Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Autonomy at All Costs: An

Ethnography of Metacognitive
Self-Assessment and
Self-Management among
Experienced Language Learners
WILLIAM P. RIVERS
Bryn Mawr College and the National
Foreign Language Center at the University of
Maryland
1029 Vermont Avenue, NW, Suite 1000
Washington, DC 20005
Email: wrivers@nflc.org

Research in cognition has shown that expert learners in diverse fields, including chess,
mathematics, physics, and language learning, approach new learning tasks differently than
novice learners. More recent research in neuropsychology makes a strong claim that metacog-
nition is separate from cognition and consists of two types of behavior: self-assessment and
self-management. This article analyzes self-directed language learning behaviors of adult
third-language learners based on qualitative data. The data were gathered in 1993 and 1994
from 11 learners of Georgian and Kazakh at the University of Maryland at College Park. All
learners had 2/2/2 (L/R/S) proficiency in Russian according to the Federal Interagency
Language Roundtable (FILR) scale. Data were analyzed using the Grounded Method for
analyzing qualitative data (Strauss & Corbin, 1990). All learners were found to assess their
progress, learning styles, strategy preferences, and conflicts with teaching styles and with the
behaviors of other learners regularly. Based on these assessments, the majority of learners
made attempts at specific self-directed learning behaviors, focused primarily on changes to
course materials and classroom activities, and targeted specific learning tasks and strategies.

METACOGNITION, EXECUTIVE learning: Individuals can be taught to regulate


FUNCTIONS, AND THE EXPERIENCED their behaviors, and these regulatory activities en-
LANGUAGE LEARNER able self-monitoring and executive control of
ones performance (Bransford, Brown, & Cock-
Research in several disparate fields makes a
ing, 1999, p. xii). One term for metacognition,
strong claim that metacognition is fundamentally
different than cognition. A recent comprehen- derived from research in developmental neurol-
sive review of the literature on educational re- ogy and neuropsychology, is executive functions
search, published by the National Research (Denckla, 1996). Executive functions consist of
Council, summarizes the role of metacognition in two types of metacognition: metacognitive self-as-
sessment, i.e., the ability to assess ones own cog-
nition (Paris, Lipson, & Wixson, 1983; Flavell,
The Modern Language Journal, 85, ii, (2001) Miller, & Miller, 1993) and metacognitive self-
0026-7902/01/279290 $1.50/0 management, i.e., the ability to manage ones
2001 The Modern Language Journal
further cognitive development (Brown & Palin-
280 The Modern Language Journal 85 (2001)
scar, 1982; Flavell, Miller, & Miller, 1993). More- creased linguistic risk taking, a factor tied to gains
over, research in neuropsychology has demon- in speaking proficiency (Pellegrino, 1994, 1996;
strated specific locations for metacognition in the Frank, 1997). The attributes of the good lan-
brain, and metacognitive assessment measures guage learnera personal learning style, a tol-
have come to be used for the differential diagno- erant and outgoing approach to the target lan-
sis of brain trauma and developmental disorders guage, technical know-how about how to tackle
(Denckla, 1996; Taylor, Schatschneider, Petrill, a language, willingness to use the language in
Barry, & Owens, 1996; Dennis, Barnes, Donnelly, real communication, and strategies for testing
Wilkinson, & Humphreys, 1996; Meichenbaum, and revision of hypotheses about the Target Lan-
Burland, Gruson, & Cameron, 1985). guage (Rubin, 1975, pp. 44-48)are charac-
Of self-management and self-assessment, the teristic of the Self-Directed Language Learner.
latter emerges as the more critical skill. Owings, The general notion that the expert in any field
Petersen, Bransford, Morris, and Stein (1980) approaches learning differently than the novice
showed that fifth-graders with better self-monitor- is well established.1 In fields as diverse as chess
ing ability performed better in self-regulated lan- (de Groot, 1965; Chase & Simon, 1973), mathe-
guage learning. They demonstrated that while matics (Brown & Burton, 1978; Lewis, 1981; An-
both of their groups were able to manage their derson, Green, Kline, & Neves, 1981), computer
learning, those without accurate assessments of programming (McKeithen, Reitman, Reuter, &
their knowledge and abilities were less successful Hirtle, 1981; Adelson, 1981), and physics (Chi,
in that management. In a study of the reading Glaser, & Rees, 1983), the expert learner ap-
strategies of second-graders and fourth-graders, proaches the learning task differently than the
Walczyk and Hall (1991) demonstrated that self- novice. Each of these studies showed broad simi-
monitoring emerges before self-management. Fi- larities among expert learners: more use than
nally, Schraw (1994) noted that college students novices of cognitive or metacognitive strategies to
with poor monitoring skills were less able to man- organize input and knowledge, including a gen-
age their learning, and performed worse, than eral tendency to reorganize learning tasks along
good monitors. deeper abstract and conceptual structures and
The parallel between the attributes of success- schemata, rather than along surface structure.
ful autonomous/self-directed learners and ex- The recent comprehensive review of educational
ecutive function (metacognitive self-assessment research by the National Research Council con-
and metacognitive self-management) is clear, if cluded that experts notice features and mean-
one considers either the classic descriptions of ingful patterns of information that are not no-
the autonomous learner as a learner who fixes ticed by novices, experts have acquired a great
objectives, defines the content and program of deal of content knowledge that is organized, and
learning, selects the methods and techniques of their organization reflects a deep understanding
learning, and monitors and evaluates his or her of the student matter, and experts have varying
progress toward his or her objectives (Holec, levels of flexibility in their approaches to new
1981), or the attributes of self-directed learning: situations (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 1999,
Learning builds on experience, is internally mo- p. xiii).
tivated, and is task-and problem-centered, rather Turning to language learning, several studies
than referent-centered (Knowles, 1975; also Bere- reinforce the general findings on expert learners.
iter, 1992, for a more general consideration of Ben Zeev (1977) reported that bilingual children
learning as problem-centered). Self-managed/ demonstrate greater flexibility in the use of learn-
self-directed learning requires the learner to as- ing strategies than monolingual children. Bia-
sess accurately and to manage actively his or her lystok (1992) supports this conclusion: In her
learning goals, behaviors, environment, and out- research on selective attention, she found that
comes. Results of experiments in self-directed bilinguals performed significantly better on tasks
language learning include increased productivity, requiring high levels of selective attention. The
higher motivation, less frustration, and higher bulk of studies on third language acquisition fo-
retention rates, when self-directed language cus on some aspect of phonetic, morphological,
learning is allowed (Ellis, 1994; Bachman, 1964; lexical, and syntactic transfer in closely related or
Holec, 1980, 1987; Dickinson, 1987; Gardner, cognate languages; transfer effects in topologi-
Ginsberg, & Smythe, 1976; Gardner & McIntyre, cally dissimilar or genetically unrelated lan-
1991). Investigations of American college stu- guages;2 and the global effects of prior linguistic
dents studying abroad have also shown links be- experience on universal grammar and parameter
tween self-directed language learning and in- setting (Zobl, 1992; Enomoto, 1994, Klein, 1995)
William P. Rivers 281
or proficiency gain in a third language.3 In a Languages of the Former Soviet Union Project,
study on interference from second language held in the Russian section of the Department of
(Spanish) in third language (English) among Germanic and Slavic Languages and Literatures
multilingual Yaqui Indians in Arizona, Bartlet at the University of Maryland at College Park
(1989) found evidence of broad use of metacog- during the 1993-94 academic year. The author
nitive and communicative strategies in oral dis- was the Program Manager and an instructor in
course and oral narrative. Mhle (1989) exam- the Russian section of the Department of Ger-
ined learning strategies in German multilingual manic and Slavic Languages and Literatures at
university students taking a variety of Indo-Euro- the University of Maryland at College Park. The
pean languages (French, Spanish, and English). students were adult learners, all of whom were
Mhle hypothesized that the narration of a film employed as translators or interpreters, and who
with no overt linguistic information would be in- had a minimum oral proficiency in Russian of 2
fluenced by cognitive processing.4 Mhle found on the Federal Interagency Language Round-
evidence of controlled lexical transfer, again a table (FILR) scale in reading, listening, and
metacognitive strategy. speaking. Their ages ranged from 26 to 64 years;
Ramsay (1980) presented a study of 10 multil- all had a BA in Russian or Russian area studies;
inguals and 10 monolinguals learning Euskera some had additional coursework or the MA in a
(Basque) in self-paced, self-instructional format. variety of fields. None had studied abroad in Rus-
A variety of materials, including vocabulary flash sia or the former Soviet Union.
cards, audiovisual material, and a grammar They were enrolled in courses in one of three
primer were available to the students. Student languages: Georgian, Kazakh, and Kyrgyz. The
portfolios of materials studied were maintained courses met 5 hours per day, 5 days per week. The
throughout the course, providing a record of the Georgian course met for 20 weeks; the Kazakh
materials accessed by each student. An achieve- and Kyrgyz courses met for 37 weeks. In all three
ment test was administered at the end of the courses, the goal was for the participants to reach
course, and the performance of both groups was 2-level proficiency (FILR scale) in reading and
compared. Ramsay discovered that multilinguals listening. The instructors were language peda-
tended to perform far better than monolinguals. gogues from Georgia, Kazakhstan, and Kyrgyzia.
A group of 5 multilinguals and 1 monolingual There were two instructors per course, each typi-
comprised a set of successful learners, who cally teaching one-half of the day, although the
scored more than one standard deviation better schedule varied with workload and activities. A
than the mean score on the achievement test. variety of teaching methodologies were used, in-
Successful learners were characterized by: the use cluding the structural approach in the Georgian,
of more cognitive and metacognitive strategies, Kyrgyz, and Kazakh courses, with the Kazakh
including substantially more verbalization and vo- course especially emphasizing a bottom-up ap-
cal practice; use of mnemonic devices; a more proach to the acquisition of the language; some
positive attitude towards the learning process (an use of the communicative approach, especially in
affective strategy); use of positive affect reinforce- the Georgian course, and to a lesser extent in the
ment; use of more sources of information; and Kazakh and Kyrgyz courses; and the Emotional-
more risk taking and less fear of errors. Metacog- Semantic Method in the Kyrgyz course.5
nitive strategies, thus, are indicated as a primary
difference between the novice and expert Analytical Methodology
learner, across a broad set of abstract systems of
knowledge, and specifically in language, at both The approach taken for this project is a quali-
a very discrete level (the processing of specific tative description of observed phenomena, based
constituent units) and at the discourse level. Wen- on extensive written, self-reported data. Because
den (1999) reviewed the literature on metacogni- there was no control group, no claims of causality
tion in language learning, and drew a similar or efficacy of treatment are made. Instead, I at-
conclusion: good language learners as well as self- tempt to document rigorously and describe cer-
directed language learners exhibit metacognitive tain behaviors and attitudes among a set of expe-
behaviors. rienced language learners in a particular
environment.
THE STUDY The collected data were analyzed by the author
using the Grounded Method for the analysis of
This study is based on open-ended, retrospec- qualitative data, as detailed by Strauss and Corbin
tive survey data collected from the students of the (1990). The Grounded Method requires rigorous
282 The Modern Language Journal 85 (2001)
inductive analysis and verification of a given phe- naire. The Georgian group returned 11 question-
nomenon through a multistage analysis of quali- naires (3 students, 32% response rate), the Kyr-
tative, narrative data. The data were first read by gyz group returned 31 questionnaires (5 stu-
the author without any attempt to categorize dents, 17%), and the Kazakh group returned 48
them. A second reading was performed, again by questionnaires (8 students, 18%). The answers to
the author, and general categories of reported the questions on this survey constitute the second
events were noted. A third reading involved the major component of the corpus.
development of systematic coding and chrono- The results reported here derive from an analy-
logical tracking of events including: learner pro- sis of the Georgian daily and weekly surveys, and
gress self-assessments, learner style assessments, from the Kazakh daily surveys. The Kyrgyz data
learner-teacher style conflicts, learner-learner were abandoned, as it proved impossible to iden-
style conflicts, and autonomy requests. At this tify and, therefore, to track individual respon-
level, the events remain a set of isolated occur- dents from the surveys.
rences, unconnected through time or by other
variables. These second two steps taken together RESULTS
constitute open coding. Coded events were
then re-coded by the author into axial groups of Students in both the Georgian and the Kazakh
similar events and phenomena. At all points in groups exhibited similar behaviors in three gen-
the process, each narrative must be treated as a eral areas: (self-)assessment, learner autonomy,
whole and in context, to the extent made feasible and self-directed language learning. Learners
by researcher notes external to the narratives, by gave ample evidence of their assessment of sev-
the narratives of other students with whom the eral areas: learner-teacher style conflicts (see Ox-
given student may have interacted, and by the ford & Lavine, 1992); learner style conflicts
conditions under which the narratives were writ- within the group, and the effects thereof on the
ten. class; self-assessment of learner style; and individ-
The results presented here represent the major ual learning strategy preferences. All students re-
observed phenomena. The narratives presented peatedly exhibited learner autonomy, in the form
below are presented in full, and are selected as of demands for the modification of some aspect
representative of the observed events and phe- of the courses, including methodology, teacher
nomena. feedback, classroom environment, sequencing,
and activities. Most importantly, various students
Data Collection demonstrated different self-directed language
learning behaviors, directed at the amelioration
Two survey instruments were used to elicit data of the learner-teacher and learner-learner style
on learning behaviors. Initially, students were conflicts, and at the individuals need for learner
given a questionnaire to complete each day. This autonomy. Self-directed learning behaviors in-
one-page questionnaire consisted of a grid that cluded the prioritizing of classroom and home-
students were to use to assess their progress and work assignments, selection of classroom and
the utility of various activities. A single, open- homework tasks, and inclusion of an inde-
ended question invited general comments on the pendent study day into the program. In this sec-
back of the form. The overall student response tion, I will examine each of these three phenom-
rate to the grid portion of the questionnaire was ena with respect to the corpus of survey responses
initially less than 50%. This response rate de- from the Languages of the Former Soviet Union
clined dramatically, and after 3 weeks, the grid Project. I will start with the Georgian course, and
questionnaire was abandoned in favor of a weekly then proceed to the Kazakh course.
questionnaire. The Georgian group (3 students) Learner-teacher style conflicts arise when the
returned 11 daily questionnaires (30%); the Kyr- teaching methodology in a given classroom ill
gyz group returned 71 (5 students, 71% response suits the cognitive style of a particular learner
rate), and the Kazakh group returned 148 (8 (Oxford & Lavine, 1992; Leaver, 1993). In the
students, 93%). In the main, the questionnaires Georgian class, one of three students reported
were not completed, save for the open-ended that the emphasis on translation during the read-
commentary question. The responses to that ing portion of the class was a source of stress and
question form the first component of the corpus tension. The student correctly identified the
of survey responses. source of this stress, namely, that there was a
The second instrument consisted of a weekly, learner-teacher style conflict in those two por-
six-item, open-ended retrospective question- tions of the course. The student writes:
William P. Rivers 283
A week ago Thursday and Friday we had some excuses instead of listening. I know speaking practice
trouble. Wed. we had begun to experience the classic is important, but when one students practice gets in
Soviet-trained teacher w/American students syn- the way of general progress, its too much (Georgian
drome when [Instructor A] expressed irritation that student 2, Weekly survey 1).
we did not know the material well enough, which led
into the classic I read it, I didnt/couldnt memorize Student 1 was also well aware of this particular
it rut (Georgian student 1, Daily survey 6). conflict:

Here, the student refers to the instructors empha- Im global extrovert. The other two are introvert
sis on rote memorizationa common pedagogi- types, and you can see that our different capabilities
cal method in the Soviet Union. This insistence on come out in different activities. Im more creative in
composition, they have a better memory of vocab,
rote memorization, and American students un-
etc. (Georgian student 1, Weekly survey 1).
willingness to do it, is a possible source of conflict
in classes with Soviet-trained instructors and This learner style conflictbetween an extro-
American students with little exposure to Soviet- verted learner and two introverted class-
trained instructors (Leaver & Flank, 1987). It matesgenerated considerable difficulty and
should be noted that the majority of learners had frustration:
little exposure to Soviet-trained instructors, be-
cause none had studied abroad. Any such expo- My only concern is, now that were running short of
sure presumably came during the students under- time, that we cant afford to spend a half hour to an
graduate careers. The same student continues: hour on [Student 1s] experiments in poetry, fairy
tales ancient history any more (Georgian student
I brought in a Georgian art book that related to a 2, Weekly survey 1).
book [Instructor A] showed me Thursday re [sic]
Georgian history. [Instructor A] told me right off the This conflict was not resolved until the depar-
bat [that Instructor A] didnt like the bookwhich ture from the course of Student 1 (the student
was disconcerting . . . as a teacher of Classical lan- left the course for job-related reasons). Both stu-
guages, [Instructor A] may not be used to . . . cogni- dents exhibited a keen awareness of their own
tive, participatory teaching (Georgian student 1, learner styles and learning strategy preferences.
Daily survey 6). Georgian student 1 was aware of a difficulty
Student 2 had a different conflict: with short-term memory and the acquisition of
lexicon, and resultant problems in activities re-
They try to keep us talking, which I wouldnt have quiring the use of short-term memory and re-
considered all that important before, but I can see cently taught lexicon:
how it is activating a lot of vocabulary (Georgian
student 2, Daily survey 4). The hard part comes when we have to translate it [a
newspaper article] on the spot in class . . . I do better
This response is illuminating in that the student at other activities, when my inability to keep up
recognizes her preference for activities other [doesnt interfere with the activity]. They [students 2
than speaking, the emphasis in the course on and 3] hear better/remember vocab better (Geor-
speaking, and some benefit of the non-preferred gian student 1, Weekly survey 1).
activity. This last traitflexibility in learner style
and learning strategy preferencesreappears Student 2 exhibited an awareness of difficulties
throughout the data. with oral activities:
Learner style conflicts within the class cohort
They try to keep us talking, which I wouldnt have
remain a less well investigated area in research on considered all that important before, but I can see
cognitive styles and Second Language Acquisi- how it is activating a lot of vocabulary (Georgian
tion (SLA). Nonetheless, some researchers, nota- student 2, Daily survey 4).
bly Leaver (1993) have recognized the potential
for problems in classrooms with divergent learner Informal consultations with the instructors and
styles and learning strategy preferences among the students tended to confirm the accuracy of
the class cohort.6 All parties recognized the con- these assessments. Student 1 did indeed have
flict: more difficulty in acquiring vocabulary, and Stu-
dent 2 was more reluctant to engage in conversa-
Ive become increasingly aware of the differences in tion.
our personalities learning styles lately (Georgian
One episode in the Georgian course stands as
student 2, Weekly survey 1).
a metaphor for learner autonomy. All students
We waste a lot of time repeating explanations because repeatedly expressed their discomfort with the
[Georgian student 1] was busy talking and making pace of the course and the amount of the mate-
284 The Modern Language Journal 85 (2001)
rial, either to the administration or to the teach- This particular example of self-directed language
ers: learning is the only clear instance occurring in
the corpus of survey responses from the Georgian
We really cant do all the homework theyre asking us
course.
to do (Georgian student 1, Daily survey 3).
Two final points concerning the Georgian
The complaint was made directly to the instruc- course: First, of the three conflicts which appear
tors, an illocutionary first step in learner auton- in the survey responses, two (the learner-teacher
omy. The instructors responded by prioritizing style conflict and the course workload and se-
the homework tasks for the students: quence conflict) were resolved by the learners
and teachers, with minimal intervention from the
It was some help today when they made an effort to administration. The thirdthe learner style con-
prioritize the tasks for us, after we talked it over [my
flict within the class, and an attendant personality
emphasis] (Georgian student 1, Daily survey 3).
clashwas resolved only by the removal of one of
Apparently, the problem recurred, as evidenced the learners, for reasons unrelated to the course.
by the responses to the problem in the later daily The ability to resolve such conflicts within the
and weekly surveys: framework of the classroom, when coupled with
remarks such as the following, indicate that the
At the end of the day, [Instructor B] agreed to cut our learners (and teachers) in this course were flex-
homework tasks back, so we have time to review vo-
ible in their approach to the learning process:
cab, and to do what we can do with better quality . . .
(Georgian student 1, Daily survey 6). I need much more practice in the areas that I like less
and tend to neglect when left to my own devices
We have boiled down all our activities, in class and at
(Georgian student 2, Daily survey 2).
home, to the kind of work we agree we need and want
most (Georgian student 2, Weekly survey 3).
On the other hand, we are getting practice where we
The first citation above addresses an additional are weakest (Georgian student 1, Daily survey 4).
issue: the students self-assessment of the quality
of the work done. The second citation above ad- They try to keep us talking, which I wouldnt have
dresses an issue besides the amount of home- considered all that important before, but I can see
work: course content. Unfortunately for the pur- how it is activating a lot of vocabulary (Georgian
poses of research, these discussions were held in student 2, Daily survey 4).
camera, as it were, between the students and
That flexibility, as seen in the research literature,
teachers. That alone, the fact that the students
is a hallmark of the experienced language learner
appealed directly to the instructors and left the
and the good language learner.
administration out of the loop, testifies to the
The Kazakh group, which consisted of 8 stu-
degree of autonomy exhibited by the students. A
dents, provided a rich set of survey responses
more typical response (as will be seen in the data
containing evidence of: two broad learner-
form the Kazakh group) would have been an
teacher style conflicts; a great deal of information
appeal to the teachers, followed by an appeal to
on the learners self-assessment of their progress,
the program administration.
learner styles, and learning strategy preferences;
Direct evidence of self-directed language learn-
and a group-wide attempt to change the structure
ing appears in the corpus of survey responses
of the course. The Kazakh students seemed less
from the participants in the Georgian course.
aware of the differences in individual learner
Student 1 claimed to have found materials of styles, often using the survey to appeal to the
personal interest (an art book), and this student
administration for a modification which would
was accused by other students of writing poetry
suit the individual making the appeal, even
and fairy tales. These are evidence of self-di-
though the same days surveys might contain re-
rected language learning, insofar as the student
sponses from other learners praising the activity
(Student 1) engaged in activities which were not
which the first respondent wanted removed. In
assigned by the instructors, which reflected the
the corpus of survey responses, the Kazakh group
students own interests (poetry, fairy tales, Geor-
evinces little awareness of this phenomenon.
gian history), and which reflected a learning
The first learner-teacher style conflict arose
strategy preference:
from the drill and kill nature of the instruc-
Im more creative in composition, they have a better tional techniques used, and the reaction of the
memory of vocab, etc. (Georgian student 1, Weekly class cohort to those techniques. The reliance on
survey 1). oral drills caused widespread conflict, with most
William P. Rivers 285
students objecting to the reliance on oral drills ing, and reading for detail. The responses to the
and requesting more written drills: newspaper texts and the approach used in analyz-
ing the texts indicated both apprehension and a
I feel like [Instructor D] is frustrated by our slow pace
general preference for bottom-up processing:7
in doing oral drills and our very slow response to
questions. This may be a cultural difference, Im not The rather lengthy newspaper article is a bit over-
sure. Perhaps they do a great deal more oral drilling whelming initially but if I keep at it, I would think that
in the former Soviet Union, but I have never had to I may learn to decipher the structures (Kazakh stu-
do so much oral drilling without extensive written dent 7, Daily survey 11).
exercise first (Kazakh student 1, Daily survey 6).
We had a long talk with our instructors today and we
We need more written practice, especially with gram- are not going to do mamoth [sic] newspaper articles
mar. We do a lot of oral practice, but it would be anymore for a while until we sort of know the gram-
easier if we did some written work first (Kazakh stu- mar and structure that goes into them (Kazakh stu-
dent 4, Daily survey 6). dent 2, Daily survey 15).
Our instructors seem to be accustomed to oral repe- Again, the conflict was partially resolved by an
tition and to oral exercises, with material we have just instance of the exercise of learner autonomyin
learned, while we are more accustomed to written this case, the talk with the instructors.
exercises, perhaps corrected orally. Some adjustment
Much as the Georgian cohort, the Kazakh stu-
is necessary on both sides (Kazakh student 5, daily
survey 7).
dents actively assessed their learner styles and
their learning strategy preferences. Among the
The resolution to this conflict lay in a direct con- assessments of learner style were several assess-
frontation with the instructors: ments of learner type, according to no particular
taxonomy:
[We] finally got up the courage today to tell our
teacher that we Americans almost never work orally Even though I rate myself as a global learner, it is
in school . . . oral drills are murder on us. Immedi- hard to do that when there are so few good reference
ately everything started getting written out on the texts or primers (Kazakh student 8, Daily survey 6).
board and we got to write down sentences and work
from our papers. It was great! (Kazakh student 1, Here, a student claimed to be a global learner.
Daily survey 8). Others claimed to be analytic learners:
Note as well that the resolution to this conflict Our instructor intends to spend several days (19) on
occurred only when the students expressed the possessive endings alone, which is a good
autonomy, by approaching the instructor and re- idea. . . . Such periodic concentration on certain as-
questing a specific change in the manner of pre- pects of the language is necessary, in my opinion, if
we are to really learn the language (Kazakh student
sentation of new material. However, not all of the
5, Daily survey 6).
students agreed with the new approach:
The fact that we have no textbook (but numerous
[Instructor D writes] sentences, words on the board
global/random handouts) makes it that much harder
it takes a long time to copy it all. By the time I
to study prepare the exercises (Kazakh student 8,
finish, she has already gone on to some new words,
Daily survey 9).
etc., which means I am missing things. Others have
said the same thing (Kazakh student 6, Daily survey Other learners classified themselves as aural or
8). visual learners:
This student was the only one who gave a negative While this [taped dialogues] will be useful, progress
response to the change from purely oral presen- will be slower, at least for me, since my visual memory
tation to a mixture of written and oral presenta- is far superior to my aural memory (Kazakh student
tion. (The others mentioned in the response 1, Daily survey 12).
did not choose to do so in the surveys.) Thus, the
Field-dependence also receives implicit mention:
tentative conclusion is that for the majority of the
students in the Kazakh group, this particular con- reciting words without context is inherently unsatisfy-
flict was resolved in a satisfactory manner. ing (Kazakh student 2, Daily survey 9).
A second conflict arose during the use of a
Our vocabulary is growing nicely, but is organized
top-down approach to newspaper texts. After ap- around phonetic sounds, rather than student matter,
proximately 2 months of instruction, the teachers i.e., words beginning with y rather than words con-
introduced authentic target language newspaper nected with a certain topic. I dont mind this, but
articles, and directed the students to perform a some students are bothered by it (Kazakh student 5,
number of tasks with the articles: scanning, gist- Daily survey 3).
286 The Modern Language Journal 85 (2001)
Although the cited examples also make refer- who intervened and convinced the instructors of
ence to affective behaviors and learning strategy the worth of an independent study day.
preferences, the clear result to be drawn from Other examples of learner autonomy included
them is that this particular cohort of experienced a request for an American-trained Turkic linguist
language learners was aware of their individual to deliver a lecture on Kazakh verbal morphol-
learner styles and learning strategy preferences, ogy:
even where they could not name the style or
Were having some problems understanding the
strategy.
manner and usage of certain verb tenses. Perhaps
The first example of self-assessment of learning [Instructor G] can help (Kazakh student 5, Daily sur-
strategy preferences analyzed abovethe strong vey 11).
and negative reaction to oral presentation, cou-
pled with the universal request for written presen- Students also made requests for dictionaries, text-
tationis a clear example of the students assess- books, and primers. A recurrent request was to
ments of their learning strategy preferences with slow the pace of the course and reduce the
respect to the mode of presentation of new mate- amount of homework. This, too, required inter-
rial. Other preferences expressed included a de- vention form the program staff, in careful con-
sire for repetition: sultation with the instructors. Throughout the
course, the Kazakh cohort, both collectively and
Review, review, review! This is very good for me individually, was quick to express learner auton-
(Kazakh student 1, Daily survey 12). omy.
In terms of learner autonomy, a pattern of re-
Another student expressed the desire for tran-
liance upon administrative intervention to
scripts, to accompany taped dialogues (Kazakh
achieve the particular goals of the cohort became
student 5, Daily survey 13). A third student stated
clear. In general, there were two possible out-
a preference for gisted dialogues with glosses of
comes to any assertion of learner autonomy:
key vocabulary:
either the adoption of the students suggestion or
Give us a loose summary of what is going on, a few of the rejection of it. In the latter case, the students
the key phrases and any new vocabulary, then send us tended to continue pressing their request rather
home to see how we do recovering the whole dia- than seeking alternatives. Some individual stu-
logue ourselves (Kazakh student 1, Daily survey 17). dents did occasionally demonstrate some self-di-
rected language learning behaviors, especially in
At the level of learning strategies, especially at
the latter stages of the course. A source for the
the level of cognitive strategies, this cohort of
relatively low incidence of self-directed language
learners had a clear conception of their own pref-
learning behaviors may have been cultural. The
erences and the interaction of those preferences
two instructors in this course tended initially to
to the course as a whole.
reject any assertion of autonomy; the perceived
The students of the Kazakh group had one
lack of autonomy may have contributed to both
singular, collective assertion of learner autonomy.
the lack of self-directed language learning behav-
Beginning in the first week of class, a request was
iors, and to the students stubborn persistence in
taken up to add independent study time to the
asserting their autonomy. Thus, the lack of auton-
course schedule:
omy for the experienced learner created a feed-
I would suggest that the week be cut back to 4 days a back mechanism, which only served to exacer-
week with one day set aside for catching up on all the bate the overall situation. A tentative conclusion
material. If we had Wednesdays off, we would be to be drawn from the Kazakh daily survey re-
fresher for the remaining class days (Kazakh stu- sponses is that autonomy is a prerequisite for
dent 8, Daily survey 3). self-directed language learning.

I do not wish to have an entire afternoon off but


would welcome taking 1 hour a week having each DISCUSSION
of them take two of us for 1/2 hour each . . . (Kazakh
student 7, Daily survey 4). The experienced language learners in the Lan-
guages of the Former Soviet Union Project at the
This request appeared in the survey responses a University of Maryland all exhibited three com-
total of 24 times in the first 3 weeks of class. All mon types of behaviors: self-assessment of pro-
students are represented at least once in that tally. gress and learner style/learning strategy prefer-
The instructors denied the initial request. An ap- ence issues; learner autonomy; and self-directed
peal was made to the program administrators, language learning. These experienced language
William P. Rivers 287
learners accurately assessed: their learner styles, that the high degree of mutual intelligibility between
any learner-teacher style conflicts, and any Portuguese and Spanish can be used in teaching Portu-
learner style conflicts within the class. guese to students having Spanish as a second language.
These experienced language learners demon- Gribble (1987) created a Bulgarian course for Russian
speakers based on that principle. Townsend followed in
strated a high tendency towards learner auton-
1995 with a Czech course for Russian speakers (Town-
omy, requesting and demanding substantive
send, 1995). Several courses at the Defense Language
changes to every aspect of the course, and espe- Institute Foreign Language Center have retrained
cially to course content and structure. These speakers of one language into another closely related
demonstrations of autonomy were based upon language: Czech L2 speakers in Serbo-Croatian (Corin,
the learners self-assessments of learner style, 1994), French speakers into Haitian Creole, and Rus-
learning strategy preferences, and their progress sian, Polish, and Czech speakers into Serbo-Croatian.
in the language. These experienced learners Thomas (1985, 1988) also reports on extensive transfer
tried to take control of the entire learning pro- from Spanish as a second language into French as a
cess. third language, although this transfer is noted as a
Also, when given the opportunity, these experi- source of error in the target language. Azevedo (1978)
provides a brief catalog of interference from L2 in the
enced language learners used self-directed lan-
same type of learner, while endorsing the promotion of
guage learning strategies to modify the learning
positive transfer. Earlier studies in transfer in Third Lan-
environment and aspects of the learning process, guage Acquisition sought to identify error as an effect of
including: type of input, mode of input, work- prior linguistic experience; Ahukanna, Lund, and Gen-
load, and course structure. tile (1981) note semantic errors in learners of French
These observations deserve fuller investigation for whom Igbo was L1 and English, L2. The latter
and replication in different environments (such trendtransfer errors due to prior linguistic experi-
as typical college classroom instruction) and with encecan be assigned to traditional (contrastive analy-
different types of learners (e.g., adolescents, col- sis) studies of transfer errors in interlanguage from L1
lege students, and study abroad participants). In (Gass, 1979; Gass & Selinker, 1992).
3 Prior linguistic experience surfaced as a predictor of
addition, the effects of cultural dissonance be-
gain among missionaries in Japan (Jacobsen & Imhoof,
tween the students and the teachers cannot be
1974). Jacobsen and Imhoof demonstrated that child-
fully accounted for here; that is, these observa-
hood bilingualism and multilingualism correlated posi-
tions may or may not obtain in a classroom with tively with language gain. Similar correlations surfaced
a teacher trained in the US. All of these questions in studies of undergraduates studying abroad in Russia
suggest further avenues for research. (Brecht, Davidson, and Ginsberg, 1990, 1991, 1993,
The accurate use of metacognitive, affective, 1995; Ginsberg, 1992) and of Foreign Service Institute
and social strategies to control the language intensive language course participants (Ehrman & Ox-
learning process and the learning environment is ford, 1995). Each of the studies cited analysed a data-
the hallmark of self-directed language learning. base of at least 600 students reporting previous linguistic
In order for such learning to occur, learners must experience (proficiency in a nonnative language other
be able to determine accurately what their needs than the target language), and in each study, prior lin-
guistic experience was statistically significant for gain in
are, and they must have the freedom to take ac-
oral proficiency. A Swedish study of the performance of
tion to meet those needs. In the absence of either
more than 2700 immigrant school children in the
accurate self-assessment or genuine autonomy, eighth grade showed mixed results (Mgiste, 1984,
self-directed language learning will not occur. 1986); children in English classes who had a passive
command of their first language, and actively used
Swedish (their second language), performed better
NOTES than either students without prior linguistic experience,
or students who had an active command of only their
1 The term expert is usually reserved for students first language. However, the study did not examine oral
with several thousand hours of experience (there are proficiency, but performance on an achievement test.
2000 working hours in a year). Flower & Hayes (1981) The results of that achievement test may be more indica-
argue that no one, not even a child prodigy, becomes a tive of performance in a formal learning environment
world-class expert without at least 20,000 hours of expe- than of proficiency in the target language.
rience (VanLehn, 1991, pp. 56061). 4 The Pear Film (Chafe, 1980) contains no overt lin-
2 Cognate languagesthose belonging to the same guistic information. The film is not silent per se, but
subfamily of a given language family, e.g. French and there is no dialogue, nor any written language (e.g.,
Spanishtypically afford the learner a similar core signage, captions). The film was designed for experi-
(phonology, morphology, lexicon, and syntax), with sys- ments in narrative production and cognitive processing.
tematic and predictable differences between the lan- 5 The Emotional-Semantic Method incorporates ele-

guages. Jensen (1989) and Jordan (1991) both suggest ments of the Total Physical Response and Silent Way
288 The Modern Language Journal 85 (2001)
methods. See Omaggio Hadley (1993) for a description pirieskoje issledovanije razvitija reevoj kompe-
of the latter two methods. A description of the Emo- tencii v uslovijax inostrannogo jazyka za rubeom.
tional-Semantic Method may be found in Sydykova [Empirical research on the development of spoken
(1989). competence during language study abroad]. In D.
6 Leaver suggests alleviating this potential conflict by Davidson (Ed.), American contributions to the VII in-
distributing students across class sections according to ternational congress of MAPRIAL (pp. 123152).
their learner styles. This would allow the strengths and Washington, DC: American Council of Teachers of
weaknesses of individual students, or individual prefer- Russian.
ences in learning strategies, to complement those of Brecht, R., Davidson, D., & Ginsberg, R. (1991). On
other individual learners in the class. Leavers sugges- evaluating proficiency gain in study abroad envi-
tionthe grouping of students by learner styles to cre- ronments: An empirical study of American stu-
ate a complementary distribution of learner styles dents of Russian (A preliminary analysis of data).
within the classroomwas impractical in a course with In Z. Dabars (Ed.), Selected papers delivered at the
three students and two instructors (Leaver, 1993). NEH symposium in Russian language and culture (pp.
7 This is a possible example of the short-circuit hy-
101130). Washington, DC and Baltimore, MD:
pothesis in action (Clarke, 1980). The National Endowment for the Humanities and
the Center of Russian Language and Culture of
Friends School.
Brecht, R., Davidson, D., & Ginsberg, R. (1993). Predic-
REFERENCES tors of foreign language gain during study abroad (pp.
3766). Washington, DC: The National Foreign
Adelson, B. (1981). Problem solving and the develop- Language Center Occasional Papers.
ment of abstract categories in programming lan- Brecht, R., Davidson, D., & Ginsberg, R. (1995). Predic-
guages. Cognition, 9, 422433. tors of foreign language gain during study abroad.
Ahukanna, J., Lund, N., & Gentile, J. (1981). Inter- and In B. Freed (Ed.), Second language acquisition in a
intra-lingual interference effects in learning a study abroad context (pp. 3766). Amsterdam, Phila-
third language. Modern Language Journal, 65, delphia: J. Benjamins.
281287. Brown, A., & Palinscar, L. (1982). Inducing strategic
Anderson, J., Greeno, J., Kline, P., & Neves, D. (1981). learning from texts by means of informed, self-
Acquisition of problem solving skills. In J. Ander- control training. Topics in Learning and Learning
son (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. Disabilities, 2, 118.
191230). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Asso- Brown, J., & R. Burton. (1978). Diagnostic models for
ciates. procedural bugs in basic mathematical skills. Cog-
Azevedo, M. (1978). Identifying Spanish interference in nitive Science, 2, 155192.
the speech of learners of Portuguese. Modern Lan- Chafe, W. (1980). The pear stories: Cognitive, cultural, and
guage Journal, 66, 1823. linguistic aspects of narrative production. Norwood,
Bachman, J. (1964). Motivation in a task situation as a NJ: Ablex.
function of ability and control over task. Journal of Chase, W., & Simon, H. (1973). Perception in chess.
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 272281. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 5581.
Bartlet, G. (1989). The interaction of multilingual con- Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Rees, E. (1981). Expertise in prob-
straints. In H. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.), Inter- lem solving. In Advances in the psychology of human
lingual processes (pp. 516). Tbingen, Germany: intelligence (Vol. 1.) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
Gunter Narr Verlag. baum Associates.
Ben Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on Clarke, M. (1980). The short circuit hypotheses of ESL
cognitive strategy and cognitive development. readingOr when language competence inter-
Child Development, 48, 10081018. feres with reading performance. Modern Language
Bereiter, C. (1992). Referent-centred and problem-cen- Journal, 64, 203209.
tred knowledge: Elements of an educational epis- Corin, A. (1994). Teaching for proficiency: The conver-
temology. Interchange, 23, 337361. sion principle. A Czech to Serbo-Croatian conver-
Bialystok, E. (1992). Selective attention in cognitive sion course at the Defense Language Institute.
processing: The bilingual edge. In R. Harris (Ed.), ACTR Letter: Newsletter of the American Council of
Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 501513). New Teachers of Russian, 20(1), 15.
York: Elsevier. Denckla, M. (1996). Research on executive function in
Bransford, J., Brown, A., & Cocking, R. (Eds.). (1999). a neurodevelopmental context: Application of
How people learn: Brain, mind, experience, and school. clinical measures. Developmental Neuropsychology,
Committee on Developments in the Science of 12, 515.
Learning, Commission on Behavioral and Social Dennis, M., Barnes, M., Donnelly, R., Wilkinson, M., &
Sciences and Education, National Research Humphreys, R. (1996). Appraising and managing
Council. Washington, DC: National Academy knowledge: Metacognitive skills after childhood
Press. head injury. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12,
Brecht, R., Davidson, D., & Ginsberg, R. (1990). Em- 77103.
William P. Rivers 289
Dickinson, L. (1987). Self-instruction in language learning. Klein, E. (1995.) Second versus third language acquisi-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. tion: Is there a difference? Language Learning, 45,
Ehrman, M., & Oxford, R. (1995). Cognition plus: Cor- 419465.
relates of language learning success. Modern Lan- Knowles, M. (1975). Self-directed learning: A guide for learn-
guage Journal, 79, 6789. ers and teachers. New York: Association Press.
Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition. Leaver, B. (1993). Teaching the whole class. Salinas, CA:
Oxford: Oxford University Press. AGSI Press.
Enomoto, K. (1994). L2 perceptual acquisition: The Leaver, B., & Flank, S. (1987). American-Soviet differences
effect of multilingual linguistic experience on the in teacher perspectives and behavior among teachers of
perception of a less novel contrast. Edinburgh foreign languages. ERIC Digest. Washington, DC:
Working Papers in Applied Linguistics, 5, 1529. ERIC.
Flavell, J., Miller, P., & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive develop- Lewis, C. (1981). Skill in algebra. In J. Anderson (Ed.),
ment (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 85110).
Hall. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. (1981). A cognitive process the- Mgiste, E. (1984). Learning a third language. Journal of
ory of writing. College Composition and Communica- Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 5, 415
tion, 32, 365387. 421.
Frank, V. (1997, March). Potential negative effects of Mgiste, E. (1986). Selected issues in second and third
homestay. Paper presented at The Middle Atlantic language learning. In J. Vaid (Ed.), Language pro-
Conference of the American Association for the cessing in bilinguals: Psycholinguistic and neurophysi-
Advancement of Slavic Studies, Albany, NY. ological perspectives (pp. 97122). Hillsdale, NJ:
Gardner, R., Ginsberg, R., & Smythe, P. (1976). Atti- Lawrence Erlbaum.
tudes and motivation in second language learn- McKeithen, K., Reitman, J., Reuter, H., & Hirtle, S.
ing: Course related changes. The Canadian Modern (1981). Knowledge organization and skill differ-
Languages Review, 3, 243266. ences in computer programmers. Cognitive Psychol-
Gardner, R., & MacIntyre, P. (1991). Motivational vari- ogy, 13, 307325.
ables in second language acquisition. Studies in Meichenbaum, D., Burland, S., Gruson, L., & Cameron,
Second Language Acquisition, 13, 5772. R. (1985). Metacognitive assessment. In S. Yussen
Gass, S. (1979). Language transfer and language univer- (Ed.), The growth of reflection in children (pp. 3-30).
sals. Language Learning, 29, 327344. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (1992). Language transfer in lan- Mhle, D. (1989). Multilingual interaction in foreign
guage learning. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: J. Ben- language production. In H. Dechert & M. Rau-
jamins. pach (Eds.), Interlingual processes (pp. 179194).
Ginsberg, R. (1992). Language gains during study abroad: Tbingen, Germany: Gunter Narr Verlag.
An analysis of the ACTR data. NFLC Working Pa- Naiman, N., Frlich, M., Stern, H., & Todesco, A.
pers. Washington, DC: The National Foreign Lan- (1978). The good language learner. Toronto: The
guage Center. Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, and
Gribble, C. (1987). Reading Bulgarian through Russian. Rowley MA: Newbury House.
Columbus, OH: Slavica. Omaggio Hadley, A. (1993). Teaching language in context
de Groot, A. (1965). Thought and choice in chess. Paris: (2nd ed.). Boston: Heinle.
Mouton. Owings, R., Petersen, G., Bransford, J., Morris, C., &
Holec, H. (1980). Learner training: Meeting needs in Stein, B. (1980). Spontaneous monitoring and
self-directed learning. In H. Altman & C. regulation of learning: A comparison of successful
Vaughan James (Eds.), Foreign language teaching: and less successful fifth graders. Journal of Educa-
Meeting individual needs (pp. 3045). Oxford: Per- tional Psychology, 72, 250256.
gamon. Oxford, R., & Lavine, R. (1992). Teacher-student style
Holec, H. (1981). Autonomy and foreign language learning. wars in the classroom: Research insights and sug-
Oxford: Pergamon. gestions. ADFL Bulletin, 23(2), 3845.
Holec, H. (1987). The learner as manager: Managing Paris, S., Lipson, M., & Wixson, K. (1983). Becoming a
learning or managing to learn. In A. Wenden & J. strategic reader. Contemporary Educational Psychol-
Rubin (Eds.), Learner strategies in language learning. ogy, 8, 293316.
(pp. 145157). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- Pellegrino, V. (1994). Conditions of risk management behav-
Hall. ior among students studying abroad: A qualitative
Jacobsen, M., & Imhoof, M. (1974). Predicting success study. Unpublished masters thesis. Bryn Mawr
in learning a second language. Modern Language College, Bryn Mawr, PA.
Journal, 58, 329336. Pellegrino, V. (1996). At the risk of speaking . . . Risk-
Jensen, J. (1989). On the mutual intelligibility of Span- management in second language use. ACTR Letter,
ish and Portuguese. Hispania, 72, 848852. 22, 13, 613.
Jordan, I. (1991). Portuguese for Spanish speakers: A Ramsay, R. (1980). Language-learning approach styles
case for contrastive analysis. Hispania, 74, of adult multilinguals and successful language
788792. learners. In V. Teller & S. White (Eds.), Studies in
290 The Modern Language Journal 85 (2001)
child language and multilingualism (pp. 7396). New Thomas, J. (1988). The role played by metalinguistic
York: New York Academy of Sciences. awareness in second and third language learning.
Rubin, J. (1975). What the good language learner can Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Develop-
teach us. TESOL Quarterly, 9, 4151. ment, 9, 235246.
Schraw, G. (1994). The effect of metacognitive knowl- Thomas, J. (1992). Metalinguistic awareness in second
edge on local and global monitoring. Contemporary and third language learning. In R. Harris (Ed.),
Educational Psychology, 19, 143154. Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 531545). Am-
Stern, H. (1975). What can we learn from the good sterdam: North-Holland.
foreign language learner? Canadian Modern Lan- Townsend, C. (1995). Teaching the Czech language through
guage Review, 31(4), 304318. Russian: Prepodavanije ekogo jazyka posredstvom
Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative re- russkogo. Columbus, OH: Slavica.
search: Grounded theory procedures and techniques, VanLehn, K. (Ed.). (1991). Architectures for intelligence.
(3rd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sydykova, D. (1989). Emocionalno-smyslovaja metodika. Walczyk, J., & Hall, V. (1991). A developmental study of
[The emotional-semantic method]. Frunze: Kyr- childrens ability to adopt perspectives and find
gyz State University. errors in text. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
Taylor, H., Schatschneider, C., Petrill, S., Barry, C., & 16, 217.
Owens, C. (1996). Executive dysfunction in chil- Wenden, A. (1999). Metacognitive knowledge and lan-
dren with early brain disease: Outcomes post guage learning. Applied Linguistics, 19, 515537.
Haemophilus Influenzae meningitis. Developmental Zobl, H. (1992). Prior linguistic knowledge and the
Neuropsychology, 12, 3551. conservatism of the learning procedure: Gram-
Thomas, J. (1985). The role played by prior linguistic maticality judgments of unilingual and multilin-
experience in second and third language learn- gual learners. In S. Gass & L. Selinker (Eds.),
ing. In R. Hall (Ed.), The Eleventh Linguistic Associa- Language transfer in language learning (pp.
tion of Canada and the United States Forum 1984 (pp. 176196). Amsterdam, Philadelphia: J. Ben-
510520). Columbia, SC: Hornbeam Press. jamins.

Forthcoming in The Modern Language Journal


Richard D. Lambert. Updating the Foreign Language Agenda

Maggie A. Broner & Elaine Tarone. Is It Fun? Language Play in a Fifth-Grade Spanish Immersion
Classroom

Peggy Buckwalter. Repair Sequences in Spanish L2 Dyadic Discourse: A Descriptive Study

Raymond Mougeon & Katherine Rehner. Acquisition of Sociolinguistic Variants by French Immersion
Students: The Case of Restrictive Exprssions, and More

Salim Khaldieh. The Relationship Between Knowledge of Icraab, Lexical Knowledge, and Reading
Comprehension of Non-Native Readers of Arabic

David P. Benseler & Suzanne S. Moore. Doctoral Degrees Granted in Foreign Languages in the United
States: 2000

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen