Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Emotional avoidance and rumination as mediators of the relation


between adult attachment and emotional disclosure q
Angela M. Garrison a,, Jeffrey H. Kahn b, Steven A. Miller c, Eric M. Sauer a
a
Western Michigan University, Department of Counselor Education and Counseling Psychology, United States
b
Illinois State University, Department of Psychology, United States
c
Rosalind Franklin University of Medicine and Science, Department of Psychology, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The authors evaluated emotional avoidance as a mediator of the relation between attachment avoidance
Received 19 May 2014 (i.e., fear of dependency) and emotional disclosure and rumination as a mediator of the relation between
Received in revised form 3 July 2014 attachment anxiety (i.e., fear of rejection) and emotional disclosure. Two operational denitions were
Accepted 4 July 2014
used for each of three variables emotional avoidance, rumination, and emotional disclosure such that
Available online 1 August 2014
hypotheses were tested on generalized self-appraisals and responses to specic emotional events. College
students (N = 116) rst completed generalized self-report measures of attachment, expressive suppres-
Keywords:
sion (i.e., emotional avoidance), rumination, and emotional-disclosure tendencies. Then, during a 7-day
Attachment avoidance
Attachment anxiety
diary study, they provided daily reports of emotional avoidance, rumination, and disclosure concerning
Emotional avoidance the days most unpleasant event. Attachment avoidance was negatively related to disclosure tendencies
Rumination and daily-event disclosure; emotional avoidance was supported as a mediator in the generalized self-
Emotional disclosure report analyses. Attachment anxiety was positively related to both measures of rumination, and daily-
event rumination was positively related to daily-event disclosure, but mediation was not supported in
either analysis. The ndings suggest implications for theories of attachment and emotion regulation.
2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction experience. That is, individuals differ in their tendency to


disclose personally distressing information across situations.
In Western cultures, when people are upset, they often look for Low-disclosing individuals experience poorer well-being than indi-
someone with whom to talk. Such emotional disclosure (i.e., talking viduals who tend to disclose distress, including lower levels of
about ones distress) often increases in concordance with the social support and self-esteem and higher levels of depression
amount of distress experienced, such that emotionally intense and loneliness (see Kahn, Hucke, Bradley, Glinski, & Malak, 2012).
experiences are disclosed more than emotionally trivial experi- Because of these negative outcomes, there is value in examining
ences (Cano, Leong, Williams, May, & Lutz, 2012; Garrison, Kahn, the processes that are associated with problematic levels of
Sauer, & Florczak, 2012). Although disclosure of ones distress is emotional disclosure.
not always adaptive, in general, emotional disclosure tends to be
benecial (Frattaroli, 2006; Kennedy-Moore & Watson, 2001). For
example, emotional disclosure to others can reduce intrusive 1.1. Attachment orientation and emotional disclosure
thoughts (Lepore, Ragan, & Jones, 2000) and reduce the intensity
of the emotion (Zech & Rim, 2005). Attachment theory may provide valuable insight into under-
Despite the advantages of disclosing distress, many individuals standing individual differences in emotional disclosure. Among
do not disclose distress even in the face of an emotionally intense adults, attachment is typically conceptualized along two dimen-
sions (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998): attachment avoidance
(i.e., fear of dependency, need for self-reliance) and attachment
q
Portions of this research were presented at the 120th Annual Convention of the anxiety (i.e., fear of rejection, need for approval). Individuals high
American Psychological Association, Orlando, FL.
Corresponding author. Address: Western Michigan University, Department of in attachment anxiety or avoidance (i.e., those with insecure
Counselor Education and Counseling Psychology, 1903 W. Michigan Ave., Kalamazoo, attachment) have distinct patterns of emotion regulation
MI 49008-5226, United States. (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007), and these patterns of emotion regula-
E-mail address: angela.m.garrison@wmich.edu (A.M. Garrison). tion have theoretical implications for emotional disclosure.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.006
0191-8869/ 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
240 A.M. Garrison et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245

1.1.1. Attachment avoidance Despite the plausibility of this theoretical statement, much of
Attachment avoidance develops when an attachment gure is the empirical research on attachment anxiety and emotional
not available. Because it does no good to engage in attachment disclosure has found a small, negative association (e.g., Pistole,
behaviors, individuals high in attachment avoidance deactivate 1993; Wei et al., 2005), not a positive one as theory would predict.
their distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). For example, when dis- We believe that this divergence between theory and research is a
tressed they may use expressive suppression (Gross & John, 2003) function of the conceptualization of emotional disclosure as a
whereby they inhibit the behavioral expression of distress. stable individual-difference tendency versus as an act in response
Expressive suppression and emotional disclosure, while strongly, to a specic event. For example, Wei et al. measured emotional-
negatively correlated, are not merely opposites (Kahn et al., disclosure tendencies using generalized self-reports and found
2012). Behavioral expression typically occurs as part of the attachment anxiety to be negatively related to emotional disclo-
momentary experience of emotion (Averill, 1997), whereas talking sure. Generalized self-reports likely tap into a lifetime of memories
about an emotion often occurs well after the emotion episode. of being unable to disclose distress because of inconsistent attach-
Thus, expressive suppression may be viewed as the inhibition of ment gures as well as cognitive expectations that disclosure is not
nonverbal expressions of emotion in the moment, whereas always possible. By contrast, Tan, Overall, and Taylor (2012)
emotional disclosure refers to the verbal sharing of emotion at measured the disclosure of specic events that had occurred over
some later point in time. Because individuals high in attachment the past week, and they found a positive association between
avoidance are attempting to avoid distress, they would have the attachment avoidance and disclosure. Similarly, in a 7-day diary
opportunity to engage in expressive suppression before they had study, Garrison et al. (2012) found a positive association between
the opportunity to engage in emotional disclosure. Thus, emotional attachment avoidance and the disclosure of intense emotional
avoidance/suppression is a potential mediating variable in the events that occurred earlier in the day. Thus, understanding the
relation between attachment avoidance and emotional disclosure. association between attachment anxiety and emotional disclosure
Empirical research supports the bivariate associations in this including the potential mediating role of rumination requires
theory-based mediation model. First, there is evidence that two methodological perspectives. The analysis of generalized
avoidantly attached individuals use suppression as an emotion self-reports (which partly assess expectations about disclosure)
regulation strategy (Caldwell & Shaver, 2012; Fraley & Shaver, and the analysis of reports of recent emotional events (which
1997). For example, when Roisman, Tsai, and Chiang (2004) asked assess actual behavior) may show differing associations between
participants to discuss potentially negative childhood experiences, attachment anxiety and emotional disclosure as well as divergent
only the avoidantly attached individuals showed increases in mediating effects. Thus, whereas we posit rumination to serve as
physiological activity that signied effortful behavioral inhibition. a mediator variable in the relation between attachment anxiety
Second, expressive suppression is strongly, negatively associated and emotional disclosure, we believe that the methodology (gener-
with emotional-disclosure tendencies (Kahn et al., 2012). Finally, alized self-reports versus reports of recent events) will, in essence,
a negative association between attachment avoidance and emo- function as a moderator variable.
tional-disclosure tendencies has been supported empirically
(Garrison et al., 2012; Wei, Russell, & Zakalik, 2005). Unfortunately,
1.2. The current study
none of these studies has examined the possible mediating relation
between attachment avoidance and emotional disclosure, as med-
The purpose of the current study was to test (a) emotional
iated by expressive suppression. Doing so was one purpose of this
avoidance (e.g., expressive suppression) as a mediator between
research.
attachment avoidance and emotional disclosure and (b) rumination
as a mediator between attachment anxiety and emotional
disclosure. Given the conceptual differences between emotional-
1.1.2. Attachment anxiety
disclosure tendencies and emotional disclosure as a response to a
Unlike attachment avoidance, the emotion-regulation strategy
specic event, we collected both generalized self-reports (which
involved with attachment anxiety is hyperactivation, that is, the
characterize how respondents appraise their typical experiences)
exaggeration of attachment strategies such as monitoring of rela-
and daily reports over a 7-day period (which characterize what
tionship partners and proximity seeking (Mikulincer & Shaver,
respondents report actually doing on a daily basis). Our hypotheses
2007). This develops because an attachment gure is nearby but
utilizing generalized self-reports were that expressive suppression
is not consistently responsive to the attachment behaviors;
would mediate the negative relation between attachment avoid-
thus, the behaviors are amplied. However, because anxiously
ance and disclosure tendencies and that rumination would mediate
attached individuals fear rejection, they may also engage in
the negative relation between attachment anxiety and disclosure
suppression which, if ineffective, will result in rumination
tendencies. Our hypotheses utilizing daily reports were that, after
(Liverant, Kamholz, Sloan, & Brown, 2011). Indeed, rumination is
accounting for the intensity of the event, emotional avoidance
a common form of hyperactivation found among anxiously
would mediate the negative relation between attachment avoid-
attached individuals (along with other forms of emotion-focused
ance and emotional disclosure, and rumination would mediate
coping; see Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Rumination involves
the positive relation between attachment anxiety and emotional
repetitively thinking about distressing emotional experiences
disclosure. We note that these hypotheses may essentially be
(Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). For example, Saffrey and Ehrenberg
construed as a test of moderated mediation, whereby we expected
(2007) found that individuals high in attachment anxiety experi-
the mediation results to differ between the two methodological
enced increased levels of both general rumination and relation-
perspectives (i.e., methodology is the moderator variable).
ship-specic rumination following a recent relationship breakup.
Consistent with the theory of social sharing, this rumination, in
turn, prompts individuals to share their distress with others 2. Method
(Rim, 2009). Thus, attachment theory and the theory of social
sharing combine to suggest that, when faced with a distressing 2.1. Participants
event, individuals high in attachment anxiety would engage in
heightened rumination which would, in turn, increase their Participants were 116 undergraduate students (73 women, 43
likelihood of engaging in emotional disclosure. men) from the midwestern United States. Most (78%) of the
A.M. Garrison et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245 241

participants identied themselves as European American, 10% when it occurred?). Responses to these items were averaged to
identied as AfricanAmerican, 3% identied as Latino/-a, 3% iden- create a measure of event intensity that ranged from 1 to 5
tied as biracial or multiracial, 2% identied as Asian or Asian (a = .81). Second, disclosure of each days event was assessed with
American, and 4% identied with another racial/ethnic group. Garrison et al.s (2012) three-item measure (e.g., To what degree
The mean age was 19.60 (SD = 3.40) years. have you shared information about this unpleasant event with
someone today, i.e., by talking with someone, texting someone,
2.2. Measures posting an online message, etc.?). The three disclosure items were
averaged to yield a measure of event disclosure (a = .92). Third,
2.2.1. Adult attachment participants completed a six-item measure of emotional avoidance
The 36-item Experiences in Close Relationships Scale (ECRS; (e.g., How hard did you try not to express your emotions related
Brennan et al., 1998) is a self-report measure of adult attachment to this unpleasant event?). Responses to these items were aver-
that ts the two-dimensional conceptualization of adult attach- aged to form a measure of emotional avoidance (a = .76). Finally,
ment as comprising attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety participants completed two items to measure rumination about
(Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007); thus, this instrument was appropriate the event (e.g., How much did you worry about this unpleasant
for our conceptualization of attachment. Participants read each event?). We averaged responses to these two items to yield an
item and rate their level of agreement with the statement using overall measure of rumination (a = .76).
a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The 18-item
Attachment Anxiety subscale represents participants wish for
extreme closeness with others and worry about the potential loss 2.3. Procedure
of relationship partners. The 18-item Attachment Avoidance
subscale reects the level to which participants emotionally dis- Participants met in small groups and provided informed con-
connect from others and feel discomfort with close relationships. sent. They then completed the ECRS, DDI, ERQ, and RRS. Next,
Coefcients alpha for scores in the current study were .91 for instructions were given for how to complete the daily diary portion
anxiety and .95 for avoidance. of the study which began that night. During the diary part of the
study, participants completed the same daily-event measures each
2.2.2. Distress disclosure evening for 7 days. Reminder emails were sent to participants each
The 12-item Distress Disclosure Index (DDI) is a self-report evening.
measure of individuals general tendencies to disclose negative Participants responses to the online questionnaire were used as
emotions and thoughts (Kahn & Hessling, 2001). Participants rate long as the responses were entered between 5 p.m. on the day they
their agreement with each item using a scale ranging from 1 were reporting about and 5 a.m. the following day. Across the 116
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Coefcient alpha in the participants, 80 (69%) completed the daily survey on time for 5 or
present study was .94. more days, 26 (22%) completed the daily survey on time for 3 or
4 days, and 10 completed the survey on time for 2 or fewer days.
2.2.3. Expressive suppression To provide an acceptable number of daily events per participant,
The 10-item Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & we only analyzed data for the 106 participants who provided at
John, 2003) is a self-report measure of the emotion-regulation least three daily reports for the daily-report analyses, but we
strategies of suppression and cognitive reappraisal. Cognitive included all 116 participants in the generalized self-report
reappraisal was not part of any of our hypotheses; thus, only the analyses.
4-item Suppression subscale was analyzed. Participants rate their
agreement with each item using a scale ranging from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The Suppression scale measures 3. Results
the extent to which individuals actively constrain their expression
of emotions. In the current study, coefcient alpha for suppression 3.1. Preliminary analyses
scores was .71.
Correlations, means, and standard deviations among the vari-
2.2.4. Rumination ables are displayed in Table 1. Correlations among daily reports
The 10-item Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS) is a self-report were based on all 570 daily reports regardless of whose report it
measure of rumination (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, was. Event disclosure was positively correlated with the degree
2003). Participants are instructed to rate each item based on to which the participant ruminated about the event but was not
whether they almost never, sometimes, often, or almost always think associated with the degree to which emotions concerning the
or act in the way described by the item. We only analyzed the event were avoided. Correlations among generalized self-reports
5-item Brooding subscale in this study; this subscale represents a indicated that attachment avoidance was associated with lower
tendency to reect on situations repeatedly without accompanying emotional-disclosure tendencies and greater expressive suppres-
action. Coefcient alpha for scores on this subscale in the current sion. Attachment anxiety was not signicantly correlated with
study was .73. emotional-disclosure tendencies but was associated with greater
rumination. We also aggregated daily reports within-person and
2.2.5. Daily-event measures correlated those aggregated daily reports with generalized
Each night for 7 nights participants were asked to think about self-reports (see dashed box on Table 1). Aggregated daily reports
the most signicant unpleasant event that you personally of disclosure, emotional avoidance, and rumination were positively
experienced since the time you woke up, and provide a very brief associated with generalized self-reports of disclosure tendencies,
description of it. Participants then completed four brief measures expressive suppression, and rumination, respectively, thus
using a scale ranging from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 5 supporting the validity of the daily reports. Finally, although not
(extremely). These four measures are described below. shown on Table 1, the number of daily reports submitted was
First, the emotional intensity of each days event was assessed not signicantly correlated with either disclosure tendencies,
with two items from Garrison et al.s (2012) study (e.g., How r = .11, p = .25, nor with aggregated daily disclosure, r = .04,
intense was your emotional reaction to the unpleasant event right p = .65.
242 A.M. Garrison et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245

Table 1
Correlations among daily reports and generalized self-reports.

Note: Daily reports variables are at the level of the daily event uncorrected for nesting (N = 570). Generalized self-reports are at the level of the person (N = 116). Correlations
between daily reports and generalized self-reports (indicated in the dashed box) used aggregated daily reports and are based on N = 106. p < .05, p < .01, p < .001.

3.2. Generalized self-report analyses 2, and the mediator variables (event avoidance and event rumina-
tion) and the dependent variable (event disclosure) were at level 1.
We estimated a single path model with attachment avoidance We therefore estimated a multilevel mediation model using Mplus
and attachment anxiety as independent variables, expressive 6.11 (Muthn & Muthn, 1998-2010). We used Preacher, Zyphur,
suppression and brooding rumination as mediator variables, and and Zhangs (2010) multilevel structural equation modeling
emotional disclosure tendencies as the dependent variable. Attach- (MSEM) approach which partitions paths into between-person
ment anxiety and avoidance were specied to be correlated with and within-person components. Because attachment was mea-
each other, and the two mediator variables were specied to be sured at level 2, the test of mediation can only occur at the
correlated with each other. Given the potential for gender affecting between-person level. Thus, the mediation analyses only incorpo-
the relations among variables, we rst estimated this model with rated the between-person effects. Gender was not correlated with
gender as a covariate of the two mediator variables and the any of the daily reports, so it was not included in the analyses.
dependent variable, but the pattern of statistical signicance was In this model (see Fig. 2) the path coefcients between the
no different from when we did not include gender in the model. mediators and the dependent variable were specied as random,
Thus, for simplicity, we report a model without gender included. meaning that these paths were allowed to vary across participants.
This model provided a good t to the data, v2 (2, N = 116) = 4.14, We also accounted for the intensity of the days emotional event in
CFI = .98, RMSEA = .10 (90% C.I. = .00.23), SRMR = .05. As Fig. 1 the prediction of disclosure of these specic emotional events.
illustrates, attachment avoidance was associated with higher Because the predictive effects of emotional avoidance and rumina-
expressive suppression, and expressive suppression was, in turn, tion on emotional disclosure might vary as a function of the
associated with lower disclosure tendencies. Although attachment emotional intensity of the event, we examined whether event
avoidance and disclosure tendencies had a zero-order correlation intensity interacted with either emotional avoidance or rumina-
of r = .38, while controlling for expressive suppression (as well tion when predicting disclosure. Neither the interaction with event
as other variables in the model) the standardized path coefcient avoidance, p = .89, nor the interaction with event rumination,
for attachment avoidance was .14 and non-signicant, p = .06. p = .45, was signicant. Thus, we specied intensity as a covariate
We tested the signicance of the indirect effect using 10,000 but not as a moderator variable.
bootstrap samples to generate 95% condence intervals around Attachment avoidance predicted higher emotional avoidance of
the unstandardized indirect effect of 0.11. The condence interval the event, meaning that individuals with relatively higher attach-
(C.I.) ranged from .18 to .06; thus, the indirect effect was statis- ment avoidance tended to avoid their emotions in response to
tically signicant, thus suggesting mediation. The correlation unpleasant events over the course of the week. At the within-
between expressive suppression and rumination was r = .19, person level, higher avoidance of the event predicted lower
p = .04, yet xing the correlation between the mediator variables disclosure of the event, meaning that when an individual avoided
to 0 did not change the pattern of the mediation results. emotions concerning an event he or she was less likely to talk
On the attachment-anxiety side of the model, attachment anx- about that event. In Preacher et al.s (2010) MSEM approach, there
iety was positively predictive of brooding rumination, yet brooding is also a between-person component to the relation between the
rumination was not related to disclosure tendencies, p = .33. The mediator (emotional avoidance, in this case) and dependent
bootstrapped unstandardized indirect effect was .02 (95% variable (see Table 2); this coefcient was also signicant but
C.I. = .07, .02), indicating a non-signicant indirect effect. positive, suggesting that participants who tend to avoid emotions
surrounding events also tend to talk about these events. The
3.3. Daily-event analyses indirect effect between attachment avoidance and event disclosure
at the between-person level, however, was just below the thresh-
For daily reports, we had multilevel data. The independent vari- old of statistical signicance, p = .06. Thus, despite a between-
ables (attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety) were at level person association between attachment anxiety and the emotional
A.M. Garrison et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245 243

avoidance of daily events, and within-person and between-person Caldwell & Shaver, 2012) and emotional suppression with lower
associations between emotional avoidance and disclosure of daily levels of disclosure (e.g., Kahn et al., 2012). Of interest was a signif-
events, the full mediation hypothesis for attachment avoidance icant positive between-person effect between emotional avoidance
and emotional disclosure was not supported. and event disclosure. Thus, people who tended to avoid the emo-
The other half of the model addressed whether rumination tions surrounding the weeks events also tended to disclose their
mediates the relation between attachment anxiety and event emotions regarding the weeks events. This highlights the idea that
disclosure. Attachment anxiety was positively predictive of even though individuals may rely on particular coping strategies
rumination, and rumination was associated with greater event more often than others, over the course of a week they might make
disclosure; this was true at both the between-person and use of multiple strategies in an effort to effectively manage their
within-person level (see Table 2). As was the case for attachment distress. This pattern of results also underscores the importance
avoidance, the indirect effect between attachment anxiety and of using both between- and within-person research approaches.
event disclosure was not signicant at the .05 level, p = .06. Thus, Despite the signicant bivariate relations, the analysis of daily
despite bivariate relations being in the predicted directions, rumi- reports failed to support the mediation hypothesis for attachment
nation did not mediate the relation between attachment anxiety avoidance. This was not the case in the analysis of generalized
and the daily disclosure of emotional events. self-reports, suggesting that peoples generalized appraisals of
their emotional-disclosure tendencies (yet not their daily disclo-
sure behaviors) might partly be a function of attachment
4. Discussion avoidance. Specically, in addition to measuring a history of
disclosure behaviors, generalized self-reports of emotional-
The purpose of this study was to explore the relation between disclosure tendencies may tap into avoidant individuals desire
attachment and emotional disclosure by investigating emotional for interpersonal distance.
avoidance and rumination as potential mediating variables. In The other half of our analyses focused on understanding the
order to obtain a methodologically comprehensive test of these relation between attachment anxiety and emotional disclosure.
relationships, we measured mediator variables and emotional dis- Although the hyperactivation of emotions inherent in attachment
closure both as generalized self-reports and as reports of daily anxiety (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007) should lead anxiously
events. This appears to be the rst time these two approaches have attached individuals to disclose distress, our analyses did not
been used together to understand potential mediating variables in support this hypothesis. Instead, we found a negative (but non-
the relation between attachment orientations and emotional signicant) zero-order correlation between attachment anxiety
disclosure. and disclosure tendencies, a nding that has now appeared multi-
Based on generalized self-reports, individuals with higher levels ple times in the literature (see Kahn et al., 2012). The internal
of attachment avoidance exhibited less of a tendency to share their working models of others held by anxiously attached individuals
distress with others than individuals lower in attachment avoid- are that other people will not be reliable attachment gures; thus,
ance. This nding is not novel (e.g., Garrison et al., 2012; Wei it does not pay to disclose distress. Thus, generalized self-reports of
et al., 2005), but we extended this literature by nding that expres- emotional-disclosure tendencies partly reect these relational
sive suppression mediated the relation between attachment avoid- schemas (Pierce, Baldwin, & Lydon, 1997). It may also be that
ance and emotional disclosure. This is consistent with attachment anxiously attached individuals fear that others will perceive them
theory, as avoidantly attached individuals seek ways to diminish as being unable to cope with lifes demands; others might choose
their experiences of distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). Thus, if to leave the relationship if that were the case. If so, they may
avoidantly attached individuals are suppressing their emotions, inhibit the disclosure of distress as a means to appear emotionally
they are unlikely to share their distress with others. stable. This is speculative, however; integrating variables such as
Analyses with daily reports supported some aspects of need for approval and social desirability into this model might be
attachment and emotion-regulation theories. We found a positive valuable in future work.
between-person relation between attachment avoidance and emo-
tional avoidance of unpleasant events, and we found the expected
negative within-person relation between emotional avoidance and
disclosure. These bivariate relations replicate the generalized
self-report analyses as well as previous research linking attach-
ment avoidance with higher levels of emotional suppression (e.g.,

Fig. 2. Daily-reports model depicting unstandardized paths between attachment


and emotional disclosure as mediated by emotional avoidance and rumination. The
Fig. 1. Generalized self-report model depicting hypothesized indirect relations circles on the mediator-to-dependent-variable paths indicate that these paths were
between attachment and disclosure tendencies as mediated by expressive sup- specied as random effects; values of the coefcients in Fig. 2 reect the
pression and brooding rumination. p < .05; p < .01; p < .001. unstandardized within-person effect for these paths. p < .05; p < .01; p < .001.
244 A.M. Garrison et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245

Table 2
Unstandardized between- and within-person paths between mediator and event disclosure and indirect effects from the daily reports analyses.

Mediator variable Level of effect Mediator to event-disclosure path Indirect effect


Estimate SE Estimate SE
Emotional avoidancea Between 1.94*** 0.53
Within 0.38*** 0.09 0.24 0.13
b
Rumination Between 0.58** 0.20
Within 0.20*** 0.05 0.10 0.06

Note: Dependent variable was event disclosure. Within estimates are xed effects (i.e., averaged across all participants). Dashes indicate the effect was not estimated.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.
a
Mediator of relation between attachment avoidance and event disclosure.
b
Mediator of relation between attachment anxiety and event disclosure.

At the level of daily events, a somewhat different pattern (Gross & Levenson, 1997), so expanding the domain of emotions
emerged. Attachment anxiety was positively related to rumination, in disclosure research would be of interest.
as theory would suggest (e.g., Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007).
Rumination about daily emotional events was also positively
4.2. Implications for research and theory
predictive of event disclosure, such that thinking about events over
and over was associated with talking about those events. Never-
Our ndings suggest the importance of incorporating attach-
theless, mediation was not strong enough to be statistically
ment into our understanding of disclosure behaviors. Specically,
signicant at the .05 level. Thus, it is best to be conservative and
attachment avoidance clearly seems to be related to lower levels
consider these relations among attachment anxiety, rumination,
of emotional-disclosure tendencies because these individuals tend
and event disclosure only at the bivariate (versus multivariate)
to suppress their emotions, yet more theoretical and empirical
level.
work is needed on understanding when and why attachment anx-
iety is related to disclosure. For example, how does disclosure to
different relationship targets vary as a function of attachment anx-
4.1. Limitations and ideas for future research iety? How does the content of disclosure affect how anxiously
attached individuals engage in disclosure? To what degree do
Our sample primarily consisted of European American individ- methodological issues affect conclusions about attachment anxiety
uals; therefore, readers should not generalize our ndings to other and disclosure? Addressing issues such as these would represent
groups of individuals. Future research could focus on understand- important points of departure from this research.
ing more about how, when, and why individuals from other Furthermore, these ndings emphasize the importance of build-
cultural groups use emotional disclosure as a potential means for ing on theory related to individual differences in the normative use
managing their distress. Similarly, it is important to note that our of daily and generalized disclosure. Our results indicate there are
participants were all college students. In future studies, research- meaningful individual differences that exist with regards to disclo-
ers may wish to explore whether the supported relations between sure behavior. Moreover, these disclosure patterns differ based on
attachment and emotional disclosure are found when non-student interpersonal and affective variables that are of interest to person-
samples are used. ality researchers, such as individuals attachment orientations and
Additionally, all measures in this study were based on partici- their emotion-regulation patterns. Thus, this research may contrib-
pant self-reports which may have been a source of bias. Whereas ute to a more complete understanding of the complex relationships
the daily measures in our study may have decreased retrospective among interpersonal factors, emotion regulation, and disclosure.
bias, participants were still providing reports at the end of each
day. Future researchers may wish to gather responses from References
participants more than once per day to capture events as they
happen. Furthermore, although we based portions of our Averill, J. R. (1997). The emotions: An integrative approach. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson,
daily-event measures on Garrison et al.s (2012) measures, we also & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 513541). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-012134645-4/50022-6.
created several new items specically for this study. Thus, the Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
validity of our daily questionnaire could not be assessed fully. romantic attachment: An integrative overview. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
Finally, other variables are worthy of including into models of (Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 4676). New York: Guilford.
Caldwell, J. G., & Shaver, P. R. (2012). Exploring the cognitive-emotional pathways
emotional disclosure. For example, we only focused on two poten- between adult attachment and ego-resiliency. Individual Differences Research,
tial mediators. Although the chosen mediators were based on 10, 141152.
theory, additional mediators may exist. For instance, self-efcacy Cano, A., Leong, L. E. M., Williams, A. M., May, D. K. K., & Lutz, J. R. (2012). Correlates
and consequences of the disclosure of pain-related distress to ones spouse.
for coping, perceived level of social support, and outcome expecta- Pain, 153, 24412447. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2012.08.015.
tions regarding disclosure may impact disclosure behavior. Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (1997). Adult attachment and the suppression of
Researchers may also wish to deepen our understanding of the dis- unwanted thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 10801091.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.5.1080.
closure behavior of insecurely attached individuals by asking about Frattaroli, J. (2006). Experimental disclosure and its moderators: A meta-analysis.
the reasoning behind their choices to disclose or not. Such informa- Psychological Bulletin, 132, 823865. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-
tion could be obtained in a mixed-method or qualitative study. In 2909.132.6.823.
Garrison, A. M., Kahn, J. H., Sauer, E. M., & Florczak, M. A. (2012). Disentangling the
addition, other personality-related variables could be examined as
effects of depression symptoms and adult attachment on emotional disclosure.
potential predictors of emotional disclosure; one candidate would Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 230239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
be emotional intelligence. On the outcome side, relatively little a0026132.
work has been done on the disclosure of positive emotions. Exper- Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation
processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of
imental research has shown that suppressing positive emotions Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
leads to a decreased subjective experience of those emotions 3514.85.2.348.
A.M. Garrison et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 70 (2014) 239245 245

Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting Pistole, M. C. (1993). Attachment relationships: Self-disclosure and trust. Journal of
negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95103. Mental Health Counseling, 15, 94106.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.106.1.95. Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM framework
Kahn, J. H., & Hessling, R. M. (2001). Measuring the tendency to conceal versus for assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15, 209233. http://
disclose psychological distress. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20, dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020141.
4165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.20.1.41.22254. Rim, B. (2009). Emotion elicits the social sharing of emotion: Theory and
Kahn, J. H., Hucke, B. E., Bradley, A. M., Glinski, A. J., & Malak, B. L. (2012). The empirical review. Emotion Review, 1, 6085. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Distress Disclosure Index: A research review and multitrait-multimethod 1754073908097189.
examination. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 139149. http://dx.doi.org/ Roisman, G. I., Tsai, J. L., & Chiang, K. S. (2004). The emotional integration of
10.1037/a0025716. childhood experience: Physiological, facial expressive, and self-reported
Kennedy-Moore, E., & Watson, J. C. (2001). How and when does emotional emotional response during the Adult Attachment Interview. Developmental
expression help? Review of General Psychology, 5, 187212. http://dx.doi.org/ Psychology, 40, 776789. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.776.
10.1037/1089-2680.5.3.187. Saffrey, C., & Ehrenberg, M. (2007). When thinking hurts: Attachment, rumination,
Lepore, S. J., Ragan, J. D., & Jones, S. (2000). Talking facilitates cognitive-emotional and post-relationship adjustment. Personal Relationships, 14, 351368. http://
processes of adaptation to an acute stressor. Journal of Personality and Social dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00160.x.
Psychology, 78, 499508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.499. Tan, R., Overall, N. C., & Taylor, J. K. (2012). Lets talk about us: Attachment,
Liverant, G. I., Kamholz, B. W., Sloan, D. M., & Brown, T. A. (2011). Rumination in relationship-focused disclosure, and relationship quality. Personal Relationships,
clinical depression: A type of emotional suppression? Cognitive Therapy and 19, 521534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.2011.01383.x.
Research, 35, 253265. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10608-010-9304-4. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered:
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247259. http://
and change. New York: Guilford. dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910315561.
Muthn, L. K., & Muthn, B. O. (19982010). Mplus users guide (6th ed.). Los Angeles, Wei, M., Russell, D. W., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, social self-efcacy,
CA: Muthn & Muthn. self-disclosure, loneliness, and subsequent depression for freshman college
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and students: A longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 602614.
mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.602.
504511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10021-843X.109.3.504. Zech, E., & Rim, B. (2005). Is talking about an emotional experience helpful? Effects
Pierce, T., Baldwin, M. W., & Lydon, J. E. (1997). A relational schema approach to on emotional recovery and perceived benets. Clinical Psychology and
social support. In G. R. Pierce, B. Lakey, I. G. Sarason, & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Psychotherapy, 12, 270287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cpp.460.
Sourcebook of social support and personality (pp. 1947). New York: Plenum
Press.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen