Sie sind auf Seite 1von 17

Developmental Psychology 2010 American Psychological Association

2010, Vol. 46, No. 5, 1159 1175 0012-1649/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0020659

Trajectories of Pure and Co-Occurring Internalizing and Externalizing


Problems From Age 2 to Age 12: Findings From the National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care

Kostas A. Fanti Christopher C. Henrich


University of Cyprus Georgia State University

How and why do internalizing and externalizing problems, psychopathological problems from different
diagnostic classes representing separate forms of psychopathology, co-occur in children? We investigated
the development of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from ages 2 to 12
with the use of latent class growth analysis. Furthermore, we examined how early childhood factors
(temperament, cognitive functioning, maternal depression, and home environment) and early adolescent
social and behavioral adjustment variables were related to differential trajectories of pure and co-
occurring internalizing and externalizing problems. The sample (National Institute of Child Health and
Human Development Study of Early Child Care) consisted of 1,232 children (52% male). Mother reports
on the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991, 1992) were used to construct the trajectories of
externalizing and internalizing problems. Analyses identified groups of children exhibiting pure and
co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems. Children exhibiting continuous externalizing or
continuous co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems across the 10-year period under
investigation were more likely to (a) engage in risky behaviors, (b) be associated with deviant peers, (c)
be rejected by peers, and (d) be asocial with peers at early adolescence. However, children exhibiting
pure internalizing problems over time were only at higher risk for being asocial with peers as early
adolescents. Moreover, the additive effects of individual and environmental early childhood risk factors
influenced the development of chronic externalizing problems, although pure internalizing problems
were uniquely influenced by maternal depression. Results also provided evidence for the concepts of
equifinality and multifinality.

Keywords: internalizing, externalizing, co-occurrence, latent class growth analysis, developmental psy-
chopathology

Internalizing and externalizing problems may develop as early logical problems from different diagnostic classes representing
as early childhood and can place children on a developmental separate forms of psychopathology, co-occur in children (Oland &
pathway to peer problems, negative interactions with parents, Shaw, 2005). We examined the co-occurrence of internalizing and
delinquency, and other negative social and behavioral outcomes externalizing problems from a developmental perspective, such
(Coie & Dodge, 1998; Keiley, Lofthouse, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, that co-occurrence was investigated in terms of varied patterns of
2003; Kovacs & Devline, 1998). Internalizing problems appear in growth trajectories over the course of childhood and not just
the form of withdrawal, anxiety, fearfulness, and depression, similar levels at a given time. We also followed Kaplan, Crawford,
whereas externalizing problems take the form of hyperactivity, Cantell, Kooistra, and Deweys (2006) suggestion that the tempo-
aggression, defiance, and destructive behavior (Achenbach, 1991, ral term co-occurrence should be used when investigating devel-
1992; Campbell, 1995). Early emotional and behavioral problems opmental problems instead of the term comorbidity, which is used
have been found to precede child, adolescent, and adult antisocial to indicate the presence of at least two psychopathological prob-
and depressive psychopathological problems. Therefore, it is of lems that are independent of each other. According to previous
great importance to identify children at risk for high and contin- research, internalizing and externalizing problems can and often do
uous internalizing and externalizing problems early in develop- co-occur at every point in development at which they have been
ment (Gilliom & Shaw, 2004; Moffitt, 1993). measured (e.g., Gilliom & Shaw, 2004; Keiley et al., 2003; Young-
A question that remains unanswered in the literature is how and strom, Findling, & Calabrese, 2003), suggesting that an apprecia-
why internalizing and externalizing problems, two psychopatho- tion of the concept of co-occurrence is essential for explaining the
development and understanding the etiology of externalizing and
internalizing problems.
We investigated the co-occurrence between internalizing and
Kostas A. Fanti, Department of Psychology, University of Cyprus,
Nicosia, Cyprus; Christopher C. Henrich, Department of Psychology,
externalizing problems within a developmental psychopathology
Georgia State University. conceptual framework. Developmental psychopathology is con-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kostas cerned with individual differences in the origins, course, and
A. Fanti, Department of Psychology, University of Cyprus, P.O. Box outcomes of normative and psychopathological developmental
20537, CY 1678, Nicosia, Cyprus. E-mail: kfanti@ucy.ac.cy processes (Cicchetti, 1984; Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). Furthermore,

1159
1160 FANTI AND HENRICH

this framework conceives of development as an active dynamic children follow a high and consistent trajectory of aggression,
process and is concerned with the time course of psychopathology conduct problems, and delinquency. Children with chronic exter-
for different groups of individuals. In addition, central to the nalizing problems are also at higher risk of experiencing peer
developmental psychopathology approach is the belief that the rejection and of being involved with deviant peers, and these
study of psychopathology can inform researchers understanding experiences appear to maintain and exacerbate their behavioral
of normal development and, conversely, that the study of norma- problems (Laird, Jordan, Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 2001).
tive development may shed light on the etiology and course of The life-course persistent trajectory of externalizing behavior
psychopathology. has been associated with prenatal and perinatal medical risks, and
Following the developmental psychopathology perspective, the these problems have been found to be related to infant neuropsy-
current study investigated how children deviate from normal de- chological risk (Brennan, Hall, Bor, Najman, & Williams, 2003;
velopment and follow trajectories of pure and co-occurring inter- Moffitt, 1993). Neuropsychological deficits can then impair the
nalizing and externalizing problems from age 2 to age 12. We also childs cognitive abilities and can also result in a difficult temper-
examined how early childhood risk factors measured during the ament. Early deficiencies in cognitive functioning and difficult
first 2 years of life, including child, family, and environmental risk temperament may set an individual on a pathway to exhibiting
factors, forecast the development of pure or co-occurring trajec- chronic externalizing problems (Lynam, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-
tories of behavioral and emotional problems. Finally, we investi- Loeber, 1993; Miner & Clarke-Stewart, 2008; Moffitt, 1990,
gated how the differential trajectories of internalizing and exter- 1993). In addition, family adversity, maternal depression, and
nalizing problems are related to early adolescent behavioral and sociodemographic risk factors, such as low socioeconomic status
social adjustment. Inclusion of early childhood risk factors and and single parent status, are among the strongest predictors of later
early adolescent adjustment variables in the trajectory models externalizing problems (Ackerman, DEramo, Umylny, Schultz, &
enabled us to investigate research questions key to developmental Izard, 2001; Brennan et al., 2003; Gross, Shaw, & Moilanen, 2008;
psychopathology: These questions pertain to equifinality, in which Shaw, Owens, Vondra, Keenan, & Winslow, 1996).
varied developmental pathways may lead to the same developmen-
tal outcome (i.e., to what extent trajectories of pure versus co- Development of Internalizing Problems
occurring risks are differentially associated with early adolescent
functioning), and multifinality, in which a single vulnerability In contrast to externalizing problems, internalizing problems on
factor or any one component or a single starting point may result average tend to increase gradually from infancy to early childhood
in diverse developmental outcomes (i.e., to what extent the early (Gilliom & Shaw, 2004), with girls showing a higher increase in
childhood risk factors forecast differential trajectories of pure vs. internalizing problems across time (Bongers, Koot, van der Ende,
co-occurring risks; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). & Verhulst, 2003). Furthermore, Achenbach, Howell, Quay, and
Conners (1991) compared children of different ages, from age 4 to
Development of Externalizing Problems age 16, and provided evidence that among clinically referred
children, internalizing problems (somatic complaints, withdrawal,
Externalizing behaviors, such as aggression and destructive anxiety, and depression) tend to increase with age. Increases in
behaviors, are often used by toddlers to solve conflicts with peers internalizing problems might be the result of cognitive maturation
or playmates, and with the development of cognitive abilities and (Kovacs & Devline, 1998), because improvements in cognitive
the skills to regulate emotions, aggressive and externalizing prob- abilities enable children to self-reflect and to remember and antic-
lems decrease and diminish over the preschool and school age ipate negative or depressive events. As with externalizing prob-
period (Coie & Dodge, 1998). However, a small number of anti- lems, children at high risk for exhibiting continuous internalizing
social children, representing 5% to 7% of the population, do not problems from toddlerhood to adolescence have been identified
outgrow the temper tantrums and the defiant and irritable behavior (Brendgen, Wanner, Morin, & Vitaro, 2005; Duggal, Carlson,
that characterizes toddlers and follow a life-course persistent tra- Sroufe, & Egeland, 2001; Sterba, Prinstein, & Cox, 2007). Sterba
jectory of externalizing problems (Moffitt, 1993). Previous work et al. (2007) used the same data as we used in the current study to
investigating heterogeneity in the course of externalizing prob- investigate the development of internalizing problems from age 2
lems, including studies that used the same data as we used in the to age 11. In addition to identifying children at high risk for
current investigation (Campbell, Spieker, Burchinal, Poe, & the developing internalizing problems, Sterba et al. provided evidence
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development for heterogeneity in the course of internalizing problems by iden-
[NICHD] Early Child Care Research Network, 2006; Fanti & tifying children exhibiting low, deceasing, and increasing internal-
Henrich, 2007; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, izing problems across development. Children exhibiting internal-
2004) and studies that used different data sets (Broidy et al., 2003; izing problems are often unable to form good peer relationships,
Nagin & Tremblay, 1999; Shaw, Gilliom, Ingoldsby, & Nagin, and they are more likely to engage in isolating behaviors and social
2003), have identified children following chronic, moderate- withdrawal (Coie & Dodge, 1998; Hogue & Steinberg, 1995;
desisting, high-desisting, and low-externalizing problem trajecto- Oland & Shaw, 2005). Because of these isolating behaviors, chil-
ries, and those differences have been found as early as the second dren who exhibit internalizing problems do not affiliate with
year of life for both boys and girls. The majority of children exhibit delinquent peers, and they are at lower risk for delinquent behav-
low levels of externalizing behavior over time, and most of the iors (Oland & Shaw, 2005).
children with moderate or high levels of externalizing behavior Environmental risk factors, such as low familial sociodemo-
problems early in development exhibit decreases in behavioral graphic status (SDS), exposure to a negative familial context, and
problems after the preschool years. Only a small percentage of maternal depression, have been associated with high levels of
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1161

internalizing problems across development (Duggal et al., 2001; in delinquent acts, and of being affiliated with deviant peers when
McLeod & Shanahan, 1996). Further, as with the development of compared with children with pure problem behaviors (Miller-
externalizing problems, difficult temperament and low cogni- Johnson, Lochman, Coie, Terry, & Hyman, 1998; Oland & Shaw,
tive abilities have been associated with the development of 2005; Wright, Zakriski, & Drinkwater, 1999). For these reasons, a
internalizing problems as well (Booth-LaForce & Oxford, 2008; greater understanding of co-occurrence is essential.
Bub, McCartney, & Willett, 2007; Keenan, Shaw, Delliquadri, The current study investigated the co-occurrence between inter-
Giovannelli, & Walsh, 1998). nalizing and externalizing problems (a) by identifying trajectories
over time that include both externalizing and internalizing behav-
Co-Occurrence Between Externalizing and iors, (b) by examining how early childhood risk factors are related
Internalizing Problems to the identified trajectories, and (c) by investigating how the
identified trajectories are related to early adolescent behavioral and
Different behavioral syndromes tend to correlate with one an- social adjustment. We expected to identify children at lower risk of
other, and children who score high in one syndrome tend to score developing internalizing or externalizing problems (representing
at average or high levels in other symptoms as well (Achenbach, the majority of the sample) and children at risk for chronically high
Conners, Quay, Verhulst, & Howell, 1989). Moreover, it has been levels of pure externalizing problems, pure internalizing problems,
proposed that pure and co-occurring psychopathologies might rep- and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems from
resent distinct, meaningful syndromes (Angold & Costello, 1992; age 2 to age 12. Early childhood risk factors, including negative
Lilienfeld, 2003; OConnor, McGuire, Reiss, Hetherington, & home environment, cognitive dysfunction, maternal depression,
Plomin, 1998). In support of this idea, studies using cross-sectional and difficult temperament, are hypothesized to place children at
and longitudinal statistical methods have provided evidence of the higher risk for developing co-occurring internalizing and external-
existence of pure internalizing, pure externalizing, and co- izing problems. On the other hand, children with pure internalizing
occurring internalizing and externalizing problems (Epkins, 2000; and externalizing trajectories are expected to experience milder
Gilliom & Shaw, 2004; Keiley, Bates, Dodge, & Pettit, 2000; forms of early childhood risk factors in comparison with children
Keiley et al., 2003; Reitz, Dekovic, & Meijer, 2005; Youngstrom who have co-occurring problems over time. In addition, we antic-
et al., 2003). These studies also suggest that the co-occurrence ipated that low individual and contextual adversity would be
between internalizing and externalizing problems has an early age associated with reduced likelihood of developing any internalizing
of onset. or externalizing problems. Further, children who have co-
Even though co-occurrence can be thought of as a distinct occurring internalizing and externalizing behavior problems over
syndrome, common environmental and individual risk factors have time are expected to face more relationship problems with peers
been proposed to account for the presence of pure and co- and to be at higher risk of exhibiting delinquent behaviors and of
occurring psychopathological problems (Angold & Costello, 1993; socializing with deviant peers during early adolescence in com-
Klein & Riso, 1993; OConnor et al., 1998). As shown in the parison with children following pure trajectories of behavioral and
previous sections, family SDS, difficult temperament, negative emotional problems. Children exhibiting pure externalizing prob-
home environment, maternal depression, and cognitive deficien- lems were also expected to be at higher risk for exhibiting risky
cies are associated with both internalizing and externalizing prob- behaviors compared with children exhibiting pure internalizing
lems, providing evidence for the process of multifinality, because problems and children at lower risk in terms of both problem
the same starting point might result in diverse behavioral prob- behaviors. Finally, we investigated the processes of equifinality
lems. These common risk factors could also account for the co- and multifinality.
occurrence between the two symptoms, with the co-occurrence
between emotional and behavioral problems being represented by Method
these overlapping risk factors and causal processes (Klein & Riso,
1993; Rutter, 1997). Research that tested this claim has found that The present study used data from the NICHD Study of Early
pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems are Child-Care, a study conducted by the NICHD Early Child Care
associated with similar negative developmental processes, al- Research Network supported by NICHD through a cooperative
though children exhibiting co-occurring behavioral and emotional agreement that called for scientific collaboration between the
problems are faced with higher levels of these risk factors (Epkins, grantees and the NICHD staff. Participants were recruited from
2000; Ge, Best, Conger, & Simons, 1996; Keiley et al., 2003; different hospitals across10 locations in the United States (Little
Lilienfeld, 2003; Milan, Pinderhughes, & the Conduct Problems Rock, Arkansas; Irvine, California; Lawrence, Kansas; Boston,
Prevention Research Group, 2006; Shaw et al., 2003). Massachusetts; Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; Pittsburgh, Pennsylva-
Additionally, previous research has suggested that co- nia; Charlottesville, Virginia; Morganton, North Carolina; Seattle,
occurrence may be related to the severity of psychopathology and Washington; Madison, Wisconsin). A total of 8,986 women gave
that individuals with co-occurring problems exceed individuals birth during the sampling period (January 1991 and November
exhibiting pure problems in terms of chronic history of mental 1991) across the different locations, and 60% (5,416) of those
illness, more physical health problems, greater functional interfer- women agreed to be contacted for a telephone interview. Of the
ence in daily life, and more impaired adaptation across domains women who agreed to participate, 56% (3,015) were selected on
such as education and social-support networks (Newman, Moffitt, the basis of conditional random sampling. The conditional random
Caspi, & Silva, 1998). Furthermore, children with co-occurring sampling procedure was used to ensure that the sample was rep-
internalizing and externalizing problems are in greater danger of resentative of single mothers, poverty status, ethnic minority, and
experiencing negative relationships with peers, of being involved low maternal education. Participants from this sample were ex-
1162 FANTI AND HENRICH

cluded if (a) mothers were younger than 18 at the time of the Risk Indexes
childs birth, (b) families were planning to move before the com-
pletion of the study, (c) children were born with disabilities or Items indicating medical risk and early SDS were dichotomized
remained in the hospital more than 7 days postpartum, and (d) into 0 (no risk) or 1 (risk). The items were dichotomized to allow
mothers could not communicate sufficiently in English (see http:// the creation of an overall medical risk score and an overall SDS
secc.rti.org). A total of 1,525 families were selected as eligible, but risk score. These risk indexes were created for the current study.
only 1,364 completed the home interview when the infant was 1 Interviews with the mother during the 1st and 6th month after birth
month old, and these families made up the final sample of the were used to establish any health problems the mother had during
study. The studys final sample was diverse in terms of gender pregnancy and any perinatal infant health problems during the 1st
(53% male), minorities (24% were minorities), maternal education and 6th month of age. Mothers also reported whether they smoked
(11% of the mothers had not completed high school), and marital during and after pregnancy and whether they or the infants were
status (14% were single). The average family income was 3.6 exposed to smoking by others (passive smoking). A score of 1 was
times the poverty threshold. given for (a) smoking by the mother, (b) passive smoking, (c)
The trajectory analyses for the current study were based on childs ear infection, (d) childs respiratory problems, (e) childs
1,232 children (52% male) whose mothers completed the Child injuries that required a visit to the doctor, and (f) maternal health
Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991, 1992) during at problems during pregnancy. The scores from these six items were
least two of the nine assessment periods, as was done in previous added together to create an overall medical risk index, which
research (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network,, 2004). The ranged from 0 to 6.
sample used for the current study was diverse in terms of ethnicity The SDS risk index reflected mothers reports of family fi-
(77.5% were White, 6% were of Hispanic descent, 11.7% were nances, maternal marital status, and maternal education, following
African American, 1.3% were Asian, and 3.5% represented other previous research (Henrich, Schwab-Stone, Fanti, Jones, & Ruch-
minority groups). In addition, 70% of the samples population kin, 2004). Mothers reported the familys income from 1 month to
reported that their income was above the poverty threshold, with a 24 months, and an income-to-needs ratio was calculated from the
mean annual income of $67,310; 20.4% scored below the poverty U.S. Census Bureau tables as the ratio of family income to the
threshold at some point during data collection, but not continu- appropriate poverty threshold for each household size and number
ously, with a mean annual income of $25,362; 9.6% scored below of children. The scores were averaged from 1 to 24 months, and
the poverty threshold continuously during data collection, with a families below the poverty threshold received a score of 1. In
mean annual income of $12,641. On the basis of interviews with addition, mothers were asked whether they were married and
mothers during the 1st and 6th month after birth, 10% of mothers whether the father was living with them in the same household.
had not completed high school, and 21% were single. During the same interview, mothers also reported their educational
level. A score of 1 was given if the mother reported a low
educational level (lower than high school) or if the mother was a
Externalizing and Internalizing Problems single parent. The SDS risk index ranged from 0 to 3.

Mothers rated their childrens externalizing and internalizing


problems at 2, 3, 4.5, 6, 7, 9, 10, 11, and 12 years of age with the Early Childhood Risk Factors
CBCL (Achenbach, 1991, 1992). There are two versions of the Early difficult temperament. Temperament was assessed
CBCL: the preschool version for children ages 23 and the school- twice with the Infant Temperament Questionnaire (Medoff-
age version for children ages 4 18. Both versions measure inter- Cooper, Carey & McDevitt, 1993). At the 1- and 6-month home
nalizing and externalizing problems, although some items vary to visits, mothers were asked to respond to 38 items that were
capture developmental changes. For the present study, following developmentally appropriate for young infants (rated on a 1 6
previous research (Gilliom & Shaw, 2004), items that appear on scale from almost never to almost always). The items provide
both versions of the CBCL were used to maximize comparability scores on five subscalesActivity, Adaptability, Approach,
over time. Nine items were used to measure externalizing prob- Mood, and Intensityand all the items can be combined into a
lems (doesnt seem to feel guilty after misbehaving, cant sit single Difficult Temperament scale. Because the stability from 1 to
still, restless, or hyperactive, cruel to animals, destroys his 6 months was high, r .77, p .001, a Difficult Temperament
own things, destroys things belonging to his family or others, scale based on the average score of all the items from 1 and 6
disobedient, gets in many fights, physically attacks people, months was created ( .70). Scores on the Infant Temperament
and temper tantrums or hot tempered). Six items were used to Questionnaire at 6 months have been shown to be related to the
measure internalizing problems (too fearful or anxious, self- development of childhood behavioral and emotional problems
conscious or easily embarrassed, shy or timid, withdrawn, (Booth-LaForce & Oxford, 2008; Miner & Clarke-Stewart, 2008).
doesnt get involved with others, unhappy, sad, or depressed, Early cognitive developmental status. The Bayley Scales of
and worries). Mothers rated how descriptive each item was of Infant Development-II (Bayley, 1993) were administered at 15 and
the childs usual behavior now or within the past 6 months on a 24 months. The Bayley Mental Development Index is the most
3-point scale (0 not true, 1 somewhat or sometimes true, 2 widely used measure of cognitive developmental status for chil-
very true or often true). The items were summed to represent a dren in the first 2 years of life. It assesses memory, learning, and
count of the number of externalizing (Cronbachs ranged from problem solving; sensory perceptual acuity and discriminations;
.75 to .80) or internalizing (Cronbachs ranged from .65 to .73) early verbal communication; and the ability to form generaliza-
behaviors weighted by frequency of occurrence. tions and classifications. The overall standardized score was based
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1163

on the average of the 15-month assessment and the 24-month study, which were based on the combined scores between the
assessment. This variable was used as an indicator for the childs mother and teacher reports, are Asocial With Peers (likes to be
early cognitive functioning. The stability from 15 to 24 months alone; .87) and Excluded by Peers (peers avoid this child;
was high, r .80, p .001. .91). Ladd, Herald-Brown, and Andrews (2009) provided
Early home environment. Quality and quantity of the social evidence for cross-informant correlations and adequate reliability
and physical resources and of support and stimulation in the and validity of the peer relational constructs measured with the
childrens home environment were assessed at ages 6 and 15 Child Behavior Scale experienced by youths ages 5 through 13.
months with the Home Observation of the Measurement of the The Asocial With Peers and Excluded by Peers subscales con-
Environment, Infant/Toddler version (Caldwell & Bradley, 1984). verged with peer reports on the same constructs and were both
This measure is a semistructured interview and observational related to relational competence constructs and a host of other
procedure designed to describe the stimulation and responsiveness negative developmental processes.
of mothers, their involvement with and acceptance of their chil-
dren, the availability of play and learning materials, and the
Analyses
organization and variety of the physical environment. An example
of an interview item is, the family has a fairly regular and Latent class growth analysis (LCGA) in Mplus 5.1 (Muthen &
predictable daily schedule for the child, and an example of an Muthen, 2007) was used to identify distinct groups of individual
observational item is, the house has at least one full shelf of trajectories for externalizing and internalizing problems and the
books. Each item is scored in a binary fashion (yesno), and the joint probabilities between externalizing and internalizing prob-
information used to score those items is obtained during the course lems. Multinomial logistic regressions in SPSS 17.0 were per-
of a home visit. The stability from 6 to 15 months was high, r formed to identify early childhood risk factors that distinguished
.72, p .001; therefore, the items from the 6- and 15-month membership in the identified groups, and analysis of covariance
assessments were added together to reflect a positive and enriched (ANCOVA) was used to compare the different trajectory groups
home environment ( .77). on measures of early adolescent social and behavioral adjustment.
Postpartum depression. When the study children were 1
month old, mothers filled out the Center for Epidemiological
Studies Depression Scale (Radloff, 1997). This scale is a widely Patterns of Pure and Co-Occurring Externalizing and
used self-report scale that measures symptoms of depression in Internalizing Problems Over Time
nonclinical populations. Respondents rate the frequency during the
LCGA identifies heterogeneous classes by modeling a mixture
past week of 20 symptoms. Response categories are rarely or none
of distinct multivariate normal distributions. LCGA uses a poly-
of the time (less than 1 day), some or a little of the time (12 days),
nomial function to model the relationship between an attribute, in
occasionally or a moderate amount of time (3 4 days), and most
this case externalizing or internalizing problems, and age (Muthen
or all of the time (57 days). The items used to create the depres-
& Muthen, 2007; Nagin, 1999; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999). The
sion score had high internal consistency ( .91).
function takes the following form:

Early Adolescent Behavioral and Social ytj jo j1 Age t ,


Adjustment Variables
where ytj is a latent variable that characterizes the level of exter-
Risky behavior at age 12. The Risky Behavior Protocol nalizing or internalizing problems for participant at time t given
(Conger & Elder, 1994) has three sections completed by the membership in class j. Aget is participant s age at time t, and
mother, the child, and a friend of the studys child. Mothers is a disturbance assumed to be normally distributed with zero mean
completed a 30-item questionnaire. The studys children and their and constant variance. The models coefficients, jo and j1, deter-
friends each completed 38 identical items that measure things mine the shape of the trajectory. The coefficients are superscripted
your friends do (19 items) and things you do (19 items). The by j to denote that they are not constrained to be the same across
questions ask whether the child or friend experiments with weap- j classes and are free to vary, which allows for cross-class differ-
ons, cigarettes, alcohol, or drugs. One total risk-taking score for ences in the shape of developmental trajectories. We also investi-
the children was created on the basis of the mother reports ( gated whether change over time is linear or whether it follows a
.71). The child self-reports and the friend reports resulted in a more complex, curvilinear pattern over time. To retain children
variable for delinquency committed by friends ( .82) and with incomplete assessments in the analysis, we used full infor-
another variable for delinquency committed by the child ( .73). mation maximum likelihood fitting in the Mplus software.
Child behavior with peers at age 12. The Child Behavior Furthermore, because all the variables were highly skewed with
Scale (Ladd & Profilet, 1996) consists of 24 filler items and 35 a large number of zeros at each time, a zero inflated Poisson model
items that are conceptually grouped into six subscales: Aggressive was used (Nagin & Land, 1993). This model is a two-class mixture
With Peers (seven items), HyperactiveDistractible (four items), model. The zero inflated Poisson model estimates a zero class,
Asocial With Peers (six items), AnxiousFearful (four items), including the children scoring zero in terms of externalizing or
Prosocial With Peers (seven items), and Excluded by Peers (seven internalizing problems, and a nonzero class, including the children
items). For the NICHD Early Child Care Research Networks with random zeros or who score higher than zero at different times.
(2004) Study of Early Child Care, mothers and teachers rated the If a child is in the nonzero class, the probability of a zero count is
childrens behaviors with peers on a 3-point scale (0 not true, expressed as a Poisson distribution. However, if a child is in the
1 sometimes true, 2 often true). The two subscales used in this zero class, a zero count has a probability of one.
1164 FANTI AND HENRICH

The LCGA model estimation in Mplus results in two outputs: peers, and peer exclusion. The ANCOVA analyses tested whether
(a) the shape and location of the different estimated class there were statistically reliable mean differences among the tra-
trajectories and (b) the posterior probability of class member- jectory groups after controlling for gender and adjusting the de-
ship. On the basis of Nagin and Tremblay (2001), when inspec- pendent variables for differences on covariates measured during
tion of the graphs suggests that a model with more classes the first 2 years of life: medical and SDS risk. As with simple
indicates the existence of similar classes of small theoretical im- regression, the covariates in the ANCOVA are used to account for
portance, the model with fewer and distinct classes is preferred. the variation they exert in the dependent variable, which increases
The average posterior probabilities and the entropy value are also the precision of comparisons among the groups.
taken under consideration to check for the precision of classifica-
tion and the degree to which the classes are distinguishable. Results
Average probabilities equal to or greater than .70 imply satisfac-
tory fit (Nagin, 2005), and an entropy value greater than .70 is Descriptive Statistics
preferred because it indicates clear classification and greater power
to predict class membership (Muthen, 2000). The means and standard deviations for externalizing and inter-
The model fit statistics used are the Bayesian information cri- nalizing problems from age 2 to age 12 are reported in Table 1.
terion (BIC) and the Lo, Mendel, Rubin (LMR) statistic. The BIC According to paired-sample t tests, there were significant mean-
is based on a maximization of a log likelihood function, and the level decreases in externalizing problems from ages 3 to 4.5,
model with a lower BIC is preferred (Schwartz, 1978). Moreover, t(1043) 13.80, p .001, from age 4.5 to 6, t(1022) 7.08, p
because the BIC criterion tends to favor models with fewer classes .001, from ages 6 to 7, t(992) 5.65, p .001, from ages 7 to 9,
by penalizing for the number of parameters (Bauer & Curran, t(948) 5.96, p .001, from ages 9 to 10, t(971) 4.19, p
2003), a likelihood statistic based on the sum of chi-square distri- .001, and from ages 10 to 11, t(981) 2.79, p .001, suggesting
butions was also used. Lo, Mendell, and Rubin (2001) adjusted the that there is a decrease in the average number of externalizing
likelihood ratio test so that it could be used in mixture modeling problems across time. Internalizing problems increased from ages
and to enable the comparison of nonnested models with different 2 to 3, t(1139) 6.25, p .001, from ages 3 to 4.5, t(1043)
numbers of classes. The LMR statistic tests k 1 classes against k 4.54, p .001, from ages 6 to 7, t(992) 3.59, p .001, and from
classes. A significant chi-square value ( p .05) indicates that the ages 10 to 11, t(981) 3.54, p .001. Furthermore, internalizing
k 1 class model is rejected in favor of the k class model. A problems decreased from ages 4.5 to 6, t(1022) 3.84, p .001,
nonsignificant chi-square value ( p .05) suggests that a model from ages 9 to 10, t(971) 1.99, p .001, and from ages 11 to
with one fewer class is preferred. 12, t(978) 2.90, p .001.
Finally, the intercept and growth parameters from the individual
class memberships for externalizing and internalizing problems Externalizing Problems Trajectories
were used to predict the probability of multiple class membership
in a joint statistical model. This statistical model, which is an To identify the optimal number of trajectories for externalizing
extension of the LCGA model, relates the entire longitudinal problems, models with one to six classes were estimated with the
course of behavioral and emotional problems and indicates the
probability that the identified subclasses co-occur between the two
types of behaviors (Muthen, 2000; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001). For Table 1
example, the chronic co-occurring group of children is composed Means, Standard Deviations, and Ranges for Externalizing and
of children who share common growth parameters for both high Internalizing Problems
externalizing and high internalizing problems. Thus, this model
Problem type
assigns class membership in trajectory classes across behaviors,
and age N M SD Min Max
taking into account longitudinal change over time. This type of
procedure is preferred over simple cross-tab analysis because it is Externalizing
based on latent classes and because it provides posterior probabil- 24 months 1,171 4.17 2.72 0 18
ities (Muthen, 2000; Nagin & Tremblay, 2001). 36 months 1,167 4.08 2.68 0 18
54 months 1,070 3.10 2.61 0 16
kindergarten 1,058 2.62 2.49 0 15
Early Childhood Risk Factors and Early Adolescent Grade 1 1,028 2.28 2.45 0 15
Grade 3 1,022 1.97 2.12 0 13
Adjustment Variables Grade 4 1,022 1.75 2.04 0 12
Grade 5 1,017 1.61 2.08 0 13
Multinomial logistic regression (MLR) analyses in SPSS were Grade 6 1,022 1.51 1.96 0 13
used to identify early childhood risk factors that discriminate Internalizing
among individuals with divergent pure or co-occurring develop- 24 months 1,171 1.16 1.26 0 8
mental trajectories. The analysis included four measures as inde- 36 months 1,167 1.41 1.42 0 11
pendent variablestemperament, cognitive functioning, maternal 54 months 1,070 1.60 1.51 0 8
kindergarten 1,058 1.44 1.52 0 8
depression, and familial environment controlling for gender, Grade 1 1,028 1.61 1.54 0 10
medical risk, and SDS risk. In addition, we used ANCOVA in Grade 3 1,022 1.64 1.70 0 12
SPSS to compare the identified trajectory groups in terms of early Grade 4 1,022 1.54 1.62 0 11
adolescent adjustment variables. The dependent variables were the Grade 5 1,017 1.68 1.71 0 10
Grade 6 1,022 1.55 1.75 0 11
childrens and the friends risky behaviors, asocial behavior with
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1165

use of LCGA. As shown in Table 2, the BIC scores kept decreas- Internalizing Problem Trajectories
ing up to the five-class model. However, the six-class model had
a higher BIC, and the LMR statistic fell out of significance, Models with one to five classes were estimated with LCGA to
suggesting that the five-class model fit the data better. Addition- identify the optimal number of trajectories for internalizing prob-
ally, inspection of the graphs suggested that the six-class model lems. The BIC statistic changed dramatically from Class 2 to Class
split the lower class into two smaller groups, which were of limited 3, but the change was much smaller from Class 3 to Class 4, which
theoretical importance. Furthermore, the mean probability score suggests that the biggest improvement in fit occurred from the
for the five externalizing classes ranged from .77 to .94, and the two-class model to the three-class model (Table 2). In addition, the
LMR statistic fell out of significance for the four-class model,
entropy value was .76, suggesting that the classes were well
suggesting that the three-class model fit the data better. Moreover,
separated. Moreover, we compared the linear LCGA model to a
the four- and five-class models indicated the existence of two and
curvilinear one to investigate whether change is linear or curvilin-
three, respectively, very similar low classes of small theoretical
ear. The BIC (33,920.43) for the curvilinear model was lower in
importance. Accordingly, the more parsimonious three-class
comparison with the BIC (33,951.81) for the linear model, indi-
model was selected. Furthermore, the BIC (28,519.71) for the
cating that the curvilinear model better represented the data.
curvilinear model was lower compared with the BIC (28,559.65)
The trajectory groups identified with LCGA are shown in Figure
for the linear model, indicating that the curvilinear model better
1A, and the unstandardized intercepts and linear and quadratic represented the data. In addition, the mean probability score for the
terms are shown in Table 3. Children assigned to the low group three internalizing classes ranged from .87 to .90, and the entropy
(27%, n 332) exhibited some externalizing problems early on, was .76, suggesting that the classes were well differentiated in this
which declined to almost zero across time. Children in the model.
moderate-desisting group (31.6%, n 389) represented the largest Figure 1B shows the three-class model, and Table 3 shows the
class of children in the sample, suggesting that this group exhibited unstandardized intercept, linear, and quadratic terms for the dif-
normative levels of externalizing problems. Children in the mod- ferent groups. Children assigned to the low group (39.3%, n
erate group (19.4%, n 239) started at the same level as the 484) scored low on internalizing problems across time. Children in
moderate-desisting group but remained at moderate levels for the moderate group represented approximately half the sample
externalizing problems across time, suggesting that these children (42.6%, n 525), suggesting that moderate levels of internalizing
are at high risk for exhibiting continuous moderate externalizing problems are normative. Children in the high group (18.1%, n
problems. Children in the high-desisting group (13.6%, n 168) 223) started at higher levels of internalizing problems compared
started at high levels of externalizing problems but desisted to low with the other two groups and showed an increase over time.
levels across time. Children in the chronic group (8.4%, n 104) According to chi-square analyses, the different classes were not
started higher on externalizing problems compared with any of the differentiated in terms of ethnicity, although more girls were in the
other groups and remained high on externalizing problems, with a high-internalizing group, 2(1, N 1232) 4.11, p .01.
quadratic deceleration across the 10-year period. According to
chi-square analyses, the identified groups were not differentiated
Joint Occurrence
in terms of ethnicity, but some gender differences emerged. More
specifically, the low group was composed of more girls, 2(1, N Initially, a 15-class joint model representing all possible classes
1232) 8.17, p .001, and the chronic group was overrepre- between the three-class model for internalizing and the five-class
sented by boys, 2(1, N 1232) 11.05, p .001. model for externalizing problems (3 5) was estimated. Table 4
shows the posterior probabilities of children in each class on the
basis of the initial joint analysis. The entropy for the 15-class
model identified in Table 4 was .72, which is considered to be
Table 2
good. However, some identified classes had very low probabilities.
Model Fit Statistics
Specifically, Groups 4, 9, 12, and 13 had probabilities below .50
Problem type and (see Table 4), suggesting that around half of the children in each
model identified BIC Entropy LMR of these classes did not fit the category to which they were
assigned. The rest of the classes had good posterior probabilities
Externalizing (.75 to .90), indicating that these classes were distinct from other
1-class model 40,344.45 n/a n/a
2-class model 36,020.01 .87 p .01
classes. To ensure that the different classes were distinct from one
3-class model 34,525.04 .85 p .01 another, Groups 4, 9, 12, and 13 were excluded from the analyses.
4-class model 34,099.31 .80 p .01 The intercept and growth terms representing these classes were not
5-class model 33,951.81 .76 p .04 included in the joint analyses, and the children in these low-
6-class model 33,961.40 .72 p .56 probability classes were forced to identify with a different class.
Internalizing
1-class model 31,887.70 n/a n/a Table 4 also shows the posterior probabilities for the 11-class
2-class model 29,203.12 .82 p .01 model. Most of the children from the dropped groups identified
3-class model 28,559.65 .76 p .01 with children in Groups 7 and 10. The entropy for the 11-class
4-class model 28,406.75 .71 p .20 model was .77, and all the classes had good posterior probabilities
5-class model 28,290.08 .69 p .15
(.72 to .90), suggesting that the classes in the 11-class model were
Note. Values in bold indicate the selected model. BIC Bayesian more distinct than those in the 15-class model. Furthermore, the
information criterion; LMR Lo, Mendel, Rubin statistic. BIC (62,569.22) for the 11-group model was lower compared with
1166 FANTI AND HENRICH

Figure 1. Latent class growth analysis model for externalizing and internalizing problems.

the BIC (62,845.02) for the 15-class model, indicating that the Low-risk groups. On the basis of Figure 2, Groups 1, 2, 5,
11-class model better represented the data. and 6 might be considered to be at low or normative risk because
Figure 2 demonstrates how the 11 identified classes changed they exhibited low or moderate desisting forms of externalizing
over time in terms of internalizing and externalizing problems. problems and low or moderate internalizing problems. Children
Mplus allows for the identified longitudinal latent classes to be assigned to Group 1 (17.1%, n 211) scored low in both problem
used as a known latent categorical variable, enabling the investi- behaviors. This lower risk group was used as a normative com-
gation of how membership in each group translates into longitu- parison group for further analyses. Children in Group 2 (13.1%,
dinal change for each measured variable. This type of multiple- n 161) exhibited low internalizing and moderate desisting
group mixture modeling is used when there is one latent externalizing problems. Children in Group 5 (8.1%, n 100)
categorical variable in the sample (i.e., individuals are placed in exhibited low externalizing and moderate internalizing problems.
different groups on the basis of their joint trajectories of internal- Children in Group 6 (15.5%, n 191) exhibited moderate inter-
izing and externalizing problems) that represents distinct groups of nalizing and moderate desisting externalizing problems.
individuals. When comparing Figures 1 and 2, we can conclude High-risk groups. Children assigned to Groups 3, 4, 7, 8, 9,
that there are no considerable deviations in terms of the develop- 10, and 11 were considered to be at higher risk. These groups can
ment of externalizing and internalizing problems, after taking into be divided in five different categories: (a) children in Group 4
account the co-occurrence between these behavioral problems. (1.9%, n 24), the pure chronic externalizing group, and Group
Furthermore, children in the chronic co-occurring group seem to 8 (3.2%, n 39), the chronic-externalizing and moderate-
be at higher risk compared with the rest of the groups in terms of internalizing group, exhibited chronic externalizing problems and
both internalizing and externalizing problems. normative levels (low and moderate) of internalizing problems, (b)
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1167

Table 3 group, the pure chronic-externalizing group, and the pure


Unstandardized Growth Factor Parameter Estimates and moderate-externalizing group compared with the low-risk group.
Standard Errors (in Parentheses) Children characterized by difficult temperament were more likely
to be classified in the chronic co-occurring group, the high-
Problem type and internalizing and high-desisting externalizing group, the chronic-
group Intercept Linear slope Quadratic term
externalizing and moderate-internalizing group, and the pure
Externalizing chronic-externalizing group compared with the low-risk group,
Low 2.44 (0.085) .304 (.056) .007 (.008) with the chronic co-occurring and the pure chronic-externalizing
Moderate desisting 3.96 (0.052) .183 (.021) .002 (.002) groups having the higher ORs. Children with lower cognitive
Moderate 3.97 (0.180) .057 (.021) .005 (.003)
abilities, on the basis of the Bayley Mental Development Index,
High desisting 6.42 (0.074) .082 (.022) .007 (.003)
Chronic 7.54 (0.057) .010 (.019) .005 (.002) were more likely to be classified in the pure chronic-externalizing
Internalizing group compared with the low-risk group. Finally, children whose
Low 0.64 (0.087) .019 (.052) .002 (.007) mothers scored high on postpartum depression were less likely to
Moderate 1.28 (0.057) .079 (.020) .005 (.002) be classified in the low-risk group than all the higher risk groups,
High 2.31 (0.076) .126 (.018) .008 (.002)
with the chronic co-occurring group having the higher OR.
The analyses proceeded to compare the seven higher risk
p .05. p .01.
groups. The MLR comparing these groups was significant, 2(42,
N 569) 108.42, p .001. The findings suggested that
children in Group 3 (7.4%, n 91), the pure moderate- children born into a low-SDS environment were at higher risk for
externalizing group, and Group 7 (15.1%, n 186), the moderate- being classified in the chronic co-occurring group compared with
externalizing and moderate-internalizing group, exhibited moder- the pure moderate-externalizing group (OR 1.46, CI [1.08,
ate externalizing problems and normative levels (low and 2.00], p .05), the pure chronic-externalizing group (OR 1.54,
moderate) of internalizing problems, (c) children in Group 9 CI [1.01, 2.41], p .05), the pure internalizing group (OR 2.03,
(2.3%, n 28), the pure internalizing group, exhibited high levels CI [1.09, 3.86], p .05), the moderate-externalizing and
of internalizing problems and low levels of externalizing problems, moderate-internalizing group (OR 1.28, CI [1.00, 1.67], p
(d) children in Group 10 (12.6%, n 155), the high-internalizing .05), and the high-internalizing and high-desisting externalizing
and high-desisting externalizing group, exhibited high internaliz- group (OR 1.37, CI [1.04, 1.81], p .05). Furthermore, children
ing and high-desisting externalizing problems, and (e) children in faced with more early childhood medical risks had a higher like-
Group 11 (3.7%, n 46), the chronic co-occurring group, exhib- lihood of being in the pure moderate-externalizing group com-
ited chronic co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems. pared with the chronic-externalizing and moderate-internalizing
Thus, there were children exhibiting pure externalizing problems group (OR 1.20, CI [1.07, 1.44], p .05) and the pure
(moderate and chronic), pure internalizing problems, high inter- internalizing group (OR 1.38, CI [1.06, 1.96], p .05).
nalizing and high-desisting externalizing problems, and co- Children who experienced a more positive and enriched early
occurring problems. home environment were more likely to be in the high-internalizing
According to chi-square analyses, the different groups were not and high-desisting externalizing group compared with the pure
differentiated in terms of ethnicity; therefore, ethnicity was not
included in further analyses. The groups exhibiting pure chronic Table 4
externalizing problems, 2(1, N 1232) 6.55, p .001, and Posterior Probabilities Based on the Joint Occurrence Analysis
moderate internalizing and chronic externalizing problems, 2(1, (N 1,232)
N 1232) 4.59, p .01, were overrepresented by boys.
Internalizing problem
MLR Analyses Externalizing
problem Low Moderate High
The analyses comparing the lower risk group with the rest of the
Initial group posterior probabilities
groups with the use of MLR was significant, 2(70, N 1232)
269.56, p .001. Table 5 incorporates odds ratios (ORs) to Low .8991 .8086 .76811
Moderate desisting .8052 .7617 .44512
compare each higher risk group with the low-risk group. In gen- Moderate .7613 .7538 .41113
eral, ORs reflect the odds likelihood of being in one group over the High desisting .4154 .4949 .74614
other, on the basis of the level of the independent variable. As Chronic .8595 .77210 .89015
shown in Table 5, children with higher SDS risk had a greater
Final group posterior probabilities
likelihood of being in the chronic co-occurring group compared
with the low-risk group. Moreover, children with more medical Low .7251 .8155 .8249
Moderate desisting .7822 .7876
risks had a higher likelihood of being in the high-internalizing and Moderate .7933 .8137
high-desisting externalizing group, the pure moderate- High desisting .81010
externalizing group, and the pure chronic-externalizing group Chronic .8374 .7868 .89911
compared with the low-risk group, with the pure moderate-
externalizing group having the higher OR. Children who experi- Note. The subscript numbers assign group membership. Values in bold
enced a less positive and enriched home environment were more indicate the groups that were excluded from the final joint occurrence
likely to be in the chronic-externalizing and moderate-internalizing analysis.
1168 FANTI AND HENRICH

Figure 2. The development of externalizing and internalizing problems based on the final group probabilities.
The left part of each groups label represents internalizing problems, and the right part represents externalizing
problems. In the Results section, the groups are labeled as follows: (1) low risk, (2) low internalizing and
moderate-desisting externalizing, (3) pure moderate externalizing, (4) pure chronic externalizing, (5) low
externalizing and moderate internalizing, (6) moderate internalizing and moderate-desisting externalizing, (7)
moderate externalizing and moderate internalizing, (8) chronic externalizing and moderate internalizing, (9) pure
internalizing, (10) high internalizing and high-desisting externalizing, and (11) chronic co-occurring. extern.
externalizing; intern. internalizing; mod. moderate; des. desisting.

moderate-externalizing group (OR .88, CI [.81, .95], p .05), Children characterized by difficult temperament were more
the pure chronic-externalizing group (OR .88, CI [.77, .99], p likely to be classified in the chronic co-occurring group com-
.05), the moderate-externalizing and moderate-internalizing group pared with the pure moderate-externalizing group (OR 1.54,
(OR .93, CI [.87, .99], p .05), and the chronic-externalizing CI [1.07, 3.94], p .05), the pure high-internalizing group
and moderate-internalizing group (OR .87, CI [.80, .95], p (OR 1.87, CI [1.18, 6.05], p .05), and the moderate-
.05). Children whose mothers scored lower on depression in early externalizing and moderate-internalizing group (OR 1.77, CI
childhood were more likely to be in the pure moderate- [1.17, 3.73], p .05). Children with lower scores on the Bayley
externalizing group compared with the chronic-externalizing and Mental Development Index, were more likely to be classified in
moderate-internalizing group (OR .94, CI [.90, .98], p .05), the pure chronic-externalizing group compared with the pure
the pure high-internalizing group (OR .94, CI [.89, .99], p moderate-externalizing group (OR .95, CI [.92, .99], p
.05), and the chronic co-occurring group (OR .92, CI [.89, .96], .05), the chronic-externalizing and moderate-internalizing
p .001) and were more likely to be classified into the moderate- group (OR .96, CI [.92, .99], p .05), and the pure inter-
externalizing and moderate-internalizing group compared with the nalizing group (OR .94, CI [.89, .98], p .01). Also,
chronic co-occurring group (OR .95, CI [.92, .98], p .01). children with lower scores on the Bayley index were less likely
Table 5
Multinomial Logistic Regression Analyses Comparing the Higher Risk Groups With the Lowest Risk Group in Terms of Early Childhood Risk Factors (N 1,232)

Group comparison

Risk factor 11 vs. 1 10 vs. 1 9 vs. 1 8 vs. 1 7 vs. 1 6 vs. 1 5 vs. 1 4 vs. 1 3 vs. 1 2 vs. 1

Gender
OR 1.73 0.94 1.53 2.33 1.23 1.15 0.71 3.51 2.02 1.78
95% CI [0.91, 3.27] [0.60, 1.47] [0.68, 3.45] [1.26, 4.33] [0.82, 1.86] [0.75, 1.76] [0.43, 1.18] [1.22, 10.09] [1.20, 3.40] [1.16, 2.73]
SDS risk
OR 1.40 1.07 0.73 1.29 1.15 1.05 1.11 0.97 1.02 1.07
95% CI [1.13, 1.89] [0.83, 1.37] [0.38, 1.39] [0.97, 1.73] [0.91, 1.45] [0.82, 1.35] [0.82, 1.48] [0.63, 1.51] [0.77, 1.34] [0.83, 1.37]
Medical risk
OR 1.18 1.26 0.95 1.17 1.15 1.13 0.99 1.29 1.38 1.02
95% CI [0.93, 1.50] [1.06, 1.49] [0.68, 1.33] [0.94, 1.47] [0.98, 1.36] [0.95, 1.34] [0.81, 1.20] [1.03, 1.78] [1.14, 1.67] [0.85, 1.20]
Home environment
OR 1.01 1.05 1.07 0.92 0.99 1.05 1.06 0.93 0.93 1.02
95% CI [0.93, 1.12] [0.98, 1.13] [0.92, 1.24] [0.84, 0.99] [0.93, 1.06] [0.99, 1.14] [0.98, 1.15] [0.83, 0.99] [1.01, 1.15] [0.95, 1.10]
Difficult temperament
OR 2.71 2.37 1.38 2.27 1.62 1.65 1.06 2.75 1.78 1.74
95% CI [1.31, 5.58] [1.40, 3.99] [0.52, 3.70] [1.14, 4.52] [0.99, 2.63] [1.18, 3.22] [0.60, 1.90] [1.01, 7.56] [0.98, 3.24] [1.06, 2.88]
Maternal depression
OR 1.11 1.08 1.09 1.09 1.06 1.07 1.02 1.06 1.03 1.03
95% CI [1.07, 1.15] [1.05, 1.11] [1.04, 1.14] [1.05, 1.13] [1.03, 1.09] [1.03, 1.10] [0.98, 1.06] [1.01, 1.12] [0.99, 1.07] [0.99, 1.06]
Bayley
OR 0.98 0.98 1.02 0.99 0.98 0.99 0.99 0.95 0.99 0.98
95% CI [0.96, 1.01] [0.96, 1.00] [0.98, 1.06] [0.64, 1.02] [0.96, 1.00] [0.97, 1.01] [0.96, 1.01] [0.91, 0.98] [0.97, 1.02] [0.96, 1.00]
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS

Note. Comparisons are based on odds ratios (ORs). Group 1 is the low-risk group; Group 2 is the low-internalizing and moderate-desisting externalizing group; Group 3 is the pure moderate
externalizing group; Group 4 is the pure chronic-externalizing group; Group 5 is the low-externalizing and moderate-internalizing group; Group 6 is the moderate-internalizing and moderate-desisting
externalizing group; Group 7 is the moderate-externalizing and moderate-internalizing group; Group 8 is the chronic-externalizing and moderate-internalizing group; Group 9 is the pure internalizing
group; Group 10 is the high-internalizing and high-desisting externalizing group; Group 11 is the chronic co-occurring group. SDS sociodemographic status; Bayley Bayley Mental Development
Index.

p .05. p .01.
1169
1170 FANTI AND HENRICH

to be classified in the pure internalizing group than in the behaviors, F(1, 1232) 13.97, 2p .02, p .001, and mother-
moderate-externalizing and moderate-internalizing group reported risky behaviors, F(1, 1232) 7.37, 2p .01, p .001,
(OR 1.04, CI [1.01, 1.07], p .05). committed by the studys children, with boys scoring higher on
both the child-reported measure (for boys, M 2.60, SD 1.97;
ANCOVAs for girls, M 1.98, SD 1.56) and the mother-reported measure
(for boys, M 7.89, SD 4.38; for girls, M 6.56, SD 4.13).
ANCOVAs were used to investigate how the trajectory groups The analysis proceeded to compare the four groups of children
were associated with early adolescent social and behavioral ad-
who scored higher than the low-risk group. No significant differ-
justment, controlling for gender and covariates measured during
ences were found for friends risky behaviors, F(3, 200) 0.38,
early childhood (SDS risk and medical risk). Table 6 uses the
2p .005, p .76, or the study childrens risky behaviors as
standardized mean difference statistic (Cohens d), as an effect size
reported by the children, F(3, 200) 0.31, 2p .004, p .82.
to compare the higher risk groups with the low-risk group. The
Significant differences were found only for risky behaviors en-
effect size was computed as the difference between the means of
gaged in by the studys children as reported by mothers, F(3,
the two groups under comparison (after these means were adjusted
200) 2.93, 2p .04, p .04, with the chronic co-occurring
for the covariate effects) divided by the root-mean-square error for
group scoring higher than the pure moderate-externalizing group
the particular model. Cohens d is a scale-free measure of the
separation between two group means. After comparing the higher (d .25, p .05).
risk groups with the normative group, the analyses proceeded to Peer exclusion. As shown in Table 6, the only high-risk group
identify the group at higher risk for early adolescent maladjust- that was not differentiated from the low-risk group in terms of peer
ment. exclusion was the pure internalizing group, and the strongest effect
Risky behaviors. According to Table 6, the three chronic- size was found for the chronic co-occurring group. The medical
externalizing groups (Groups 11, 8, 4) and the pure moderate- risk index, F(1, 1232) 4.41, 2p .01, p .01, and the SDS
externalizing group engaged in more risky behaviors and had more risk index, F(1, 1232) 7.39, 2p .01, p .01, were
friends who committed risky behaviors compared with the low- significant covariates for peer exclusion, although there were no
risk group. The strongest effect size for the child and friend risky gender differences, F(1, 1232) 0.07, 2p 0, p .79. The
behaviors (child/friend report) was found for the pure chronic- ANCOVA comparing the six groups of children who scored
externalizing group, and the strongest effect size for the mother- higher than the low-risk group in terms of peer exclusion was
reported child risky behaviors was found for the chronic co-occurring significant, F(5, 541) 4.20, 2p .04, p .001. The findings
and the chronic-externalizing and moderate-internalizing groups. suggested that the chronic co-occurring group scored higher on
The SDS risk index was a significant covariate for child- and peer exclusion compared with the high-internalizing and high-
friend-reported risky behaviors committed by friends of the desisting externalizing group (d .25, p .05), the moderate-
studys children, F(1, 1232) 27.60, 2p .02, p .001, and by internalizing and chronic-externalizing group (d .23, p
the studys children, F(1, 1232) 38.75, 2p .03, p .001. The .05), the moderate-externalizing and moderate-internalizing
medical risk index was a significant covariate for mother-reported group (d .26, p .05), the pure moderate-externalizing group
child risky behaviors, F(1, 1232) 12.84, 2p .01, p .001. (d .22, p .05), and the pure chronic-externalizing group
There were significant gender differences for child-reported risky (d .14, p .05).

Table 6
Analyses of Covariance Comparing the Early Adolescent Adjustment of the Higher Risk Groups and the Lowest Risk Group
(N 1,232)

Group comparisons

Risk behavior F 2
p 11 vs. 1 10 vs. 1 9 vs. 1 8 vs. 1 7 vs. 1 6 vs. 1 5 vs. 1 4 vs. 1 3 vs. 1 2 vs. 1

Friend risky behaviors


(child/friend report) 4.51 .04 .22 .06 .09 .19 .03 .02 .04 .31 .25 .05
Child risky behaviors

(child/friend report) 3.99 .03 .21 .03 .11 .17 .03 .04 .09 .31 .24 .03
Child risky behaviors
(mother report) 21.45 .15 .31 .11 .02 .32 .11 .05 .01 .19 .21 .08
Asocial with peers

(mother/teacher report) 9.66 .07 .38 .22 .26 .11 .12 .11 .12 .27 .09 .05
Excluded by peers
(mother/teacher report) 9.05 .07 .37 .22 .09 .20 .20 .08 .05 .29 .21 .09

Note. The group comparisons are based on effect sizes (Cohens d). Group 1 is the low-risk group; Group 2 is the low-internalizing/moderate-desisting
externalizing group; Group 3 is the pure moderate externalizing group; Group 4 is the pure chronic-externalizing group; Group 5 is the low-externalizing/
moderate-internalizing group; Group 6 is the moderate-internalizing/moderate-desisting externalizing group; Group 7 is the moderate-externalizing/
moderate-internalizing group; Group 8 is the chronic-externalizing/moderate-internalizing group; Group 9 is the pure internalizing group; Group 10 is the
high-internalizing/high-desisting externalizing group; Group 11 is the chronic co-occurring group.

p .05. p .01.
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1171

Asocial behaviors with peers. As shown in Table 6, all children with lower SDS were more likely to be classified as
higher risk groups were more asocial with peers than the low-risk having chronic co-occurring problems. Previous research has in-
group. The medical risk index, F(1, 1232) 3.75, 2p .01, p dicated that children who come from low-SDS families are more
.05, and the SDS risk index, F(1, 1232) 7.39, 2p .01, p .01, likely to be at high risk for both behavioral and emotional prob-
were significant covariates for asocial behavior with peers, al- lems (Keenan, Shaw, Walsh, Delliquadri, & Giovannelli, 1997;
though there were no gender differences, F(1, 1232) 0.27, 2p Keiley et al., 2000), and SDS risk has been considered as a general
0, p .60. The strongest effect size was found for the chronic stressor for psychopathology (Dodge, Pettit, & Bates, 1994; Dun-
co-occurring group. The analysis proceeded to compare the nine can, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994). This finding provides
groups of children who scored higher than the normative group. evidence for Angold and Costellos (1992) suggestion that one
The ANCOVA comparing these groups in terms of being asocial possible explanation for the relation between behavioral and emo-
with peers was not significant, F(8, 860) 1.92, 2p .03, tional problems in children is that it represents undifferentiated
p .06. responding to stress. Our findings also indicate that the additive
effects of early childhood risk factors, including maternal depres-
Discussion sion and difficult temperament, may set the stage for the develop-
ment of co-occurring problems.
The present study makes unique contributions by investigating In terms of early adolescent adjustment, children with chronic,
the development of co-occuring and distinct internalizing and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems were differ-
externalizing problems within a LCGA framework. In addition to entiated from the rest of the sample in that they were more
providing separate information for the development of internaliz- excluded by peers during early adolescence, which might indicate
ing and externalizing problems, the findings make three distinct that the combination of internalizing and externalizing problems
contributions. They (a) provide information on the developmental acts as a general risk for being rejected by peers (Rudolph, Ham-
pathways of pure and co-occurring internalizing and externalizing men, & Burge, 1994). For example, it may be that children who are
problems, (b) identify early childhood risk factors related to co- anxious/depressed as well as aggressive might be regarded as more
occurrence or their pure counterparts, and (c) identify ways in annoying by peers, compared with children who are just aggressive
which pure and co-occurring problem pathways are associated or just depressed (Keiley et al., 2003). Furthermore, children with
with early adolescent social and behavioral adjustment. chronic, co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems
When investigating the development of externalizing problems, were also markedly asocial with peers. These findings indicate that
we identified children following trajectories of normative, moder- children with co-occurring problems do not have the necessary
ate, high-decreasing, and chronic externalizing problems, which is social skills to associate with peers.
in agreement with previous studies investigating the development The investigation also identified a small number of children
of externalizing problems (Broidy et al., 2003; Campbell et al., exhibiting high pure externalizing problems. Children with lower
2006; Fanti & Henrich, 2007; Nagin & Tremblay, 1999; NICHD cognitive abilities in infancy were more likely to develop pure
Early Child Care Research Network, 2004; Shaw et al., 2003). In externalizing problems. According to Moffitt (1993), mild cogni-
terms of internalizing problems, the investigation is in agreement tive impairments early in development are associated to a host of
with prior research in that the majority of the children participating negative outcomes, including following a course of continuous
in the study exhibited low levels of internalizing problems from behavioral problems. Additionally, children living in an early
toddlerhood to early adolescence and in that a minority of the home environment low in support and stimulation were more
children were at higher risk of exhibiting continuous or increasing likely to develop continuous high or moderate externalizing prob-
internalizing problems (Sterba et al., 2007). However, the present lems but low or normative levels of internalizing problems. Dif-
studys findings are unique because LCGA enabled the identifica- ficult children may influence their parents negatively, resulting in
tion of heterogeneous developmental patterns of pure or combined parents being less involved and providing a less positive or devel-
internalizing and externalizing problems within a dynamic frame- opmentally appropriate environment for these children (Shaw et
work by taking trajectories of change into account. Distinct devel- al., 2003). Eventually, a negative home environment may result in
opmental patterns of pure continuous externalizing (moderate and the continuation of externalizing problems across development.
chronic), pure internalizing, high-internalizing and high-desisting Thus, the current investigation provides evidence that low cogni-
externalizing, and co-occurring problems were identified. tive abilities and a negative home environment might be more
The identification of distinct developmental patterns of high and strongly related to behavioral than emotional problems.
continuous internalizing and externalizing problems fits well with In contrast to our expectations, children following trajectories of
previous suggestions that co-occurrence should be regarded as a either pure externalizing or co-occurring internalizing and exter-
distinct syndrome (Angold & Costello, 1992; Lilienfeld, 2003; nalizing problems were not differentiated in terms of difficult
OConnor et al., 1998). On the basis of prior literature (e.g. temperament and maternal depression. These findings indicate that
Nottelman & Jensen, 1995; Oland & Shaw, 2005), we expected different individual and environmental factors similarly influence
that children experiencing higher levels of early childhood risk the development of pure externalizing and co-occurring problems.
factors would be more likely to exhibit combined internalizing and In addition, both children with pure externalizing problems and
externalizing problems and that children with chronic co-occurring children with co-occurring internalizing and externalizing prob-
problems would have poorer social and behavioral adjustment in lems had similarly high levels of deviant behaviors and friends
early adolescence, compared with those with pure internalizing or who committed risky behaviors during early adolescence, indicat-
externalizing problems. Our findings provide partial support for ing continuity in their behavioral repertoire across time. Hence,
these expectations. In terms of early childhood risk processes, multiple developmental trajectories may lead to deviant behaviors,
1172 FANTI AND HENRICH

providing evidence for the concept of equifinality. The results also exhibited less deviant behaviors and associated less with deviant
indicate that it might be the continuation of externalizing prob- peers, compared with those with pure externalizing problems.
lems, and not necessarily the levels of these problems, that lead to According to prior research, anxiety and depression in the absence
deviant associations and behaviors later in life, because both of externalizing problems may serve as a protective factor against
continuous moderate and chronic externalizing problems were later risky behaviors and being affiliated with delinquent peers
related to deviant behaviors and associations with deviant peers. (Ialongo, Edelsohn, Werthamer-Larsson, & Crockett, 1996). How-
These findings are important because delinquent peer affiliations ever, children exhibiting chronic internalizing problems but not
may function to maintain and exacerbate childhood externalizing continuous externalizing problems were similarly asocial with
problems (Laird et al., 2001). peers, which is in accordance with Oland and Shaws (2005)
The analyses also revealed a developmental pattern character- sociodevelopmental milestone model. The model suggests that
ized by high internalizing problems and initially high but desisting children exhibiting high levels of internalizing problems but low
externalizing problems. In terms of multifinality, it is useful to levels of externalizing problems should also be more likely to
compare these children to those with co-occurring internalizing exhibit isolating behavior and to be withdrawn and avoidant be-
and externalizing problems who had equivalent levels of internal- cause of the internalizing problems they exhibit. Furthermore,
izing and externalizing problems in early childhood and those with because of these characteristics, children exhibiting high levels of
pure externalizing problems who had equivalent levels of exter- internalizing problems but low levels of externalizing problems are
nalizing problems in early childhood. Children in these groups less likely to be involved with delinquent peers and less likely to
were not differentiated in terms of medical risk, difficult temper- exhibit delinquent behaviors. The studys findings align with this
ament, or maternal depression. As a result, these risk factors might theoretical description; asocial behavior with peers was the only
work together to set the stage for the initiation (but not the outcome that differentiated children with pure internalizing prob-
continuation) of behavioral problems. However, children with a lems from those with neither internalizing nor externalizing prob-
more positive and enriched home environment were more likely to lems. Thus, asocial behavior might be a common maladaptive
have high internalizing problems and initially high but desisting outcome for children following differential trajectories of exter-
externalizing problems. On the basis of these findings, we might nalizing and internalizing problems, providing an example of the
conclude that the experiences of a positive home environment concept of equifinality. Children exhibiting externalizing symp-
might be sufficient for a sharp decline in externalizing problems. toms tend to exhibit impulsive and undercontrolled behaviors
Furthermore, children with greater SDS risk were more likely to toward peers, which are considered by peers to be asocial behav-
have co-occurring internalizing and externalizing problems, com- iors (Calkins, Gill, & Williford, 1999), whereas children exhibiting
pared with high internalizing but desisting externalizing problems, internalizing problems tend to be withdrawn and avoidant and
again indicating that a positive and a low-stress familial environ- because of that they do not engage in friendly relations with peers,
ment might result in decreased externalizing problems but not which is also considered by peers to be asocial behavior (Oland &
decreased internalizing problems. As a result, early environmental Shaw, 2005; Rudolph et al., 1994).
experiences might be related to the resiliency of the high- The comparisons we discussed focus on the extremes of risk and
internalizing and high-desisting group, at least in the levels of nonrisk. Children classified into these groupslow risk, chronic
externalizing problems they exhibited. The findings also suggest co-occurring, high pure externalizing, pure internalizing, and high
that the chronicity of externalizing problems for children who also internalizing and high-desisting externalizingtogether accounted
have high levels of internalizing problems cannot be predicted for less than half of the total sample. Thus, normative develop-
from individual characteristics in early childhood. ment, at least in this sample, involves a degree of risk for devel-
In terms of equifinality, it is important to compare children with oping various levels of internalizing and externalizing problems,
high internalizing and high-desisting externalizing problems and frequently in combination. Moreover, children who exhibit behav-
children exhibiting continuous high pure internalizing symptoms ior problems early in life do not necessarily continue to exhibit
because these two groups of children had similar levels of inter- these problems at later ages, and for some children, aggressive,
nalizing and externalizing problems at age 12. Our findings indi- oppositional, and hyperactive behaviors early in life seem to be a
cated that difficult temperament and medical risk were associated normal part of development. In addition, it is the children who
with initial high levels of externalizing problems but not with the exhibited continuous behavioral problems who faced social and
development of internalizing problems. Additionally, maternal de- delinquent problems. We also found that moderate internalizing
pression was associated with increased risk of being high in problems characterized the majority of the sample, suggesting that
internalizing problems as well as high in initial or chronic exter- some internalizing feelings might be a normal part of child devel-
nalizing problems regardless of whether they were pure or co- opment. Comparisons between groups can also provide informa-
occurring. This finding suggests that maternal depression might be tion about the mechanisms involved in normal and psychopatho-
a general risk marker for both internalizing and externalizing logical development, and our findings indicate that low individual
problems and provides an example of multifinality in that a single and environmental early risk processes protect the majority of
starting point may result in diverse outcomes. The impact of children from developing continuous behavioral problems.
maternal depression on childrens psychopathological problems is
well established, and there are a number of studies linking mater- Strengths, Limitations, Future Directions, and
nal depression to childrens behavioral and emotional problems Implications
(e.g., Gross et al., 2008; Shaw et al., 2003; Sterba et al., 2007).
Moreover, children with high internalizing and high-desisting Strengths of this investigation included a large sample of chil-
externalizing problems and those with pure internalizing problems dren followed from birth to early adolescence. The nine data points
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1173

available for externalizing and internalizing problems enhanced externalizing problems (Broidy et al., 2003; Nagin & Tremblay,
the reliability and flexibility of the longitudinal analyses (Singer & 1999; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2004; Shaw et
Willett, 2003). Furthermore, the analyses incorporated data from al., 2003). Much research has focused on finding possible ways to
multiple informants and multiple methods. The investigation also identify these children, and the present study offers an additional
offered a number of methodological advances. Researchers have perspective by providing evidence that they can be differentiated
been using statistical methods, such as correlations, clinical cutoff in terms of internalizing problems. In addition, our findings sug-
scores, cluster analysis, and factor analysis, to identify syndromes gest that following a path of continuous externalizing or co-
that tend to co-occur in the individual; however, these approaches occurring internalizing and externalizing problems may lead to
are not built to take longitudinal change into account, and at most, engagement in deviant behaviors, association with deviant peers,
these methods test the association of only two assessment periods rejection by peers, and asocial behavior with peers. Moreover,
(Nagin & Tremblay, 2001). However, LCGA investigates co- multiple child and environmental risks operate in conjunction with
occurrence within a dynamic framework by taking trajectories of each other to influence the development of chronic externalizing
change into account, by investigating nonlinear change, and by and co-occurring externalizing and internalizing problems. How-
using all the available longitudinal data in the analysis. ever, experiencing a home environment high in support and stim-
One limitation of the current study might be that the external- ulation may lead to decreases in externalizing problems but not to
izing and internalizing problem trajectory analyses relied exclu- decreases in internalizing problems. Furthermore, even though
sively on mother reports. However, parents are considered to be a children exhibiting pure internalizing problems are asocial with
critical source for reporting on their childs behavior (Shaw et al., peers, they are at low risk for exhibiting deviant behaviors during
2003), and mothers might be more observant of the childrens early adolescence. The continuous internalizing problems of these
internalizing symptoms compared with teachers or other infor- children seemed to be uniquely predicted by maternal depression.
mants (Keiley et al., 2000). Another limitation pertains to the Finally, our work suggests that taking into account the concept of
combinations of the two versions of the CBCL (the preschool co-occurrence is essential for explaining the development and
version for children ages 23 and the school-age version for understanding the etiology of externalizing and internalizing prob-
children ages 4 18), because behavioral and emotional problems lems.
may manifest differently across the age range. For example, phys-
ical aggression is often used by toddlers to solve conflicts with
References
peers or playmates, although this type of aggression decreases over
the course of development and nonaggressive behaviors increase Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/418
(Tremblay, 2000). Additionally, the inclusion of other biological, and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University
individual, and sociocultural risk factors and the consideration of of Vermont.
both risk and protective developmental processes might further Achenbach, T. M. (1992). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/23
elucidate the differences between pure and co-occurring problems. and 1992 Profile. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University
For instance, the presence of callous unemotional traits has been of Vermont.
Achenbach, T. M., Conners, C. K., Quay, H. C., Verhulst, F. C., & Howell,
related to higher externalizing problems, although these traits
C. T. (1989). Replication of empirically derived syndromes as a basis for
suppress the development of internalizing problems (Frick & Ellis, taxonomy of child/adolescent psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal
1999). Child Psychology, 17, 299 323.
Investigating the trajectories of behavioral and emotional prob- Achenbach, T. M., Howell, C. T., Quay, H. C., & Conners, C. K. (1991).
lems over time can also inform the timing and construction of National survey of problems and competencies among 4- to 16-year-
interventions (Loeber & Farrington, 1994). The current study olds: Parents reports for normative and clinical samples. Monographs of
suggested that high pure and co-occurring internalizing and exter- the Society for Research in Child Development, 56, 1131.
nalizing problems may start as early as the second year of life, and Ackerman, B. P., DEramo, K. S., Umylny, L., Schultz, D., & Izard, C. E.
according to Dodge and Pettit (2003), preventive interventions (2001). Family structure and externalizing behavior of children from
should start early in life before antisocial outcomes or other types economically disadvantaged families. Journal of Family Psychology, 15,
288 300.
of psychopathology become inevitable. In support of this idea,
Angold, A., & Costello, E. (1992). Comorbidity in children and adoles-
prior work provided evidence that interventions during toddler- cents with depression. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North
hood are successful in reducing externalizing (Dishion et al., America, 1, 3151.
2008), internalizing (Shaw, Connell, Dishion, Wilson, & Gardner, Angold, A., & Costello, E. (1993). Depressive comorbidity in children and
2009), and co-occurring behavioral and emotional problems (Con- adolescents: Empirical, theoretical, and methodological issues. Ameri-
nell et al., 2008). The current findings may have the ability to can Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1779 1791.
inform the construction of interventions by providing information Bauer, D., & Curran, P. (2003). Distributional assumptions of growth
on unique or common factors influencing the development of pure mixture models: Implications for overextraction of latent trajectory
externalizing, pure internalizing, and co-occurring behavioral and classes. Psychological Methods, 8, 338 363.
emotional problems. Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant DevelopmentII. San Antonio,
TX: Psychological Corporation.
In conclusion, the current investigation provides important in-
Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2003). The
formation for the co-occurrence between different behavioral normative development of child and adolescent problem behavior. Jour-
problems, which is considered to be a major research challenge for nal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 179 192.
developmental psychopathology (Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). For ex- Booth-LaForce, C., & Oxford, M. L. (2008). Trajectories of social with-
ample, the studys findings on co-occurrence can inform previous drawal from Grades 1 to 6: Prediction from early parenting, attachment,
research concerned with children following a chronic trajectory of and temperament. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1298 1313.
1174 FANTI AND HENRICH

Brendgen, M., Wanner, B., Morin, A. J. S., & Vitaro, F. (2005). Relations problems in community and clinic-referred children. Journal of Clinical
with parents and with peers, temperament, and trajectories of depressed Child Psychology, 29, 199 208.
mood during early adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Fanti, K. A., & Henrich, C. C. (2007). The relation of home and childcare/
33, 579 594. school environment to differential trajectories of externalizing problems.
Brennan, P. A., Hall, J., Bor, W., Najman, J. M., & Williams, G. (2003). International Journal About Parents in Education, 1, 117123.
Integrating biological and social processes in relation to early-onset Frick, P. J., & Ellis, M. (1999). Callous unemotional traits and subtypes of
persistent aggression in boys and girls. Developmental Psychology, 39, conduct disorder. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 2,
309 323. 149 168.
Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., Tremblay, R. E., Bates, J. E., Brame, B., Ge, X., Best, K. M., Conger, R. D., & Simons, R. L. (1996). Parenting
Dodge, K. A., . . . Vitaro, F. (2003). Developmental trajectories of child- behaviors and the occurrence and co-occurrence of adolescent depres-
hood disruptive behaviors and adolescent delinquency: A six-site cross- sive symptoms and conduct problems. Developmental Psychology, 32,
national study. Developmental Psychology, 39, 222245. 717731.
Bub, K. L., McCartney, K., & Willett, J. B. (2007). Behavior problem Gilliom, M., & Shaw, D. S. (2004). Codevelopment of externalizing and
trajectories and first-grade cognitive ability and achievement skills: A internalizing problems in early childhood. Development and Psychopa-
latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, thology, 16, 313333.
653 670. Gross, H. E., Shaw, D. S., & Moilanen, K. L. (2008). Reciprocal associ-
Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation of the ations between boys externalizing problems and mothers depressive
Measurement of the Environment. Little Rock, AK: University of Ar- symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 693709.
kansas at Little Rock. Henrich, C. C., Schwab-Stone, M., Fanti, K., Jones, S. M., & Ruchkin, V.
Calkins, S. D., Gill, K., & Williford, A. (1999). Externalizing problems in (2004). The association of community violence exposure with middle
two-year-olds: Implications for patterns of social behavior and peers school achievement: A prospective study. Journal of Applied Develop-
responses to aggression. Early Education and Development, 10, 267 mental Psychology, 25, 327348.
288. Hogue, A., & Steinberg, L. (1995). Homophily of internalizing distress in
Campbell, S. B. (1995). Behavior problems in preschool children: A adolescent peer groups. Developmental Psychology, 31, 897906.
Ialongo, N., Edelsohn, G., Werthamer-Larsson, L., & Crockett, L. (1996).
review of recent research. Journal of Child Psychology and Allied
The course of aggression in first-grade children with and without co-
Disciplines, 36, 113149.
morbid anxious symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24,
Campbell, S. B., Spieker, S., Burchinal, M., Poe, M. D., & the National
445 456.
Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care
Kaplan, B., Crawford, S., Cantell, M., Kooistra, L., & Dewey, D. (2006).
Research Network. (2006). Trajectories of aggression from toddlerhood
Comorbidity, co-occurrence, continuum: Whats in a name? Child:
to age 9 predict academic and social functioning through age 12. Journal
Care, Health and Development, 32, 613756.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 791 800.
Keenan, K., Shaw, D. S., Delliquadri, E., Giovannelli, J., & Walsh, B.
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology.
(1998). Evidence for the continuity of early problem behaviors: Appli-
Child Development, 55, 17.
cation of a developmental method. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol-
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. (1996). Equifinality and multifinality in
ogy, 26, 441 454.
developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychology, 8, 597
Keenan, K., Shaw, D. S., Walsh, B., Delliquadri, E., & Giovannelli, J.
600.
(1997). DSMIIIR disorders in preschool children from low-income
Coie, J. D., & Dodge, K. A. (1998). Aggression and antisocial behavior. In
families. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.
Psychiatry, 36, 620 627.
3. Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 779 862). New Keiley, M. K., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2000). A
York, NY: Wiley. cross-domain growth analysis: Externalizing and internalizing behaviors
Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. (1994). Families in troubled times: Adapting during 8 years of childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28,
to change in rural America. New York, NY: de Gruyter. 161179.
Connell, A., Bullock, B. M., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D., Wilson, M., & Keiley, M. K., Lofthouse, N., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S.
Gardner, F. (2008). Family intervention effects on co-occurring early (2003). Differential risks of covarying and pure components in mother
childhood behavioral and emotional problems: A latent transition anal- and teacher reports of externalizing and internalizing behavior across
ysis approach. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 12111225. ages 5 to 14. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 267283.
Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Connell, A., Gardner, F., Weaver, C., & Klein, D., & Riso, L. (1993). Psychiatric disorders: Problems of boundaries
Wilson, M. (2008). The family check-up with high-risk indigent fami- and comorbidity. In C. Costello (Ed.), Basic issues in psychopathology
lies: Outcomes of positive parenting and problem behavior from ages 2 (Vol. 9, pp. 19 66). New York, NY: Guilford.
through 4. Child Development, 79, 13951414. Kovacs, M., & Devline, B. (1998). Internalizing disorders in childhood.
Dodge, K., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the devel- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1, 47 63.
opment of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Developmental Ladd, G. W., Herald-Brown, S. L., & Andrews, R. K. (2009). The Child
Psychology, 39, 349 371. Behavior Scale (CBS) Revisited: A longitudinal evaluation of CBS
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1994). Socialization mediators subscales with children, preadolescents, and adolescents. Psychological
of the relation between socioeconomic status and child conduct prob- Assessment, 21, 325339.
lems. Child Development, 65, 649 665. Ladd, G. W., & Profilet, S. M. (1996). The Child Behavior Scale: A
Duggal, S., Carlson, E. A., Sroufe, A., & Egeland, B. (2001). Depressive teacher-report measure of young childrens aggressive, withdrawn, and
symptomatology in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psy- prosocial behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1008 1024.
chopathology, 13, 143164. Laird, R. D., Jordan, K. Y., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E.
Duncan, G. J., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. K. (1994). Economic (2001). Peer rejection in childhood, involvement with antisocial peers in
deprivation and early childhood development. Child Development, 65, early adolescence, and the development of externalizing behavior prob-
296 318. lems. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 337354.
Epkins, C. (2000). Cognitive specificity in internalizing and externalizing Lilienfeld, S. (2003). Comorbidity between and within childhood external-
INTERNALIZING AND EXTERNALIZING PROBLEMS 1175

izing and internalizing disorders: Reflections and directions. Journal of children and adolescents: Developmental perspectives. In T. H. Ollen-
Abnormal Child Psychology, 31, 285291. dick & R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology (Vol. 17,
Lo, Y., Mendell, N., & Rubin, D. B. (2001). Testing the number of pp. 109 155). New York, NY: Plenum.
components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767778. OConnor, T. G., McGuire, S., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin,
Loeber, R., & Farrington, D. P. (1994). Problems and solutions in longi- R. (1998). Co-occurrence of depressive symptoms and antisocial behav-
tudinal and experimental treatment of child psychopathology and delin- ior in adolescence: A common genetic liability. Journal of Abnormal
quency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 887900. Psychology, 107, 2737.
Lynam, D., Moffitt, T., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1993). Explaining the Oland, A. A., & Shaw, D. S. (2005). Pure versus co-occurring externalizing
relation between IQ and delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school and internalizing symptoms in children: The potential role of socio-
failure, or self-control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 187196. developmental milestones. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Re-
McLeod, J. D., & Shanahan, M. J. (1996). Trajectories of poverty and view, 8, 247270.
childrens mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37, Radloff, L. S. (1997). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for
207220. research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement,
Medoff-Cooper, B., Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1993). Early infancy 1, 385 401.
temperament questionnaire. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Reitz, E., Dekovic, M., & Meijer, A. M. (2005). The structure and stability
Pediatrics, 14, 230 235. of externalizing and internalizing problem behavior during early adoles-
Milan, S., Pinderhughes, E. E., & the Conduct Problems Prevention Re- cence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 577588.
search Group. (2006). Family instability and child maladjustment tra- Rudolph, K. D., Hammen, C., & Burge, D. (1994). Interpersonal function-
jectories during elementary school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychol- ing and depressive symptoms in childhood: Addressing the issues of
ogy, 34, 4356. specificity and comorbidity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22,
Miller-Johnson, S., Lochman, J. E., Coie, J. D., Terry, R., & Hyman, C. 355371.
(1998). Comorbidity of conduct and depressive problems at sixth grade: Rutter, M. (1997). Comorbidity: Concepts, claims and choices. Criminal
Substance use outcomes across adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Behavior and Mental Health, 7, 265285.
Psychology, 26, 221232. Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology:
Miner, J. L., & Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (2008). Trajectories of externalizing Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 265
behaviors from age 2 to age 9: Relations with gender, temperament, 296.
ethnicity, parenting, and rater. Developmental Psychology, 44, 771786. Schwartz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of
Moffitt, T. E. (1990). The neuropsychology of delinquency: A critical Statistics, 6, 461 464.
review of theory and research. In N. Morris & M. Tonry (Eds.), Crime Shaw, D. S., Connell, A., Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., & Gardner, F.
and justice (Vol. 12, pp. 99 169). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago (2009). Improvements in maternal depression as a mediator of interven-
Press. tion effects on early childhood problem behavior. Development and
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course persistent anti- Psychopathology, 21, 417 439.
social behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, Shaw, D. S., Gilliom, M., Ingoldsby, E. M., & Nagin, D. S. (2003).
100, 674 701. Trajectories leading to school-age conduct problems. Developmental
Muthen, B. (2000). Latent variable mixture modeling. In G. A. Marcou- Psychology, 39, 187200.
lides & R. E. Schumacker (Eds.), Advanced structural equation model- Shaw, D. S., Owens, E. B., Vondra, J. I., Keenan, K., & Winslow, E. B.
ing: New developments and techniques (pp. 133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- (1996). Early risk factors and pathways in the development of early
baum. disruptive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 8,
Muthen, L. K., & Muthen, B. (2007). Mplus users guide. Los Angeles, 679 699.
CA: Authors. Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis:
Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semipara- Modeling change and event occurrence. Oxford, England: Oxford Uni-
metric, group-based approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139 157. versity Press.
Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group based modeling of development. Cambridge, Sterba, S. K., Prinstein, M. J., & Cox, M. J. (2007). Trajectories of
MA: Harvard University Press. internalizing problems across childhood: Heterogeneity, external valid-
Nagin, D. S., & Land, K. (1993). Age, criminal careers, and population ity, and gender differences. Development and Psychopathology, 19,
heterogeneity: Specification and estimation of a nonparametric mixed 345366.
Poisson model. Criminology, 31, 327362. Tremblay, R. E. (2000). The development of aggressive behavior during
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (1999). Trajectories of boys physical childhood: What have we learned in the past century? International
aggression, opposition, and hyperactivity on the path to physically Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 129 141.
violent and nonviolent juvenile delinquency. Child Development, 70, Wright, J., Zakriski, A., & Drinkwater, M. (1999). Developmental psycho-
11811196. pathology and the reciprocal patterning of behavior and environment:
Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Analyzing developmental trajec- Distinctive situational and behavioral signatures of internalizing, exter-
tories of distinct but related behaviors: A group based method. Psycho- nalizing, and mixed-syndrome children. Journal of Consulting and Clin-
logical Methods, 6, 18 34. ical Psychology, 67, 95107.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Youngstrom, E., Findling, R., & Calabrese, J. (2003). Who are the comor-
Care Research Network. (2004). Trajectories of physical aggression bid adolescents? Agreement between psychiatric diagnosis, youth, par-
from toddlerhood to middle childhood. Monographs of the Society for ent, and teacher report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31,
Research in Child Development, 69, 1128. 231245.
Newman, D., Moffitt, T., Caspi, A., & Silva, P. (1998). Comorbid mental
disorders: Implications for treatment and sample selection. Journal of Received February 4, 2009
Abnormal Psychology, 107, 305311. Revision received June 15, 2010
Nottelman, E. D., & Jensen, P. S. (1995). Comorbidity of disorders in Accepted June 21, 2010

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen