalou, 966978.
Enhanced excitability of the human and stimulus-driven attention in L. W. (2012). Mind wandering Liotti, M., Woldorff, M. G., Perez,
visual cortex induced by short-term the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, and attention during focused R., and Mayberg, H. S. (2000).
light deprivation. Cereb. Cortex 10, 201215. meditation: a fine-grained tem- An ERP study of the temporal
529534. Davis, L., and Kurzban, S. (2012). poral analysis of fluctuating course of the Stroop color-word
Brefczynski-Lewis, J. A., Lutz, A., Mindfulness-based treatment for cognitive states. Neuroimage 59, interference effect. Neuropsychologia
Schaefer, H. S., Levinson, D. B., people with severe mental illness: a 750760. 38, 701711.
and Davidson, R. J. (2007). Neural literature review. Am. J. Psychiatr. Hlzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J.
correlates of attentional expertise in Rehabil. 15, 202232. Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., D., and Davidson, R. J. (2008).
long-term meditation practitioners. Dosenbach, N. U., Fair, D. A., Miezin, and Ott, U. (2011). How does Attention regulation and monitor-
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, F. M., Cohen, A. L., Wenger, K. mindfulness meditation work? ing in meditation. Trends Cogn. Sci.
1148311488. K., Dosenbach, R. A., et al. (2007). Proposing mechanisms of action 12, 163169.
Brickenkamp, R., and Zilmer, E. Distinct brain networks for adaptive from a conceptual and neural Macleod, C. M. (1991). Half a century
(1998). D2 Test of Attention: and stable task control in humans. perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, of research on the Stroop effect: an
Manual. Oxford: Hogrefe and Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 537559. integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109,
Huber. 1107311078. Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., and Baime, 163203.
Brown, K. W., Ryan, R. M., and Dosenbach, N. U., Visscher, K. M., M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training Malinowski, P. (2008). Mindfulness as
Creswell, J. D. (2007). Mindfulness: Palmer, E. D., Miezin, F. M., Wenger, modifies subsystems of attention. psychological dimension: concepts

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 10


Malinowski Attentional control in meditation

and applications. Irish J. Psychol. 29, human brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. Slagter, H. A., Davidson, R. J., and for refining and examining con-
155166. 13, 2542. Lutz, A. (2011). Mental training as sciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 6,
Malinowski, P. (2012). Wirkme- Posner, M. I., and Rothbart, M. K. a tool in the neuroscientific study 175187.
chanismen der Achtsamkeitspraxis. (2007). Research on attention net- of brain and cognitive plasticity. Wallace, B. A. (2006). The Attention
Wie frdert Meditation positive works as a model for the integration Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:17. doi: Revolution: Unlocking the Power of
psychologische Vernderungen? of psychological science. Annu. Rev. 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00017 the Focused Mind. Somerville, MA:
[Mechanisms of mindfulness. How Psychol. 58, 123. Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. Wisdom Books.
meditation fosters positive psycho- Raz, A., and Buhle, J. (2006). L., Francis, A. D., Nieuwenhuis, Wallace, B. A., and Shapiro, S. (2006).
logical change], in Achtsamkeit: Typologies of attentional networks. S., Davis, J. M., et al. (2007). Mental balance and well-being:
Ein buddhistisches Konzept erobert Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 367379. Mental training affects distribu- building bridges between Buddhism
die Wissenschaft [Mindfulness: A Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, tion of limited brain resources. and Western Psychology. Am.
Buddhist concept conquers science], J., Baddeley, B. T., and Yiend, J. PLoS Biol. 5:e138. doi: 10.1371/jour- Psychol. 61, 690701.
eds M. Zimmermann, C. Spitz, and (1997). Oops!: performance corre- nal.pbio.0050138 Weinberg, A., Riesel, A., and Hajcak,
S. Schmidt (Berlin: Hans Huber), lates of everyday attentional failures Slagter, H. A., Lutz, A., Greischar, L. G. (2012). Integrating multi-
91100. in traumatic brain injured and nor- L., Nieuwenhuis, S., and Davidson, ple perspectives on error-related
Malinowski, P. (2013). Flourishing mal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35, R. J. (2009). Theta phase syn- brain activity: the ERN as a
through meditation and mind- 747758. chrony and conscious target per- neural indicator of trait defen-
fulness, in Oxford Handbook of Roelofs, A., Van Turennout, M., and ception: impact of intensive men- sive reactivity. Motiv. Emot. 36,
Happiness, eds S. David, I. Boniwell, Coles, M. G. (2006). Anterior cingu- tal training. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 84100.
and A. Conley Ayers (Oxford: late cortex activity can be indepen- 15361549. Wenk-Sormaz, H. (2005). Meditation
Oxford University Press), 384396. dent of response conflict in Stroop- Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., and can reduce habitual responding.
Marois, R., and Ivanoff, J. (2005). like tasks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for Altern. Ther. Health Med. 11, 4258.
Capacity limits of information pro- U.S.A. 103, 1388413889. the right fronto-insular cortex in Wickens, C., Kramer, A., Vanasse,
cessing in the brain. Trends Cogn. Sahdra, B. K., Maclean, K. A., Ferrer, switching between central-executive L., and Donchin, E. (1983).
Sci. 9, 296305. E., Shaver, P. R., Rosenberg, E. and default-mode networks. Performance of concurrent tasks:
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van L., Jacobs, T. L., et al. (2011). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, a psychophysiological analysis of
Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, Enhanced response inhibition dur- 1256912574. the reciprocity of information-
S. T., and Macrae, C. N. (2007). ing intensive meditation training Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interfer- processing resources. Science 221,
Wandering minds: the default predicts improvements in self- ence in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. 10801082.
network and stimulus-independent reported adaptive socioemotional Psychol. 18, 643661. Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., and
thought. Science 315, 393395. functioning. Emotion 11, 299312. Suedfeld, P. (1975). Society The Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, Schmertz, S. K., Anderson, P. L., and Benefits of Boredom: sensory basis of error detection: conflict
M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, Robins, D. L. (2008). The rela- Deprivation Reconsidered: the monitoring and the Error-Related
A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The tion between self-report mindful- effects of a monotonousenviron- Negativity. Psychol. Rev. 111,
unity and diversity of executive ness and performance on tasks of ment are not always negative; 931959.
functions and their contributions sustained attention. J. Psychopathol. sometimes sensory deprivation has Zylowska, L., Ackerman, D. L., Yang,
to complex Frontal Lobe tasks: Behav. Assess. 31, 6066. high utility. Am. Sci. 63, 6069. M. H., Futrell, J. L., Horton,
a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Schooler, J. W., Smallwood, J., Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., N. L., Hale, T. S., et al. (2008).
Psychol. 41, 49100. Christoff, K., Handy, T. C., Reichle, Feng, S., Lu, Q., et al. (2007). Short- Mindfulness meditation training in
Moore, A., Gruber, T., Derose, J., and E. D., and Sayette, M. A. (2011). term meditation training improves adults and adolescents with ADHD:
Malinowski, P. (2012). Regular, Meta-awareness, perceptual decou- attention and self-regulation. a feasibility study. J. Atten. Disord.
brief mindfulness meditation prac- pling and the wandering mind. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 11, 737746.
tice improves electrophysiological Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 319326. 1715217156.
markers of attentional control. Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, Tang, Y. Y., and Posner, M. I. (2009).
Conflict of Interest Statement: The
Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:18. doi: A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Attention training and attention
author declares that the research
10.3389/fnhum.2012.00018 Kenna, H., et al. (2007). Dissociable state training. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13,
was conducted in the absence of any
Moore, A., and Malinowski, P. (2009). intrinsic connectivity networks 222227.
commercial or financial relationships
Meditation, mindfulness and cogni- for salience processing and exec- Tang, Y. Y., Rothbart, M. K., and
that could be construed as a potential
tive flexibility. Conscious. Cogn. 18, utive control. J. Neurosci. 27, Posner, M. I. (2012). Neural cor-
conflict of interest.
176186. 23492356. relates of establishing, maintaining,
Pagnoni, G., and Cekic, M. (2007). Shapiro, K. L., Arnell, K. M., and and switching brain states. Trends
Age effects on gray matter vol- Raymond, J. E. (1997). The atten- Cogn. Sci. 16, 330337. Received: 05 October 2012; accepted:
ume and attentional performance tional blink. Trends Cogn. Sci. 1, Teper, R., and Inzlicht, M. (2013). 11 January 2013; published online: 04
in Zen meditation. Neurobiol. Aging 219296. Meditation, mindfulness and execu- February 2013.
28, 16231627. Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, tive control: the importance of emo- Citation: Malinowski P (2013) Neural
Pitskel, N. B., Merabet, L. B., Ramos- J. A., and Freedman, B. (2006). tional acceptance and brain-based mechanisms of attentional control in
Estebanez, C., Kauffman, T., Mechanisms of mindfulness. J. Clin. performance monitoring. Soc. Cogn. mindfulness meditation. Front. Neurosci.
and Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Psychol. 62, 373386. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 8592. 7:8. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00008
Time-dependent changes in cor- Shaywitz, B. A., Shaywitz, S. E., Valentine, E. R., and Sweet, P. L. G. Copyright 2013 Malinowski. This is
tical excitability after prolonged Blachman, B. A., Pugh, K. R., (1999). Meditation and attention: an open-access article distributed under
visual deprivation. Neuroreport 18, Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., a comparison of the effects of the terms of the Creative Commons
17031707. et al. (2004). Development of concentrative and mindfulness Attribution License, which permits use,
Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300: an left occipitotemporal systems meditation on sustained atten- distribution and reproduction in other
integrative theory of P3a and P3b. for skilled reading in children tion. Ment. Health Relig. Cult. 2, forums, provided the original authors
Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 21282148. after a phonologically- based 5970. and source are credited and subject to any
Posner, M. I., and Petersen, S. E. intervention. Biol. Psychiatry 55, Wallace, B. A. (1999). The Buddhist copyright notices concerning any third-
(1990). The attention system of the 926933. tradition of samatha: methods party graphics etc.

Frontiers in Neuroscience www.frontiersin.org February 2013 | Volume 7 | Article 8 | 11

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen