Sie sind auf Seite 1von 28

Psychological Review Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association

2005, Vol. 112, No. 4, 951978 0033-295X/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.951

Why Most People Disapprove of Me: Experience Sampling


in Impression Formation
Jerker Denrell
Stanford University

Individuals are typically more likely to continue to interact with people if they have a positive impression of
them. This article shows how this sequential sampling feature of impression formation can explain several
biases in impression formation. The underlying mechanism is the sample bias generated when the probability
of interaction depends on current impressions. Because negative experiences decrease the probability of
interaction, negative initial impressions are more stable than positive impressions. Negative initial impres-
sions, however, are more likely to change for individuals who are frequently exposed to others. As a result,
systematic differences in interaction patterns, due to social similarity or proximity, will produce systematic
differences in impressions. This mechanism suggests an alternative explanation of several regularities in
impression formation, including a negativity bias in impressions of outgroup members, systematic differences
in performance evaluations, and more positive evaluations of proximate others.

Keywords: impression formation, sampling, judgment bias, stereotypes

Why do evaluations and impressions depend systematically on Suppose, for example, that a colleague in your field asks about
gender, ethnicity, proximity, and attractiveness? Much research in the social skills of a faculty member at your school. Approaching
impression formation has attributed such systematic biases to flaws in life systematically, you begin by listing all your interactions with
information processing and hypothesis testing and to motivational this individual and evaluating his or her social skills during these
influences. Prior expectations, including stereotypes, confirmation interactions. Being statistically trained, you take the average and
bias and self-fulfilling prophecies, as well as the desire to maintain a report this as your best estimate. Although this procedure seems
positive social identity have been argued to distort the perception, fair, it will often result in systematically biased estimates. More-
interpretation, and recall of behavior of individuals and groups (All- over, the estimate will also vary systematically depending on the
port, 1954; Asch, 1946; Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Hilton & von Hippel, type of individual that you are evaluating. In particular, even if you
1996; Rosenthal & Jacobsen, 1968; Snyder & Stukas, 1999; Snyder & accurately register and recollect all your interactions, there is a risk
Swann, 1978; Tajfel, Flament, Billig, & Bundy, 1971; Word, Zanna, of reporting systematically lower estimates for members of minor-
& Cooper, 1974). ity groups to which you do not belong.
This article argues that there is an important alternative source The reason is that the above procedure ignores a major feature
of bias in impression formation. Even if impressions would be of experience sampling (Denrell & March, 2001; Eiser, Fazio,
based on all available evidence, and the evidence would be accu- Stafford, & Prescott, 2003; Gilovich, 1991; T. M. Newcomb,
rately interpreted, impressions might nevertheless be biased. The 1947). The experience of interacting with an individual with seem-
reason is the sample selection bias introduced by experience ingly low social skills decreases the likelihood of future interac-
sampling.
tions, whereas the experience of interacting with an individual with
seemingly high social skills increases the likelihood of future
This article has benefited from prior joint work with James March. I interactions. As a result of such experience sampling, false nega-
thank Chip Heath, James March, and Dale Miller for their extensive tive initial impressions are unlikely to be corrected. A false posi-
comments and help. I am also grateful for comments and help from Bill tive initial impression, however, will lead to further sampling,
Barnett, Marilynn Brewer, Lawrence Brown, Glenn Carroll, Christian
which might disconfirm the initial belief. This asymmetry implies
Czernich, Hillary Elfenbein, Christina Fang, Deborah Gruenfeld, Mike
Hannan, Richard Harrison, Rod Kramer, Dan Levinthal, Steve Lippman, that even if impressions are based on all available observations,
Anders Martin-Lof, Maggie Neal, Jeffrey Pfeffer, Patrick Regner, Lee and each observation is accurately interpreted and remembered,
Ross, Zur Shapira, Udo Zander, and participants in seminars at the Haas impressions may nevertheless be biased (Denrell & March, 2001;
School of Business at the University of California, Berkeley, Harvard Business Eiser et al., 2003; Fazio, Eiser, & Shook, 2004). In fact, even if
School Scandinavian Consortium for Organizational Research, Stanford each experience is equally likely to be positive or negative, a
Graduate School of Business, the Department of Psychology at Stanford majority of individuals, when asked about their impression, will
University, Stockholm School of Economics, and the Wharton School of report that their impression is negative.
Management. Erica Carranza, Deborah Prentice, Jeff Larsen, Noah Gans,
This article shows how this sequential sampling feature of
George Knox, Rachel Croson, James Shah, Paige Brazy, and Tory Higgins
generously allowed me to use their data. All errors are my own. impression formation can explain a variety of biases in impression
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jerker formation. I demonstrate that experience sampling implies that
Denrell, Graduate School of Business, Stanford University, 518 Memorial individuals with similar characteristics and skill levels may be
Way, Stanford, CA 94305-5015. E-mail: denrell@gsb.stanford.edu evaluated differently as a result of differences in social ties and
951
952 DENRELL

interactions. The implication is that experience sampling can pro- heuristics or motivational influences. Sample bias may also be due
vide an alternative explanation for several systematic biases in to unrepresentative samples, based on the available information
impression formation. In this explanation, biased evaluations do (Combs & Slovic, 1979; Dawes, 1993; Denrell, 2003; Fiedler,
not result from the influence of prior expectations on the interpre- 2000) or based on the self (Krueger, 1996; Krueger & Zeiger,
tation of subsequent observations, biased hypothesis testing and 1993; Ross, Greene, & House, 1977). Mere differences in sample
self-fulfilling prophecies, or motivational influences. Rather, bi- sizes can also produce systematic differences in evaluations of
ased evaluations are the result of systematic differences in social groups in situations in which information aggregation has a sys-
interactions, which determine the information to which evaluators tematic effect on judgments (Fiedler, 1996, 2000; Kashima et al.,
have access. Individuals whom evaluators are likely to meet, 2000; Van Rooy et al., 2003). Finally, how much and what type of
irrespective of their opinions of them, will have more chances to information people search for and how they process it has been
correct a false negative impression. In contrast, individuals whom shown to depend on the valence of their current impression
evaluators are less likely to meet are less likely to get a second (Ybarra, 2002). Positive impressions lead to more testing and more
chance. For such individuals, false-negative first impressions are critical information processing than negative impressions do, be-
more likely to be stable. cause negative behaviors are assumed to be more diagnostic.
Such a model of experience sampling cannot, and does not try In social interactions, however, there is an important source of
to, provide an alternative explanation of the findings of experi- sample bias that has remained largely unexplored. In many im-
ments in which the information available to subjects is fixed and pression formation contexts, the information available depends on
exogenous. A long line of work has established that cognitive and decisions about social interactions. Unless an individual decides to
perceptual biases can influence impression formation in such sit- interact with another individual, no additional information may be
uations. This article suggests a complementary explanation of available (T. M. Newcomb, 1947; Towles-Schwen & Fazio, 2003).
systematic biases in impression formation in many real-life con- Decisions about interactions, however, are influenced by the va-
texts in which information is not automatically provided but has to lence of impressions formed on the basis of previous interactions.
be actively sampled. In particular, I demonstrate that pragmatic In particular, one of the more common ideas about social interac-
constraints in sampling (Einhorn & Hogarth, 1978; Friedrich, tions is that future interactions are more likely if previous inter-
1993) may contribute to what researchers typically interpret as actions suggest that future interactions will be rewarding or plea-
cognitiveperceptual biases. In line with recent work on sampling surable (Altman & Taylor, 1973; Berscheid, 1985; Homans, 1961;
(Fiedler, 1996, 2000; Kashima, Woolcock, & Kashima, 2000; Van Lott & Lott, 1972; Montoya & Horton, 2004; T. M. Newcomb,
Rooy et al., 2003), this article suggests that cognitive and percep- 1953; Sunnafrank, 1986; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). If good out-
tual biases are not necessary to explain systematic biases in im- comes are experienced in initial contacts or if these contacts lead
pression formation and judgment. the persons to anticipate good outcomes in the future, the interac-
tion is likely to be repeated (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959, p. 20).
Sampling in Impression Formation Such a tendency has been demonstrated in a variety of experimen-
tal and field studies. Experiments manipulating rewards during inter-
Much research in impression formation has examined how actions have shown that willingness to continue the interaction is
impressions are influenced by cognitive heuristics in information higher for individuals who have experienced rewarding interactions
processing and perceptual biases. By manipulating the order of (Chambliss, 1966; D. A. Taylor, Altman, & Sorrentino, 1969, p. 333).
observation or by inducing different expectations before the infor- Ratings of attraction, based on initial information about a future
mation is presented, studies have demonstrated how information interaction partner, also predict willingness to continue the interaction
order and expectations influence the interpretation, recall, and (Carranza, Prentice, & Larsen, 2004; Montoya & Horton, 2004, p.
integration of information (Asch, 1946; Jussim & Eccles, 1995; 700; M. S. Schwartz, 1966, cited in Byrne, 1971, p. 231). For
Lord, Ross, & Lepper, 1979; Rosenthal & Jacobsen, 1968; Snyder, example, in the study by Carranza et al. (2004), 66 subjects each
Campbell, & Preston, 1982; Snyder & Stukas, 1999; Snyder & listened to statements by 60 individuals. Subjects were asked to rate
Swann, 1978). By manipulating group membership, other studies how much they liked each individual and how likely they would be,
have demonstrated how group identification influences informa- on a scale from 1 to 5, to be friends with him or her, to hang out with
tion integration and evaluation (Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996; Hew- him or her, to enjoy spending time with him or her, to stay away from
stone, Rubin, & Willis, 2002; Krueger, 1996; Mullen, Brown, & him or her, and so forth. The correlations with ratings of liking were
Smith, 1992; Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel et al., 1971). .76 (be friends), .71 (hang out with), .74 (enjoy spending time),
However, impressions do not only depend on how observations and .70 (stay away).1 Experiments and field studies have also
are interpreted and processed but also on the observations that are shown that negative impressions and outcome expectancies predict
sampled. In many settings observations vary widely, and impres- avoidance (S. Levin, van Laar, & Sidanius, 2003; Plant, 2004; Plant
sions may depend more on decisions about sampling than on & Devine, 2003; Towles-Schwen & Fazio, 2003). In particular, Plant
information integration. Any systematic biases in sampling will and Devine (2003, Study 2; see also Plant, 2004) showed that subjects
thus lead to systematic biases in impression formation (Fiedler, with negative expectations about future interactions were less likely to
2000). Such systematic sample biases have of course been ex- return for an additional session in which they would continue the
plored in the literature. They have been prominent in discussions interaction. Finally, several experimental studies have shown that the
of confirmation bias (Klayman & Ha, 1987; Snyder & Swann,
1978), selective exposure (Kunda, 1990), and availability and
salience (Nisbett & Ross, 1980; S. E. Taylor, 1982; Tversky & 1
N 3,960. I calculated these correlations on the basis of data provided
Kahneman, 1973), in which sample bias has been attributed to by Carranza et al. (2004).
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 953

frequency of communication is higher for individuals who have social skills are relevant for expectations of how pleasurable future
experienced rewarding interactions (D. A. Taylor et al., 1969), have interactions will be. Impressions of many other personal attributes,
positive impressions of others (Kelley, 1950, pp. 437 438; Thibaut & such as skills, creativity, or reasoning ability, may also influence
Coules, 1952, pp. 774 775), and for groups that have more favorable expectations about how rewarding or productive future interac-
attitudes to each other (Lott & Lott, 1961; Moran, 1966). tions will be. Nevertheless, this does not hold for all personal
The general tendency that attraction breeds interaction is also attributes and interactions. For example, your impressions of the
consistent with sociometric data. Sociometric studies have typi- bowling skills of others may be irrelevant to your expectation of
cally shown that similarity, an important determinant of attraction the pleasures of future social interactions.
(Byrne, 1971), is a good predictor of affiliation (for a review, see In social situations satisfying these conditions, the available
McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2001). More generally, socio- information will not be a random sample of fixed size as in
metric studies have shown that ratings of attraction and liking are experiments. Rather, the composition and size of the sample will
positively correlated with sociometric nomination measures depend on past observations. To illustrate this, consider a sche-
(Bukowski, Sippola, Hoza, & Newcomb, 2000; Hoffman, 1962; matic representation of the sampling process depicted in Figure 1.
Maassen, van der Linden, Goossens, & Bokhorst, 2000) and that As shown, the information available depends on decisions about
popular individuals tend to have more likable characteristics (Bon- interactions, which depend on current impressions. Current im-
ney, 1947; French, 1956; French & Mensh, 1948; Jennings, 1950; pressions, however, depend on observations obtained through pre-
Leman & Solomon, 1952; A. F. Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, vious interactions. It follows that the set of observations available
1993). There is also some evidence that sociometric rank (i.e., will depend on the outcome of past interactions. In particular, a
popularity) is positively correlated with the number of actual positive impression implies that the probability of further obser-
interactions and conversations (Homans, 1954, Table 1). vations is high and the sample size will expand. A negative
However, attraction does not always lead to interaction (Ber- impression, in contrast, implies that the probability of further
scheid, 1985). Highly attractive individuals may be difficult to observations is low and the sample size will be constrained (Gilo-
access. Attractive and competent individuals are also less likely to vich, 1991; T. M. Newcomb, 1947; Towles-Schwen & Fazio,
be chosen as interaction partners if one fears that the choice will 2003; Ybarra, 2002, p. 437). Thus, the valence of current impres-
not be reciprocated (Berscheid, 1985; Huston & Levinger, 1978; sions will determine not only how much and what type of infor-
Shaw & Gilchrist, 1955). Highly attractive and competent individ- mation people search for and how they process it (Ybarra, 2002),
uals may also be avoided for fear of low evaluations (Berscheid, but also whether new information will be available at all.
1985; Montoya & Horton, 2004) or due to intimidation (Aronson, Because historical outcomes influence sampling, experience sam-
Willerman, & Floyd, 1966; Helmreich, Aronson, & LeFan, 1970). pling produces a sample bias (Denrell & March, 2001; Eiser et al.,
Notwithstanding these caveats, positive impressions do seem to be 2003; Fazio et al., 2004; Heckman, 1979; March, 1996). Despite such
an important determinant of the probability of future interaction. bias, experience sampling is often the only practical way of sampling.
Such a tendency, however, implies that the sample of informa- If productive interactions are important and impressions are relevant
tion available will be biased. In particular, if decisions about social for expectations about future interactions, it may not be sensible to
interactions depend on impressions based on the available infor- continue sampling if the impression is sufficiently negative. Few
mation, and decisions about social interactions determine the avail- people continue to interact with people they disapprove of just to see
able information, the sample of information available will depend if their initial impressions were false. Similarly, few people actively
on the outcomes of past observations. As a result, the available seek out professional colleagues (who may not be working in the
sample will not be exogenous but endogenous. Although not same organization) if their initial impressions suggest they cannot
universal, such endogenous sampling is likely in social settings gain, professionally or socially, by future interactions.
satisfying the following conditions. Such behavior is not unintelligent. People may recognize that
First, impressions are mainly based on, or at least influenced by, taking more observations will increase the accuracy of evaluations,
impressions formed by personal observations. For example, assess- but accuracy is seldom the only objective of social interactions
ments of social skills can mainly be based on impressions obtained (Friedrich, 1993). The goal of accuracy has to be balanced against the
through personal interactions. Impressions of several other personal goal of engaging in productive interactions (Einhorn, 1986; Einhorn
attributes are also influenced by personal observations, even if infor- & Hogarth, 1978; Friedrich, 1993; Klayman & Ha, 1987; Peeters,
mation from tests and from other individuals may be available. In 1971; B. Schwartz, 1982). The basic trade-off is the same as in the
these situations, future interaction, and thus the available sample, will classical one-armed bandit problem (e.g., DeGroot, 1970), in which a
be influenced by impressions based on previous interactions. In other
situations, such as in exams, the set of observations available is
exogenously determined.
Second, individuals can choose with whom they want to inter-
act. If individuals cannot voluntarily choose their future interaction
partners, such as when interaction patterns are circumscribed by
the division of labor, status hierarchies, or roles, impressions based
on previous interactions cannot influence the probability of future
interactions.
Third, impressions based on observations from previous inter-
actions are relevant for expectations about how rewarding or Figure 1. The relation between the current impression, decisions about
productive future interactions will be. For example, impressions of interactions, and the information available under experience sampling.
954 DENRELL

decision maker can repeatedly choose between a certain alternative It is also assumed that the impression in the first period is equal
with a known payoff and an unknown alternative with an uncertain to bx1. Here, b is a positive fraction regulating the weight of the
payoff. In this situation, more information about the uncertain alter- most recent observation. This specific process implies that As
native is valuable because it makes possible more informed choices in current estimate is an exponentially weighted average of all ob-
the future. Nevertheless, if the uncertain alternative is perceived to be servations of B. Such a weighted-average model is in the spirit of
inferior, obtaining such information is costly because information can the models suggested in the literature on information integration
only be obtained by choosing a seemingly inferior alternative. As a and belief adjustment (Anderson, 1959, 1981; Hogarth & Einhorn,
result, it may be optimal to stop sampling in order to exploit the 1992; Kashima & Kerekes, 1994). The specific model above is
alternative that is believed to be better. For a similar reason, limited also consistent with a recency effect, in which late observations are
sampling in impression formation may be optimal when individuals given more weight than early observations. A recency effect has
care about the outcome of interactions as well as the accuracy of their been consistently observed in experimental studies of impression
evaluations. Such limited sampling has also been observed in exper- formation if subjects are required to formulate an impression after
iments on rule learning when payoffs depend on outcomes. Subjects each piece of information is presented (i.e., serial responding; for
who were instructed to maximize their outcomes were much less
reviews, see Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992; Kashima & Kerekes,
likely to experiment with alternative rules if they had found a rule that
1994). Such serial responding seems representative of situations in
generated a satisfactory outcome than were subjects who were told
which individuals have to decide, after each impression, whether
that they would be assessed on whether they found the correct rule (B.
to continue the interaction.
Schwartz, 1982).
In contrast to more elaborate models in the literature (e.g.,
This implies that in many situations in which information gath-
Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992; Kashima & Kerekes, 1994; Van Over-
ering is not the only priority, impressions have to be based on
limited and biased samples. Some of the complications introduced walle & Labiouse, 2004), the above model assumes that the weight
by this type of sample bias have been noted in the statistical of the most recent observation is constant and independent of the
literature (Duflo, 1997, chap. 9; Lee, 1989, chap. 7; Melfi & Page, value of the observation and of the distance between the observa-
2000; Wetherill, 1966, chap. 8), and some of its behavioral con- tion and the existing impression, which may not hold (Hogarth &
sequences have been noted in psychology, economics, and orga- Einhorn, 1992; Kashima & Kerekes, 1994; Skowronski & Carl-
nization theory (Cohen & Levinthal, 1994, Proposition 1; Denrell ston, 1989; Stangor & McMillan, 1992; Van Overwalle & Labi-
& March, 2001; Eiser et al., 2003; Gilovich, 1991, pp. 46 48; ouse, 2004). Moreover, in contrast to theories of impression for-
March, 1996; Rabin, 2002, p. 807). Here, I examine how this bias mation as driven by schemata and categorizations (Fiske & Taylor,
may explain systematic differences in impression formation. 1991), the above model assumes that all observations are correctly
registered and interpreted and, thus, does not capture the signifi-
cance of schemata, the effect of first impressions, or differential
A Simple Example of the Effect of Experience Sampling memory for consistent or inconsistent information (Stangor &
Model McMillan, 1992). Also, the model does not allow for confirmation
bias or self-fulfilling prophecies, which can contribute to a pri-
To illustrate the implications of experience sampling, consider macy effect. These omissions, however, also imply that any bias in
the process by which an individual, A, forms an impression of evaluations resulting from the above belief-formation process can-
another individual, B. Because Bs behavior may vary, any single not be due to such errors of information processing or to the effects
observation of Bs behavior may be regarded as a sample from a of attention. I also demonstrate that the general results hold for
random variable, X. For the purposes of this illustration, assume more general assumptions about how impressions are formed.
that X is a normally distributed random variable with a mean of 0 Assume, for simplicity, that A always interacts with B in the first
and a standard deviation of 1. period. In later periods, however, As current impression, xt, forms the
As belief about B is assumed to evolve over time as a function of basis for whether A will choose to interact with B. In particular,
new observations of the behavior of B. I assume that this process of assume that A is more likely to interact with B in period t 1 if the
belief formation can be modeled as a sequential revision of current current impression, xt, is high than if it is low. The reason is that a low
beliefs in view of new evidence (Anderson, 1981; Hogarth & Ein- estimate implies a low expectation of the benefits of future interac-
horn, 1992; Kashima & Kerekes, 1994). Specifically, As current tions. A low expectation of the benefits of future interactions, in turn,
belief is assumed to provide an anchor that is adjusted on the basis of implies a low probability of future interactions (Altman & Taylor,
new evidence. The revised belief then becomes the anchor for the next
1973; Homans, 1961; Lott & Lott, 1972; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959).2
adjustment, and the process continues sequentially.
A simple way to model such a sequential process of belief
revision is to assume that the revised belief is some weighted 2
combination of the previous belief and the new evidence, if any. If The problem of sequentially choosing whether to interact with B can be
seen as a version of the one-armed bandit problem. The optimal solution to
the belief, or estimate, in period t is denoted xt, one way to
this problem implies that the sampling probability is either 0 or 1 (e.g.,
represent such a process is as follows:
DeGroot, 1970). In experiments, however, individuals do not behave in this
way but switch quite often (Gans, Knox, & Croson, 2004; Meyer & Shi,


1 bxt bxt1 1995). Moreover, on the basis of a comparison of several models, Gans et
if an observation is made in period t 1 al. (2004) concluded that the model presented in this section (assuming a
xt1 xt . (1)
weighted average and an exponential-choice model) provided the best
if no observation is made in period t 1 overall fit to the behavior of subjects in an experiment on bandit problems.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 955

Figure 2. The distribution of As estimate at the end of the 10th period compared with a standard normal
distribution. The probability of sampling is e3xt/(1e3xt), and b 0.5 (based on 5 million simulations).

A simple model of such a choice process, which also can handle estimate is negative, the estimate then remains at a low level for
negative impressions, is the exponential version of the Luce choice the rest of this simulation run. More generally, because the prob-
model (Luce, 1959). In this model, the probability that A will ability of sampling is lower if the estimate is low than if it is high,
interact with B in period t 1 is low estimates are more stable than high estimates (Gilovich, 1991;
T. M. Newcomb, 1947).4
ecSxt
. (2) This stability of negative impressions was illustrated in an early
1 ecSxt
experiment by Kelley on impression formation (Kelley, 1948;
Here, S is a parameter regulating how sensitive the choice proba- cited in Kelley, 1950, p. 438), which showed that individuals with
bility is to the value of the estimate, and c determines the baseline a negative initial impression of another person interacted less with
probability of sampling. Although several alternative specifica- that person. Individuals who interacted less also had more stable
tions are possible, this model has the advantage that it can be given impressions. As noted by Nisbett and Smith (1989, p. 72), another
an axiomatic foundation as a choice model (Luce, 1959; McFad- illustrative example of the stability of negative estimates can be
den, 1981). The exponential version of the Luce choice model also found in T. M. Newcombs (1961) longitudinal study of attitudes
has some empirical support as a choice model (e.g., Guadagni & and attraction among undergraduates living in the same dorm.
Little, 1983; Yechiam & Busemeyer, 2005) and is often used in Although attraction ratings were relatively unstable for most indi-
models of adaptive learning (e.g., Camerer & Ho, 1999). As viduals, 4 out of 17 individuals had stable ratings (T. M. New-
demonstrated below, the above choice model also provides a good comb, 1961, p. 225). Of these 4, 3 were individuals who were
fit to data on how the probability of future interactions varies with ranked among the least attractive (14, 15, and 16 on a scale where
the current impression.
17 is the least attractive) during the 1st week (T. M. Newcomb,
1961, p. 226, Table 12.2). Such persistence of negative ratings has
Implications also been found in later studies of sociometric ranks (Cillessen,
Given the above assumptions, what will be the distribution of Bukowski & Haselager, 2000; A. F. Newcomb & Bukowski,
As estimate at the end of the 10th period, for example? Figure 2 1984).5
shows this distribution when b 0.5, S 3, and c 0.3 A An experiment on attitude formation (Fazio et al., 2004) pro-
majority of the estimates are negative (75%), and the mean is also vides an illustration of how the stability of negative estimates can
negative (0.33). These results also hold if one examines the
distribution of As estimate in later periods. In fact, as t 3 , the
distribution of As estimate quickly converges to a stationary 3
distribution, which can be solved analytically (see Appendix A). This was based on 5 million simulations. All simulations were pro-
grammed in both C and Crystal Ball, an add-on to Excel.
For example, in the above case, the expected estimate can be 4
The stability of negative evaluations does not imply that all estimates
shown to converge to 0.82 and the probability of a negative
will eventually become negative. Even in the long term, some estimates
estimate to 0.87.
will be positive. In addition, if the average observation were positive rather
The reason for these results is the tendency to avoid sampling than 0, the average estimate would be positive but below the average
when the estimate is low. To illustrate this, consider the develop- observation.
ment of As estimate over time in one simulation run, plotted in 5
Table 5.1 in Cillessen et al. (2000) summarized stability ratings for
Figure 3. Here, As estimate is initially positive. However, after different sociometric status groups in studies of children. Computation of
some random negative observations, the estimate turns negative. the average and the median of Cohens kappa shows that the rejected group
Because the probability of taking another sample is low if the was the most stable.
956 DENRELL

Figure 3. The development of As estimate during one simulation run.

lead to a negativity bias. Subjects in this experiment engaged in a Figure 4 also shows that As average estimate will only be free
survival game, in which they had to eat beans with positive energy from any bias (i.e., 0) if r 1. In this case, A will sample in every
levels and avoid beans with negative energy levels in order to period and, thus, always obtain new information about B. This
survive. Subjects were initially uninformed about the energy value theoretical result is consistent with the finding that no bias
of beans with different shapes, and they could only obtain infor- emerged in the experiment on attitude formation (Fazio et al.,
mation about the energy values of different beans by eating them. 2004) if subjects were always informed about the value of beans
In each period of the game, subjects were presented with a bean of with which they were presented, regardless of whether they ate
a particular shape. Because subjects were less likely to eat beans them.
they believed had negative energy levels, false negative beliefs The effect of r also implies that evaluations can differ system-
about the energy levels of beans were more likely to persist than atically between people with whom one tends to interact socially
false positive beliefs. As a result, at the end of the game, subjects and people one is less likely to meet socially. By meeting people
were more likely to have false negative than false positive beliefs, often, one will obtain further information about their abilities and
consistent with the experience sampling model. correct any false negative initial impressions. Often this process
Figure 3 also suggests one way in which false negative impres- implies that evaluations will be systematically lower for individ-
sions may change. Namely, suppose that A does interact with B uals of minority groups to which one does not belong. The reason
again even if As estimate is negative. For example, A may be is that individuals are most likely to interact with socially similar
forced to interact with B if they work in the same department. others (Fischer, 1982; Marsden, 1987; McPherson & Smith-Lovin,
Alternatively, A may have a low estimate of the ability of B, but 1987; McPherson et al., 2001). Because one is less likely to meet
interact with B for noninstrumental and social reasons (Blau, 1962, individuals of minority groups to which one does not belong to, a
1963). In this case, A will obtain another sample of X. Because As low estimate of such individuals will be more stable.
estimate is below 0 and because the mean of X is 0, it follows that This effect also implies that social contacts may compensate for
the effect of taking another sample is most likely that As estimate ability in evaluations. Consider, for example, two individuals: The
will be adjusted upward, toward 0. This reasoning suggests that first has an average ability equal to 0, whereas the second has an
evaluations based on experience sampling may be sensitive to average ability equal to 0.3. Suppose that the first is likely to
exogenous differences in the probability of interactions. To include interact socially with evaluators (i.e., r 0.8). The second indi-
this in the model, suppose that the probability that A will interact vidual, however, never interacts socially with evaluators (i.e., r
with B in period t 1 is given by 0). In this case, the average evaluation of the first and the second
individual, at the end of the 10th period, is roughly equalthat is,
ecSxt 0.04 (when b 0.5, c 0, and S 3, based on 500,000
r (1 r) . (3)
1 ecSxt simulations). Thus, despite having a higher average ability, the
Here, r can be interpreted as the probability of nonvoluntary second individual will typically be rated as equal to the well-
(Thibaut & Kelley, 1959) or noninstrumental (Blau, 1962) inter- connected first individual. Moreover, if the difference in abilities
actions, which both imply that A may interact with B for reasons were smaller, the well-connected first individual would, on aver-
unrelated to As estimate of Bs ability, for example. Such inter- age, be rated higher than the more able second individual.
actions may nevertheless provide further observations of the be-
havior of B relevant for evaluating the ability of B. 6
Cross and Parker (2004) found several examples of this effect of
As the above reasoning suggests, a higher probability of non- involuntary interactions in a study of networks in organizations: One of
voluntary or noninstrumental interactions implies a higher average the most surprising discoveries in our interviews was that many productive
evaluation.6 This is illustrated in Figure 4, which shows As relationships begin with poor first impressions that change over time when
average estimate at the end of the 10th period as a function of r. people have no choice but to work together (p. 105).
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 957

Figure 4. As average estimate at the end of the 10th period as a function of r. The probability of sampling is
r (1 r)[e3xt/(1 e3xt)], and b 0.5. Each entry is based on averages from 500,000 simulations.

Relation to Existing Literature which X was symmetrically distributed, and the mean and initial
belief were both equal to 0. Models relying on information aggre-
As the above example illustrates, experience sampling suggests gation also imply that differences in impressions will eventually
an alternative explanation of systematic differences in impressions.
disappear as sample sizes increase (Van Rooy et al., 2003, p. 540).
In contrast to theories emphasizing the influence of prior expec-
Experience sampling, however, suggests that the bias can be
tations (Fiske & Taylor, 1991) or behavioral confirmation and
permanent if individuals stop sampling. Nevertheless, whereas the
self-fulfilling prophecies (Klein & Snyder, 2003; Snyder & Stukas,
models differ in their mechanisms and in some of their predictions,
1999), experience sampling does not assume that prior expecta-
they are best viewed as complementary mechanisms by which
tions influence the interpretation of subsequent observations or
sampling, rather than cognitiveperceptual biases, may contribute
change future behavior. In contrast to theories emphasizing moti-
to biases in impression formation. Moreover, experience sampling
vational influences on attitude formation (Kunda, 1990; Tajfel &
is likely to reinforce the bias produced by information aggregation.
Turner, 1979) or how impressions about members of the ingroup
A negative impression of an individual or a group, due perhaps to
are anchored on the self (Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996; Krueger,
a small sample size and information aggregation, may not be
1996), experience sampling does not assume that impressions are
revised because future sampling might be avoided.
influenced by motivations or are anchored on the self. The impli-
cation is not that cognitive and motivational influences are not Of course, a negativity bias in impressions would also occur if
important. Rather, experience sampling suggests an additional negative observations had a larger impact on impressions than
mechanism that can lead to persistence of negative impressions positive observations, as several studies have shown (for a review,
and to systematic differences in impressions. see Skowronski & Carlston, 1989). The reason may be that unde-
The underlying mechanism also differs from recent models of sirable behaviors are more diagnostic than desirable behaviors
impression formation and sampling (Fiedler, 1996; Kashima et al., (Reeder & Brewer, 1979; Skowronski & Carlston, 1989; Ybarra,
2000; Van Rooy et al., 2003), which also have shown how differ- 2002). If so, negative observations will have a larger weight in
ences in sample sizes can give rise to systematic differences in domains in which such behavior is unusual, such as in the morality
impressions of groups or individuals. Sampling in these models is domain, whereas positive observations can have a larger weight in
assumed to be independent of the current impression, although the other domains, such as in the competence domain (Reeder &
probability of sampling may differ among groups. Fiedlers (1996) Brewer, 1979; Skowronski & Carlston, 1989; Wojciszke, Brycz, &
BIAS exemplar model, as well as Kashima et al.s (2000) tensor Borkenau, 1993). As a complement to theories relying on diag-
product model and Van Rooy et al.s (2003) connectionist model, nosticity, the model of experience sampling suggests that a nega-
instead rely on information aggregation to explain the effect of tivity bias can emerge when information has to be actively sam-
sample size. The basic intuition behind their results is that larger pled even if all observations are given equal weight. This was also
sample sizes will reduce noise and better reveal systematic ten- illustrated in the experiment on attitude formation discussed above
dencies. As a result, if members of two groups have primarily (Fazio et al., 2004), in which a negativity bias emerged in the
positive traits, the impression will be higher for the most fre- absence of differential diagnosticity. In particular, there was no
quently sampled group. Thus, in these models the effect of sample negativity bias if subjects were always told about the energy values
size is akin to the set-size effect (Anderson, 1981, p. 131). of the beans presented to them regardless of whether they ate them.
In contrast to the model of experience sampling, these models If negative observations also have a larger impact, this will
only predict an effect of the sample size if the proportion of strengthen the bias from experience sampling. However, in cases
positive versus negative observations is unequal or differs from in which positive observations have a larger weight, the bias from
initial beliefs (Fiedler, 1996, p. 199; Kashima et al., 2000, p. 923). experience sampling will be reduced. Even if negative observa-
These models would not predict a bias in the above example, in tions have a larger weight, however, such a tendency would not, by
958 DENRELL

Table 1
Estimates of Models of Belief Updating

Estimate of constant weight Estimates of declining weight


Pieces of
Experiment information b % deviation % deviation

Stewart (1965) 8 0.19 0.05 0.45 0.47 0.01


Stewart (1965) 6 0.24 0.04 0.49 0.53 0.01
Stewart (1965) 4 0.24 0.03 0.63 0.50 0.01
Anderson (1968) 6 0.53 0.03 0.58 0.37 0.03
Anderson and Farkas (1973) 4 0.36 0.04 0.61 0.45 0.02
Levin and Schmidt (1969) 6 0.45 0.12a 0.86 0.38 0.11a
Levin and Schmidt (1970) 3 0.38 0.05a 0.48 0.10 0.05a
Average 0.34 0.59 0.40

Note. Values are based on minimizing the absolute deviation. Percentage of deviation is the average of the absolute difference between the actual and
the estimated impressions divided by the range of the scale used.
a
Average absolute difference between the estimated probability and the actual proportions.

itself, lead to systematic differences in impressions as a result of The Magnitude of the Bias
systematic differences in interaction patterns.
Finally, experience sampling is related to Ybarras (2002) the- Whereas the above result holds under very general assumptions,
ory of how the valence of impressions influences subsequent the magnitude of the bias depends on the specific assumptions
information processing. As reviewed by Ybarra (2002; see also made. In particular, a large bias requires that sampling is unlikely
Ybarra, Schaberg, & Keiper, 1999), positive impressions elicit when the estimate is negative (i.e., that S is large), so that negative
more testing and critical information processing than negative estimates are unlikely to be corrected. Moreover, a large bias
impressions, because people are more likely to think that negative requires that the weight of the most recent observation is large (i.e.,
traits are diagnostic. Ybarra (2002, p. 437) also notes that negative that b is large), so that a positive estimate can turn negative even
impressions may imply that no additional information is sampled. after one or two negative observations. To evaluate the practical
As illustrated above, this may be sufficient to produce biases in significance of experience sampling, one thus needs information
impression formation. This implies that negative impressions may about how strongly people avoid others if their impression is
be more stable even if negative behaviors are not (and are not
negative and information about the weight of the most recent
perceived to be) more diagnostic. More generally, experience
observation.
sampling is likely to reinforce a negativity bias produced by more
Consider first the weight of the most recent observation, b. A
stringent testing of positive impressions.
large number of studies have shown that if individuals have to
formulate an impression after each piece of information is pre-
Examining the Influence of Experience Sampling sented, there is a recency effect in the sense that the most recent
A General Model observation has the largest weight (for reviews, see Hogarth &
Einhorn, 1992; Kashima & Kerekes, 1994). Most studies, how-
The above illustration relied on specific assumptions about ever, have focused on qualitative properties of the data (i.e., the
belief updating and sampling. The weight of the most recent
existence of a recency effect), and few studies have estimated the
observation was assumed to be constant, and sampling was as-
weight of the most recent observation. However, data from some
sumed to follow the exponential-choice rule. However, the results
studies on impression formation can be used to obtain tentative
hold for a wide range of alternative assumptions. Instead of being
estimates.
constant, the weight of the most recent observation may shift over
time. For example, it may decline as suggested by several exper- Table 1 shows estimates from seven experiments on person-
imental studies of impression formation (Anderson & Farkas, impression formation that provide data on the development of
1973; Dreben, Fiske, & Hastie, 1979; Kashima & Kerekes, 1994). impressions over time.7 In each experiment, subjects were pre-
The estimate can also simply be the average of all observations, or sented with a sequence of descriptions (typically adjectives) of a
it may even follow Bayess rule. As discussed in Appendix B, the hypothetical individual, and they stated their impression after each
only assumption required is that the new estimate can be written as piece of information was presented. In the experiments of I. P.
some weighted combination of the old estimate and the new Levin and Schmidt (1969, 1970), however, subjects only stated
observation, xt1 (1 bt1)xt bt1xt1, where bt1 can differ
between periods. Similarly, one does not have to assume that
sampling follows the exponential-choice rule. One has to assume 7
Dreben et al. (1979) also estimated the impact of serial position on
only that sampling is more likely if the impression is high. For all impression formation. No data on the development of impressions over
processes satisfying these assumptions, Appendix B shows that for time are available, however. Nevertheless, their method of estimating the
all t 1, the expected estimate, E(Xt), will be below the average relative influence implies that the weight of the fourth observation must be
observation E(X). at least 71/200 0.36 to 0.23.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 959

whether they liked the individual.8 Both a model with a constant were likely to enjoy spending time with him or her (Carranza et
weight (as in the above illustration) and a model with a declining al., 2004). These studies, however, do not provide any direct
weight were estimated. In the model of a declining weight, I information about how the probability of future interactions varies
assumed that the weight of the most recent observation could be with the initial impression. To obtain some tentative information
modeled as bn (1/n) (cf. Hertwig, Barron, Weber, & Erev, in about this function, I distributed a questionnaire to 109 undergrad-
press), where is the weight of the first impression, n is the uates as a part of a larger package of questions, completed for
number of observations made so far, and is the rate at which the payment. The questions were variations of the following:
weight declines. As mentioned above, several studies of impres-
Before the first lecture of one of your courses, you briefly talk to
sion formation have found that the weight of the most recent another student taking the same course. Based on your first impres-
observation tends to decline. Details of the estimation procedure sion, you believe that 84% of all students you have met at campus are
are given in Appendix D.9 friendlier than this student.
Although the estimates in Table 1 show great variation, they In this case, would you choose to:
also illustrate that there are situations in which the weight of the
1. Initiate a brief hallway conversation with this student?
most recent observation can be large even after several pieces of
information have been presented. For example, estimates from the YES___ NO___
model of a declining weight, based on the experiment of Anderson 2. Sit next to this student when you arrive to class 10 minutes early?
(1968), imply that the weight of the sixth piece of information is YES___ NO___
0.30. Thus, a negative observation after five previous observations
3. Invite this individual out for a coffee?
could substantially change the impression. This is also consistent
with the findings of I. P. Levin and Schmidt (1969), who found YES___ NO___
that the proportion of subjects who said that they liked the indi-
Subjects answered five questions in which the percentage (84% in
vidual about whom they obtained information almost always
the example) varied across five levels: 84, 69, 50, 34, and 16%,
dropped below 50% whenever the most recently presented adjec-
corresponding to an impression 1.0 or 0.5 standard deviations
tive was negative, even if several positive adjectives had been
below the mean, at the mean, and 0.5 or 1.0 standard deviations
presented earlier.10 Also, Dreben et al. (1979, p. 1765) found that above the mean (assuming a normal distribution). The order of the
there was a stronger recency effect in experiments with a filler task percentages (starting with 84 or 16%) and the order of the alter-
between each observation. This suggests that the weight of the natives (beginning with yes or no) were counterbalanced.
most recent observation might be larger when individuals are The results, summarized in Figure 5, show that subjects were
involved in several tasks between successive encounters, as in more likely to initiate another interaction if their initial impression
many real-life situations. was positive. Estimates of the above exponential-choice model
These estimates can be compared with estimates from experi- (Equation 2), based on minimizing the absolute deviation, also
ments of repeated choices between uncertain alternatives with show a very good fit for the data; the average absolute difference
unknown payoffs, a task similar to choosing between interaction
partners. Using data from an experiment of about 100 choices
8
between two alternatives, Gans et al. (2004) estimated a model To estimate the weight of the most recent observation, I assumed that
identical to the basic model described in the previous section (A the probability of a like response followed the exponential-choice model.
In particular, the probability of a like response was assumed to be
Simple Example of the Effect of Experience Sampling), with a
1/1 exp[S(xt 3)]}, where 3 is the middle point of the scale used in
constant weight and an exponential-choice rule. The median esti- these experiments. I used the data from individuals in I. P. Levin and
mate of the weight of the most recent observation was 0.25 and the Schmidt (1969) and the data from Experiment 1 in I. P. Levin and Schmidt
average was 0.40. Using data from an experiment of repeated (1970).
choices between four alternatives, Yechiam and Busemeyer (2005) 9
There are also data available from experiments that have examined
estimated the same model. Their estimate of the weight of the most belief updating in evaluation tasks (Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992)that is,
recent observation was 0.31. when subjects have to decide whether the data support a hypothesis.
Consider next the parameters regulating how the probability of Estimates based on Furnhams (1986) data on formation of guilt and
innocence judgments gives b 0.22, assuming a constant weight, and
sampling varies with the impressionthat is, S and c. As men-
0.25 and 0.08, assuming a declining weight. Pennington and Hastie
tioned above, several studies have examined the effect of attraction (1992, p. 200) reported that the best fitting model, based on data from a
on interaction and communication. Some of these studies provide similar experiment, gives b 0.55. Estimates based on Hogarth and
data about the effect of initial impressions on the willingness to Einhorns (1992) experiment on hypothesis evaluation (Experiment 4) give
continue the interaction. In particular, M. S. Schwartz (1966; cited b 0.21, assuming a constant weight, and 0.29 and 0.08,
in Byrne, 1971, p. 231), Montoya & Horton (2004, p. 700), and assuming a declining weight. Finally, Busemeyer and Myung (1988, p. 7),
Carranza et al. (2004) showed that ratings of attraction, based on in an experiment on prototype learning, reported that their findings can be
approximated with a model with a constant weight of 0.5.
questions of how much subjects like their future interaction part- 10
I. P. Levin and Schmidt (1969) also estimated a linear learning model
ner, are positively and significantly correlated with the willingness
(Bush & Mosteller, 1955), assuming that the change in the probability of
to continue to the interaction, on the basis of questions such as (a) a like response after a negative adjective was Pn 1 (1 W)Pn. The
if they were willing to sit next to this person in a class or meeting estimated value of W for a negative adjective presented at the sixth position
(M. S. Schwartz, 1966), (b) if they would like to meet my future was 0.76 (p. 286), implying that the probability of a like response would be
interaction partner (Montoya & Horton, 2004), and (c) if they reduced from 1 to 0.24 after a single piece of negative information.
960 DENRELL

Figure 5. The probability of continued interaction as a function of the first impression. Each entry is based on
responses from 109 subjects.

between the estimated probabilities and the actual proportions was of a negative estimate is still 0.61 when S 2.51 and c 2.75.13
0.01. The estimates obtained were S 1.74 and c 1.06 for Overall, these simulations suggest that the bias can be expected to
Initiate a brief hallway conversation, S 1.94 and c 0.24 for be significant whenever interactions require some investment and
Sit next to this student, and S 2.51 and c 2.75 for Invite sampling is sensitive to the impression.
this individual out for a coffee.11 Although these estimates are Obviously, these simulations are based on a number of simpli-
based on responses to a hypothetical situation and only provide fying assumptions. Nevertheless, the main result holds and is
information about the effect of the first impression, they provide sometimes even stronger if these assumptions are changed. For
some indication of possible parameter values. In particular, they example, suppose that negative observations have a larger impact
suggest that when interactions require some investment in time (as on impressions than positive observations (Dreben et al., 1979;
in Invite this individual out for a coffee), the probability of
Skowronski & Carlston, 1989), as reported in three studies (Ander-
sampling can be relatively sensitive to the impression (cf. Einhorn
son, 1968; I. P. Levin & Schmidt, 1969, 1970). In such cases, the
& Hogarth, 1978; Friedrich, 1993). Such time consuming interac-
bias will be stronger. However, if positive observations have
tions probably also provide more opportunities for information
greater weight, as reported in Anderson & Farkas (1973, p. 91), the
exchange and revision of the impression than do brief
conversations. bias will be reduced. An observation that deviates substantially
Whereas the above estimates of the weight of the most recent from the current estimate may also be more weighted.14 Simula-
observation and the sensitivity of sampling to the impression must tions show that such a contrast effect would generally increase the
be regarded as tentative, they provide some indication of reason-
able values. These values can then be used as inputs to a simula-
tion, to estimate the possible effect size of the bias produced by
11
experience sampling. Table 2 shows the effect size for three Estimates based on maximizing the likelihood give very similar
different assumptions about belief updating and sampling. As values. Such estimates, however, assume independence within subjects,
illustrated, when both the weight of the most recent observation which may not hold.
12
and the sensitivity of sampling to the impression are small, the The effect size is larger after 50 periods because the probability of
effect size is small. However, when the weight of the most recent sampling under these assumptions is low, and few people have sampled
observation is set to the average value in Table 1, and the sensi- more than once after 10 periods.
13
tivity of sampling is set at the median value, the effect size is This was after 50 periods, based on 500,000 simulations.
14
noticeable. For example, the probability of a negative impression A contrast effect can also be modeled in at least two other ways. First,
is 0.62 after the 10th period, and the average impression is 0.16. the weight of the most recent observation may be a function of the absolute
value of the observation. For example, more extreme observations may be
However, the effect size is much stronger when both the weight of
given more weight (e.g., Fiske, 1981). In this case, simulations suggest that
the most recent observation and the sensitivity of sampling to the the bias will be stronger than if the weight were constant and equal to the
impression are large.12 For example, the probability of a negative average weight. The weight of the most recent observation may also
estimate is 0.82. It can also be noted that the probability of a depend on the value of the estimate. For example, the weight may be
negative estimate will be high whenever sampling is sensitive to greater for more extreme estimates (Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992). This will
the impression. For example, even if b is only 0.19, the probability typically reduce the bias.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 961

Table 2 other influences on the sampling process. Because the negativity


Simulation of the Effect Size bias is due to the fact that individuals believed to be inferior are not
given a second chance, evaluations are more likely to be positive
After 10 periods After 50 periods whenever sampling is likely, even when impressions are negative.
Probability Average Probability Average Such a pattern was also observed in the experiment on the devel-
Assumptions negative impression negative impression opment of attitudes described above (Fazio et al., 2004). In par-
ticular, manipulations that increased the probability of approaching
Small 0.54 0.05 0.54 0.02
beans with negative energy values reduced the probability of
Medium 0.62 0.16 0.61 0.10
Large 0.68 0.33 0.82 0.50 erroneously classifying beans as negative. As a result, the proba-
bility increased that a bean would be classified as having a positive
Note. Small: 0.45, 0.47, S 1.74, and c 1.06. Medium: energy level.
0.59, 0.40, S 1.94, and c 0.24. Large: 0.86, 0.38, S
2.51, and c 2.75. Each entry is based on 500,000 simulations.
In the above model, sampling when the estimate is negative can
occur for several different reasons, illustrated graphically in Figure
6. First, as discussed above, individuals may be forced to continue
bias, although the difference is not large.15 The size of the bias is the interaction, even if their impression is negative. For example,
also essentially unchanged if one assumes that impression forma- individuals may have to continue to interact with colleagues or
tion follows a different model, suggested by Anderson (1959) and with friends of their friends, even if their impression is negative.
Anderson and Farkas (1973), in which impressions can be decom- More generally, interactions are sometimes determined by exoge-
posed into two components: a surface component, which is sensi- nous factors. This case is illustrated in Figure 6A, where Line (B)
tive to the most recent observations, and a basal component, which starts at a baseline probability of sampling equal to 0.4, whereas
is adjusted to the cumulated data.16 Finally, suppose that the Line (A) starts from 0. As illustrated in Figure 4, such forced
impression can change even if no new information is provided, sampling implies that the bias will be smaller and that evaluations
perhaps due to memory decay (Broadbent, 1958; Erev & Roth, will be higher. This is also demonstrated formally in Appendix C.
1998; Yechiam & Busemeyer, 2005). Simulations show that this Second, individuals may feel that sampling is justified even if
would weaken the bias but not eliminate it. their impression is low. For example, individuals may be moti-
The bias would also be stronger and more permanent if one vated by accuracy and put little weight on the expected outcome of
assumed that the probability of sampling changed over time. In
particular, as more information is accumulated and confidence
grows, individuals may be less likely to interact with an individual 15
To model such a contrast effect, I assumed that the weight of the latest
of whom they have a negative estimate. For example, A may observation was bn [(1/n)]1/(1Dk), where D is the absolute difference
follow a strategy of never sampling B again if As impression after between the observation and the prior estimate, divided by the standard
10 observations is sufficiently negative. If so, these negative deviation of the observation. Thus, the weight of the most recent observa-
estimates will never change, and the bias will be permanent even tion is an increasing function of the absolute difference between the
if the estimate was the average of all observations so far, which estimate and the observation. I estimated this model using Stewarts (1965)
data with eight, six, and four adjectives, Andersons (1968) data, and
otherwise would converge to 0. The bias would also be stronger if
Anderson and Farkass (1973) data. I then simulated the bias using these
one assumed, following prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky, estimates and compared the bias, as measured both by the average estimate
1979), that the probability of sampling is not symmetric around c and the probability of a negative estimate, with the bias if no contrast effect
but more sensitive to estimates below c. is assumed (i.e., using estimates from the best fitting model when k 0).
Finally, all the examples in this section have assumed that the The results show that the bias is larger in four cases and smaller in one
true mean of the observation is 0. The bias in the estimate, case.
however, depends on the mean of the observation (relative to c). If 16
The estimate was modeled as


the mean is very high so that sampling is very likely, or very low
1
so that sampling is very unlikely, the bias is much smaller. This xt1 (1 bn) xi bnxt1.
n1
also implies that the average bias in As estimate of many different
individuals, with different means, is smaller.17 This does not Here,
change the fact that As bias of many individual members of a
population can be significant. Still, it does suggest that the average
impression of a large and widely varying group of individuals is
1
n1 xi

not likely to be biased very much if each impression is formed


is the average of all observations so far including the prior (assumed to be
independently and then averaged. However, if the impression of
0), capturing the basal component, and bn (1/n) captures the weight of
one individual influences the probability of interacting with other the surface component. A model based on these assumptions does provide
individuals of this group, the bias for the group as a whole will be a better fit to Anderson and Farkass (1973) data (the estimates are
larger. 0.56 and 0.43) than does a model assuming only a declining weight.
Nevertheless, simulations using estimates from this model show that the
The Influence of the Sampling Process bias is as strong as or stronger than it is when only a declining weight is
assumed.
Although the general negativity bias is interesting, the most 17
For example, if b 0.34, the probability of a negative impression is
important theoretical implication of the model of experience sam- 0.67 if the mean is 0, whereas it is 0.58 if the mean is normally distributed
pling is that evaluations will be sensitive to interaction patterns and with a mean of 0 and a variance of 1 (S 3, c 0).
962 DENRELL

even if the impression is somewhat negative. As demonstrated in


Appendix C, such less selective sampling implies that the bias will
be smaller and that evaluations will be higher. For example,
although the probability of a negative impression after the 10th
period is 0.75 for Line (A), it is only 0.63 for Line (B).18
Finally, the probability of sampling may not be very sensitive to
the impressionthat is, S may be low. This case is illustrated in
Figure 6C, where Line (B) is less sensitive to the impression than
Line (A). As illustrated by the survey reported above, such less
sensitive sampling may happen for interactions that do not require
much engagement or investment (such as in engage in a brief
conversation). It may also happen if the current impression is not
very important for expectations about how rewarding future inter-
actions will be. As illustrated in the above simulations, a small
value of S implies that the bias will be smaller and the average
estimate higher.

The Influence of First Impressions


The above models assumed that there is a recency effect in
impression formation, consistent with many experiments on belief
updating. Nevertheless, these experiments typically exclude, by
design, confirmatory hypothesis testing and self-fulfilling proph-
ecies (Hilton & von Hippel, 1996; Snyder & Stukas, 1999), which
can both lead to a primacy effect. If there was a strong primacy
effect, perhaps due to confirmatory hypothesis testing and self-
fulfilling prophecies, simulations show that the bias from experi-
ence sampling would be smaller. In fact, if only the first impres-
sion mattered, the expected estimate would be the expected value
of the first observation, and there would be no bias. Thus, confir-
matory hypothesis testing and self-fulfilling prophecies will tend
to weaken the bias from experience sampling.
There is little reason to expect, however, that confirmatory
hypothesis testing and self-fulfilling prophecies would nullify the
effect of experience sampling. Although much research has dem-
onstrated that primacy effects can emerge through confirmatory
hypothesis testing and self-fulfilling prophecies, the magnitude of
the effect produced by these phenomena varies depending on the
context (Snyder & Stukas, 1999). Thus, for example, although the
existence of confirmatory hypothesis testing is well-established,
how general it is remains at issue (Fiske & Taylor, 1991, p. 543).
Similarly, although self-fulfilling prophecies have been established
in many contexts, meta-analyses of experimental studies and field
studies have shown that the average effect size is quite small
(Jussim & Eccles, 1995; Madon, Jussim, & Eccles, 1997). In
Figure 6. How the probability of sampling can vary with the impression.
addition, several studies have shown that subjects are sensitive to
data violating expectations (Fiske & Taylor, 1991). In particular,
individuating information has been shown to reduce the effects of
interactions. Individuals may also have a low comparison level stereotypes and categorizations (Gawronski, Ehrenberg, Banse,
(Thibaut & Kelley, 1959) for deciding whether to continue sam- Zukova, & Klauer, 2003; Hilton & Fein, 1989; Weber & Crocker,
pling. For example, few satisfactory interaction partners may exist, 1983), sometimes even after only one piece of information (Locks-
perhaps because the individual has a low status or reputation. As ley, Borgida, Brekke, & Hepburn, 1980). Thus, although first
a result, the individual may have to be less selective in sampling. impressions and expectations can matter, there is no reason to
Consistent with this, Van Duuren and Di Giacomo (1997) showed suggest that impressions are not sensitive to subsequent observa-
that individuals who had experienced failure had a greater propen- tions. Instead, the bias produced by experience sampling should be
sity to affiliate with others and were also less discriminating in seen as a different and complementary mechanism, by which
choosing a partner. Such less selective sampling is illustrated by false-negative expectations may fail to be disconfirmed.
Line (B) in Figure 6B. The probability of sampling is always
higher (for any given impression) for Line (B) than for Line (A).
Moreover, the probability of sampling for Line (B) is quite high 18
This was based on 500,000 simulations when b 0.5.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 963

The Possibility of Correcting for the Bias positive evaluations of the self to members of their own group
(Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996; Krueger, 1996). Finally, experiments
If an individual is aware of the bias, is there any way of have shown that subjects can develop illusory correlations between
correcting for it? Suppose, for example, an individual has sampled negative attributes and minority group membership (Fiedler, 1996;
n times out of t times possible, and the observations are x1, x2, . . . , Fiedler & Walther, 2004; Hamilton & Gifford, 1976).
xn. As explained above, a simple average of these observations
The model of experience sampling suggests an additional mech-
would be biased. To obtain an unbiased estimate, one would have
anism for why outgroup members are evaluated more negatively
to control for the sample bias produced by experience sampling.
than ingroup members. To develop this, suppose first that impres-
Although not impossible, it would be very difficult to perform this
sions of individuals belonging to a specific group are determined
correction intuitively.19 Nevertheless, the biggest obstacle to ob-
in part by experiences in previous encounters with individuals of
taining unbiased estimates is probably not the computational dif-
this group (Fiedler, 1996). Positive previous encounters lead to
ficulty of correcting for the bias, but the fact that most people are
more positive impressions and negative encounters lead to more
likely to be unaware of the sampling bias and its consequences.
negative impressions. Although strongly held stereotypes about
Most individuals would probably not intuitively recognize that an
groups imply that impressions may be relatively insensitive to new
estimate, such as the average of all observations taken, is biased if
experiences (Fiske & Taylor, 1991) or that positive impressions
sampling depends on the current impression. Given the difficulties
may not generalize to other members of the group (Brewer &
many individuals have in dealing with other subtle points in
Miller, 1984; Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000; Rothbart & John, 1985),
probability, such as regression to the mean (Kahneman & Tversky,
numerous studies have shown that positive encounters can, under
1973), and with selection bias in data and experiments (see Fiedler,
the right conditions, lead to changes in group impressions (e.g.,
2000, for a discussion), it seems unlikely that individuals would be
Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000, chap. 5; Pettigrew, 1998; Pettigrew &
aware of this bias. People may be aware, however, that it is
Tropp, 2000; Stephan & Stephan, 1984; Triandis, 1994). Plant and
valuable to continue sampling even if the initial impression is
Devine (2003, Study 1) also showed that individuals with more
negative, to obtain additional information. Because continued sam-
positive previous contacts with outgroup members had more pos-
pling will reduce the bias, it provides a different way to correct for
it. However, if accuracy is not the only goal, continued sampling itive expectations about future interactions with outgroup
is not always a reasonable strategy. If the current impression is members.
sufficiently negative, the value of additional information may not Second, the probability of future interactions with outgroup
compensate for a negative expected value of future interactions, members depends on current impressions of members from this
and further sampling may be avoided. Because sampling should group. In particular, individuals may avoid members of a specific
only be avoided if the current impression is negative, this also group if their experiences with other members of this group have
implies that false negatives will be more common than false been negative (T. M. Newcomb, 1947; Pettigrew, 1997, 1998;
positives.20 Plant, 2004; Towles-Schwen & Fazio, 2003). Such avoidance was
demonstrated in the experiment of Plant and Devine (2003, Study
1), who showed that negative outcome expectancies lead to anxiety
Judgment Biases Originating in Experience Sampling
in interracial interactions, which in turn was a good predictor of the
Interaction Patterns and Evaluation Bias desire to avoid future interactions. Another study, in which actual
behaviors rather than intentions were studied, showed that negative
The model of experience sampling shows how systematic dif- outcome expectancies reduced the probability that subjects would
ferences in evaluations can emerge as the result of systematic return for an additional session (Plant & Devine, 2003, Study 2;
differences in interaction patterns. If A is more likely to interact see also Plant, 2004). In addition, Henderson-King and Nisbett
with individuals of Type B than with individuals of Type C, for (1996) showed that exposure to negative behavior of a Black
any given level of impression, evaluations of individuals in Group person increased the probability that subjects would avoid Black
C are more likely to be negative than evaluations of individuals of people in the future. Finally, S. Levin et al. (2003) found that
Group B. This section illustrates how this mechanism can provide negative attitudes toward an outgroup after the first year of college
a novel explanation for several biases in impression formation.
Population structure and attitudes toward outgroups. Numer-
ous studies have shown that outgroup members are typically 19
Obtaining an unbiased estimate would require calculating the value of
evaluated more negatively than ingroup members (Brewer & E[X] that maximizes the likelihood of the data, given the sampling and
Kramer, 1985; Hewstone et al., 2002; Mullen et al., 1992). Al- updating processes. This would be possible if the character of the distri-
though a variety of different processes and historical events can bution of X was known (e.g., normal), but computationally challenging. For
contribute to such a bias (Hewstone et al., 2002), existing psycho- example, if it was known that X was normally distributed with a variance
logical explanations have emphasized the motive to maintain a of 1 and that the weight of the most recent observation was constant and
equal to 0.5, and the sampling rule was known, simulations could be used
positive social identity (Tajfel, 1982; Tajfel & Turner, 1979). In
to calculate the value of E[X] that is most likely if the estimate after 10
this explanation, an ingroup bias emerges because individuals
periods was, say, 0.4. However, if the sampling and updating rules were
bolster evaluations of their own group. Persistent negative attitudes not knownthat is, if one only had information about the impressions at
toward outgroups have also been attributed to stereotypes or sche- which others had arrivedit could be impossible to identify E[X] (cf.
mata that influence the interpretation and recall of observations or Heckman, 2001; Manski, 1995).
induce self-fulfilling prophecies (Chen & Bargh, 1997; Hilton & 20
Indeed, Denrell (2005) showed that this result holds even if individ-
von Hippel, 1996). Alternatively, an ingroup bias can emerge if uals follow a strategy that maximizes expected value in a one-armed bandit
individuals rely on information about the self and generalize problem.
964 DENRELL

reduced the number of outgroup friends in subsequent years, even views about outgroups (Ellison & Powers, 1994; Pettigrew, 1997;
when initial attitude and the initial number of outgroup friends van Dick et al., 2004).
were controlled for. The probability of such encounters, however, depends on group
Given these assumptions, the model of experience sampling size. As noted by Blau, in his work on intergroup contact (Blau,
implies that average evaluations of outgroup members will contain 1977, 1994), due to the numerical dominance of the majority,
a negativity bias. This occurs even if negative impressions would members of the majority group are less likely than members of
not influence information processing or behavior during interac- minority groups to meet members of other groups (unless there is
tions and lead to a self-fulfilling prophecy. Thus, experience substantial segregation). More generally, outgroup size should be
sampling suggests a different mechanism that leads to negative positively associated with outgroup contact and familiarity, as
impressions of outgroup members. demonstrated in several studies of intergroup contact (Islam &
The result will likely be different for evaluations of members of Hewstone, 1993a; Liebkind, Nystrom, Honkanummi, & Lange,
the ingroup, however. As demonstrated in several field studies 2004), familiarity with the outgroup (Kalin, 1996; Kalin & Berry,
(Islam & Hewstone, 1993a; S. Levin et al., 2003) individuals are 1982), and intergroup marriage (Blau & Schwartz, 1984).
likely to interact with members of the ingroup, even if the ingroup Because one is less likely to encounter members of small
is relatively small. A tendency to associate with members of the outgroups, negative impressions will be more persistent for small
ingroup was also illustrated in an experiment by Shah, Brazy, & outgroups. Thus, attitudes toward the outgroup will be positively
Higgins (2004). In one of their studies (Experiment 2), group associated with the size of the outgroup. In particular, because
membership was manipulated, and the seating preferences of sub- members of the minority are more likely to meet members of the
jects were observed. Subjects sat at a significantly smaller average majority than members of the majority are to meet members of the
distance from a chair when they thought it was occupied by an minority, the attitudes of members of the minority toward mem-
ingroup member than when they thought it was occupied by an bers of the majority should be more favorable than vice versa.
outgroup member.21 If the ingroup is defined so that it includes Several studies have found this pattern. Brewer and Campbell
members of the family, close colleagues, or fellow students, it may (1976, pp. 65 66), on the basis of a survey of 30 ethnic groups in
also be difficult to avoid interacting with ingroup members. Fi- Kenya, Tanzania, and Uganda, showed that the favorability of the
nally, observations of oneself may be integrated into ingroup attitude toward the outgroup was positively and significantly cor-
evaluations (Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996). related with outgroup population size (the average correlation was
When individuals are likely to interact with members of the 0.37). Although it is possible that the effect of size is a result of
ingroup but can more easily avoid members of the outgroup if they differences in group status, Kalin and Berry (1982), on the basis of
have had negative experiences with them, the model of experience a census in Canada and a survey of attitudes toward seven ethnic
sampling implies that average evaluations of the ingroup will be groups, showed that the favorability of the attitude toward the
higher than average evaluations of the outgroup. Thus, experience outgroup was positively and significantly correlated with the pro-
sampling suggests a different mechanism leading to an ingroup portion of the outgroup in the respondents census unit. Outgroup
bias. The argument is not that experience sampling could provide proportion was also associated with familiarity with the outgroup.
an alternative explanation for the findings of experiments with Kalin (1996) replicated these findings using a later survey and
minimal groups (Tajfel et al., 1971), in which subjects are arranged census. Liebkind et al. (2004), on the basis of a national probability
randomly into groups and do not have any opportunity for inter- sample in Sweden and Finland, also showed that outgroup size was
action.22 Rather, the claim is that experience sampling provides a associated with outgroup contact, which was associated with more
different mechanism by which ingroup bias can develop in real favorable attitudes toward the outgroup.23
groups, in which there are opportunities for interactions. Meta- In a series of studies, Verkuyten (1992 [Table 1], 1996 [Table
analyses have also shown that the effect size is larger in real 1], 2005 [Table 1]) and Kinket and Verkuyten (1999) have also
groups than it is in artificially created groups (Mullen et al., 1992). shown that members of minority groups in Holland have more
Moreover, experience sampling is likely to reinforce any existing favorable attitudes toward the Dutch majority than the Dutch
biases that may have been generated by the motive to maintain a majority has toward minority members. Similarly, Tropp and
positive social identity, self-fulfilling prophecies, or illusory cor- Wright (2003) found that Mexican American children had more
relations. If outgroup members are avoided, negative views about favorable attitudes toward European American children than vice
them are less likely to be exposed to disconfirming evidence. versa. Stephan and Stephan (1984, Table 11.1) also found that
Beyond this, the model of experience sampling also suggests
that attitudes toward outgroups depend on group size. In particular,
21
negative attitudes are especially likely to develop toward members The tendency to avoid the outgroup was also stronger for individuals
of minority groups. Whereas individuals may avoid individuals with a higher motivation for interpersonal security. I am grateful to James
Shah, Paige Brazy, and Tory Higgins for providing these data, which were
belonging to outgroups with which they have negative experi-
not reported in Shah et al. (2004).
ences, they may nevertheless sometimes encounter such individu- 22
This is unless it is assumed that ingroup judgments are anchored on
als. They may have to interact with them at work, in school, or in the self (Cadinu & Rothbart, 1996). In such a case, because the sample size
their neighborhood. Such encounters provide sources of additional of performances by the self is larger than the sample size of the perfor-
information and may lead to acquaintances and friendship with mances by others, an ingroup bias can still emerge.
outgroup members, as demonstrated by van Dick et al. (2004). 23
This was found even when controlling for age, education, reported
Acquaintances and friendship with outgroup members, in turn, ingroup identification, and perceived status difference between the ingroup
have been shown to be especially important in reducing negative and the outgroup.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 965

students of Hispanic origins had more favorable attitudes toward findings of the effects of a high concentration of African Ameri-
Anglo American students in a New Mexico high school than vice cans (e.g., Pettigrew, 1959; M. C. Taylor, 1998; Williams, 1964).
versa.24 Several other surveys of students and children have found Homophily and biases in performance evaluation. Experience
similar results (Aboud & Skerry, 1984; Bagley & Verma, 1975; sampling also suggests a different mechanism leading to system-
Brand, Ruiz, & Padilla, 1974; DSouza, 1978; Rice, Ruiz, & atic biases in performance evaluations and selection decisions.
Padilla, 1974). Although such results could be due in part to Most research on performance evaluation has focused on cognitive
differences in status between minorities and majorities, Li and and social factors that influence impression formation (DeNisi,
Hong (2001) also showed that the mainland Chinese minority in 1997; Feldman, 1981; Flynn, Chatman, & Spataro, 2001; Hogan,
Hong Kong had a more accurate perception of the cultural values 1987; Ilgen, Barnes-Farrell, & McKellin, 1993; Judge & Ferris,
of the majority than vice versa. Some studies, however, have found 1993) and systematic biases have been attributed to prior expec-
a conflicting pattern. Islam and Hewstone (1993a) showed that the tations based on schemas and stereotypes of ethnicity or gender
Hindu minority in Bangladesh had less favorable attitudes toward (Arvey, 1979; Heilman, 2001; Kanter, 1977; Klein & Snyder,
the Muslim majority than vice versa.25 Corenblum and Stephan 2003). Numerous studies have also demonstrated the influence of
(2001, Table 1) showed that native Canadians had a less favorable stereotypes and group membership on evaluations, attributions,
attitude toward the white majority than vice versa.26 and selection decisions (e.g., Arvey, 1979; Biernat & Kobrynow-
The proposition that a small group would show more favorable icz, 1997; Davison & Burke, 2000; Elliott & Smith, 2004; Heilman
attitudes to outgroups may seem to go against arguments and & Guzzo, 1978; Kraiger & Ford, 1985; Nieva & Gutek, 1980;
findings that ingroup bias is inversely associated with the relative Olian, Schwab, & Haberfeld, 1988; Sackett & DuBois, 1991;
size of the ingroup (for a meta-analysis, see Mullen et al., 1992). Swim, Borgida, Maruyama, & Myers, 1989). Stereotypes and prior
However, attitudes toward outgroups are not the same as ingroup expectations have also been shown to lead to behavioral confir-
bias, usually defined as the difference between the evaluation of mation and self-fulfilling prophecies (e.g., Chen & Bargh, 1997;
the ingroup and the outgroup.27 Nevertheless, the different argu- Klein & Snyder, 2003; Word et al., 1974).
ments can be reconciled by noticing that relative ingroup size is As a complement to explanations emphasizing the influence of
likely to have two different effects on attitudes toward the out- stereotypes, experience sampling suggests that the tendency to
group. First, as the relative size of the ingroup increases, the associate with socially similar others can lead to systematic dif-
probability of contact with the outgroup decreases, which leads to ferences in evaluations as a function of group membership. In
less favorable attitudes toward the outgroup. Second, as the rela- particular, systematic differences in evaluations of individuals of
tive size of the ingroup increases, members of the ingroup may feel different groups would emerge under the following four
less threatened by the outgroup (Giles & Evans, 1986; R. A. conditions.
Levine & Campbell, 1972; D. M. Taylor & Moghaddam, 1994), First, impressions and evaluations are based on, or influenced
and ingroup membership may become less salient (Leonardelli & by, observations obtained through personal encounters. By work-
Brewer, 2001), both of which may lead to more favorable attitudes ing together with others, seeking their advice, or discussing pro-
toward the outgroup. The above discussion focused on the first fessional matters with them, individuals form impressions about
effect. To examine the second effect in field studies, one needs to the abilities of colleagues. In many organizations, as well as in
control for the probability of contact. If the second effect is also professions, such impressions provide the basis for, or provide an
present, the effect of ingroup size on outgroup attitudes should be important input to, evaluation and selection decisions. In other
positive when the probability of contact is controlled for. Liebkind settings, evaluations may be based on information about perfor-
et al. (2004) found precisely this pattern of results. Ingroup size mance on tasks with measurable outcomes, such as sales, and
was negatively associated with favorable attitudes toward out- personal observations in interactions matter less.
groups, and this was due to the negative effect of ingroup size on Second, the probability of future interactions is influenced by
outgroup contact. However, if the probability of contact was current impressions of abilities. In some organizations this may not
controlled for, ingroup size had a positive effect on attitudes be possible because assignment and interactions are regulated by
toward outgroups. senior management. However, in several professions and in many
In summary, whereas differences in contact have long been organizations, individuals, and especially managers, have some
argued to be an important predictor of outgroup attitudes (Allport, discretion over the individuals with whom they will work. In
1954; Pettigrew, 1998), the model of experience sampling suggests addition, individuals can often decide whom they will approach for
a simple mechanism that can explain both the origins of a nega- advice on professional matters (Blau, 1962; Borgatti & Cross,
tivity bias in attitudes toward outgroups as well as the conse- 2003; Lazega, 2001; Wilensky, 1967). In such cases, current
quences of additional contact. It should be noted, however, that the impressions of ability may influence the probability of future
argument assumes that contact provides information that can lead
to more positive attitudes. Even if there is broad support for this 24
According to the 1990 census, 38% of all individuals in New Mexico
effect (Pettigrew, 1998; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2000), there are ex- had Hispanic origins.
ceptions. In particular, if a majority is strongly prejudiced against 25
The Muslim majority made more negative attributions about the
a minority, contact may have the opposite result. Islam and Hew- minority than vice versa, however (Islam & Hewstone, 1993b).
stone (1993a) argue that this explains why Hindus (the minority in 26
The majority had more negative stereotypes about native Canadians,
Bangladesh) who had more contact with the Muslim majority than however.
vice versa also had more negative attitudes to the majority. Sim- 27
The meta-analytic findings are also mainly based on experiments with
ilarly, Kalin (1996) suggests that this may explain the conflicting little opportunity for interaction.
966 DENRELL

contact and, thus, the probability of future sampling. If a manager (1992) found that male employees in an advertising agency were
believes that a colleague is especially competent, the manager may most likely to choose another male employee when asked about
be more likely to ask this colleague for advice and to include him whom you see socially outside of work and you know you can
or her in future working groups. Consistent with this, several count on, and were also most likely to choose another male
studies have shown that perceived competence, based on observa- employee when they were asked with whom you discuss what is
tions of successful task performance in previous interactions, going on in the organization and who are important sources of
strongly influences the choice of future working partners (Colella, professional advice (Ibarra, 1992, p. 431). Similarly, Lazega
DeNisi, & Varma, 1998; Gilchrist, 1952; Hinds, Carley, Krack- (2001, pp. 121123) showed that lawyers in a law firm were both
hardt, & Wholey, 2000; J. M. Levine, Snyder, & Mendez-Caratini, more likely to seek advice from and socialize outside work with
1982; Miller & Suls, 1977; Zander & Havelin, 1960). Studies of lawyers of the same gender.
advice networks in organizations have also shown that individuals Social interactions also make it possible to observe personal
are more likely to be asked for advice if they are perceived to be attributes of others, such as leadership ability and creativity. In
competent (Blau, 1962; Borgatti & Cross, 2003; Lazega, 2001, pp. addition, through social interactions one may hear about accom-
9596). plishments of which one was unaware. Information about such
Current impressions of abilities may also influence the proba- accomplishments can also come from other people in the same
bility of future observations of abilities if task assignments depend social network. Finally, social connections can directly influence
on current impressions of abilities. A subordinate who is believed the choice of working partners. As demonstrated in experiments
to be inferior may only be given less challenging tasks and, thus, and field studies (French, 1956; Hinds et al., 2000), individuals are
have little chance to display his or her talents. On the basis of a more likely to continue to work with another whose past perfor-
study of career paths in a large firm, Rosenbaum (1979) suggested mance is low if he or she is a friend. If this is true, individuals are
that this is an important reason for the limited opportunities facing more likely to get additional observations of individuals who are
individuals with poor initial performance: socially similar and, thus, more likely to be a friend.
Early winners are seen as high-potential people who can do no If these conditions are satisfied, the model of experience sam-
wrong and who are given additional opportunities and challenges, pling implies that negative impressions will be less stable, and thus
while those who do not win in the early competition are given little or evaluations will be higher for individuals who are socially similar
no chance to prove themselves again. (p. 236) to evaluators, consistent with findings from numerous field studies
(Elliott & Smith, 2004; Ferris et al., 1994; Judge & Ferris, 1993;
Similarly, Manzoni and Barsoux (2002), on the basis of interviews
Kraiger & Ford, 1985; Sackett & DuBois, 1991; Tsui & OReilly,
with managers and subordinates, argued that managers typically
1989; Turban & Jones, 1988; Varma & Stroh, 2001).28 The model
give seemingly poor performers routine assignments and projects.
is also consistent with findings that interpersonal attraction is
Third, evaluators sometimes interact with the individuals they
associated with high performance ratings (Cardy & Dobbins, 1986;
may be evaluating for reasons unrelated to their professional
Ferris et al., 1994; Judge & Ferris, 1993; Tsui & OReilly, 1989;
estimate of them. In particular, the reason for interaction may be
noninstrumental and social (Blau, 1962; Greenberger & Sorensen, Varma & Stroh, 2001; for a review, see Lefkowitz, 2000), a result
1971). In many organizations and professions, there are numerous usually attributed to a direct influence of attraction on either
occasions for such socially motivated interactions. A large amount perceived or actual performance (Robbins & DeNisi, 1994).29
of research has also shown that such social interactions are more In the model of experience sampling, a bias emerges even if it
likely to occur among individuals with similar characteristics, such is assumed that stereotypes and cognitive and perceptual biases do
as gender, ethnicity, education, and age (Byrne, 1971; Fischer, not influence observations or evaluations. Again, this does not
1982; Marsden, 1987; McPherson & Smith-Lovin, 1987; McPher- imply that stereotypes and cognitive biases are not important.
son et al., 2001). Research on informal interactions in organiza- Rather, experience sampling is likely to reinforce biases due to
tions also has shown that this principle holds for social and other flaws in information processing and preexisting stereotypes. Nev-
informal networks within organizations (Blau, 1962; Brass, 1985; ertheless, the model of experience sampling implies that theories
Greenberger & Sorensen, 1971; Ibarra, 1992, 1993; Lazega, 2001; relying on cognitive or perceptual biases are not necessary to
Lincoln & Miller, 1979; Sykes, Larntz, & Fox, 1976). Studies have explain biased evaluations. Thus, observations of biases in evalu-
also shown that social similarity between raters and ratees leads to ations in field studies should not necessarily be attributed to
interpersonal attraction (Ferris, Judge, Rowland, & Fitzgibbons, cognitive biases. The model of experience sampling also suggests
1994; Judge & Ferris, 1993; Tsui & OReilly, 1989; Varma & that biases may be underestimated in experiments on performance
Stroh, 2001). evaluation in which information is exogenously provided or sam-
Fourth, socially motivated interactions provide information
about behaviors and characteristics that may influence impressions 28
Moreover, the effect sizes observed in field studies, which have
and evaluations. Social interactions may lead to discussions about
seldom been large (e.g., Kraiger & Ford, 1985), are well within the range
professional events and issues that provide information about the
of what the model can generate under reasonable assumptions about
knowledge and insight of others. For example, studies have shown parameter values.
that friendship predicts whom individuals ask for advice and 29
Varma, DeNisi, and Peters (1996) found that diary keeping strength-
information (Cross, Rice, & Parker, 2001). If individuals are likely ened the influence of affect, which seems inconsistent with arguments
to be friends with socially similar others (Byrne, 1971), they are based on cognitive biases but is consistent with a model with experience
likely to have many conversations about professional events and sampling, if memories of past impressions are strengthened by diary
issues with socially similar others. Consistent with this, Ibarra keeping (see the above discussion about the effect of memory).
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 967

pling is unrelated to impressions. Even if biases are small in such competency evaluations. Ferris et al. (1994) also found that per-
contexts, as they sometimes are, they may be larger in field formance ratings were negatively correlated with spatial distance
settings in which information has to be actively sampled. Meta- between the supervisor and the subordinate.
analyses have also shown that effect sizes in studies of biases in The influence of physical attractiveness. Experience sampling
performance evaluation are larger in field studies (Kraiger & Ford, can also lead to systematic differences in evaluations of individ-
1985). uals who have attributes that make them more or less attractive to
If biases in evaluations can emerge even in the absence of any interact with. Consider, for example, the effect of physical attrac-
cognitive bias, this also implies that efforts to eliminate cognitive tiveness. Several studies have documented that physically attrac-
biases may not eliminate biases in evaluations. To eliminate biases tive individuals are often perceived in a more positive light than
in evaluations one may also have to formalize interactions among others (Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972; Eagly, Ashmore,
raters and ratees. The bias should be lower in firms with more Makhijani, & Longo, 1991; Feingold, 1992; Landy & Sigall, 1974;
formal interactions among raters and ratees, consistent with find- Langlois et al., 2000; Webster & Driskell, 1983), even for traits
ings that the proportion of women in managerial jobs is higher in that are not objectively associated with physical attractiveness
firms with more formalized personnel practices (Reskin & (Feingold, 1992). Existing explanations of this phenomenon have
McBrier, 2000). Moreover, because interactions are often more focused on evolutionary theories and on how positive feelings for
regulated in jobs at lower levels in the organization, and job attractive people may contribute to behavioral confirmation or a
performance at lower levels is typically easier to measure, the bias self-fulfilling prophecy (e.g., Feingold, 1992; Langlois et al.,
should be weaker at lower levels in the organization, consistent 2000).
with findings of decreasing disparity in promotions and wages of Experience sampling suggests an additional mechanism of how
minorities and majorities at lower levels in organizations (Cotter, attractiveness could influence impressions of other traits. Suppose
Hermsen, Ovadia, & Vanneman, 2001 [but see Baxter & Wright, that people are more willing to interact with physically attractive
2000]; Elliott & Smith, 2004; Maume, 2004). people. Such an assumption is consistent with studies that have
Finally, experience sampling is likely to be especially important found that physically attractive individuals are paid more attention
in professional communities, such as academia, in which interac- and are less likely to be avoided (for a review, see Langlois et al.,
tions are seldom structured but are determined mainly by assess- 2000). In addition, studies have shown that physical attractiveness
ments of professional and social benefits. If professionals tend to predicts popularity, both with the opposite and the same sex
associate with socially similar others, and thus get more informa- (Feingold, 1992). Reis et al. (1982) also showed that physical
tion about their accomplishments and capabilities, evaluations will attractiveness of males was associated with a larger quantity of
be higher for socially similar others, consistent with findings of social interactions with the opposite sex. Suppose also that phys-
systematic differences in promotion rates between men and ical attractiveness can compensate, to some extent, for other neg-
women in academia (Long, Allison, & McGinnis, 1993; McBrier, ative traits. People may be willing to interact with a physically
2003; Rosenfeld, 1981) and professions (Hagan & Key, 1995; attractive individual even if they believe that he or she is not very
Spurr & Sueyoshi, 1994). competent, for example. If this is true, false negative impressions
Proximity. Several studies have documented that proximity is regarding other traits, such as competence, will be less likely to
an important determinant of liking (Brewer & Campbell, 1976; persist for physically attractive individuals. Thus, experience sam-
Festinger, Schachter, & Back, 1950; Segal, 1974). Even if students pling suggests a simple mechanism for how an illusory correlation
are randomly assigned to rooms, individuals are more likely to can develop between physical attractiveness and other traits, such
become friends with and have a favorable impression of individ- as competence.
uals who are nearby (Segal, 1974). Although this effect can be
explained by several different mechanismsincluding simple Other Areas of Application
availability, the possibility that people tend to reward each other
more often than they punish one another (T. M. Newcomb, 1956), Although this article has focused mainly on systematic differ-
as well as mere exposure (Moreland & Beach, 1992; Zajonc, ences in evaluations, experience sampling also has implications
1968)it is also an implication of experience sampling. Numerous for beliefs about the prevalence of attitudes and individual
studies have shown that spatial proximity is likely to lead to characteristics.
informal communication through frequent exposure to others Trust. The bias produced by experience sampling suggests that
(Allen, 1978; Bochner, Duncan, Kennedy, & Orr, 1976; Hagstrom, individuals will, on average, underestimate the trustworthiness of
1965/1975; Kraut, Egido, & Galegher, 1990; Zahn, 1991). Prox- others. To develop this, note that perceptions of trustworthiness are
imity also predicts whom individuals ask for advice in organiza- often based on events in which individuals have trusted other
tions (Borgatti & Cross, 2003; Lazega, 2001, p. 121). Because individuals and such trust has not been betrayed (e.g., Boyle &
individuals are likely to be frequently exposed to spatially proxi- Bonacich, 1970; Deutsch, 1958; Lindskold, 1978). In other words,
mate others, and because such people are easily accessible, indi- beliefs are based on historical observations. Beliefs about trust-
viduals are more likely to interact with spatially proximate others worthiness, however, are also likely to influence the probability
of whom they may have a negative impression than with spatially that an individual will put him or herself in a situation so that he
distant others of whom they have a negative impression. The above or she will have to trust another individual (Hardin, 1992). If an
arguments then imply that individuals who are nearby will, on individual, based on previous observations, is considered to be less
average, be evaluated higher. This applies to both competency and than trustworthy, such an individual may be avoided (Gambetta,
liking evaluations, whereas other explanations, such as T. M. 1988). Moreover, even if this individual cannot be avoided, safe-
Newcombs (1956) thesis, are more plausible for liking than for guards can be used, such as formal contracts or other types of
968 DENRELL

incentives that protect against any adverse consequences of op- sion formation. It also suggests a number of new hypotheses about
portunistic behavior (Williamson, 1975). However, because such moderators of such biases.
safeguards typically imply that individuals will be punished for The fact that sampling can explain systematic biases in impres-
opportunistic behavior, the absence of opportunistic behavior does sion formation does not imply that cognitiveperceptual biases are
not provide much information about whether an individual is not significant. Many systematic biases in impression formation,
trustworthy (Eagly, Wood, & Chaiken, 1978; Strickland, 1958). As such as ingroup bias, are probably overdetermined and are the
demonstrated in an experiment by Malhotra and Murnighan result of both sampling and cognitiveperceptual biases. Never-
(2002), a binding contract may lead partners to attribute coopera- theless, as emphasized by Fiedler (1996, 2000), sampling models
tive behavior to the restrictions imposed by the contract rather than provide an alternative null model for evaluating the significance of
to the individuals themselves. This also implies that an initial false cognitiveperceptual biases. Standard null models typically as-
belief about an opponent can result in actions (i.e., the use of sume that if no systematic mental operations of distortions are at
safeguards) that make it very difficult to disconfirm the belief. This work, no systematic bias in impressions is expected. Sampling
analysis also suggests that in social settings in which safeguards models challenge this assumption and suggest that the appropriate
are considered inappropriate or are not used, such as in families or baseline, against which psychological effects should be evaluated,
in some organizations, the level of trust may be higher. In such is not 0.
settings, individuals cannot avoid others and have to rely on them, More generally, the model of experience sampling suggests that
and false beliefs about untrustworthiness will be less stable. More pragmatic constraints in impression formation are important. Much
generally, this reasoning suggests that (a) several moral traits may research on impression formation implicitly assumes that the goal
be underestimated if individuals avoid others who seem to lack of impression formation is, or should be, accuracy. However,
such traits, and (b) the perceived level of morality will be higher accuracy is seldom the only goal regulating impression formation
in settings in which avoidance or the use of safeguards is not processes, because outcomes also matter (Einhorn & Hogarth,
possible. 1978; Friedrich, 1993). Except in laboratory experiments, people
Social influence. Experience sampling also suggests a novel cannot always afford to continue to sample events or individuals if
way in which beliefs may be influenced by the immediate social they believe the outcome will be negative. As discussed above, and
environment (e.g., Festinger et al., 1950). Even if individuals have as illustrated by the experiment by Fazio et al. (2004) on attitude
no intrinsic preference for agreeing with the individuals they formation, such a concern for outcomes implies that negative
interact with most, they may nevertheless have more positive impressions may be persistent even if beliefs and evaluations
opinions about activities in which several of their friends and would be sensitive to new evidence. It also implies that biases in
colleagues are engaged than about activities in which few of their impression formation are likely to be stronger in situations in
friends and colleagues are engaged. The reason is that frequent which information has to be actively sampled and individuals care
interaction with friends and colleagues will provide individuals about the outcome of interactions as well as accuracy. Incorporat-
with many observations of these activities, irrespective of their ing such active sampling into experiments on impression forma-
opinion of these activities. Thus, false negative impressions are tion would make it possible to examine the relative magnitude of
more likely to be disconfirmed for such activities. For an illustra- experience sampling and flawed information processing in the
tion, consider researchers opinions about various theories or fields persistence of negative impressions.
of research. Because one cannot read everything, impressions Finally, experience sampling also implies that interventions that
about fields of research in which one is not personally involved try to eliminate individual biases, such as confirmation bias, may
must be based on a limited set of observations. If the initial not be sufficient. For example, although accountability can reduce
impression is negative, one is less likely to examine this field in the effect of first impressions and expectations (Tetlock, 1983), it
more detail. As a result, a negative opinion will be stable. How- may not eliminate the bias produced by experience sampling.
ever, if there are several researchers at your school who are active Evaluators may still be able to justify their decisions by referring
in a particular field of research, you may have to interact, in to information to which they have access, without realizing that the
seminars and otherwise, with individuals doing research in this sample to which they have access may be biased. To minimize the
field. Thus, more observations will be available, and the opinion effect of experience sampling, the evaluation process should be
may be corrected if unjustifiably negative, which might not have made more formal, with structured interactions (cf. Desforges et
happened otherwise. al., 1991). Although this may be possible in formal organizations,
it is difficult in most other settings, such as social and professional
Conclusion communities.

Even if impressions are based on all available evidence, and the References
evidence is accurately interpreted, impressions may nevertheless
be biased. This result is an implication of a common characteristic Aboud, F. E., & Skerry, S. A. (1984). The development of ethnic attitudes:
of impression formation: the tendency for individuals with a pos- A critical review. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15, 334.
Allen, T. J. (1978). Managing the flow of technology: Technology transfer
itive impression to continue sampling and for individuals with a
and the dissemination of technological information within the R&D
negative impression to stop sampling. Though not necessary, such organization. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
sequential sampling is often the only practical way to allocate Allport, G. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-
attention between alternative interaction partners. When sampling Wesley.
is sufficiently sensitive to the impression, this simple mechanism Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development
can provide an alternative explanation of several biases in impres- of interpersonal relationships. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 969

Anderson, N. H. (1959). Test of a model for opinion change. Journal of (Eds.), Groups in contact: The psychology of desegregation (pp. 281
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 371381. 302). New York: Academic Press.
Anderson, N. H. (1968). Application of a linear-serial model to a Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Perga-
personality-impression task using serial presentation. Journal of Person- mon Press.
ality and Social Psychology, 10, 354 362. Bukowski, W. M., Sippola, L., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (2000). Pages
Anderson, N. H. (1981). Foundations of information integration theory. from a sociometric notebook: An analysis of nomination and rating scale
New York: Academic Press. measures of acceptance, rejection, and social preference. New Directions
Anderson, N. H., & Farkas, A. J. (1973). New light on order effects in for Child and Adolescent Development, 88, 1126.
attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, Busemeyer, J. R., & Myung, I. J. (1988). A new method for investigating
88 93. prototype learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Aronson, E., Willerman, B., & Floyd, J. (1966). The effect of a pratfall on Memory, and Cognition, 14, 311.
increasing interpersonal attractiveness. Psychonomic Science, 4, 227 Bush, R. R., & Mosteller, F. (1955). Stochastic models for learning. New
238. York: Wiley.
Arvey, R. D. (1979). Unfair discrimination in the employment interview: Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.
Legal and psychological aspects. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 736 765. Cadinu, M. R., & Rothbart, M. (1996). Self-anchoring and differentiation
Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Ab- processes in the minimal group setting. Journal of Personality and
normal and Social Psychology, 41, 1230 1240. Social Psychology, 70, 661 677.
Bagley, C., & Verma, G. (1975). Inter-ethnic attitudes and behaviour in Camerer, C., & Ho, T.-H. (1999). Experience-weighted attraction learning
British multi-racial schools. In G. Verma & C. Bagley (Eds.), Race and in normal form games. Econometrica, 67, 837 874.
education across cultures (pp. 236 262). London: Heinemann. Cardy, R. L., & Dobbins, G. H. (1986). Affect and appraisal accuracy:
Baxter, J., & Wright, E. O. (2000). The glass ceiling hypothesis: A Liking as an integral dimension in evaluating performance. Journal of
comparative study of the United States, Sweden, and Australia. Gender Applied Psychology, 71, 672 678.
and Society, 14, 275294. Carranza, E., Prentice, D. A., & Larsen, J. (2004). Is whats good for the
Berscheid, E. (1985). Interpersonal attraction. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson goose better for the gander? Reactions to masculine women and femi-
(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., pp. 413 484). New nine men. Unpublished manuscript, Princeton University.
York: Random House. Chambliss, W. J. (1966). The selection of friends. Social Forces, 43,
Biernat, M., & Kobrynowicz, D. (1997). Gender and race-based standards 370 380.
of competence: Lower minimum standards but higher ability standards Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirmation
for devalued groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype ac-
544 557. tivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 541560.
Blau, P. M. (1962). Patterns of choice in interpersonal relations. American Cillessen, A., Bukowski, W., & Haselager, G. (2000). Stability of socio-
Sociological Review, 27, 4155. metric categories. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Develop-
Blau, P. M. (1963). The dynamics of bureaucracy: A study of interpersonal ment, 88, 7593.
relations in two governmental agencies. Chicago: University of Chicago Cohen, W., & Levinthal, D. (1994). Fortune favors the prepared firm.
Press. Management Science, 40, 227251.
Blau, P. M. (1977). Inequality and heterogeneity: A primitive theory of Colella, A., DeNisi, A. S., & Varma, A. (1998). The impact of ratees
social structure. New York: Free Press. disability on performance judgments and choice as partner: The role of
Blau, P. M. (1994). Structural contexts of opportunities. Chicago: Univer- disabilityjob fit stereotypes and interdependence of rewards. Journal of
sity of Chicago Press. Applied Psychology, 83, 102111.
Blau, P. M., & Schwartz, J. E. (1984). Crosscutting social circles: Testing Combs, B., & Slovic, P. (1979). Newspaper coverage of causes of death.
a macrostructural theory of intergroup relations. New York: Academic Journalism Quarterly, 56, 837 843.
Press. Corenblum, B., & Stephan, W. G. (2001). White fears and native appre-
Bochner, S., Duncan, R., Kennedy, E., & Orr, F. (1976). Acquaintance hensions: An integrated threat theory approach to intergroup attitudes.
links between residents of a high rise building: An application of the Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 33, 251268.
small world method. Journal of Social Psychology, 100, 277284. Cotter, D. A., Hermsen, J. M., Ovadia, S., & Vanneman, R. (2001). The
Bonney, M. E. (1947). Popular and unpopular children: A sociometric glass ceiling effect. Social Forces, 80, 655 681.
study. Sociometry Monographs (No. 9). Cross, R., & Parker, A. (2004). The hidden power of social networks:
Borgatti, S. P., & Cross, R. (2003). A relational view of information Understanding how work really gets done in organizations. Boston:
seeking and learning in social networks. Management Science, 49, Harvard Business School Press.
432 445. Cross, R., Rice, R. E., & Parker, A. (2001). Information seeking in social
Boyle, P., & Bonacich, P. (1970). The development of trust and mistrust in contexts: Structural influences and receipt of information benefits. IEEE
mixed-motives games. Sociometry, 33, 123139. Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics: Part C. Applications
Brand, E. S., Ruiz, R. A., & Padilla, A. M. (1974). Ethnic identification and and Reviews, 13, 438 448.
preference: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 860 890. Davison, H. K., & Burke, M. J. (2000). Sex discrimination in simulated
Brass, D. J. (1985). Mens and womens networks: A study of interaction employment contexts: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Voca-
patterns and influence in an organization. Academy of Management tional Behavior, 56, 225248.
Journal, 28, 327343. Dawes, R. M. (1993). Prediction of the future versus an understanding of
Brewer, M. B., & Campbell, D. T. (1976). Ethnocentrism and intergroup the past: A basic asymmetry. American Journal of Psychology, 106,
attitudes: East African evidence. New York: Sage. 124.
Brewer, M. B., & Kramer, R. M. (1985). The psychology of intergroup DeGroot, M. H. (1970). Optimal statistical decisions. New York: McGraw-
attitudes and behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 219 243. Hill.
Brewer, M. B., & Miller, N. (1984). Beyond the contact hypothesis: DeNisi, A. S. (1997). A cognitive approach to performance appraisal: A
Theoretical perspectives on desegregation. In N. Miller & M. B. Brewer program of research. London: Routledge.
970 DENRELL

Denrell, J. (2003). Vicarious learning, under-sampling of failure, and the Fiedler, K., & Walther, E. (2004). Stereotyping as inductive hypothesis
myths of management. Organization Science, 14, 227243. testing. New York: Psychology Press.
Denrell, J. (2005). Uncertainty aversion in experiential learning. Working Fischer, C. S. (1982). To dwell among friends: Personal networks in town
paper, Stanford Graduate School of Business. and city. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Denrell, J., & March, J. G. (2001). Adaptation as information restriction: Fiske, S. T. (1981). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact
The hot stove effect. Organization Science, 12, 523538. of negative and extreme behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Desforges, D. M., Lord, C. G., Ramsey, S. L., Mason, J. A., Van Leeuwen, Psychology, 38, 889 906.
A. D., West, S. C., & Lepper, M. R. (1991). Effects of structured Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (1991). Social cognition (2nd ed.). New York:
cooperative contact on changing negative attitudes toward stigmatized McGraw-Hill.
social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 531 Flynn, F. J., Chatman, J. A., & Spataro, S. E. (2001). Getting to know you:
544. The influence of personality on impressions and performance of demo-
Deutsch, M. (1958). Trust and suspicion. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 2, graphically different people in organizations. Administrative Science
265279. Quarterly, 46, 414 442.
Dion, K. K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. French, E. G. (1956). Motivation as a variable in work-partner selection.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285290. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 53, 96 99.
Dreben, E. K., Fiske, S. T., & Hastie, R. (1979). The independence of French, R. L., & Mensh, I. N. (1948). Some relationships between inter-
evaluative and item information: Impression and recall order effects in personal judgments and sociometric status in a college group. Sociom-
behavior-based impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social etry, 11, 335345.
Psychology, 37, 1758 1768. Friedrich, J. (1993). Primary error detection and minimization (PEDMIN)
DSouza, M. B. (1978). Intergroup attitudes in multi-ethnic schools. Ox- strategies in social cognition: A reinterpretation of confirmation bias
ford Review of Education, 4, 149 160. phenomena. Psychological Review, 100, 298 319.
Duflo, M. (1997). Random iterative models. New York: Springer-Verlag. Furnham, A. (1986). The robustness of the recency effect: Studies using
Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). legal evidence. Journal of General Psychology, 113, 351357.
What is beautiful is good, but . . .: A meta-analytic review of research on Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The
the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 109 common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.
128. Gambetta, D. (1988). Can we trust trust? In D. Gambetta (Ed.), Trust:
Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Chaiken, S. (1978). Causal inferences about Making and breaking cooperative relationships (pp. 213238). Cam-
communicators and their effect on opinion change. Journal of Person- bridge, MA: Blackwell.
ality and Social Psychology, 36, 424 435. Gans, N., Knox, G., & Croson, R. (2004). Simple models of discrete choice
Einhorn, H. J. (1986). Accepting error in order to make less error. Journal and their performance in bandit experiments. Working paper, Wharton
of Personality Assessment, 50, 387395. School of Management, University of Pennsylvania.
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1978). Confidence in judgment: Persis- Gawronski, B., Ehrenberg, K., Banse, R., Zukova, J., & Klauer, K. C.
tence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review, 85, 395 416. (2003). Its in the mind of the beholder: The impact of stereotypic
Eiser, R. J., Fazio, R. H., Stafford, T., & Prescott, T. (2003). Connectionist associations on category-based and individuating impression formation.
simulation of attitude learning: Asymmetries in the acquisition of pos- Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 16 30.
itive and negative evaluations. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- Gilchrist, J. C. (1952). The formation of social groups under conditions of
letin, 29, 12211235. success and failure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47,
Elliott, J. R., & Smith, R. A. (2004). Race, gender, and workplace power. 174 187.
American Sociological Review, 69, 365386. Giles, M. W., & Evans, A. S. (1986). The power approach to intergroup
Ellison, C. G., & Powers, D. A. (1994). The contact hypothesis and racial hostility. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 30, 469 486.
attitudes among Black Americans. Social Science Quarterly, 75, 385 Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isnt so: The fallibility of human
400. reason in everyday life. New York: Free Press.
Erev, I., & Roth, A. E. (1998). Predicting how people play games: Rein- Greenberger, E., & Sorensen, A. (1971). Interpersonal choices among a
forcement learning in experimental games with unique, mixed strategy junior high school faculty. Sociology of Education, 44, 198 216.
equilibria. American Economic Review, 88, 848 881. Guadagni, P., & Little, J. D. C. (1983). A logit model of brand choice based
Fazio, R. H., Eiser, J. R., & Shook, N. J. (2004). Attitude formation on scanner data. Marketing Science, 2, 203238.
through exploration: Valence asymmetries. Journal of Personality and Hagan, J., & Key, F. (1995). Gender in practice: A study of lawyers lives.
Social Psychology, 87, 293311. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
Feingold, A. (1992). Good-looking people are not what we think. Psycho- Hagstrom, W. O. (1975). The scientific community. Carbondale: Southern
logical Bulletin, 111, 304 341. Illinois University Press. (Original work published 1965)
Feldman, J. M. (1981). Beyond attribution theory: Cognitive processes in Hamilton, D., & Gifford, R. (1976). Illusory correlation in interpersonal
performance appraisal. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, 127148. perception: A cognitive bias of stereotypic judgments. Journal of Ex-
Ferris, G. R., Judge, T. A., Rowland, K. M., & Fitzgibbons, D. E. (1994). perimental Social Psychology, 12, 392 407.
Subordinate influence and the performance evaluation process: Test of a Hardin, R. (1992). The street-level epistemology of trust. Analyse & Kritik,
model. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 14, 152176.
101135. Heckman, J. J. (1979). Sample selection bias as a specification error.
Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. W. (1950). Social pressures in Econometrica, 47, 153161.
informal groups: A study of human factors in housing. Stanford, CA: Heckman, J. J. (2001). Micro data, heterogeneity, and the evaluation of
Stanford University Press. public policy: Nobel Lecture. Journal of Political Economy, 109, 673
Fiedler, K. (1996). Explaining and simulating judgment biases as an 748.
aggregation phenomenon in probabilistic, multiple-cue environments. Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription: How gender stereo-
Psychological Review, 103, 193214. types prevent womens ascent up the organizational ladder. Journal of
Fiedler, K. (2000). Beware of samples! A cognitive ecological sampling Social Issues, 57, 657 674.
approach to judgment bias. Psychological Review, 107, 659 676. Heilman, M. E., & Guzzo, R. A. (1978). The perceived cause of work
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 971

success as a mediator of sex discrimination in organizations. Organiza- Kalin, R. (1996). Ethnic attitudes as a function of ethnic presence. Cana-
tional Behavior and Human Performance, 21, 346 357. dian Journal of Behavioral Science, 28, 171179.
Helmreich, R., Aronson, E., & LeFan, J. (1970). To err is humanizing Kalin, R., & Berry, J. W. (1982). The social ecology of ethnic attitudes in
sometimes: Effects of self-esteem, competence, and a pratfall on inter- Canada. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 14, 97109.
personal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, Kanter, R. M. (1977). Men and women of the corporation. New York:
259 264. Basic Books.
Henderson-King, E. I., & Nisbett, R. E. (1996). Anti-Black prejudice as a Karlin, S., & Taylor, H. M. (1975). A first course in stochastic processes.
function of exposure to the negative behavior of a single black person. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 654 664. Kashima, Y., & Kerekes, A. R. Z. (1994). A distributed memory model for
Hertwig, R., Barron, G., Weber, E. U., & Erev, I. (in press). Risky averaging phenomena in person impression formation. Journal of Ex-
prospects: When valued through a window of sampled experiences. In perimental Social Psychology, 30, 407 455.
K. Fiedler & P. Juslin (Eds.), Information sampling and adaptive cog- Kashima, Y., Woolcock, J., & Kashima, E. S. (2000). Group impressions
nition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. as dynamic configurations: The tensor product model of group impres-
Hewstone, M., Rubin, M., & Willis, H. (2002). Intergroup bias. Annual sion formation and change. Psychological Review, 107, 914 942.
Review of Psychology, 53, 575 604. Kelley, H. H. (1948). First impressions in interpersonal relations. Unpub-
Hilton, J. L., & Fein, S. (1989). The role of typical diagnosticity in lished doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
stereotype-based judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Kelley, H. H. (1950). The warm cold variable in first impressions of
ogy, 57, 501511. persons. Journal of Personality, 18, 431 439.
Hilton, J. L., & von Hippel, W. (1996). Stereotypes. Annual Review of Kinket, B., & Verkuyten, M. (1999). Intergroup evaluations and social
Psychology, 47, 237271. context: A multilevel approach. European Journal of Social Psychology,
Hinds, P. J., Carley, K. M., Krackhardt, D., & Wholey, D. (2000). Choos- 29, 219 237.
ing work group members: Balancing similarity, competence, and famil- Klayman, J., & Ha, Y.-W. (1987). Confirmation, disconfirmation, and
iarity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 81, information in hypothesis testing. Psychological Review, 94, 211228.
226 251. Klein, O., & Snyder, M. (2003). Stereotypes and behavioral confirmation:
Hoffman, L. R. (1962). A note on ratings versus choices as measures of From interpersonal to intergroup perspectives. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
group attraction. Sociometry, 25, 313320. Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 153234).
Hogan, E. A. (1987). Effects of prior expectations on performance ratings: New York: Academic Press.
A longitudinal study. Academy of Management Journal, 30, 354 368. Kraiger, K., & Ford, J. K. (1985). A meta-analysis of ratee race effects in
Hogarth, R. M., & Einhorn, H. J. (1992). Order effects in belief updating: performance ratings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 56 65.
The belief-adjustment model. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 155. Kraut, R. E., Egido, C., & Galegher, J. (1990). Patterns of contact and
Homans, G. C. (1954). The cash posters: A study of a group of working communication in scientific research collaborations. In J. Galegher,
girls. American Sociological Review, 19, 724 733. R. E. Kraut, & C. Egido (Eds.), Intellectual teamwork: Social and
Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behavior: Its elementary forms. New York: technological foundations of cooperative work (pp. 149 171). Hillsdale,
Harcourt, Brace & World. NJ: Erlbaum.
Huston, T. L., & Levinger, G. (1978). Interpersonal attraction and rela- Krueger, J. (1996). Personal beliefs and cultural stereotypes about racial
tionships. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 115156. characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 536
Ibarra, H. (1992). Homophily and differential returns: Sex differences in 548.
network structure and access in an advertising firm. Administrative Krueger, J., & Zeiger, J. (1993). Social categorization and the truly false
Science Quarterly, 37, 422 447. consensus effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
Ibarra, H. (1993). Personal networks of women and minorities in manage- 670 680.
ment: A conceptual framework. Academy of Management Review, 18, Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bul-
56 87. letin, 108, 480 498.
Ilgen, D. R., Barnes-Farrell, J. L., & McKellin, D. B. (1993). Performance Landy, D., & Sigall, H. (1974). Beauty is talent: Task evaluation as a
appraisal process research in the 1980s: What has it contributed to function of the performers physical attractiveness. Journal of Person-
appraisals in use? Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro- ality and Social Psychology, 29, 299 304.
cesses, 54, 321368. Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M.,
Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993a). Dimensions of contact as predic- & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and
tors of intergroup anxiety, perceived out-group variability, and out- theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390 423.
group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Lazega, E. (2001). The collegial phenomenon: The social mechanisms of
Bulletin, 19, 700 710. cooperation among peers in a corporate law partnership. Oxford, En-
Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993b). Intergroup attributions and affec- gland: Oxford University Press.
tive consequences in majority and minority groups. Journal of Person- Lee, P. M. (1989). Bayesian statistics: An introduction. London: Edward
ality and Social Psychology, 64, 936 950. Arnold.
Jennings, H. H. (1950). Leadership and isolation: A study of personality in Lefkowitz, J. (2000). The role of interpersonal affective regard in super-
inter-personal relations. New York: Longmans, Green. visory performance ratings: A literature review and proposed causal
Judge, T. A., & Ferris, G. R. (1993). Social context of performance model. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 73,
evaluation decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 36, 80 105. 67 85.
Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1995). Naturally occurring interpersonal expec- Leman, T. B., & Solomon, R. L. (1952). Group characteristics as revealed
tancies. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 74 108. in sociometric patterns and personality ratings. Sociometry, 15, 790.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Leonardelli, G. J., & Brewer, M. B. (2001). Minority and majority dis-
Psychological Review, 80, 237251. crimination: When and why? Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of ogy, 37, 468 485.
decisions under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263291. Levin, I. P., & Schmidt, C. F. (1969). Sequential effects in impression
972 DENRELL

formation with binary intermittent responding. Journal of Experimental Evidence from a random-effects model of managerial attainment. Work
Psychology, 79, 283287. and Occupations, 31, 250 274.
Levin, I. P., & Schmidt, C. F. (1970). Differential influence of information McBrier, D. B. (2003). Gender and career dynamics within a segmented
in an impression formation task with binary intermittent responding. professional labor market: The case of law academia. Social Forces, 81,
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 374 376. 12011266.
Levin, S., van Laar, C., & Sidanius, J. (2003). The effects of ingroup and McFadden, D. (1981). Structural discrete probability models derived from
outgroup friendships on ethnic attitudes in college: A longitudinal study. theories of choice. In C. F. Manski & D. McFadden (Eds.), Structural
Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 76 92. analysis of discrete data with econometric applications (pp. 198 272).
Levine, J. M., Snyder, H. N., & Mendez-Caratini, G. (1982). Task perfor- Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
mance and interpersonal attraction in children. Child Development, 53, McPherson, J. M., & Smith-Lovin, L. (1987). Homophily in voluntary
359 371. organizations: Status distance and the composition of face-to-face
Levine, R. A., & Campbell, D. T. (1972). Ethnocentrism: Theories of groups. American Sociological Review, 52, 370 379.
conflict, ethnic attitudes, and group behavior. New York: Wiley. McPherson, J. M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a
Li, Q., & Hong, Y.-Y. (2001). Intergroup perceptual accuracy predicts feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27,
real-life intergroup interactions. Group Processes and Intergroup Rela- 415 444.
tions, 4, 341354. Melfi, V. F., & Page, C. (2000). Estimation after adaptive allocation.
Liebkind, K., Nystrom, S., Honkanummi, E., & Lange, A. (2004). Group Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference, 87, 353363.
size, group status, and dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup Meyer, R. J., & Shi, Y. (1995). Sequential choice under ambiguity:
attitudes. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 7, 145159. Intuitive solutions to the armed-bandit problem. Management Science,
Lincoln, J. R., & Miller, J. (1979). Work ties and friendship ties in 41, 817 834.
organizations: A comparative analysis of relation networks. Administra- Miller, R. L., & Suls, J. M. (1977). Affiliation preferences as a function of
tive Science Quarterly, 24, 181199. attitude and ability similarity. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social
Lindskold, S. (1978). Trust development, the GRIT proposal, and the comparison processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 103
effects of conciliatory acts on conflict and cooperation. Psychological 124). Washington, DC: Wiley-Halsted.
Montoya, R. M., & Horton, R. S. (2004). On the importance of cognitive
Bulletin, 85, 772793.
evaluation as a determinant of interpersonal attraction. Journal of Per-
Locksley, A., Borgida, E., Brekke, N., & Hepburn, C. (1980). Sex stereo-
sonality and Social Psychology, 86, 696 712.
types and social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Moran, G. (1966). Dyadic attraction and oriental consensus. Journal of
ogy, 39, 821 831.
Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 94 99.
Long, J. S., Allison, P. D., & McGinnis, R. (1993). Rank advancement in
Moreland, R. L., & Beach, S. (1992). Exposure effects in the classroom:
academic careers: Sex differences and the effects of productivity. Amer-
The development of affinity among students. Journal of Experimental
ican Sociological Review, 58, 703722.
Social Psychology, 28, 255276.
Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and
Mullen, B., Brown, R., & Smith, C. (1992). Ingroup bias as a function of
attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently con-
salience, relevance, and status: An integration. European Journal of
sidered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,
Social Psychology, 22, 103122.
2098 2109.
Newcomb, A. F., & Bukowski, W. H. (1984). A longitudinal study of the
Lott, A. J., & Lott, B. E. (1961). Group cohesiveness, communication
utility of social preference and social impact sociometric classification
level, and conformity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62,
schemes. Child Development, 55, 1434 1447.
408 412.
Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Childrens peer
Lott, A. J., & Lott, B. E. (1972). The power of liking: Consequences of relations: A meta-analytic review of popular, rejected, neglected, con-
interpersonal attitudes derived from a liberalized view of secondary troversial, and average sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113,
reinforcements. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social 99 128.
psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 109 148). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Newcomb, T. M. (1947). Autistic hostility and social reality. Human
Luce, R. D. (1959). Individual choice behavior: A theoretical analysis. Relations, 1, 69 86.
New York: Wiley. Newcomb, T. M. (1953). An approach to the study of communicative acts.
Maassen, G. H., van der Linden, J. L., Goossens, F. A., & Bokhorst, J. Psychological Review, 60, 393 404.
(2000). A ratings-based approach to two-dimensional sociometric status Newcomb, T. M. (1956). The prediction of interpersonal attraction. Amer-
determination. Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 88, ican Psychologist, 11, 575585.
5573. Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York: Holt,
Madon, S., Jussim, L., & Eccles, J. (1997). In search of the powerful Rinehart & Winston.
self-fulfilling prophecy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Nieva, V. F., & Gutek, B. A. (1980). Sex effects on evaluation. Academy
72, 791 809. of Management Review, 5, 267276.
Malhotra, D., & Murnighan, J. K. (2002). The effects of contracts on Nisbett, R. E., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and
interpersonal trust. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 534 559. shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Manski, C. F. (1995). Identification problems in the social sciences. Nisbett, R. E., & Smith, M. (1989). Predicting interpersonal attraction from
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. small samples: A reanalysis of Newcombs acquaintance study. Social
Manzoni, J.-F., & Barsoux, J.-L. (2002). The set-up-to-fail syndrome: How Cognition, 7, 6773.
good managers cause great people to fail. Boston: Harvard Business Olian, J. D., Schwab, D. P., & Haberfeld, Y. (1988). The impact of
School Press. applicant gender compared to qualifications on hiring recommendations:
March, J. G. (1996). Learning to be risk averse. Psychological Review, 103, A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Organizational Behavior and
309 319. Human Decision Processes, 41, 180 195.
Marsden, P. V. (1987). Core discussion networks of Americans. American Peeters, G. (1971). The positivenegative asymmetry: On cognitive con-
Sociological Review, 52, 122131. sistency and positivity bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1,
Maume, D. J., Jr. (2004). Is the glass ceiling a unique form of inequality? 455 474.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 973

Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1992). Explaining the evidence: Tests of the Shah, J. Y., Brazy, P. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Promoting us or
Story Model for juror decision making. Journal of Personality and preventing them: Regulatory focus and manifestations of intergroup
Social Psychology, 62, 189 206. bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 433 446.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1959). Regional differences in anti-Negro prejudice. Shaw, M. E., & Gilchrist, J. C. (1955). Repetitive task failure and socio-
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 28 36. metric choice. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 50, 29 32.
Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Generalized intergroup contact effects on preju- Skowronski, J. J., & Carlston, D. E. (1989). Negativity and extremity
dice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 173185. biases in impression formation: A review of explanations. Psychological
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Bulletin, 105, 131142.
Psychology, 49, 65 85. Snyder, M., Campbell, B. H., & Preston, E. (1982). Testing hypotheses
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2000). Does intergroup contact reduce about human nature: Assessing the accuracy of social stereotypes. Social
prejudice? Recent meta-analytic findings. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing Cognition, 1, 256 272.
prejudice and discrimination: The Claremont Symposium (pp. 93114). Snyder, M., & Stukas, A. A. (1999). Interpersonal processes: The interplay
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral activities in social interaction.
Plant, E. A. (2004). Responses to interracial interactions over time. Per- Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 273303.
sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1458 1471. Snyder, M., & Swann, W. B. (1978). Hypothesis-testing processes in social
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2003). The antecedents and implications of interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1202
interracial anxiety. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1212.
790 801. Spurr, S., & Sueyoshi, G. T. (1994). Turnover and promotion of lawyers:
Rabin, M. (2002). Inference by believers in the law of small numbers. An inquiry into gender differences. Journal of Human Resources, 20,
Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117, 775 816. 813 842.
Reeder, G. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic model of disposi- Stangor, C., & McMillan, D. (1992). Memory for expectancy-congruent
tional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review, 86, and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and
6176. social development literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 42 61.
Reis, H. T., Wheeler, L., Spiegel, N., Kernis, M. H., Nezlek, J., & Perri, M. Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1984). The role of ignorance in
(1982). Physical attractiveness in social interactions: II. Why does intergroup relations. In N. Miller & M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Groups in
appearance affect social experience? Journal of Personality and Social
contact: The psychology of desegregation (pp. 229 256). New York:
Psychology, 45, 979 996.
Academic Press.
Reskin, B. F., & McBrier, D. B. (2000). Why not ascription? Organiza-
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of
tions employment of male and female managers. American Sociological
Social Issues, 41, 157175.
Review, 65, 210 233.
Stewart, R. H. (1965). Effect of continuous responding on the order effect
Rice, A. S., Ruiz, R. A., & Padilla, A. M. (1974). Person perception,
in personality impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social
self-identity, and ethnic group preference in Anglo, Black, and Chicano
Psychology, 1, 161165.
preschool and third-grade children. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol-
Strickland, L. H. (1958). Surveillance and trust. Journal of Personality, 26,
ogy, 5, 100 108.
200 215.
Robbins, T. L., & DeNisi, A. S. (1994). A closer look at interpersonal
Sunnafrank, M. (1986). Predicted outcome value during initial interactions:
affect as a distinct influence on cognitive processing in performance
A reformulation of uncertainty reduction theory. Human Communication
evaluation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 341353.
Research, 13, 333.
Rosenbaum, J. E. (1979). Tournament mobility: Career patterns in a
Swim, J. K., Borgida, E., Maruyama, G., & Myers, D. G. (1989). Joan
corporation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 220 241.
Rosenfeld, R. A. (1981). Academic mens and womens careers. Social McKay versus John McKay: Do gender stereotypes bias evaluations?
Science Research, 10, 337363. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 409 429.
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobsen, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Sykes, R. E., Larntz, K., & Fox, J. C. (1976). Proximity and similarity
Teacher expectations and childrens intellectual development. New effects of frequency of interaction in a class of naval recruits. Sociom-
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. etry, 39, 263269.
Ross, L., Greene, D., & House, P. (1977). The false consensus effect: An Tajfel, H. (Ed.). (1982). Social identity and intergroup relations. Cam-
egocentric bias in social perception and attribution process. Journal of bridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 279 301. Tajfel, H., Flament, C., Billig, K., & Bundy, R. (1971). Social categoriza-
Ross, S. M. (2000). Introduction to probability models (7th ed.). San tion and intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 1,
Diego, CA: Harcourt Academic Press. 149 175.
Rothbart, M., & John, O. P. (1985). Social categorization and behavioral Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup
episodes: A cognitive analysis of the effects of intergroup contact. conflict. In G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of
Journal of Social Issues, 41, 81104. intergroup relations (pp. 33 47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Sackett, P. R., & DuBois, C. L. Z. (1991). Raterratee race effects on Taylor, D. A., Altman, I., & Sorrentino, R. (1969). Interpersonal exchange
performance evaluations: Challenging meta-analytic conclusions. Jour- as a function of rewards and costs and situational factors: Expectancy
nal of Applied Psychology, 76, 873 877. confirmation disconfirmation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Schwartz, B. (1982). Reinforcement-induced behavioral stereotypy: How ogy, 5, 324 339.
not to teach people to discover rules. Journal of Experimental Psychol- Taylor, D. M., & Moghaddam, F. M. (1994). Theories of intergroup
ogy: General, 111, 2359. relations: International social psychological perspectives (2nd ed.).
Schwartz, M. S. (1966). Effectance motivation and interpersonal attrac- Westport, CT: Praeger.
tion: Individual differences and personality correlates. Unpublished Taylor, M. C. (1998). How White attitudes vary with the racial composi-
doctoral dissertation, University of Texas. tion of local populations: Numbers count. American Sociological Re-
Segal, M. W. (1974). Alphabet and attraction: An unobtrusive measure of view, 63, 512535.
the effect of propinquity in a field setting. Journal of Personality and Taylor, S. E. (1982). The availability bias in social perception and inter-
Social Psychology, 30, 654 657. action. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment
974 DENRELL

under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 190 200). Cambridge, ethnic identity among adolescents in the Netherlands. Journal of Social
England: Cambridge University Press. Psychology, 132, 741750.
Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Accountability and the perseverance of first impres- Verkuyten, M. (1996). Personal self-esteem and prejudice among ethnic
sions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 285292. majority and minority youth. Journal of Research in Personality, 30,
Thibaut, J. W., & Coules, J. (1952). The role of communication in the 248 263.
reduction of interpersonal hostility. Journal of Abnormal and Social Verkuyten, M. (2005). Ethnic group identification and group evaluation
Psychology, 47, 770 777. among minority and majority groups: Testing the multiculturalism hy-
Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social psychology of groups. pothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 121138.
New York: Wiley. Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revision of
Towles-Schwen, T., & Fazio, R. H. (2003). Choosing social situations: The stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45,
relation between automatically activated racial attitudes and anticipated 961977.
comfort interacting with African Americans. Personality and Social Webster, M., & Driskell, J. E. (1983). Beauty as status. American Journal
Psychology Bulletin, 29, 170 182. of Sociology, 89, 140 165.
Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York: McGraw- Wetherill, G. B. (1966). Sequential methods in statistics. London:
Hill. Methuen.
Tropp, L. R., & Wright, S. C. (2003). Evaluations and perceptions of self, Wilensky, H. (1967). Organizational intelligence. New York: Basic Books.
ingroup, and outgroup: Comparisons between Mexican-American and Williams, D. (1991). Probability with martingales. Cambridge, England:
European-American children. Self and Identity, 2, 203221. Cambridge University Press.
Tsui, A. S., & OReilly, C. A. (1989). Beyond simple demographic effects: Williams, R. M. (1964). Strangers next door: Ethnic relations in American
The importance of relational demography in superiorsubordinate dyads. communities. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Academy of Management Journal, 32, 402 423. Williamson, O. E. (1975). Markets and hierarchies: Analysis and anti-trust
Turban, D. B., & Jones, A. P. (1988). Supervisorsubordinate similarity: implications. New York: Free Press.
Types, effects, and mechanisms. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, Wojciszke, B., Brycz, H., & Borkenau, P. (1993). Effects of information
228 234. content and evaluative extremity on positivity and negativity biases.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 327335.
frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 207232. Word, C. O., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). The non-verbal mediation
van Dick, R., Wagner, U., Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wolf, C., Petzel, T., of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction. Journal of Exper-
et al. (2004). Role of perceived importance in intergroup contact. Jour- imental Social Psychology, 10, 109 120.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 211227. Ybarra, O. (2002). Naive causal understanding of valenced behaviors and
Van Duuren, F., & Di Giacomo, J. P. (1997). Degrading situations, affil- its implications for social information processing. Psychological Bulle-
iation, and social dependency. European Journal of Social Psychology, tin, 128, 421 441.
27, 495510. Ybarra, O., Schaberg, L., & Keiper, S. (1999). Favorable and unfavorable
Van Overwalle, F., & Labiouse, C. (2004). A recurrent connectionist target expectancies and social information processing. Journal of Per-
model of person impression formation. Personality and Social Psychol- sonality and Social Psychology, 77, 698 709.
ogy Review, 8, 28 61. Yechiam, E., & Busemeyer, J. R. (2005). Comparison of basic assumptions
Van Rooy, D., Van Overwalle, F., Vanhoomissen, T., Labiouse, C., & embedded in learning models for experience based decision-making.
French, R. (2003). A recurrent connectionist model of group biases. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 387 402.
Psychological Review, 110, 536 563. Zahn, G. L. (1991). Face-to-face communication in an office setting: The
Varma, A., DeNisi, A. S., & Peters, L. H. (1996). Interpersonal affect and effects of position, proximity, and exposure. Communication Research,
performance appraisal: A field study. Personnel Psychology, 49, 341 18, 737754.
360. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of
Varma, A., & Stroh, L. K. (2001). The impact of same-sex LMX dyads on Personality and Social Psychology Monograph, 9(2, Pt. 2), 127.
performance evaluations. Human Resource Management, 40, 309 320. Zander, A., & Havelin, H. H. (1960). Social comparison and interpersonal
Verkuyten, M. (1992). Ethnic group preferences and the evaluation of attraction. Human Relations, 13, 2132.
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 975

Appendix A

Derivation of the Asymptotic Density

Let Z be the random variable that the estimate, Xt, would converge to as The density, h(y), is thus
t 3 , if instead of sequential sampling it was assumed that the individual
was sampled in each period. Denote the density of Z by g(z). In addition, 1
ecSy 1 ey / 2v
2 2

let Y be the random variable that Xt converges to as t 3 . To derive the 2 v



density of Y, note that the process can be viewed as a semi-Markov process. , (A7)

cSy
1 y2 / 2v2
In a semi-Markov process, transitions between states follow a Markov e 1 e dy
process. The duration between transitions, however, is not fixed, as in
2 v
ordinary Markov processes, but follows a probability distribution that may
depend on the current state (e.g., Karlin & Taylor, 1975). In the above or
process, the (continuous) state space is the set of estimates. Because the
transitions between these states depend only on the existing state and the 1 1
ey / 2v Syc ey / 2v
2 2 2 2

value of the new observation, transitions are Markovian. Transitions are 2 v 2 v



. (A8)
only made, however, if the alternative is sampled. In this case, durations
1
1
ey / 2v Sycdy ey / 2v dy
2 2 2 2
between transitions follow a geometric probability distribution with mean
w(z) 1/p(z).
2 v
2 v
Using the theory of semi-Markov chains, it is possible to derive the
limiting distribution of the density of the estimates, h(y). To do so, note that Note that
for a semi-Markov chain with finite state space, the limiting proportion of
y2 (y Sv2 ) 2 S2 v2
time that the process will be in state i is 2 Sy . (A9)
2v 2v2 2
i i

i i i
, (A1) The limiting density can thus be written as

1 1
where i is the limiting probability that the underlying Markov process will 2 2
eS v / 2c
2 2
eySv ) ]/ 2v
2
ey / 2v
2 2

be in state i, and i is the expected duration in state i (e.g., Karlin & Taylor, 2 v 2 v

. (A10)
1975). In this case, the underlying Markov process has a limiting distri-
1 ySv2 ) 2 ]/ 2v2
1 y2 / 2v2
eS2 v2 / 2c
e dy e dy
bution with density g(z), and the expected duration in state z is w(z). The 2 v 2 v
probability that the underlying Markov process will be in the region [z, z

z] is thus G(z z) G(z). The expected duration in this region is


Because the integrals equal 1 (the expressions in the integrals are normal
approximately w(z). The limiting proportion of time that the process will be
densities), the limiting density is
in state [z, z z] is thus
1 1
Gz z Gzwz 2 2
eySv ) ]/ 2v ey / 2v

2 2 2 2 2
eS v / 2c
. (A2) 2 v 2 v
Gz z Gzwz (A11)
eS v / 2c 1
2 2

Dividing with z and taking limits as z goes to 0, it is the case that the
limiting density of the proportion of time that the process spends in state y or
is 2 2
eS v / 2c 1 2 2
1 1
eySv ) ]/ 2v ey / 2v .
2 2 2
(A12)
wygy 1 eS v / 2c
2 2
2 v 1 eS v / 2c
2 2
2 v

hy . (A3)

wygydy Thus, the density is a weighted combination of the density of a normal
random variable with a mean of Sv2 and the density of a normal random

variable with a mean of 0. It follows that the density of Y will be shifted
If the probability of sampling the alternative is to the left in comparison with the density of a normal random variable with
a mean of 0. It is also the case that the expected estimate is lower the higher
ecSy the value of S (S both reduces the mean of the first term and increases its
, (A4)
1 ecSy weight). Moreover, the expected estimate is higher the higher the value of
c (which increases the weight of the second term). Finally, because v2 is an
S 0, the mean duration of an estimate Y y is a geometrically distributed increasing function of b and the expected value of Equation A12 is a
random variable with an expected value of decreasing function of v2 (v2 reduces the mean and increases the weight of
the first term), the expected estimate is inversely associated with the value
1 1 ecSy
wy ecSy 1. (A5) of b.
py ecSy To illustrate the resulting distribution, Figure A1 plots the density
when S 3, c 0, b 0.5, and 2 1 together with the density that
Moreover, if X is a normally distributed variable with a mean of 0 and
would have resulted from random sampling. As illustrated, the distri-
variance 2, it follows that Z is normally distributed with a mean of 0 and
bution produced by experience sampling has been shifted to the left. In
variance
this case, the expected estimate, E(Y), can be shown to be 0.82, and
b2 2 the probability of a negative estimate is 0.87. As noted above, the bias


v2 . (A6) would be smaller if S were lower and sampling were less sensitive to the
2 b 1
2 1 estimate.
b
(Appendixes continue)
976 DENRELL

Figure A1. The asymptotic density of the estimate Y for random and experience sampling. In this example, the
probability of sampling is e3xt/1 e3xt, and b 0.5.

Appendix B

A General Model

Belief Formation For all processes satisfying these assumptions, and for any random
variable X with finite expectation and variance, the expected estimate,
Assume that the estimate in period t 1, xt1 , is a weighted combina- E(Xt), will always be below E(X):
tion of the past estimate, xt, and the new observation in period t 1, xt1,
if any. Formally,
Proposition


1 bt1 xt bt1 xt1
if an observation is made in period t 1 For all t 1, E(Xt) E(X).
xt1 xt . (B1)
if no observation is made in period t 1 Proof
Assume also that bt (0, 1). Note that the value of bt can differ between The expected value of Xt1, conditional upon the fact that Xt xt is
periods. As a result, the above specification includes several different
belief-formation processes. For example, it includes a process in which the EXt1 Xt xt pt1 xt1 bt1 xt bt1 EX 1 pt1 xtxt
first impression is especially important (i.e., where b1 is large). Moreover,
it includes a belief-formation process in which the estimate at the begin- (B3)
ning of period t 1 is the average of all observations made in period t or
or
before. In this case,

1 EXt1 Xt xt xt bt1 pt1 xtEX bt1 pt1 xtxt. (B4)


bt1 , (B2)
nt 1
Because
where n(t) is the number of observations made in period t and before. If X
is normally distributed and the prior is also normally distributed, the above EX tEXt1 Xt xt EXt1 (B5)
process also includes Bayesian updating. In this case, the mean of the
(e.g., Williams, 1991), this implies that
posterior distribution is simply a weighted average of the mean of the prior
and the new observation.
EXt1 EX tXt bt1 EX tpt1 XtEX bt1 EX tpt1 XtXt. (B6)

Sampling Process Because

Let pt1(xt) be the probability of sampling in period t 1 given the EX tpt1 XtX covX tpt1 Xt, Xt EX tpt1 XtEX tXt, (B7)
estimate Xt xt at the beginning of period t 1. Note that the function
pt1(xt) may differ between periods. Assume that pt1(xt) is a strictly this equals
increasing functionthat is, if xt,1 xt,2, then pt1(xt,1) pt1(xt,2).
Assume also that for all values of xt, pt1(xt) (0, 1) that is, assume that EX tXt1 bt1 EX tpt1 Xt
pt1(xt) cannot be 0. Finally, assume that all individuals sample in the first
period. bt1 EX tpt1 XtEX bt1 covX tpt1 Xt, Xt. (B8)
EXPERIENCE SAMPLING IN IMPRESSION FORMATION 977

Note that covXt[pt1(Xt), Xt] 0 if pt1() is a strictly increasing function EXt1 EX bt1 covX tpt1 Xt, Xt EX. (B11)
(Ross, 2000, p. 626).
Because EX1(X1) E(X), one gets The proposition now follows from the principle of induction. Q.E.D.
This proof assumes that the first impression is equal to the first obser-
EX2 EX b2 covX 1p2 X1 , X1 EX. (B9) vation. However, exactly the same proof holds if there is a prior and the
prior is equal to the expected value of the observation. If the prior differs
More generally, suppose that EXt(Xt) E(X). It then follows that E(Xt1) from the expected value of the observation, however, it is possible that
is strictly less than experience sampling will, temporarily, lead to overestimation. This could
happen if the expected value of the observation is strongly negative while
EX1 bt1 EX tpt1 Xt bt1 EX tpt1 XtEX
the prior is, say 0. In this case, individuals might avoid sampling after the
bt1 covX tpt1 Xt, Xt, (B10) first observation. If the estimate is a weighted average of the prior and the
first observation, however, the estimate, although strongly negative, might
or still be higher than the expected value of the observation.

Appendix C

The Influence of the Sampling Process

Case A If w(y) [dw1 (1 w1)p(Y)]/[dw2 (1 w2)p(Y)], this implies that

To model the influence of other factors on sampling, suppose that the 1



E1 wYY


probability of sampling is wd (1 w) pt1(xt), where w (0, 1), and E2 Y ywyh1 ydy , (C4)
E1 wY
d (0, 1) is an arbitrary constant. Here, w is a parameter regulating the wyh1 ydy

probability that sampling will be independent of the estimate and will be


determined exclusively by the arbitrary constant, d. It may be expected that
as w increases, the bias will decrease, and the estimate will increase. To where the index denotes that the expectation is with respect to the random
show this, consider the model in which bt is identical for all t (as in the A variable with density h1(y) or h2(y). This can be expressed as
Simple Example of the Effect of Experience Sampling section) and X is
normally distributed with a mean of and variance 2. For this model, it cov1 wY, Y E1 wYE1 Y cov1 wY, Y
E2 Y E1 Y (C5)
is the case that the expected asymptotic estimate, E(Y), is an increasing E1 wY E1 wY
function of w.
or

Observation 1
cov1
dw1 1 w1 pY
dw2 1 w2 pY
,Y

E(Y) is an increasing function of w. E2 Y E1 Y . (C6)
dw1 1 w1 pY
E1
dw2 1 w2 pY
Proof
Because
Let w2 w1. As demonstrated in Appendix A, the corresponding
densities are

1

dw1 1 w1 py
y dw2 1 w2 py
pydw2 w1
w2 1 w2 py 2
0, (C7)

gy this implies that [dw1 (1 w1)p(y)]/[dw2 (1 w2)p(y)] is a strictly


dw1 1 w1 pY


h1 y (C1) increasing function of y. It follows (Ross, 2000, p. 626) that

1


gydy
dw1 1 w1 pY dw1 1 w1 pY
cov1 , Y 0. (C8)
dw2 1 w2 pY
and
Thus, E2 (YD d) E1 (YD d) for all values of d, which implies that
1 E2(Y) E1(Y).
gy Notice also that because
dw2 1 w2 pY


h2 y . (C2)
1
1
gydy (C9)
dw2 1 w2 pY dw 1 wpy

is a strictly decreasing function of y for all w (0, 1), it still follows that
Notice that h2(y) can be written as E(Y) E(X).
dw1 1 w1 pY
h y Case B
dw2 1 w2 pY 1


h2 y . (C3)

dw1 1 w1 pY To illustrate the effect of the selectivity of sampling, suppose that the
h ydy probability of sampling is pt1(xt)m, where m (0, 1). Here pt1(xt) can be
dw2 1 w2 pY 1
any strictly increasing function. A smaller value of m implies that the
(Appendixes continue)
978 DENRELL

probability of sampling, for any given estimate, is higher, as illustrated in Let r m1 m2 0. It then follows that
Figure 6B. This also implies that the bias will be smaller and the estimate
higher.
py m1 1
py r , (C11)
Observation 2 py m2 py r

E(Y) is a decreasing function of m.


which is a strictly decreasing function of y. This implies (Ross, 2000, p.
Proof 626) that

Let m2 m1. Following the same reasoning as above, it is the case that

cov1
pY m1
,Y
cov1 pY m1
pY m2,Y 0 (C12)
pY m2


E2 Y E1 Y . (C10)
pY m1
E1 and, thus, that E2(Y) E1(Y).
pY m2

Appendix D

Estimates of Belief Updating

The available data consist of the development of the average impression be above the highest observed average impression and below the lowest
over time for different presentation orders and combinations of high-valued observed average impression, an implication of both a model with a
and low-valued adjectives. Because most studies display only the average constant and a model with a declining weight. Thus, if the average
impressions in graphs, the values had to be approximated from these impression after six highly valued adjectives was 17.5, I required that the
graphs, although exact figures are available in Anderson (1968; for the last value of the highly valued adjective be at least 17.5. In only one case
impression, see Table 2), Furnham (1986), Hogarth and Einhorn (1992), (Anderson, 1968) did this constraint influence the estimates (by producing
and Levin and Schmidt (1969, 1970, on like responses). Although this a lower estimate of the weight of the most recent observation).
procedure introduces errors, small changes in values do not change the Finally, some assumptions must be made about the initial impression
estimates much. Because no data on individuals are available, and because (i.e., the starting value for belief updating). One possibility is to assume
I am interested in parameter values that provide a good fit rather than tests that the initial impression is equal to the middle point of the scale used.
of significance, I chose to estimate parameters by minimizing the mean Alternatively, to incorporate any upward or downward bias in evaluations,
absolute deviation between the predictions of the model and the data. The the initial impression can be set equal to the average impression after
numerical minimizations were performed with Excels solver function, receiving the first piece of information, where the average is taken over a
using several different starting points, and were checked by numerical counterbalanced set of combinations of high and low adjectives. This
simulations programmed in C. procedure is consistent with a model in which the weight of the most recent
In addition to estimating the parameters of the models (i.e., b, , , etc.), observation is constant or declining. I made use of the second assumption.
the values of the stimulithat is, the adjectives presented often have to However, both assumptions produce very similar estimates. For the data in
also be estimated. Few experiments (with the exception of Levin and Levin and Schmidt (1969, 1970), which only present the proportion of like
Schmidt, 1969, 1970) have reported the average impression of the adjec- responses and not the average impression, I took the middle point of the
tives presented. Thus, if an experiment made use of high- and low-valued scale they used (i.e., 3).
adjectives, in different orders, the values of both of these had to be
estimated. The estimates of the values of the adjectives were constrained to Received November 11, 2003
fall within the scale used (i.e., 1 8 for the data presented in Stewart, 1965). Revision received March 6, 2005
In addition, I also required that the estimates of the values of the adjectives Accepted March 24, 2005

The author has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen