Sie sind auf Seite 1von 26

European Journal of Psychology of Education

2006, Vol. XXI, n 2, 183-208


2006, I.S.P.A.

Bullying among students and its consequences on


health
Barbara Houbre
Cyril Tarquinio
Isabelle Thuillier
University of Metz, France

Emmanuelle Hergott
CEFP, France

Violence among students at school is an ever-growing problem.


Bullying can be defined as all forms of repeated physical or mental
violence performed by an individual on another person who is not
capable of defending him/herself (Roland & Idsoe, 2001). The three
studies conducted here reveal some of the characteristics and
implications of this type of aggression. Whether the attacker(s) or the
attacked, all protagonists in a bullying episode suffer the consequences
of this behavior. Study 1 showed that students who were both victims
and bullies had the lowest self-concepts in all areas studied. Victims
exhibited inferior self-concepts to bullies, who in turn obtained lower
scores than students not involved in bullying at all. Study 2 showed, as
expected, that the group of bully/victims reported more psychosomatic
problems than all other groups. In addition, there was a positive link
between behavioral problems and the onset of psychosomatic disorders.
Study 3, which was mainly exploratory, looked at the traumatic impact
of bullying and the emergence of addictive behavior. Children who had
vivid memories of being the victim of an aggressive act manifested a
high level of post-traumatic stress, although no link was observed
between post-traumatic stress and the type of aggression (physical,
verbal, or relational). A dependency relationship was found between
post-traumatic stress and substance use. The results of these studies
suggest that the many complexities of the different protagonists of
bullying should be taken into account in view of developing servicing
that is geared to each individual.

Within the past few decades, aggressive behavior among students has been an important
issue for both researchers and policy makers. The findings of studies on this subject appear to

The authors would like to thank the Moselle Board of Education and the various school principals, teachers,
students, and all other individuals, close and far, who made this study possible.
184 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

be the same in Europe and in the United States: serious bullying affects about 5% of all
students, less serious bullying, between 15% and 30% (Baldry, 1998; OMoore, 1989; Pepler,
Craig, Ziegler, & Charach, 1993; Slee & Rigby, 1993; Withney & Smith, 1993). In parallel
with prevention programs, an abundant body of literature has been developing on this issue.
According to Olweus (1989), a student is being bullied or victimized when he or she is
exposed, repeatedly and over time, to negative actions on the part of one or more students.
Bullying has three specific characteristics: frequency, the intention to hurt, and an asymmetric
relationship between the bully and the victim. This kind of aggression can be direct or indirect;
it can be expressed in words (threats, mocking, teasing, name calling), via physical contact
(hitting, shoving, kicking, pinching, holding someone back), or by way of social relations
(ostracizing, manipulating friendships) (Berkowitz, 1993; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Olweus,
1984; Smith & Sharp, 1994). In addition to these distinctions, Austin and Joseph (1996)
classified students according to their roles in bullying. Four types were defined: victims,
bullies, bully/victims (who are both the attackers and the attacked), and not involved.
Naturally, these aggressive acts have an impact on the students who are involved. We
will address this impact in terms of various psychological characteristics. Whether the effects
show up as an altered self-concept, health problems (psychosomatic symptoms and addictive
behavior), or psychologically traumatic consequences, we will try to determine the extent to
which bullying is an explanatory factor in their emergence.
Concerning the self-concept, many authors have raised the question of how identity
might be related to a childs status, itself defined by his/her role in the bullying process (bully,
victim, bully/victim, or not involved). It seems that children who are victims of bullying
invariably see themselves as socially incompetent. They are generally unpopular among
peers, are more anxiety-ridden and unstable, and display little self-confidence (Craig, 1997;
Kahtri, Kupersmidt, & Patterson, 2000; Olweus, 1989; Perry, Kusel, & Perry, 1988; Slee,
1995). More specifically, high scores on certain victimization scales are often associated with
low scores in scholastic competence, social acceptance, athletic competence, physical
appearance, and global self-worth (Andreou, 2000; Boulton & Smith, 1994). Victims tend to
have more negative self-concepts than individuals in the other two groups involved in bullying
(Boulton & Underwood, 1992; Largerspets, Bjrkqvist, Berts, & King, 1982; Olweus, 1978,
1984). The findings regarding the self-concepts of bullies are not so clear-cut and are
sometimes even contradictory. According to some authors (Boulton & Underwood, 1992;
Johnson & Lewis, 1999), there is no link between aggressive behavior and self-worth in
students. On the other hand, Andreou (2000) and OMoore (1997) showed that bullies have
less overall self-worth than children who are not involved in bullying. OMoore and Kirkham
(2001) mentioned two factors that could account for the discrepancy in these results. The first
concerns the psychometric properties of the assessment tools used. If we separate one-
dimensional scales (self-worth) from multidimensional ones (self-concept), however,
differences show up once again. For OMoore and Hillery (1991), for example, bullies have
poorer self-concepts than children who are not involved, whereas for Mynard and Joseph
(1997), bullies have better self-concepts in most areas. The second factor concerns the
typology used to differentiate the students. In a study by Olweus (1993), typical bullies were
differentiated from passive bullies, but then certain passive bullies may also be victims.
Students who were both victims and bullies were found to have lower global self-worth scores
than students in all other groups (Austin & Joseph, 1996; Mynard & Joseph, 1997). Thus,
discrepancies across studies may be due to the criteria used to classify the children.
In matters of bullying, the implications in terms of health have mainly been examined
among bullied victims. Several studies have found a link between being victimized and the
psychosomatic effects of the aggressive events on students health (Forero, McLellan, Rissel, &
Bauman, 1999). In 1998, Rigby noted a strong connection between being bullied and headaches.
More recently, Rigby (1999) showed that severe victimization was often associated with poor
physical health. In particular, victims were found to suffer more from sleep disorders, bed-
wetting, headaches, stomachaches, and feeling unhappy (Williams, Chambers, Logan, &
Robinson, 1996). The symptoms also varied according to the victimized childs gender. Boys
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 185

tended to have more headaches and backaches, and to be more irritable than girls, who were
more nervous and had more sleep disorders. Moreover, the greater the exposure to bullying, the
more numerous the symptoms. The number of symptoms appears to be additionally dependent
on the distress level, and also on the social support provided by the teacher for girls, and by
peers for boys. The less support received, the greater the symptoms. The data also show that
girls are more inclined to report a wider variety of symptoms than boys (Natvig, Albrektsen, &
Qvarnstrom, 2001). Only one study found that the presence of psychosomatic symptoms was a
function of the childs bullying status (Natvig, Albrektsen, & Qvarnstrom, 2001). Bullies
manifested more symptoms than children not involved, but still fewer than victims and
bully/victims. The bully/victim group had the highest proportion of symptoms.
Note that there are no bullying studies to date that have related psychosomatic symptoms
to behavioral disorders.
Being victimized also generates a great deal of distress in a child. Anxiety, depression,
loneliness, isolation, fear of going to school all the necessary elements are present for
provoking emotional and behavioral disorders in the bullied child (Boulton & Underwood,
1992; Boulton & Smith, 1994; Perry et al., 1988; Rigby & Slee, 1993). As such, victimization
can be a precursor to mental-health disorders. However, for some authors, these same disorders
are the cause, not the consequence, of being bullied (Hodges & Perry, 1999), so the link can be
envisaged in both directions. Bullies are dominating (and they like domination; Olweus, 1994)
and anxious (Lagerspetz et al., 1982). While being popular in some cases (Olweus, 1994), they
are rejected by their peers in others (Boulton & Smith, 1994). Bully/victims differ from both
victims and bullies by their personality; they obtain high scores on neurotic and psychotic
scales, are at the bottom of the social acceptance ranking (Mynard & Joseph, 1997), and are
rejected by peers (Bower, Smith, & Binney, 1992). Studies have shown that children who play a
bully/victim or bully role are subject to hyperactivity and manifest many externalization
behaviors extraversion, inability to sit still, need to shout, etc. By contrast, victims exhibit
mainly internalization behaviors withdrawal, introversion, etc. (Kumpulainen et al., 1998;
Laukkanen, Shemeikka, Notkola, Koivumaa-Honkanen, & Nissinen, 2002). Beyond these
studies on the traits of each group, no research has been conducted on the behavioral problems
associated with the four bullying statuses identified by Austin and Joseph (1996).
Another possible impact of bullying is addictive behavior. Research on this topic is
scarce, although there are a few studies on the problem of substance abuse and its link not
only to aggressive acts but also to stressful life events. Relationships of dependency between
aggressive behavior and alcohol consumption are found very early. Accordingly, if children
exhibit behavioral problems at the age of 8 (aggressiveness, fighting), then they are 1.9 times
more likely to drink alcohol and twice as likely to smoke or take drugs (Lynskey & Ferguson,
1995). By the age of 13, aggressive children differ from non-aggressive ones by their greater
consumption of alcohol, cigarettes, and illicit drugs (Choquet, Menke, & Manfredi, 1991).
These results are comparable to those obtained in other studies on aggressive behavior and its
link to substance use (Hore, 1988; Kandel, 1986; Tygard, 1989). Concerning victimization, we
know that by the age of 10, alcohol and tobacco consumption is associated with stressful life
events and daily problems (Steinhausen & Metzke, 1998). It seems that regular consumers of
alcohol, tobacco, and/or drugs, as compared to non-consumers, are more often victims of
insults, violence, theft, and rackets (Oubrayrie-Roussel & Safont-Mottay, 2001). From this
angle, substance use is considered as a response, adjustment, or coping strategy that is non-
adaptive for the child. The results suggest that such substances are consumed by children in an
attempt to reduce behavioral problems related to peer perceptions. Alcohol is thought to
restore self-perceptions about ones social skills. As such, substance consumption would be
highly affected by peer attributions (Adler & Latecka, 1973; McKillip, Johnson, & Petzel,
1973; Smith, Canter, & Robin, 1987). In the case of bullying, however, Kaltiala-Heino,
Rimpela, Rantanen, and Rimpelas (2000) study showed that bullies are greater consumers of
substances than victims. In short, the relationships between substance use, aggression, and
victimization are not easy to define. Can addictive behavior be considered to generate
aggressive acts and/or to serve as a way of maintaining a satisfactory sense of self-worth?
186 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

The consequences of bullying can also be approached in terms of its traumatic impact.
The notion of trauma appeared along with traumatic neurosis, described by Oppenheim
(1889) as a state characterized by nightmares, slow onset of disorders, and irritability. Later,
the Americans described post-traumatic stress in terms of diagnosis criteria defined in the first
version of DSM III (1980). Six criteria are currently listed in DSM IV: (A) The person has
experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events that involved actual or
threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the physical integrity of self or others, and the
persons response involved intense fear, helplessness, or horror; (B) the traumatic event is
persistently reexperienced (dreams, flashbacks, physiological reactivity); (C) persistent
avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma and numbing of general responsiveness
(diminished interest in work and family); (D) persistent symptoms of increased arousal
(irritability, difficulty concentrating); (E) the duration of the disturbance (symptoms in Criteria
B, C, and D) is more than one month; and (F) the disturbance causes clinically significant
distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning. In the
child, in addition to the fact that post-traumatic stress may depend on development, two types
of psychotraumatic disorders are generally distinguished (Terr, 1991): those following a single
deleterious event, and those following a repeated or long-lasting deleterious event. The present
study looks in particular at the second type of psychotraumatic disorder, which involves
avoidance behavior, intrusive thoughts, fears related to the traumatic event, neurovegetative
activation, mood disorders, somatic complaints, fits of rage, anger, forgetting of childhood
memories, and personality problems (Terr, 1991). Because the childs age and developmental
level can affect the manifestations of these symptoms, the disorder varies with development
(Schwarz & Perry, 1994). Children under six rarely exhibit symptoms of post-traumatic stress
as they are described in DSM IV. Cognitive development also appears to play a preponderant
role in the interpretation of events, the ability to cope, and anxiety about the future (Keppel-
Benson & Ollendick, 1993). However, in Pynoos et al.s (1993) study following an earthquake,
no link was found between post-traumatic stress severity and the childrens ages.
These highly specific descriptions of the state of post-traumatic stress have been
challenged by many authors, who feel they do not reflect reality due to the many existing
interindividual differences (Bowman, 1999). The theory proposed by Horowitz offers a good
alternative. According to this author, post-traumatic stress is a cognitive process involving
fluctuation between avoidance reactions and intrusive thoughts (which are likened to the
processes of assimilation and accommodation; Horowitz, 1974, 1979; Horowitz, Stinson, &
Field, 1991). Avoidance (or denial) is characterized by numbing, suppression of stressful
material from the consciousness, and avoidance of memories related to stressful stimuli. It
satisfies the need to protect the ego from the crushing power of the traumatic event. Intrusion
(or approach), which is characterized by invasive thoughts (constant reliving of the event,
flashbacks, dreams, etc.), satisfies the need to adapt to reality.
But in the case of bullying, can we really speak of post-traumatic stress, knowing that the
events in question are not deleterious? Various studies on this topic suggest that the answer
is yes. An investigation on victims of bullying at work showed that 76% of the bullied
individuals were suffering from a post-traumatic stress disorder (Mikkelsen & Einarsen,
2002). A case study on a young victimized adolescent showed that she developed a post-
traumatic stress disorder after being subjected to repeated acts of aggression. Other studies
concur with these in pointing out the devastating impact of bullying on the lives of the affected
persons (Bjrkvist, sterman, & Hjelt-Bck, 1994; Leymann & Gustafsson, 1996). In the
same vein, Scott and Stradling (1994) described three individuals who did not exhibit DSM
IVs Criterion A (having experienced an event that provoked intense fear and involved death
or was a threat to physical integrity) but exhibited post-traumatic stress without trauma, which
this author called prolonged duress stress disorder (PDSD).
In the three experiments reported below, we are going to address each of these points by
looking at the effects of bullying on three levels: (1) its impact on identity, (2) somatic and
behavior implications, and (3) traumatic consequences.
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 187

STUDY 1: THE SELF-CONCEPT OF BULLIES AND VICTIMS

It is clear from a brief overview of the literature that the available findings are highly
contradictory. In the present study, we examine the self-concept in terms of the childs status
as defined by Austin and Joseph (1996), who differentiated bullies, victims, and bully/victims
(persons who are not involved make up the control group). Based on the main findings
presented above, we expected the lowest self-worth among bully/victims. Within this subgroup,
we also examined the self-concept for various areas of life, while paying particular attention to
the social-relations dimension and its manifestations in each group.

Method

Participants

The sample was composed of 116 pupils ages 9 to 12 (m=10.67), 47.4% of whom were
boys. The pupils were from three elementary schools where they were attending fourth or fifth
grade (50.9% in fifth grade). The occupational categories of the parents were representative of
the general population of France. Of the 145 requests for consent given to the parents, 120
agreed to have their child participate in the study. Four of the children refused to take part.

Measures

The pupils filled out two questionnaires. The first one was used to measure the self-
concept, and the second to assess aggressive acts executed and received.

Self-concept. Harters (1982) Self-Perception Profile for Children (SPPC) was


administered. The questionnaire had already been translated from English into French and
validated (Pierrehumbert, Plancherel, & Jankech-Caretta, 1987). It is the scale most
commonly used in studies on school bullying. This self-evaluation instrument measures the
self-concept in a multidimensional perspective and was designed to offset the tendency to give
socially desirable responses, which may be particularly unreliable in the youngest children.
The questionnaire requires the self-appraisal of competency in several areas of daily life. The
areas covered are school (scholastic or cognitive aptitude), social competence (making friends
easily, popularity), athletic competence (physical and sports abilities), appearance (satisfaction
with ones looks), conduct (self-control), and global self-worth (overall self-esteem). The
answers are scored on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 to 4, with 4 representing the most
favorable rating from the standpoint of the self-concept. Thus, for each dimension, the score
varied between 5 and 20.

Bullying. The second questionnaire included two subscales, the Peer Victimization
Scale and the Bullying Behaviour Scale, published by Austin and Joseph (1996) and
constructed specifically for use in conjunction with Harters (1982) self-concept scale. It
measures aggressive acts executed and received. Each subscale is composed of six items
scored on a four-point scale. A sample item is: Some children do not hit and push other
children about, but other children do hit and push other children about. To answer, the child
had to pick which group of children he/she resembled the most and then state the degree of
resemblance (really true for me versus sort of true for me). A mean score was calculated
for each scale (range 1 to 4). A high mean score indicated a high frequency of aggressive acts
executed and/or received. A cutoff point (2.50) was used to classify the children by their status
as a victim, a bully, a bully/victim, or not involved (control group).
188 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

Procedure

The schools that participated in the study were chosen at random. Once permission to
conduct the study had been obtained from the school principals and teachers, a request for
parental consent containing a brief description of the study was given to each child. The data
was gathered in May and June. For each questionnaire, the children had to answer in reference
to the past school year.
The questionnaires were administered collectively during class by a single experimenter,
who reminded the children that the questionnaires were anonymous and that no one in their
surroundings (parents, teachers, or friends) would find out what answers they gave. The pupils
were also told that this was not a test or homework, and that there were no right or wrong
answers. Before filling in the questionnaires, the children were given a definition of bullying:
We say a pupil is being bullied, or picked on, when another pupil, or group of pupils, say
nasty and unpleasant things to him or her. It is also bullying when a pupil is hit, kicked,
threatened, locked inside a room, sent nasty notes, when no one ever talks to them or things
like that. These things can happen frequently, and it is difficult for the pupil being bullied to
defend himself or herself. It is also bullying when a pupil is teased repeatedly in a nasty way.
But it is not bullying when two pupils of about the same strength have the odd fight or quarrel
(Piers, 1984). Then the questionnaires were handed out and the children were given as much
time as they needed to answer them.

Results

For the sample as a whole, 38.8% of the pupils were involved in bullying in one way or
another (Table 1). Victims were the most numerous (15.52%), followed by bullies (12.93%)
and then children who were both victims and bullies (10.34%). There was no significant
difference across ages (2=7.05, df=9, p=NS), school grades (2=3.75, df=3, p=NS), or the
occupational categories of the father (2=17.79, df=15, p=NS) or mother (2=11.14, df=15,
p=NS). However, the childs gender had an impact on the distribution (2=9.365, df=3, p<.05).
Girls were less involved in bullying than boys, and most of the involved girls were victims.
Boys primarily played the role of aggressor.

Table 1
Number of students (and percentage of the row), by gender and bullying status
Bullies Victims Bully/victims Control group
Girls (n=61) 06 (9.84) 11 (18.03) 05 (8.20)0 39 (63.93)
Boys (n=55) 09 (16.36) 07 (12.73) 07 (12.73) 32 (52.46)
Total (n=116) 15 (12.93) 18 (15.52) 12 (10.34) 71 (61.21)

Concerning the pupils self-concepts (Table 2), we can see that the childrens bullying status
affected the scores on the following dimensions: social competence (F(3,112)=15.439, p<.001),
athletic competence (F(3,112)=4.078, p<.01), physical appearance (F(3,112)=8.665, p<.001),
self-control (F(3,112)=8.624, p<.001), and global self-worth (F(3,112)=10.687, p<.001).
More specifically, bully/victims obtained the lowest scores on dimensions related to
self-control, social competence, physical appearance, and global self-worth. However, their
self-perceptions of their athletic abilities were better than those of victims, but still below
those of the bully group and the control group. Concerning the victims, they obtained higher
scores than the bully/victims but lower ones than the bullies and controls on the social
competence, physical appearance, and global self-worth dimensions. At the same time, their
self-control score was lower than the control groups but higher than the scores of the other
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 189

two groups. They also obtained the lowest score on self-perceptions of athletic competence.
Finally, concerning the bullies as compared to the other two groups involved in bullying, they
were the ones who had the best self-concepts, the highest opinions of their physical
appearance, and the most global self-worth. They even obtained higher scores than the control
group on athletic and social competence. However, their perceived self-control, although
above that of bully/victims, was lower than for victims and control subjects.

Table 2
Mean score (and standard deviation) on the self-concept scale, by bullying status
Scholastic Social Athletic Physical Self- Global
competence competence competence appearance control self-worth
=.81 =.78 =.85 =.79 =.80 =.89
Bullies 2.78 (0.67) 3.47 (0.46) 2.96 (0.72) 3.00 (0.61) 2.54 (0.50) 3.13 (0.74)
Victims 2.60 (0.60) 2.37 (0.97) 2.30 (0.86) 2.53 (0.83) 2.61 (0.45) 2.52 (0.80)
Bully/victims 2.64 (0.69) 2.28 (0.77) 2.51 (0.62) 2.36 (0.85) 2.23 (0.65) 2.46 (0.78)
Control group 2.93 (0.66) 3.15 (0.53) 2.80 (0.61) 3.12 (0.58) 2.98 (0.54) 3.21 (0.51)
Significance NS p<.001 p<.01 p<.001 p<.001 p<.001
Note. The classification was based on a 2.50 cutoff point for the victimization scale and the aggression scale.

A closer look at the strength of the links between the various self-concept dimensions and
aggressive acts executed and received indicated a strong negative correlation between bullying
and self-control (r=-.52, p<.001), and positive but more moderate correlations between bullying
and social competence (r=.22, p<.05), athletic competence (r=.30, p<.01), physical appearance
(r=.23, p<.05), and global self-worth (r=.20, p<05). On the victimization side, there were
significant negative correlations between being bullied and social competence (r=-.50,
p<.001), athletic competence (r=-.26, p<.01), physical appearance (r=-.47, p<.001), and self-
worth (r=-.53, p<.05). In addition, there was a strong positive correlation between self-control
and being bullied (r=.52, p<.001).
However, a linear regression analysis (Table 3) showed that being bullied accounted for
the childs overall self-worth (=-.55, p<.001) while bullying did not (=.11, p=NS). The same
was true of scholastic competence and appearance. However, both receiving and executing
acts of aggression explained social competence and self-control.

Table 3
Linear regressions of the self-concept dimensions, for aggressive acts received and aggressive
acts executed
Aggressive acts received =.82 Aggressive acts executed =.87
R2 p R2 p
School .056 -.23 .050 .010 -.12 NS
Social .245 -.37 .001 .036 -.17 .050
Athletic .013 -.18 NS .291 -.46 .001
Appearance .276 -.50 .001 .001 -.03 NS
Self-control .252 -.38 .001 .237 -.34 .001
Global self-worth .301 -.55 .001 .009 -.11 NS

Discussion of Study 1

The proportions of our pupils in the different bullying-status categories were lower than
those published by Austin and Joseph (1996). Only 38.8% of our children were involved in
190 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

one way or another in bullying, whereas Austin and Joseph (1996) obtained 46% (bullies 9%,
victims 22%, bully/victims 15%). Our figures were lower especially for victims (15%) and
bully/victims (10%). Note also that girls were less often involved in bullying than were boys.
This gender difference in aggressive behavior has already been observed elsewhere (Olweus,
1984) aggressive acts were shown to be three or four times more frequent among boys than
among girls (Baldry, 1998; Boulton & Underwood, 1992; OMoore, 1989; Pepler et al., 1993;
Slee & Rigby, 1993; Withney & Smith, 1993). However, the boy-girl difference appears to be
more than one of mere number, since qualitative differences have also been observed.
Violence seems to be expressed differently by the two genders, with girls engaging more
readily in indirect, more subtle forms of bullying (backbiting, rumors, manipulation of
friendships) rather than in the more overt, physical forms of bullying employed by boys.
These distinctions are thought to be rooted in biological differences (Maccoby & Jacklin,
1980) and in gender roles (Eagly & Steffen, 1986).
We can also see that pupils who were both victims and bullies had the poorest self-
concepts in nearly every area. These findings are consistent with a number of earlier studies
(Austin & Joseph, 1996; OMoore & Kirkham, 2001) but contradict the results obtained by
Andreou (2000) and Boulton and Smith (1994). It seems that bully/victims differ from victims
and bullies by their relationships to others: they are rejected the most by peers (Bowers et al.,
1992) and have the lowest social-acceptance scores (Mynard & Joseph, 1997). It could be,
then, that poor relations with peers account for why a child has a particularly bad self-image
and substantial identity problems. However, the individual correlations found here indicate
that it is especially being bullied that is negatively linked to social competence.
Pupils who are victims alone also seem to be affected by identity problems but in lesser
proportions. Our victims self-perceptions concerning their social competence, physical
appearance, global self-worth, and above all, athletic competence were low. These results
corroborate past studies and support the idea that victims are not popular (OMoore &
Kirkham, 2001) and tend to see themselves in a negative way (Olweus, 1984).
On the aggressor side, bullies seem to suffer less than all others involved in bullying, at
least as far as identity is concerned, although their scores were still below those of the control
group. This cannot be said for self-ratings of social and athletic competence, however, which
exceeded those of the control group. Regarding social competence, several studies have shown
that bullies are in fact quite popular, but only among their immediate peer group (Boulton &
Underwood, 1992; Cairns, Cairns, Neckerman, Gest, & Gariepy, 1988; Funk, 2001; Olweus,
1993). In particular, we know that social values circulating in a classroom (positive versus
negative attitudes toward school) may be such that the prestige of a given childs conduct is
positively correlated with conformity (Hargreaves, 1967). Finally, in the area of athletic
competence, our results are consistent with Olweuss (1984) study, which showed that bullies
benefit from greater physical strength. This could explain why bully/victims perceive their
physical and sports abilities more favorably than do victims.
We now know that bullying is likely to affect a pupils identity on both the cognitive
(self-concept) and affective (self-worth) levels. But this finding does not suffice. Modified
self-perceptions may alter a childs relationship to the outside world, which in turn could lead
to a deeper disorder. This was our rationale for addressing the issue of bullying from a
pathological angle. Somatic disorders were assessed by searching for signs of health problems,
and mental disorders were examined by looking into potential behavioral problems.

STUDY 2: PSYCHOSOMATIC SYMPTOMS AND BEHAVIORAL PROBLEMS

This study was conducted in two phases. In the first, we looked at how a childs bullying
status affects manifestations of psychosomatic symptoms. It was assumed for bully/victims
that, because of the characteristics of children with this status (anxiety, instability,
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 191

unpopularity), these children would be the most affected by psychosomatic problems. In the
second phase, we focused on behavioral problems and their potential link to the onset of
psychosomatic symptoms. It was assumed that a positive link exists between these two
variables.

Method

Participants

The sample was composed of 291 subjects, 148 fourth graders and 143 fifth graders. The
pupils ranged in age between 9 and 12 (m=10.14) and were attending five different schools.
The occupational categories of the parents were representative of the general population of
France. Of the 305 parents who were given requests for consent, 296 agreed to have their child
participate in the study. Five of these children did not want to take part.

Measures

Four scales were used to test our hypotheses. Only two will be presented here, because
the other two are described above (self-concept and aggressive acts executed and received).

Psychosomatic symptom scale. This scale was derived from Boyer and Guelfis
Checklists for the Evaluation of Somatic Symptoms (CHESS, 1978). We used the second
version of the scale (CHESS-2: Guelfi & Pull, 1993) composed of 47 items. From these, we
eliminated 3 items concerning changes in sexual desire for ones partner and menstruation
problems (9- to 12-year-old children suffer little if at all from these types of problems). This
gave us a 44-item scale assessing cognitive difficulties (trouble concentrating, memory
problems: 5 items), neurovegetative disorders, part 1 (dizziness, vision problems, tingling
sensations: 5 items), sleep disorders (difficulty falling asleep, waking at night: 4 items),
digestive disorders (nausea, abdominal pain, stomachaches: 4 items), neurovegetative
disorders, part 2 (heart palpitations, trouble breathing: 3 items), somatic pain (pain in the
abdomen, lower back, muscles: 3 items), eating disorders (anorexia and bulimia: 3 items), skin
conditions (itching, pimples: 2 items), vegetative symptoms and dysuria (dry mouth,
perspiration, feeling tense: 13 items), and diarrhea and constipation (2 items). Each item was
scored on a scale ranging from 0 (never) to 4 (every day). The answers were to pertain to the
past school year.

Inventory of youth behavioral problems (ages 11 to 18). Developed by Aschenbach


(Youth Self-Report, 1991) and derived from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL,
Aschenbach, 1991), this self-report questionnaire was translated into French by Vermeersch
and Fombonne (1997). It enables the child to evaluate his/her psychopathological profile, and
provides measures of social competence and behavioral problems. The social-competence
scale includes an activity subscale (ranging from 0 to 12), a social subscale (0 to 12), and a
school subscale (0 to 4). The behavioral-problem scale consists of 119 forced-choice items to
which the child has to answer not true (0), somewhat or sometimes true (1), or very true
or often true (2). We asked our children to base their answers on the past six months.

Procedure

The procedure was the same as in the first study, and the data was also collected in May
and June. However, for validity reasons concerning the use of the behavioral-problem scale
(which can only be administered to children between the ages of 11 and 18), only the fifth
192 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

graders rated themselves on this scale (n=143). The children in this portion of the sample were
11 or 12 years old (m=11.12).

Results

In all, 48.8% of the children were involved in bullying (Table 4). There were no
significant differences across ages (2=10.12, df=9, p=NS), school grades (2=1.08, df=4,
p=NS), or occupational categories of the father (2=12.58, df=15, p=NS) or mother (2=7.89,
df=15, p=NS).

Table 4
Number of students with each bullying status (using a cutoff point of 2.50)
Number (n) Percentage (%)
Bullies 057 19.6
Victims 044 15.1
Bully/Victims 041 14.1
Control Group 149 51.2

Psychosomatic symptoms

The number of symptoms for each bullying status differed significantly ( 2=22.462,
df=12, p<.05). For the set of all children involved in bullying, 60% had more than 15
symptoms. Among these, 78% were bully/victims, 68% were victims, and 40% were bullies
(Table 5).

Table 5
Number of students (and percentage of the column), by number of psychosomatic symptoms
exhibited
Number of symptoms Bullies Victims Bully/victims Control group
[0-10] 10 (17.54%) 05 (11.36%) 04 (9.76%) 55 (36.91%)
[11-15] 24 (42.10%) 09 (20.45%) 05 (12.49%) 24 (16.10%)
[16-20] 08 (14.04%) 06 (13.64%) 07 (17.07%) 32 (21.48%)
[21-30] 08 (14.04%) 11 (25%)0.0 11 (26.83%) 23 (15.44%)
[31-44] 07 (12.28%) 13 (29.55%) 14 (34.15%) 15 (10.07%)
Note. 2=22.462; df=12; p<.05.

More specifically, differences across statuses were found on cognitive problems


(F (3,287) =2.978, p<.05), neurovegetative disorders part 1 (vision problems, dizziness:
F(3,287)=3.227, p<.05), digestive problems (F(3,287)=3.679, p<.05), neurovegetative disorders
part 2 (difficulty breathing, heart palpitations: F(3,287)=4.186, p<.01), somatic disorders
(F(3,287)=3.006, p<.05), and skin conditions (F(3,287)=3.360, p<.05) (Table 6).
The control group obtained the lowest score on every dimension; pupils who were both
victims and bullies obtained the highest score on every dimension (except for cognitive
problems, where victims scored higher). Victims had higher means than bullies in all other
areas except digestive and neurovegetative disorders part 2, (heart palpitations, difficulty
breathing) where their scores were lower. Thus, bullies can be said to fall between victims and
bully/victims along the digestive and neurovegetative 1 dimensions. But they fell between
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 193

victims and controls along the neurovegetative 2, somatic, and skin dimensions. For all
symptoms combined (F (3,287) =4.185, p<.01), bully/victims had the highest number of
symptoms (m=9.52, SD=4.62), followed by victims (m=7.49, SD=3.78), bullies (m=7.28,
SD=4.14), and finally pupils not involved in bullying at all (m=6.29, SD=3.70).

Table 6
Mean score (and standard deviation) on the psychosomatic symptom subscales, by bullying
status
=.73 Bullies Victims Bully/victimes Control group Significance
Cognitive difficulties 0.51 (0.70) 0.64 (0.68) 0.56 (0.45) 0.41 (0.50) p<.05
Neurovegetative disorders, part 1 0.57 (0.43) 0.62 (0.46) 0.84 (0.69) 0.50 (0.41) p<.05
Sleeping disorders 1.38 (0.99) 1.39 (0.97) 1.56 (0.85) 1.25 (0.95) NS
Digestive disorders 0.80 (0.62) 0.66 (0.68) 1.13 (0.66) 0.63 (0.58) p<.05
Neurovegetative disorders, part 2 0.69 (0.81) 0.55 (0.55) 1.00 (0.83) 0.48 (0.54) p<.01
Somatic pain 0.66 (0.51) 0.73 (0.54) 1.06 (0.84) 0.47 (0.61) p<.05
Eating disorders 0.96 (0.48) 0.87 (0.53) 1.02 (0.55) 0.76 (0.51) NS
Skin conditions 0.75 (1.11) 0.87 (0.72) 1.27 (0.86) 0.62 (0.81) p<.05
Vegetative symptoms 0.77 (0.57) 0.75 (0.46) 0.86 (0.58) 0.63 (0.41) NS
Diarrhea and constipation 0.49 (0.67) 0.36 (0.46) 0.27 (0.37) 0.33 (0.47) NS

A stepwise ascending multiple regression analysis on the psychosomatic symptom data


pointed out a link with the self-concept. Eight predictors were input (the six self-concept
dimensions and the aggressive-acts-executed and aggressive-acts-received variables). Three
models were retained by the multiple regression analysis. For the first, there was a significant
regression on aggressive acts received (=.72, p<.01) that accounted for 22% of the observed
variance in the psychosomatic-symptom variable (R 2=.220, p<.01). The second model
explained aggressive acts received (=.56, p<.01) and social competence (=-.47, p<.01).
Combined, these two variables explained 32% of the observed variance in psychosomatic
symptoms (R2=.317, p<.01). Finally, the third model had significant regressions for aggressive
acts received (=.39, p<.01), social competence (=-.39, p<.01), and aggressive acts executed
(=.38, p<.01). This model accounted for 42% of the observed variance (R2=.419, p<.01).

Behavioral problems

Recall that the sample used to study behavior disorders was composed of 143 children
who were at least 11 years old. The bullying-status distribution of these pupils was similar to
that of all subjects pooled (Table 7). Here again, nearly 45% of the pupils were involved in
bullying.

Table 7
Distribution of students, by bullying status (2.50 cutoff point)
Number (n) Percentage (%)
Bullies 25 17.6
Victims 20 13.9
Bully/Victims 19 13.3
Not involved 79 55.2

Comparing our results with Fombonnes obtained during the validation of the French
Child Behavior Checklist (Table 8), we can see that the scores were equivalent on the
194 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

activities (m=5.98, SD=2.36), social (m=5.47, SD=1.86), and social competence (m=16.07,
SD=3.47) subscales. Note the differences, however, for the subscales assessing school
(m=4.73, SD=0.87) and behavioral problems (m=52.36, SD=22.03). In our study, we found
significant status-related differences on the social (F (3,139) =2.035, p<.05), school
(F(3,139)=2.124, p<.05), and behavior (F (3,139)=2.437, p<.05) subscales. It is particularly
interesting to note that the lowest score on the social dimension was obtained by the
bully/victims (m=4.27, SD=1.66), and the highest score, by the bullies (m=6.31, SD=1.76),
even higher than the control group (m=6.03, SD=1.86). These same bullies were the ones who
obtained the lowest mean score on the school dimension (m=3.58, SD=0.75), followed by
bully/victims (m=4.07, SD=0.78) and then victims (m=5.51, SD=0.93), whose overall mean
was close to the control groups (m=5.77, SD=8.83). The bully/victims (m=64.38, SD=27.0)
gave themselves the highest mean rating on behavioral problems, as compared to victims
(m=57.29, SD=18.8) followed by bullies (m=51.12, SD=19.7).

Table 8
Mean score (and standard deviation) on the behavioral problems scale by bullying status in
Study 2, and in two samples tested by Fombonne using the French Child Behavior Checklist
(1989)
Study 2 Fombonne
Bullies Victims Bully/victims Control group p Clinical sample Normal sample
Activities =.67 05.59 (2.47) 05.25 (2.78) 06.17 (2.25) 06.41 (1.91) NS 05.5 (2.4)0 6.1 (2.3)0
Social =.50 06.31 (1.76) 05.32 (2.11) 04.27 (1.66) 06.03 (1.86) p<.05 04.2 (2.1)0 5.4 (1.7)0
School =.79 03.58 (0.75) 05.51 (0.93) 04.07 (0.78) 05.77 (8.83) p<.05 03.6 (1.3)0 5.0 (0.9)0
Social comp. =.81 15.48 (3.50) 15.03 (3.67) 15.56 (3.36) 18.21 (3.30) NS 13.5 (4.0)0 16.6 (3.5)0
Behav. problems =.92 51.12 (19.7) 57.29 (18.8) 64.38 (27.0) 36.04 (20.7) p<.05 51.1 (22.2) 30.0 (18.3)

We might also mention the importance of behavioral problems in the manifestation of


psychosomatic symptoms (=.60, p<.001, R2=.342). The more behavioral problems the
children had, the greater the number and frequency of psychosomatic problems. We also
found a negative link with the social dimension: the lower the social score, the greater the
psychosomatic symptoms (=-.17, p<.05, R2=.053). However, no associations were noted with
school ( =-.03, p=NS, R 2 =.001), activities ( =.08, p=NS, R 2 =.003), or overall social
competence (=.13, p=NS, R2=.012).

Discussion of Study 2

Here again, we obtained a bullying-status distribution that differed from Austin and
Josephs (1996) our proportion of bullies was twice as high. However, the proportions of
victims and bully/victims were similar.
The results of this experiment showed that children involved in bullying (victims, bullies,
or bully/victims) exhibited a large number of psychosomatic symptoms. More specifically,
bully/victims had the highest mean scores and were the most affected by neurovegetative
disorders, digestive problems, somatic pain, and skin conditions. This finding is comparable to
that obtained by Natvig et al. (2001), who noted that nearly 71% of these subjects had the
highest number of psychosomatic symptoms. Victims had health problems too, especially
cognitive difficulties, neurovegetative disorders, somatic pain, and skin conditions. Again,
these results are consistent with the findings of other studies (Natvig et al., 2001; Rigby, 1998,
1999; Williams et al., 1996). Bullies had a higher psychosomatic symptom level than controls,
particularly symptoms of digestive and neurovegetative disorders, which corroborates earlier
findings (Natvig et al., 2001; Kaltiala-Heino, Rimpel, Rantanen, & Rimpel, 2000). The
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 195

manifestation of psychosomatic symptoms was partially explained by aggressive acts


received, low self-perceived social competence, and aggressive acts executed. Thus, being a
victim, but also a perpetrator, of bullying can be dangerous for a pupils health. We are not
suggesting that there is a direct causal relation between aggressive behavior and health, but
that the various protagonists of bullying must be taken into consideration in their entirety and
with all of the complexities involved.
This study also looked for a possible link between behavioral problems and the childs
status with respect to bullying. Bully/victims were deficient in social and scholastic
competence, and had significantly more behavioral problems than other children involved or
not involved in aggressive acts. This finding also applies to victims and to bullies. More
specifically, bullies had low scholastic competence, as observed by Fombonne in a clinical
sample. Many studies have found a positive link between failure in school and aggressive
behavior (Baranger, 1999; Dornbush, Mounts, Lamborn, & Steinberg, 1991). However, the
direction of the link is difficult to determine. For these authors, the two are interdependent
they mutually reinforce each other and thereby increase the feeling of being left out (Aubert,
2001; Perrenoud, 1989). We can also see that bullies had particularly high social competence
scores. This result is in line with our preceding study.
We now know that bullying is likely to have repercussions on identity (altered self-
concept), health (psychosomatic symptoms), and behavior. This set of manifestations, which
are only some of the problems examined in the bullying research, are indicative of the
probable ill-being suffered by victims of bullying. The next question we raised concerned
whether these indicators merely represent the tip of the iceberg or whether bullying has a
deeper impact. What is the real impact of bullying? Is it simply a factor of stress or anxiety, as
various studies have already shown, or, as we suspect, does it have psychotraumatic
consequences that jeopardize the very mental soundness of the individual? We will try to
answer this question in Study 3.

STUDY 3: POST-TRAUMATIC STRESS AND ADDICTIVE BEHAVIOR

Few studies have taken an interest in the traumatic effects of aggressive acts among
students at school. This is one of the issues tackled in this study. We assumed that a childs
post-traumatic stress level varies with the extent to which he/she is bullied. We were also
interested in the manifestations of addictive behavior. To this end, were looked for links
between substance use, victimization, and acts of aggression. Our hypothesis was that these
three variables are positively correlated. In addition, we predicted that there would be a positive
link between substance use and post-traumatic stress level. Two student populations were
compared: ones attending normal schools, and ones enrolled in a special-education program1.
Children enrolled in a special-education program exhibit a number of social and
psychological characteristics that make them incapable of adjusting to a normal school
environment. They are placed in a special school due to a slight or moderate intellectual
deficiency or to maladjustment. In most cases, they are from families with many children
who live in crowded conditions, in homes with a very low socioeconomic standing.
Psychologically speaking, these students are mentally unstable, suffer from feelings of failure
and inferiority, and are not very resistant to frustrations. In addition, they are highly emotional
and adult-dependent. Academically, their scholastic history is marked by repeated failures and
disappointments (San Jos, 1992).
Students who are doing poorly in school are known to have below-average levels of self-
worth (Chapman, 1988). A study by San Jos (1992) pointed out three profiles: uprooted
children, who are the offspring of immigrants but have no apparent personality problems;
welfare children, who are from underprivileged, culturally deprived homes; and pathological
cases or children who require therapy because of behavior and personality problems. For all
196 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

of these characteristics which make them unusual, we wanted to compare special-education


children to children attending regular schools, by looking at their self-concept, their
involvement in bullying, and its impact on them. We predicted that aggressive behavior and
victimization would be more prevalent in a special-education setting than in a regular middle
school. We also expected to find more signs of post-traumatic stress in students attending a
special school.

Method

Participants

The subjects in the sample (n=162) were attending either a regular middle school (n=70)
or a special-education school (n=92). Unlike the preceding studies, this study dealt with
adolescents. The reason for this difference is that children in France are not tracked and oriented
by level until they enter middle school, so this was the only way we could compare regular
and special-ed groups. In addition, as Oubrayrie-Roussel and Safont-Montay (2001) showed
in a study on substance use, less than 3% of 11- to 13-year-olds are regular substance
consumers. Studying middle school children thus provided us with an older sample (age range
12 to 17 years).
In each school, equal-size groups were selected from every grade between the sixth and
ninth grades. There was some degree of imbalance across schools in the students genders and
in the parents occupational categories (Table 9). The special-ed group had a majority of boys
(68%), whereas the regular-school group had a more balanced gender distribution (41% boys).
In addition, parents who were laborers or unemployed made up 73% of the special-ed
population, versus 61% for the regular-school group. This group difference was significant for
the fathers occupation (2=5.575, df=1, p<.05): 93.5% of the fathers of special-ed students
were farmers, employees, laborers, retired, or unemployed, whereas this figure was 81.4% for
the adolescents from regular schools.

Table 9
Characteristics of the samples
Special education Regular schooling Whole sample
(n=92) n(%) (n=70) n(%) (n=162) n(%)
Gender Male 63 (68.5)0 29 (41.4)0 092 (56.84)
Female 29 (31.5)0 41 (58.6)0 070 (43.2)0
Age [range] [13-16] [12-17] [12-17]
Mean 14.38 13.99 14.21
Median 14.00 14.00 14.00
Origin French 84 (91.3)0 70 (100)0. 154 (95.10)
Foreign 08 (8.7)00 00 (0)0.00 008 (4.9)00
Parents occupational category Shop owner 08 (4.35)0 18 (12.86) 026 (8.02)0
Manager/professional 39 (21.19) 58 (41.43) 097 (29.94)
Employee 51 (27.72) 28 (20)0.0 079 (24.38)
Laborer 03 (1.63)0 01 (0.71)0 004 (1.24)0
Retired/unemployed* 83 (45.11) 35 (25)0.0 118 (36.42)
Note. *Unemployed or housewife.

Procedure

The procedure was the same as in the first two studies. This time, though, data collection
took place in March and April.
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 197

Measures

Four scales were used to test our hypotheses: a self-concept assessment scale, a scale of
aggressive acts executed and received, an addictive behavior scale, and a post-traumatic stress
scale. Here again, the first two scales will not be presented since they are described above in
Study 1.

Addictive behaviors. The addictive behavior scale was derived from Currie and
Hurremans (2000) Health Behavior in School Aged Children Survey. A few questions were
added from the ISPA (Swiss Institute for the Prevention of Alcoholism), which is an essentially
epidemiological questionnaire pertaining to alcohol, tobacco, and illegal drugs. The questions
pertained to both how much and how often a given substance was consumed, such as If you
smoke, how many cigarettes do you smoke per week? The tobacco subscale consisted of six
items, with a total score ranging from 0 to 16. Alcohol drinking was measured on an eight-
item subscale (total score: 0-29), and drug use, on a fifteen-item subscale (total score: 0-40).
The overall score combining the three subscales thus ranged between 0 and 85. In our study,
the internal consistency ratings of the tobacco, alcohol, and drug subscales were .53, .81, and
.82, respectively.

Post-traumatic stress. The students each had to state whether, during that year, they had
been the victim of one or more aggressive acts that they remembered vividly. If they said yes,
they had to state what type of aggression it was (physical, verbal, or relational) and when the
event(s) had taken place. Next, the students had to fill out the Impact Event Scale
(Horowitz, 1979), which assesses the frequency of avoidance and intrusion phenomena linked
to the experience of a particular event. This instrument is used to diagnose a state of post-
traumatic stress, but also and especially, to measure the post-traumatic stress level following a
specific event. Avoidance behaviors are measured on eight items, giving a total score between
0 and 40. Intrusion is assessed on seven items, for a total score of 0 to 35. In our study, the
internal consistency ratings of the avoidance and intrusion subscales were .87 and .91,
respectively.

Results

We can see that the special-ed students were more involved in bullying than students in
regular schools ( 2 =13.655, df=3, p<.01). Fifty percent of the former had committed
aggressive acts and/or been victimized, whereas only 22.9% of the latter had been involved in
bullying (Table 10).

Table 10
Number of students (and percentage), by bullying status and schooling (regular schooling
versus special education), with a cutoff point of 2.50
Special education (n=92) Regular schooling (n=70) Whole sample (n=162)
n (%) n (%) n (%)
Bullies 18 (19.6) 05 (7.1)0 23 (14.2)
Victims 21 (22.8) 10 (14.3) 031 (19.1)
Bully/victims 07 (7.6)0 01 (1.4)0 008 (4.9)0
Not involved 46 (50)0. 54 (77.1) 100 (61.7)
Note. The difference between special education and regular schooling was significant: 2(3)=13.655, p<.01.
198 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

This finding was further supported by the fact that special-ed students obtained a higher
mean score on the scale of aggressive acts executed (m=2.19, SD=0.59) than did regular-
school students (m=1.92, SD=0.57; t(160)=-2.964, p<.01). However, this difference was no
longer found on the scale of aggressive acts received (t(160)=-1.914, p=NS). Also, students
whose father was a craftsman, shop owner, manager, head of a firm, or in one of the liberal
professions obtained lower average scores on the aggressive-acts-executed scale (m=1.67,
SD=0.45) than did the adolescents whose father was a laborer, a farmer, an employee, retired,
or unemployed (m=2.12, SD=0.59) (t(160)=2.048, p<.01). However, this finding was not
obtained for the mothers occupational category (t(160)=-0.074, p=NS). These occupation-related
differences did not show up in the aggressive-acts-received scores, whether for the father
(t(160)=0.902, p=NS) or the mother (t(160)=1.025, p=NS).

Self-concept

Setting aside other considerations, it is interesting to note that the special-ed students
obtained a lower score than the regular-school students on several dimensions of the self-
concept. This was true of scholastic competence (special m=2.60, SD=0.60; regular m=2.79,
SD=0.55; t(160)=2.084, p<.05), social competence (special m=2.98, SD=0.73; regular m=3.20,
SD=0.56; t (160)=2.108, p<.05), athletic competence (special m=2.59, SD=0.70; regular
m=2.82, SD=0.69; t(160)=2.061, p<.05), and self-control (special m=2.67, SD=0.53; regular
m=2.91, SD=0.55; t(160)=2.794, p<.01). However, these differences were not obtained for
appearance (t(160)=0.853, p=NS) or global self-worth (t(160)=1.948, p=NS).

Table 11
Mean score (and standard deviation) of students who had/had not experienced an event they
remembered vividly, by self-concept dimension
Bullied (n=73) Not Bullied (n=72) p
Scholastic competence 2.60 (0.6)0 2.78 (0.57) NS
Social competence 2.94 (0.72) 3.20 (0.62) p<.05
Athletic competence 2.60 (0.74) 2.77 (0.66) NS
Physical appearance 2.61 (0.74) 2.79 (0.66) NS
Conduct 2.67 (0.53) 2.88 (0.57) p<.05
Global self-worth 2.84 (0.67) 3.11 (0.56) p<.01

Out of the 145 students tested, 73 said they had undergone at least one aggressive act that
they remembered vividly. Note that 17 students were removed from the sample for having
experienced impactful events that were unrelated to our theme (death, relational problems with
an adult). We can see that students who reported having undergone a particularly impactful act
of aggression had poorer self-concepts on several dimensions, namely: social competence
(t(143)=-2.261, p<.05), conduct (t(143)=-2.277, p<.05), and global self-worth (t(143)=-2.670,
p<.01) (Table 11). These differences did not occur for scholastic competence (t(143)=-1.787,
p=NS), athletic abilities (t(143)=-1.538, p=NS), or physical appearance (t(143)=-1.572, p=NS).

Post-traumatic stress

This year, were you the victim of one or more aggressive acts by your classmate(s) that
you left a marked impression on you? To this question, 42 special-ed students and 31 regular-
school students answered yes. A majority of these students (65.8%) obtained a high post-
traumatic stress level, 17.8% had an intermediate level, and 8.2% a low level. This portion of
the sample included 39 boys (53.4%) and 34 girls (46.6%), but there was no significant
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 199

difference between the mean post-traumatic stress scores obtained by the two genders
(t(71)=0.299, p=NS). However, the students age did have an effect (F(5,67)=2.998, p<.05). The
12- to 15-year-olds obtained mean scores that dropped as their age rose (age 12 n=10,
m=34.80, SD=19.81; age 13 n=11, m=29.64, SD=12.36; age 14 n=20, m=27.75, SD=15.20;
age 15 n=19, m=18.42, SD=14.70). The older students (ages 16 and 17) obtained mean scores
of 34.96 (age 16 n=12, SD=21.31) and 64 (age 17 n=1). As predicted, the post-traumatic stress
level depended on the students bullying status (F(3,69)=4.862, p<.01). Students who were both
victims and bullies had the highest level (m=27.62, SD=23.64), followed by victims (m=20.71,
SD=23.64), with the post-traumatic stress level tending to be linked to being victimized
(r=.20, p<.10). Moreover, intrusion behaviors were positively correlated with being bullied
(r=.27, p<.05). Linear regressions on post-traumatic stress level and being victimized also
yielded a number of tendencies. Intrusive thoughts (=.25, p<.10, R2=.053) and post-traumatic
stress level ( =.20, p<.10, R 2 =.029) tended to be positively affected by the extent of
victimization. However, this effect no longer occurred for avoidance behaviors ( =.16, p=NS,
R2=.013). Regarding the type of aggression, the bullying was verbal in 16.4% of the cases
(n=12), relational in 23.3% (n=17), and physical in 60.3% (n=44). However, the type of
aggression had no effect on post-traumatic stress (F(2,70)=0.049, p=NS), nor did the time lapse
between being bullied and filling out the questionnaires (F(3,69)=0.855, p=NS). In addition,
there was no significant difference between the regular-school and special-ed students as far as
post-traumatic stress was concerned (t(71)=-0.441, p=NS). Note also that post-traumatic stress was
negatively correlated with global self-worth (r=-.31, p<.01). More specifically, self-worth was
linked to intrusive thoughts (r=-.41, p<.01) but not to avoidance behaviors (r=-.20, p=NS).

Table 12
Correlation Matrix (n=162)
School Social Athletic Appearan. Conduct Self-worth Bully Victim Tobacco Alcohol Drugs Addictions
School 1.00
Social -.190** 1.00
Athletic -.258** -.346** 1.00
Appearance -.282** -.410** -.348** 1.00
Conduct -.311** -.039** -.094** -.145** 1.00
Self-worth -.418** -.404** -.282** -.626** -.265** 1.00
Bully -.283** -.005** -.015** -.034** -.497** -.193** 1.00
Victim -.295** -.598** -.226** -.441** -.154* -.509** -.159** 1.00
Tobacco -.259** -.112** -.075** -.044** -.304** -.217** -.383** -.004* 1.00
Alcohol -.157** -.092** -.168** -.130** -.059** -.006** -.158** -.178* .499** 1.00
Drugs -.187** -.091** -.099** -.030** -.211** -.076** -.227** -.078* .579** .487** 1.00
Tot. addictions -.244** .119** -.077** -.049** -.228** -.121** -.311** -.107* .844** .818** .812** 1.00

Note. *p<.05, **p<.01.

Substance use

In general, there were strong positive intercorrelations between the consumption of alcohol,
tobacco, and drugs (Table 12). Aggressive behavior was positively correlated with addictive
behavior in general (r=.31, p<.01), and with smoking (r=.38, p<.01), drugs (r=.23, p<.01), and
alcohol (r=.16, p<.05) in particular. Regressions on the various types of substances pointed
in the same direction (tobacco =.00, p=NS, R2=.006; alcohol =-.18, p<.05, R2=.032; drugs
=-.08, p=NS, R2=.006). Contrary to our hypothesis, a negative correlation was found between
being victimized and alcohol consumption (r=-.18, p<.05). Linear regressions supported this
finding (tobacco =.38, p<.01, R2=.147; alcohol =.16, p<.05, R2=.025; drugs =.23, p<.01,
R2=.052). Note also that each of the addictive-behavior variables correlated negatively with
the school dimension. Similar associations were observed with the conduct dimension.
Among the 73 students who had been victims of one or more impactful bullying events,
the special-ed students and the ones attending regular schools did not differ significantly on
200 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

substance use. This was true for tobacco (t(71)=-1.686, p=NS), alcohol (t(71)=0.356, p=NS), and
drugs (t(71)=1.387, p=NS). Moreover, no link was found between post-traumatic stress level
and substance use, although there was a positive correlation between intrusions and tobacco
consumption (r=.28, p<.05).

Discussion of Study 3

This study revealed, first of all, that special-education students had more identity
problems than students enrolled in regular schools. In various areas (scholastic competence,
social competence, athletic abilities, and self-control), their self-concept was poorer.
According to professionals who work in this field, these students are highly stigmatized and
have a hard time accepting that they must go to a special school (Coslin, Agou, & Majoux,
1985). In addition, the special-ed students were found to be more involved in bullying than the
students attending regular schools. But we also noted that the parents of these adolescents
were from a lower social class than those of regular-school students. Poor living conditions
are known to constitute a powerful explanatory factor in the appearance of violent behavior
(Reid, 1984; Tibbenheim, 1977). Living in a cramped home (Kultus, Wissenschaft, & Kunst,
1994, cited by Funk, 2001), having an unemployed parent (May, 1975), or simply being from
the lower class (Reid, 1984) all add up to increasing the likelihood of aggressive behavior.
Many studies have already established the link between poverty and violence (Byrne et al.,
1999; McLloyd, 1990).
Another result obtained here was the lack of a significant difference between the two
schooling groups on the impact of bullying (post-traumatic stress). This finding is particularly
interesting since it means that even though the special-ed students were more involved in
bullying, they apparently did not suffer greater consequences than the students attending regular
schools. We can hypothesize that social norms about aggressive behaviors account for this
difference. In special schools, more acts of aggression may be committed, but they may also be
accepted better. Such differences are known to exist at the intercultural level (Fraczek, 1985;
Lagerspetz & Westman, 1980; Tomlinson, 1970) depending on the culture or social group in
which they are expressed, various forms of aggression are tolerated to a greater or lesser extent.
Note in addition that the victims of aggression exhibited a higher level of post-traumatic
stress (prolonged duress stress disorder) than the rest of the sample. More specifically,
victimization tended to be associated with greater post-traumatic stress. Furthermore, as
expected, bullying was associated with intrusive thoughts, as reported in the case study by
Weaver (2000). However, we found no gender-linked differences, unlike earlier studies where
girls were shown to be more severely affected than boys (Shannon, Lonigan, Finch, & Taylor,
1994). Age appears to have an impact too, with younger students being affected more than
older ones. However, our 16- and 17-year-olds were also highly affected by bullying. We can
assume that their age, a testimony to the fact that they were lagging behind in school, was an
indication of deficient cognitive development. Moreover, post-traumatic stress was highly
linked to low self-worth, which supports the findings of many past studies (Jind, 2001). It is
interesting to note that while intrusion was negatively linked to self-worth, avoidance was not.
Thus, in line with the observations made here, avoidance of anxiety-generating stimuli in the
face of bullying may play a protective role (Naylor, Cowie, & Rey del, 2001).
Concerning substance use (alcohol, tobacco, or drugs), it seems that the consumption of
any one of these substances generally leads to the use of the other two. Here again, no
schooling-related differences were found. However, smoking and taking drugs were linked to
committing aggressive acts. This finding is in line with Kaltiala-Heinos (2000) study, which
showed that substance use was particularly prevalent among bullies and bully/victims.
Unexpectedly, we did not find a positive link between addictive behavior and being victimized.
Note, however, that the higher the level of intrusive thoughts, the greater the consumption of
tobacco. Thus, the more the adolescents had thoughts or nightmares related to being bullied,
the greater their tendency to smoke. Two possible hypotheses can be set forth to account for
this addiction. The first concerns the fact that the psycho-stimulating effect of nicotine is
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 201

comparable to that of amphetamines, so high doses have a relaxing and euphoric effect. These
mixed effects (the impression of increased productivity followed by the appearance of a sense
of well-being) can only contribute to making the smoker more dependent. The other
hypothesis concerns the psychosocial dimension of cigarette smoking. Indeed, smoking may
restore a childs social image and thereby sporadically reinforce a failing level of self-esteem.
As usual, the most probable explanation is certainly a combination of the two.

General discussion

These three studies showed that the implications of bullying are numerous and varied.
Based on Study 1, it seems that bully/victims have the lowest opinions of themselves, followed
by victims and then bullies. However, although we showed that being a victim of aggression
could play a critical role in a childs sense of self-worth, the question that arose was the
direction of the link between the self-concept and the different possible statuses of the child in
the bullying phenomenon. According to some authors (Aubert, 2001; Olweus, 1978), there is a
certain predisposition to being a victim, a bully, or even a bully/victim. At the same time,
repeated harassment by peers can considerably reinforce feelings of distress, lack of self-
confidence, and low self-esteem in victims. We also know that children attach more
importance to traits that characterize them as bullies (e.g., traits of temperament like impulsive,
extroverted, conceited, leader, and physical traits like strong) than to other traits.
Study 2 showed that students involved in bullying may have health problems too,
especially psychosomatic symptoms. Bully/victims seem, once again, to be the most highly
affected (mainly by neurovegetative disorders, digestive problems, somatic pain, and skin
conditions). Victims, on the other hand, seem to suffer in particular from cognitive difficulties;
bullies have digestive and neurovegetative disorders. A link was also found between the
childs bullying status and behavioral problems. As shown in earlier work, behavioral
problems appeared in all three status groups (in the following decreasing order: bully/victims,
victims, and bullies). This helps account for the variations observed in the manifestation of
psychosomatic symptoms.
Study 3 on middle school students showed that bully/victims and victims exhibited high
post-traumatic stress levels, and that there tends to be a positive correlation between being
victimized and post-traumatic stress. In addition, it seems that the children who suffer the
most from intrusions related to being bullied are the ones to smoke the most. Thus, we can
assume that when the well-being of adolescents is threatened in upsetting situations (like
being bullied), risky behaviors such as substance use are attempts at maintaining a satisfactory
level of self-esteem. As Peele (1985) suggested, addiction may help a child strengthen his/her
sense of self-worth.
The prevalence of bullying differed in the three studies conducted here. In Study 1,
nearly 39% of the children were involved in bullying, in Study 2, the proportion was nearly
49%, and in Study 3, it was about 38%. The figures for Study 2 are comparable to those
obtained in research on elementary school pupils. However, this cannot be said for Study 1,
where our figures were lower than those obtained elsewhere. This discrepancy seems to be due
to environmental factors, since different populations were studied. Indeed, as Carra and Sicot
(1997) showed, bullying can vary considerably from one school to the next. Environmental
factors in this case refer to physical characteristics of the school likely to favor the emergence
of aggressive behavior (Ahmad et al., 1991, cited by Karatzias, Power, & Swanson, 2002).
The most commonly mentioned factor is size. As early as 1983, the Lon Report in France
indicated that the risk of violence is greater in large schools. Class size also seems to have an
impact. Funk and Passenbergen (1999, cited by Funk, 2001) showed that the smaller the
student-to-teacher ratio, the fewer the acts of aggression. However, other studies suggest that
children who belong to small classes are more often subjected to bullying, as observed in
England (Wolke, Woods, Standford, & Shulz, 2001). In fact, a direct causal relation cannot be
202 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

established between these factors and the emergence of aggressive behavior. Certain studies
suggest that in large schools where supervision is not as great, it is more difficult to find out
who is at fault (Lon, 1983). In this case, both anonymity and size may be risk factors in the
emergence of aggressive behavior at school.
The results of Study 3 are inconsistent, once again, with those of earlier work, since
bullying was more prevalent in our study. A gradual drop in bullying with age was observed
by Olweus (1984) and other investigators, who showed that bullying declined and finally
disappeared altogether at the age of 16 (Elsea & Smith, 1994, cited by Eslea & Rees, 2001;
Whitney & Smith, 1993). This overall effect did not occur in our third study. However, the
distribution we obtained was different for the two types of schools studied. In regular schools,
only about 23% of the students were involved in bullying, whereas in special-education
schools, this figure jumped to 50%. Thus, for regular-school students there was indeed a
decline in bullying with age, as already observed elsewhere. However, the reason given by the
authors for the decrease is that bullying in fact changes in nature over the years. Several
studies (Owens, 1996; Rivers & Smith, 1994) have shown that younger children more often
resort to direct forms of aggression, whereas older ones prefer more relational, indirect forms.
The bullying scale we used only assessed direct forms of aggression (physical and verbal). So
the phenomenon we observed was essentially a decrease in direct aggression in regular middle
school students and stabilization of direct aggression among students attending a special
school. Why? One hypothesis is related to the characteristics of the population. As stressed
above, these socioeconomically, cognitively, and/or behaviourally disadvantaged students
were also lagging behind in the development of social competence and abilities, so that
recourse to indirect forms of aggression could not emerge until later. Indeed, we found a link
between the type of bullying and social cognition skills. Clearly, indirect and relational
aggressions cannot be executed unless the child has some awareness of, and ability to,
manipulate the mental states and beliefs of others (Bjrkqvitz, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen,
1992; Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Moreover, Kaukiainen et al. (1999) found a positive
correlation in 8-, 12-, and 14-year-old age groups between indirect aggressive acts and social
intelligence, but not when the aggressions were physical or verbal.
To conclude, it would seem worthwhile in future research to address the issue of the
specific characteristics of each bullying status examined here, particularly for the bully/victim
role, and in doing so, to take a more in-depth approach to child behavior. This is especially
important for these children, who cannot seek the necessary support from parents or siblings,
with whom they feel little closeness or connectedness (Bowers et al., 1992). In addition, we
can assume that the various symptoms exhibited by these children are interdependent, as
already shown in some studies (Kaltiala-Heino et al., 2000; Laukkanen et al., 2002), and that
this leads to a cumulative effect (aggressive behavior, substance use, psychosomatic
symptoms) called cumulative continuity (Brook & Newcomb, 1995). This interaction style
prompts the individual to choose and create an environment that will reinforce that style.
Through the mechanism of cumulative continuity, the deviant child is likely to adopt
another deviant behavior such as substance use. Furthermore, in some cases, the
relationships between psychosomatic symptoms, substance use, and aggression are interactive.
This type of interactional continuity refers to the effects of reciprocal interactions between
an individual and his/her environment, such as the link between aggressive acts and substance
use (Vuchinick, Bank, & Paterson, 1992). Reciprocal interactions like these are thought to
prevent the situation from changing (Ensminger, & Slusarcick, 1992). Henceforth, anyone
striving to reduce bullying in the schools should take the wide range of factors underlying its
occurrence into account, so that an effective investigation can be conducted and conclusive
results can be obtained.

Notes

1 The special-education children were in a SEGPA school Section for General Occupational and Adapted Teaching.
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 203

References

Aschenbach, T.M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University
of Vermont Department of Psychiatry.

Adler, P.T., & Latecka, L. (1973). Drug use among high school students: Patterns and correlates. International
Journal of addiction, 8, 537-548.

American Psychiatric Association. (1996). DSM-IV: Manuel diagnostique et statistique des troubles mentaux.
French tranlsation. Paris: Masson.

Andreou, E. (2000). Bully/victim problems and their associations with psychological constructs in 8 to 12 year
old Greek schoolchildren. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 49-56.

Aubert, J.L. (2001). La violence dans les coles. Paris: Odile Jacob.

Austin, S., & Joseph, S. (1996). Assessment of bully/victim problems in 8 to 11 year-olds. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 66, 447-456.

Baldry, A.C. (1998). Bullying among Italian middle school students. School and Psychology Intervention, 19,
361-374.

Baranger, P. (1999). Absentisme, indiscipline, violence: Les incidences ducatives du cadre, des rgles et des rituels
lcole. In P. Baranger (Ed.), Cadres, rgles et rituels dans linstitution scolaire. Nancy: Presses Universitaires de Nancy.

Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences and control. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Bjrkqvist, K., sterman, K., & Hjelt-Bck, M. (1994). Aggression among university employees. Aggressive
Behavior, 20, 173-184.

Bjrkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K.M.J., & Kaukiainen, A. (1992). Do girls manipulate and boys fight? Developmental
trends in regard to direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 18, 117-127.

Boulton, M.J., & Smith, P.K. (1994). Bully/victim problems in middle school children: Stability self-perceived
competence, peer perceptions and peer acceptance. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 315-329.

Boulton, M.J., & Underwood, K. (1992). Bully/victim problems among middle school children. British Journal of
Education and Psychology, 62, 73-87.

Bowers, L., Smith, P.K., & Binney, V. (1992). Coercion and power in the families of children involved in
bully/victims in middle childhood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11, 215-232.

Bowman, M.L. (1999). Individual differences in posttraumatic distress: Problems with the DSM-IV model. The
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 44, 21-33.

Boyer, P., & Guelfi, J.D. (1978). Troubles somatiques et tats dpressifs. Annales mdico-psychologiques, 139,
1167-1182.

Brook, J.-S., & Newcomb, M.-D. (1995). Childhood aggression and unconventionality: Impact on later academic
achievement, drug use, and workforce involvement. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156(4), 393-410

Byrne, C.A., et al. (1999). The socioeconomic impact of interpersonal violence on women. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology, 67, 362-366.

Cairns, R.B., Cairns, B.D., Neckerman, H.J., Gest, S.D., & Gariepy, J.L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive
behavior. Developmental Psychology, 25, 320-330.

Carra, C., & Sicot, F. (1997). Une autre perspective sur les violences scolaires: Lexprience de la victimation. In
B. Charlot & I.C. Emin (Eds.), Violence lcole: Etat des savoirs (pp. 61-82). Paris: Armand Colin.

Chapman, J.W. (1988). Learning disabled childrens self-concepts. Review of Educational Research, 58, 347-371.

Choquet, M., Menke, H., & Manfredi, R. (1991). Interpersonal aggressive behavior and alcohol consumption
among young urban adolescents in France. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 26(4), 381-390.

Coslin, P.G., Agou, M.C., & Majoux, C. (1985). Vandalism in school. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 5, 444.

Craig, W.M. (1997). The relationship among bullying, victimization, depression, anxiety, and agression in
elementary school children. Personal and Individual Differences, 24(1), 123-130.
204 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

Crick, N.R., & Grotpeter, J.K. (1995). Relational aggression, gender and social-psychological adjustment. Child
Development, 66, 710-722.

Currie, L., & Hurreman, H. (2000). HBSCs is WHO/EURO collaborative study. International Coordinator of the
1997-98 study/Candace Currie, University of Edinburgh, Scotland. Data Bank Manager: Bente Wold, University of
Bergen, Norway.

Dodge, K., & Coie, J. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression in
childrens peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1158.

Dornbush, S.M., Mounts, N., Lamborn, S.D., & Steinberg, L. (1991). Patterns of competence and adjustment
among adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful families. Child Development, 62, 1049-
1065.

Eagly, A.H., & Steffen, V.J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social
psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100(3), 309-330.

Eslea, M., & Rees, J. (2001). At what age are children most likely to be bullied at school? Agressive Behavior, 27,
419-429.

Ensminger, M.-E., & Slusarcick, A.L. (1992). Path to high school graduation or dropout: A longitudinal study of a
first grade cohort. Sociology of Education, 65, 95-113.

Fraczek, A. (1985). Moral approval of aggressive acts, a polish-Finnish comparative study. Journal of Cross-
Cultural Psychology, 16(1), 41-54.

Forero, R., McLellan, L., Rissel, C., & Bauman, A. (1999). Bullying behaviour and psychological health among
students in New South Wales, Australia: Cross sectional survey. British Medical Journal, 319, 344-348.

Funk, W. (2001). La violence lcole en Allemagne: Etat des lieux. In R. Debarbieux & C. Blaya (Eds.), La vio-
lence lcole: 10 approches en Europe. Paris: ESF.

Guelfi, J.D., & Pull, C.B. (1993). Les check-lists dvaluation des symptmes somatiques: CHESS (2). In
J.D. Guelfi (Ed.), Lvaluation clinique standardise en psychiatrie (pp. 633-649). Lavaur: Editions mdicales Pierre Fabre.

Hargreaves, D.H. (1967). Social relations in a secondary school. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Harter, S. (1982). The perceived competence scale for children. Child Development, 53, 87-97.

Hodges, E., & Perry, D. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and consequences of victimization by
peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677-685.

Hore, B. (1988). Alcohol and crime. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 23, 435-439.

Horowitz, M.J. (1974). Stress response syndromes: Character style and dynamic psychotherapy. American
Journal of Psychotherapy, 27, 506-515.

Horowitz, M.J. (1979). Stress response syndromes. New York: Jason Aronson.

Horowitz, M.J., Stinson, C., & Field, N. (1991). Natural disaster and stress response syndromes. Psychiatric
Annals, 21, 556-562.

Jind, L. (2001). Do traumatic events influence cognitive schemata? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 113-
120.

Johnson, D., & Lewis, G. (1999). Do you like what you see? Self-perceptions of adolescent bullies. British
Educational Research, 13, 665-676.

Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpel, M., Rantanen, P., & Rimpel, A. (2000). Bullying at school, an indicator of
adolescents at risk for mental disorders. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 661-674.

Kandel, D. (1986). Risk factors for delinquency and illicit drug use from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal
of Drug Issues, 16, 67-90.

Karatzias, A., Power, K.G., & Swanson, V. (2002). Bullying and victimization in Scottish secondary schools:
Same or separate entities. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 45-61.

Kaukiainen, A., Bjrkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., sterman, K., Salmivalli, C., Forsblom, S., & Ahlbom, A. (1999).
The relationships between social intelligence, empathy and three types of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 25(2), 81-89.
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 205

Keppel-Benson, J.M., & Ollendick, T.H. (1993). Post-traumatic stress disorder in children and adolescents. In
C.F. Saylor (Ed.), Children and Disaster (pp. 29-44). New York: Plenum Press.

Khatri, P., Kupersmidt, J.B., & Patterson, C. (2000). Aggression and peer victimization as predictors of self-
reported behavioral and emotional adjustment. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 345-358.

Kumpulainen, K., Rsnen, E., Henttonen, I., Almqvist, F., Kresanov, K., Moilanen, I., Linna, S.L., Piha,
J. Puura, K., & Tamminen, T. (1998). Bullying and psychiatric symptoms among elementary school-age children. Child
Abuse and Neglect, 22(7), 705-717.

Lagerspetz, K., & Westman, M. (1980). Moral approval of aggressive acts: A preliminary investigation.
Aggressive Behavior, 6, 119-130.

Lagerspetz, K.M., Bjrkqvist, K., Berts, M., & King, E. (1982). Group aggression among schoolchildren in three
schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 23, 45-52.

Laukkanen, E., Shemeikka, S., Notkola, I. L., Koivumaa-Honkanen, H., Nissinen, A. (2002). Externalizing and
internalizing problems at school as signs of health-damaging behavior and incipient marginalization. Health Promotion
International, 17(2), 139-146.

Lon, J.-M. (1983). Violence et dviance chez les jeunes: Problmes de lcole, problmes de la cit. Rapport du
Ministre de lEducation.

Leymann, H., & Gustafsson, A. (1996). Mobbing at work and the development of post-traumatic stress disorders.
European Journal of Work and Organizational Psycholgy, 5, 251-275.

Lynskey, M.T., & Fergusson, D.M. (1995). Childhood conduct problems, attention deficit behaviors, and
adolescent alcohol, tobacco, and illicit drug use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 23(3), 281-302.

May, D. (1975). Truancy, school absenteeism and delinquency. Scottish Educational Studies, 7(2), 97-107.

MacCoby, E.E., & Jacklin, C.N. (1980). Sex differences in aggression: A rejoinder and reprise. Child
Development, 51, 364-380.

McKillip, J., Johnson, J.E., & Petzel, T.P. (1973). Patterns and correlates of drug use among urban high school
students. Journal of Drugs and Education, 3, 1-12.

McLloyd, V.C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on black families and children: Psychological distress,
parenting and socioeconomic development. Child Development, 61, 311-371.

Mikkelsen, E.G., & Einarsen, S. (2002). Basic assumptions and symptoms of post-traumatic stress among victims
of bullying at work. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psycholgy, 11(1), 87-111.

Mynard, H., & Joseph, S. (1997). Bully/victim problems and their association with Eysencks personality
dimensions in 8 to 13 year-olds. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 51-54.

Naylor, P., Cowie, H., & Rey del, R. (2001). Coping strategies of secondary school children in response to being
bullied. Child Psychology and Psychiatry Review, 6(3), 20-34.

Natvig, G.K., Albrekten, G., & Qvarnstrom, U. (2001). Psychosomatic symptoms among victims of school
bullying. Journal of Health Psychology, 6(4), 365-377.

OMoore, A.M. (1989). Bullying in Britain and Ireland: An overview. In E. Roland & E. Munthe (Eds.), Bullying:
An international perspective. London: Fulton.

OMoore, A.M. (1997). What do teachers need to know. In M. Elliot (Ed.), Bullying: A practical guide to coping
for schools (pp. 56-59). London: Pitman in association with Kidscape.

OMoore, A.M., & Hillery, B. (1991). What do teachers need to know? In M. Elliot (Ed.). Bullying: A practical
guide to coping for schools (pp. 151-166). London: Pitman in association with Kidscape.

OMoore, A.M., & Kirkham, C. (2001). Self-esteem and its relationship to bullying behavior. Aggressive
Behavior, 27, 296-283.

Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the school: Bullies and whipping boys. Washington: Wiley.

Olweus, D. (1984). Aggressors and their victims: Bullying at school. In N. Frude & H. Gault (Eds.), Disruptive
behavior in schools. New York: Wiley.
206 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

Olweus, D. (1989). Bully/Victim problems among school children: Basic facts and effects of school-based
intervention programs. In K. Rubin & D. Pepler (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression. New
Jersey: Erlbaum.

Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at school. Cambridge: Blackwell.

Olweus, D. (1994). Annotation: Bullying at school: Basic facts and effects of a school-based intervention
programs. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(7), 1171-1190.

Oppenheim, H. (1989). Die traumatischen neurosen. Berlin: August Hirschwald.

Oubrayrie-Roussel, N., & Safont-Montay, C. (2001). Conduites risques et dvalorisation de soi: Etude de la
consommation de toxiques (tabac, alcool et drogues) chez les adolescents scolariss. Psicologia, Saude & Doenas,
2(1), 59-75.

Owens, L.D. (1996). Sticks and stones and sugar and spice: Girls and boys aggression in schools. Australian
Journal of Guidance and Counseling, 6, 45-55.

Peele, S. (1985). The meaning of addiction: Compulsive experience and its interpretation. London: Lexington.

Pepler, D.S., Craig, W.M., Ziegler, S., & Charach, A. (1993). A school based anti-bullying intervention:
Preliminary evaluation. In D. Tattun (Ed.), Understanding and managing bullying (pp. 76-91). Oxford: Heinenmann
Educational.

Perrenoud, Ph. (1989). Parle comme il faut! Rflexions sociologique sur lordre linguistique. In G. Schoni,
J.P. Bronckart, & Ph. Perrenoud (Eds.), La langue franaise est-elle gouvernable? Normes et activit langagire (pp.
79-108). Paris: Delachaux et Niestl.

Perry, D.G., Kusel, S.J., & Perry, L.C. (1988). Victims of peer aggression. Developmental Psychology, 24, 807-814.

Pierrehumbert, B., Plancherel, B., & Jankech-Caretta, C. (1987). Revue de Psychologie Applique, 37(4), 359-377.

Piers, E.V. (1984). Piers-Harris childrens self-concept scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

Pynoos, R.S., Groenjian, A., Tashjian, M., et al. (1993). Post-traumatic stress reactions in children after the 1988
Armenian earthquake. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 239-247.

Reid, K. (1984). Some social, psychological and educational aspects related to school absenteeism. Research in
Education, 31, 63-82.

Rigby, K. (1998). The relationship between reported health and involvement in bully/victim problems among
male and female secondary schoolchildren. Journal of Health Psychology, 3(4), 465-476.

Rigby, K. (1999). Peer victimization at school and the health of secondary school students. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 69, 95-104.

Rigby, K., & Slee, P. (1993). Dimensions of interpersonal relations among Australian children and implication of
psychological well-being. Journal of Social Psychology, 133, 33-42.

Rivers, I., & Smith, P.K. (1994). Types of bullying behavior and their correlates. Aggressive Behavior, 20, 359-368.

Roland, E., & Idsoe, T. (2001). Aggression and bullying. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 446-462.

San Jos, A. (1992). Approche clinique dun chantillon dlves de section dducation spcialise (SES). Revue
Franaise de Pdagogie, 90, 49-61.

Schwarz, E.D., & Perry, B.D. (1994). The post-traumatic response in children and adolescents. Psychiatric
Clinics of North America, 17(2), 311-326.

Scott, M.J., & Stradling, S.G. (1994). Post-traumatic disorder without the trauma. British Journal of Clinical
Psychology, 33, 71-74.

Shannon, M.P., Lonigan, C.J., Finch, A.J., & Taylor, C.M. (1994). Children exposed to disaster. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 33(1), 80-93.

Slee, P.T. (1995). Peer victimization and its relationship to depression among Australian primary school students.
Personal and Individual Differences, 18(1), 57-62.

Slee, P.T., & Rigby, K. (1993). The relationship of Eysenks personality factors and self-esteem to bully/victim
behavior in Australian school boys. Personal and Individual Differences, 14, 371-373.
BULLYING AT SCHOOL AND HEALTH 207

Smith, P., & Sharp, S. (1994). School bullying: Insights and perspectives. London: Routledge.

Smith, M.B., Canter, W.A., & Robin, A.L. (1987). A path analysis of an adolescent drinking behavior model
derived from problem behavior theory. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 50(2), 128-142.

Steinhausen, H.-C., & Metzke, C.W. (1998). Frequency and correlates of substance use among preadolescents and
adolescents in a Swiss epidemiological study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(3), 387-397.

Terr, L.C. (1991). Childhood traumas: An outline and overview. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148(1), 10-20.

Tibbenheim, A. (1977). Housing and truancy. New Society, 39, 501-502.

Tygart, C. (1989). Public school vandalism: Toward a synthesis of theories and transition to paradigm analysis.
Adolescence, 23(89), 187-200.

Tomlinson, T.M. (1970). Ideological foundations for Negro action: A comparative study of militant and non-
militant views of a Los Angeles riot. Journal of Social Issues, 26, 93-120.

Vermeersch, S., & Fombonne, E. (1997). Le Child Behavior Checklist: Rsultats prliminaires de la standardisa-
tion de la version franaise. Neuropsychiatrie de lEnfance et de lAdolescence, 45(10), 615-620.

Vuchinick, S., Bank, L., & Paterson, G.-R. (1992). Parenting, peers, and the stability of antisocial behavior in
preadolescent boys. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 510-521.

Weaver, A. (2000). Can post-traumatic stress disorder be diagnosed in adolescence without a catastrophic
stressor? A case report. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 5(1), 77-83.

Williams, K., Chambers, M., Logan, S., & Robinson, D. (1996). Association of common health symptoms with
bullying in primary school children. British Medical Journal, 313, 17-19.

Withney, I., & Smith, P.K. (1993). A survey of the nature and extent of bullying in junior/middle and secondary
schools. Educational Research, 35, 3-25.

Wolke, D., Woods, S., Standford, K., & Shulz, H. (2001). Bullying and victimization of primary school children
in England and Germany: prevalence and school factors. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 673-696.

Les agressions entre lves en milieu scolaire constituent un


problme grandissant. Le bullying peut tre dfini comme toutes formes
de violences physiques ou mentales rptes, effectues par un individu
sur une personne qui nest pas capable de se dfendre elle-mme (Roland
& Idsoe, 2001). Trois tudes rvlent partiellement les tenants et les
aboutissants de cette violence. Quils soient victimes ou agresseurs, les
diffrents protagonistes en subissent les consquences. Ltude 1 montre
que les lves simultanment victimes et agresseurs ont lexpression du
concept de soi la plus faible, et ce, dans diffrents domaines. On constate
galement que les victimes manifestent des concepts de soi infrieurs aux
agresseurs, qui eux-mmes, obtiennent des scores infrieurs ceux du
groupe contrle. Ltude 2, conformment nos attentes, montre que les
victimes/agresseurs rapportent les atteintes psychosomatiques les plus
leves. En outre nous pouvons observer un lien positif entre la
manifestation de troubles du comportement et lapparition de troubles
psychosomatiques. Ltude 3, principalement exploratoire, sintresse
limpact traumatique du bullying et lmergence de conduites additives.
Ainsi, les enfants affirmant avoir vcu une (ou des) agression(s) les ayant
particulirement marque(s) manifestent un niveau lev de stress post-
traumatique. Cependant, nous ne trouvons pas de lien entre ce dernier et
le type dagression subie (physique, verbal ou relationnel). En outre, un
rapport de dpendance apparat entre le stress post traumatique et la
consommation de toxiques. Les rsultats de ces tudes suggrent de tenir
compte des diffrents protagonistes du bullying dans toute leur
complexit afin de dfinir avec prcisions les directions emprunter dans
le cadre dune prise en charge.
208 B. HOUBRE, C. TARQUINIO, I. THUILLIER, & E. HERGOTT

Key words: Addictive behavior, Behavioral problems, Health, Post-traumatic stress, School
bullying, Self-concept.

Received: April 2004


Revision received: December 2005

Barbara Houbre. Laboratory of Psychology, University of Metz UFR SHA, Ile du Saulcy, 57000 Metz,
France; E-mail: barbara_houbre@hotmail.com; Web site: www.sha.univ-metz.fr

Current theme of research:


School bullying. Self-schemata. Adaptation and coping. Exclusion.

Most relevant publications in the field of psychology of education:

Houbre, B., Fischer, G.-N., & Tarquinio, C. (2003). Les violences scolaires. In G. Fischer (Ed.), Psychologie des
violences sociales (pp. 77-105). Paris: Dunod.

Cyril Tarquinio. Department of Psychology, University of Metz, UFR SHA, Ile du Saulcy, 57000 Metz,
France; E-mail: tarquinio@univ-metz.fr; Web site: www.univ-metz.fr

Current theme of research:


Self-schemata and health. Therapeutic compliance and HIV. Value systems and coping.

Most relevant publications in the field of psychology of education:

Tarquinio, C., & Fischer, G.N. (2001). Therapeutic compliance methodologies in HIV-Infection treatment: A
comparative study. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 60(3), 136-160.

Tarquinio, C., & Somat, A. (2001). Scholastic achievement, academic self-schemata, and normative clearsightedness.
European Journal of Psychology of Education, XVI(1), 117-129.

Tarquinio, C., Fischer, G.N., Gauchet, A., & Perarnaud, J. (2001). The self-schema and addictive behaviors: Studies of
alcoholic patients. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 60(2), 73-81.

Tarquinio, C., Fischer, G.N., Gauchet, A., Dodeler, V., Grgoire, A., & Romary, B. (2003). Compliance et concept de
soi chez des patients atteints par le VIH: Une approche dynamique du soi. Revue Internationale de Psychologie
Sociale, 16(1), 21-54.

Isabelle Thuillier. University of Metz, 68 rue Sainte Elisabeth, 57100 Thionville, France; E-mail:
picln@procie.fr

Current theme of research:


School bullying and self-schemata.

Most relevant publications in the field of psychology of education:

Emmanuelle Hergott. Centre Educatif et de Formation Professionnelle Charles Thilmont (CEFP), 45


route de Metzvisse, 57310 Gunange, France; E-mail: hergott_emma@hotmail.com

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen