Sie sind auf Seite 1von 3

Infinitives

Infinitives: Raising, control and ECM (6) a. IP


(Handout 6, MA Seminar English Syntax) I
Andrew McIntyre VP
IP
1. Introduction
I
Recall (Handout 2, section 2.4): infinitive particle to instantiates the category I (not P). DP I V DP I VP
Here we look at various types of infinitives, noting that all of them involved a subject in
spec,IP, but this subject is not usually pronounced. Schema: Johni seems ti to ti know the answer
(1) ....[IP (silent subject) [I' to [VP ... ]]]
Mostly this type of IP appears as complement to a verb. b. IP
I
2. Infinitives with raising verbs VP
CP
(2) a. JOHNi seemed/appeared [IP ti to know the answer]
IP
b. JOHNi turned out [IP ti to have the right qualities]
DP I V C
c. JOHNi happened [IP ti to see the article]
The underlined verbs in (2) are called raising verbs, since the DP that initially seems to be It seems (that) John knows the answer
their subject, John, has been raised out of the bracketed IP, as the traces indicate. We will
To understand this, recall the following observations:
see that raising verbs dont take DP arguments, rather they only take a single IP argument.
(7) a. The infinitive particle to is an instance of Infl (handout 2)
Thus, in (2) John is an argument of the verbs in the bracketed IPs, not of the raising verbs.
b. Subject requirement: In English, the specifier of IP must be filled. (handout 3)
In the raising structure in (6)a), John starts in the specifier of the smallest VP (since John
2.1. Evidence that raising verbs dont take DP arguments
is an argument of know), moves to the spec. of the innner IP headed by to and then to the
The sentences in (2) have close paraphrases in the non-raising constructions in (3), where spec. of the outer IP. Both cases of movement to spec,IP are due to (7)b).
John stays in the lower clause and is clearly not an argument of the underlined verbs. (In In (6)b), the requirement in (7)b) is fulfilled by the expletive it. (We saw a similar use of
(3), it is an expletive subject like there mentioned in handout 3.) expletive there in structures like there is someone waiting in handout 3.)
(3) a. It appeared/seemed [that JOHN knew the answer] The infinitive particle to differs from other instances of Infl in not being able to assign
b. It turned out [that JOHN had the right qualities] case to its specifier. This means that the DP must move to spec,IP in the main clause to
c. It happened [that JOHN saw the article] receive case and rules out a structure like:
In constructions like (2), the subject DPs are always interpreted as arguments of the verbs (8) *It seems [IP John to know that]
in the bracketed IPs. (E.g. John in (2)a) is an experiencer, which is the thematic role that Raising verbs are like unaccusative and passive verbs in that the DP appearing as their
know assigns to its subject.) subject really starts as (part of) their complement. Passive, unaccusative and raising
Recall idioms consisting of verb, subject and object (handout 3). The fixed subject can structures all involve A-movement (movement of a DP to an A(rgument) Position, namely
become subject in a raising construction, and it is still interpreted as part of an idiom: spec, IP, motivated by the need for the DP to receive case and the need for IP to have a
(4) a. All hell seemed to break lose. specifier.
b. The chickens appeared to be coming home to roost.
c. The cat turned out to be out of the bag already. 2.3. Other instances of raising
There are certain (parts of) DPs that normally only occur as part of particular idioms (take
Some verbs allow semantically near-identical raising and non-raising uses (cf. (2)/(3)), but
umbrage at, raise Xs hackles, read the riot act to X). Sometimes these idioms can be
other raising verbs dont have structures parallel to (3).
passivised (umbrage was taken at, etc.). If we combine these passives with a raising
(9) a. There needed/had/continued/were supposed to be inquiries.
verb, the special idiomatic DP can quite happily appear as subject of the raising structure,
b. All hell started/is going/tends to break lose here.
as in (5). It would be implausible to treat these DPs as arguments of seem, since no other
c. John needed/had/continued/was supposed to know the answers.
verbs apart from the verbs in the subordinate clauses can take these DPs as arguments.
d. It needed/had/continued/was supposed that John knows the answers.
(5) a. Considerable umbrage seems to have been taken at this.
b. Everyones hackles seemed to have been raised by this. Given that subjects of sentences always come from inside the VP (handout 3), it appears
c. The riot act doesnt seem to have been read to those creeps. that all modal verbs are raising verbs, since the subject is not an argument of the auxiliary,
but is moved passed the auxiliary from somewhere inside the auxiliarys complement.
2.2. An analysis for raising verbs This should not shock us, since semantically modals are very close to certain types of
adverbs/adjectives which clearly dont have DP arguments: I must go means it is
The syntax of raising constructions, compared with expletive constructions of the type in (3): necessary that I go and he may have gone means he has possibly gone.
There are also raising adjectives (which take an IP complement that a DP moves out of):
(10) Shei is [AP certain/likely [IP ti to go]. [cf. It is likely that she will go]

2
Infinitives Infinitives

3. Control: Infinitives with a silent pronoun subject (PRO) Unlike raising verbs, which allow any kind of subject that the verb in the IP
complement allows, control verbs only allow DP arguments with which they are
With some infinitives we have grounds for assuming that the specifier of to is occupied by a
semantically compatible. Thus, control verbs fail tests like (4) and (5): We dont find
silent pronoun called PRO which is interpreted as being identical to (technical term: is
parts of idioms functioning as controllers of PRO because outside the idiom they either
controlled by) an argument of the verb in the main clause:
make no sense or do not have an idiomatic interpretation:
(11) Subject control:
(15) a. *Considerable umbragei tried PROi to be taken at his behaviour.
a. IP
b. *The rioti act managed PROi to be read to those losers.
I
c. *I asked the riot acti PROi to be read to them.
VP
If the inner IP has an expletive subject, it can be moved into the main clause with
V
raising verbs, but not with control verbs, since expletives dont have the right semantics
IP
to appear as arguments of control verbs:
I
(16) a. There is someone waiting. (expletive there)
VP
b. Raising: Therei seems [ti to be someone waiting].
DPi I DP V DP I
c. Subject control: *Therei managed [PROi to be someone waiting].
Elvis ti decided PROi to tPRO leave the building
d. Object control: *I persuaded therei [PROi to be someone waiting].
(17) a. It is raining . (expletive it)
b. Paraphrase of (a): Elvis has decided that he should leave the building
b. Raising: Iti seemed [ti to be raining].
c. Other such verbs: try, hope, manage, opt, aspire, fail, need, pretend
c. Subject control: *Iti managed [PROi to be raining].
(12) Object control:
d. Object control: *I persuaded iti [PROi to be raining].
a. IP
I
3.2. Why linguists believe that PRO exists
VP
V Parallels like (18) show that subordinate clauses in control sentences are interpreted as if
IP they have an understood/implicit subject. By assuming PRO, we are maximising the
I parallels between syntax and semantics.
VP (18) a. I promised PRO to leave = I promised that I would leave
DPk I DP V DP DP I b. I am sorry PRO to have put you out = I am sorry that I have put you out
They tk advised Elvisi PROi to tPRO leave the building Other evidence comes from binding (=the area of grammar dealing with reflexive
pronouns). Reflexives require an antecedent in the same IP, cf. (19)a). If PRO exists, we
b. Interpretation of (a): They advised Elvis that he should leave the building can explain why it is possible to have reflexives in an IP where there is no other antecedent
c. Other such verbs: tell, tempt, force, persuade, order, ask, instruct in the clause, cf. (19)b-d).
We call decide a subject control verb since its subject controls (i.e. provides the (19) a. We want [IP Elvisi to trust himselfi/*ourselves]
interpretation for) PRO. Advise is an object control verb, because its object controls PRO. b. Theyi promised him [IPPROi to trust themselvesi/*himself]
c. Wei want [IP PROi to help ourselvesi/*himself/*yourselves]
3.1. How control and raising verbs differ d. They asked himi [IPPROi to trust himselfi/*themselves]
Both types of verbs have in common that the main clause contains a DP which is In (20), the reflexives act as if they are bound to a generic pronoun (one). If we assume a
interpreted as an argument of a verb in the subordinate IP, e.g. Mary is interpreted as the PRO here, this receives an explanation.1
agent of solve in the following sentences: (20) [PRO to love yourself/oneself/*herself/*themselves/*yourselves/*ourselves] is hard
(13) a. Raising: Maryi seemed [IP ti to solve the problem].
3.3. Miscellaneous comments on PRO
b. Subj. control: Maryi tried [IP PROi to solve the problem].
c. Obj. control: I asked Maryi [IP PROi to solve the problem]. Normally, if a control verb has an object, the object will control PRO. This suggests that
The difference is that, while raising verbs dont take DP arguments, control verbs clearly PRO is controlled by the nearest DP in the main clause. An exceptional case is promise:
do. How we know this: Hei promised them PROi to leave early. Sentences of this type are indeed exceptional:
Very often, the fact that the individual expressed by the DP in question has two They arent possible to all speakers and cause problems for children learning languages.
thematic roles can be detected intuitively: PRO doesnt need case, hence it can occupy the specifier of an IP headed by to.
(14) a. Mary hoped PRO to study: Mary is both experiencer (of hope) and agent (of study)
b. I persuaded Mary PRO to leave: She is both persuaded and the one who will leave.
The same point can be made using paraphrases like those in (11)b)/(12)b): Here we see
that Elvis is an argument of decide/advise, but there is also a pronoun referring to Elvis
1
which is argument of leave. In (11)a)/(12)a), PRO functions like he in (11)b)/(12)b). Here PRO has an arbitrary interpretation, i.e. is not controlled. More examples: PRO to err
is human; Thats hard PRO to see; I dont know whether PRO to go; PRO surfing is fun.

3 4
Infinitives Infinitives

4. Exceptional Case Marking (ECM) (26) [CP [C For] [IP him to go there] would be silly
An indication that for assigns case here:
The third type of infinitive is one with exceptional case marking (ECM):
(27) For them to go to the slaughterhouse would be inadvisable.
(21) IP
PRO to go to the slaughterhouse would be inadvisable.
I'
*Them to go to the slaughterhouse would be inadvisable.
VP
V
IP A. Decide whether the following sentences involve raising, control or ECM. Some verbs
can be used in more than one way.
I' a. want: 1. I want it to rain. 2. I want to go. 3. *There wants to someone smoking here.
VP b. need: 1. I need it to rain. 2. I need to go. 3. It needs to rain heavily.
DP I DP V DP I c. cease: There ceased to be people interested in the job.
shek tk believed himi to ti have dishonest motives d. agree: He agreed to stop ruining everyones life.
B. Draw trees for the following.
Here him in the lower spec,IP will be shown not to be an argument of the verb believe,
1. The problem seems to have been fogotten. 2. What did he appear to forget?
although it receives case from it. (such phrases are exceptionally case marked DPs).
3. She knew him to have been arrested by that time.
Other ECM verbs: expect, know, prove, assume, believe, need, want, think, show, consider.

4.1. How ECM verbs differ from (object) control verbs


Exceptionally case marked DPs dont receive thematic roles from the verb in the main
clause (i.e. arent arguments of it). How we know this:
In these constructions, the interpretation doesnt correspond to that found in cases where
the verb selects a DP without an infinitive (if it can take a DP at all):
(22) a. I believe/found him to be a liar I believe/found him
b. I need the psycho to be locked up I need the psycho
c. I proved the theory to wrong I proved the theory
d. I expect him not to turn up I expect him
e. I assume/think her to be a genius *I assume/think her
In (23) we see that parts of idioms can be exceptionally case marked DPs. We saw from
(15) that unacceptability results when we try to put part of an idiom in a position where
it has to be treated as an argument of two verbs at once, and in (23) this problem is not
found, so we conclude that the ECM DP is only an argument of the verb in the inner IP.
(23) a. I considered the chickens to have come home to roost.
b. I need all hell to have broken loose by then.
c. I expect the riot act to have been read to those louts by tomorrow.
Same deal for expletives: If ECM verbs assigned a thematic role to their objects, we
would expect them to be incompatible with expletives (just like object control verbs, cf.
(16), (17)), but what we find is that they are compatible with them:
(24) a. I expect there to be someone waiting for him.
b. She knew there to be people controlling the situation.

4.2. What is meant by exceptional case marking


The DP in the lower spec,IP receives accusative, not nominative:
(25) I expect {her/*she} to win the race.
Recall that infinitive particle to doesnt assign case to its specifier.
This suggests that the case of the objects in ECM constructions must come from the verb.
This is exceptional because the verb assigns case to the specifier of its complement, in
contrast where to the more normal situation where it assigns case to its complement (not to
a constituent within it).
Another instance of ECM is the complementiser for (discussed in handout 2):

5 6

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen