Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

Smoking History and Cognitive Function


in Middle Age From the Whitehall II Study
Severine Sabia, MSc; Michael Marmot, PhD, FFPHM, FRCP; Carole Dufouil, PhD; Archana Singh-Manoux, PhD

Background: Studies about the association between OR, 1.69; 95% CI, 1.41- 2.02 among women). At phase 5
smoking and dementia necessarily involve those who have in age- and sex-adjusted analyses, smokers compared with
survived smoking. We examine the association be- those who never smoked were more likely to be in the low-
tween smoking history and cognitive function in middle est quintile of cognitive performance. After adjustment for
age and estimate the risk of death and of nonparticipa- multiple covariates, this risk remained for memory (OR,
tion in cognitive tests among smokers. 1.37; 95% CI, 1.10-1.73). Ex-smokers at phase 1 had a 30%
lower risk of poor vocabulary and low verbal fluency. In
Methods: Data are from the Whitehall II study of 10 308 longitudinal analysis, the evidence for an association be-
participants aged 35 to 55 years at baseline (phase 1 [1985- tween smoking history and cognitive decline was incon-
1988]). Smoking history was assessed at phase 1 and at sistent. Stopping smoking during the follow-up period was
phase 5 (1997-1999). Cognitive data (memory, reason- associated with improvement in other health behaviors.
ing, vocabulary, and semantic and phonemic fluency) were
available for 5388 participants at phase 5; 4659 of these Conclusions: Smoking was associated with greater risk
were retested 5 years later. of poor memory. Middle-aged smokers are more likely
to be lost to follow-up by death or through nonpartici-
Results: Smokers at phase 1 were at higher risk of death pation in cognitive tests. Ex-smokers had a lower risk of
(hazard ratio [HR], 2.00; 95% confidence interval [CI], poor cognition, possibly owing to improvement in other
1.58-2.52 among men and HR, 2.46; 95% CI, 1.80-3.37 health behaviors.
among women) and of nonparticipation in cognitive tests
(odds ratio [OR], 1.32; 95% CI, 1.16-1.51 among men and Arch Intern Med. 2008;168(11):1165-1173

T
HE ASSOCIATION BETWEEN would support the hypothesis that smok-
smoking and dementia has ing is involved in the pathogenesis of pre-
been much discussed in re- clinical cognitive deficit and decline.
cent years,1-3 with a meta- We investigated the association be-
analysis4 concluding that tween the history of tobacco consump-
smoking is a risk factor for dementia. This tion (smoking status and pack-years of
association is thought to be primarily smoking) and the multiple domains of cog-
Author Affiliations: Institut through the effect of smoking on vascu- nition in middle-aged individuals. We ex-
National de la Sante lar disease.2,4 Examining this effect in older amined associations with cognitive per-
et de la Recherche Medicale
persons is problematic because of loss to formance and change in cognitive function
(INSERM) Unite 687, Institut
Federatif de Recherche 69, follow-up, misdiagnosis of dementia, and during 5 years in analyses adjusted for the
Hopital Paul Brousse, Villejuif, smoking-related premature mortality be- effects of socioeconomic status, health be-
France (Ms Sabia and fore the onset of dementia.2,3 To avoid haviors, and a range of health indicators.
Dr Singh-Manoux); INSERM some of these problems, one approach en- A further objective was to assess the ex-
Unite 708 and Universite Paris tails exploring the association between tent to which middle-aged smokers are lost
6 (Dr Dufouil) and Centre de smoking and cognition before the onset of to follow-up by death or through nonpar-
Gerontologie, Hopital Sainte dementia. There is increasing evidence to ticipation in cognitive tests.
Perrine, Assistance suggest the importance of midlife risk fac-
PubliqueHopitaux de Paris, tors for later dementia.5 Furthermore, the
Paris, France (Dr Singh- METHODS
link between cognitive impairment and
Manoux); and Department of
Epidemiology and Public later-life dementia 6-8 is clearly estab-
Health, University College lished. Therefore, it is important to exam- Data are drawn from the Whitehall II study,16
London, London, England ine if the risk of cognitive impairment in established in 1985 to examine the socioeco-
(Drs Marmot and smokers is also present in midlife9-15; evi- nomic gradient in health and disease among
Singh-Manoux). dence of this association at younger ages 10 308 civil servants (6895 men and 3413 wom-

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1165

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


10 308 Participants, phase 1: 1985-1988
Smoking status Smoking status

7830 Participants, phase 5: 1997-1999 Smoking status 7246 Participants, history


10 212 Participants 274 Deaths of tobacco use until phase 5
were included 2204 Nonresponses
in the survival
study until
phase 7 5885 Participants underwent 5388 Participants complete data for
(Table 1) first assessment of study of cognitive deficit
cognitive function (Table 2 and Table 4)

6944 Participants, phase 7: 2002-2004 6302 Participants underwent 4659 Participants complete data for
605 Deaths second assessment of study of cognitive decline
2759 Nonresponses cognitive function (Table 5)

Figure 1. Flowchart showing the formation of the study population (1985-2004). *The number of deaths since phase 1. Complete means with exclusion of
persons without all adjustment behavioral variables available (154 with missing sociodemographic variables, 236 with missing behavioral variables, and 156 with
missing health variables), and excluding history of stroke (42 participants).

en). All civil servants aged 35 to 55 years in 20 London-based lable or 2 syllables) at 2-second intervals and were then asked
departments were invited by letter to participate, and 73% agreed. to recall in writing as many of the words in any order and had
Baseline examination (phase 1) took place from 1985 to 1988 2 minutes to do so.
and involved a clinical examination and a self-administered ques-
tionnaire containing sections on demographics, health, and life- Reasoning
style factors such as smoking habits, work characteristics, so-
cial support, and life events. Clinical examination included The Alice Heim AH4 Group Test of General Intelligence18
measures of blood pressure, anthropometry, biochemical vari- (AH4-I) is composed of a series of 65 verbal and mathematical
ables, neuroendocrine function, and subclinical markers of car- reasoning items of increasing difficulty. It tests inductive rea-
diovascular disease. Subsequent phases of data collection have soning, measuring the ability to identify patterns and to infer
alternated between postal questionnaire alone (phase 2 [1988- principles and rules. Participants had 10 minutes to complete
1990], phase 4 [1995-1996], phase 6 [2001], and phase 8 [2006]) this section.
and postal questionnaire accompanied by a clinical examina-
tion (phase 3 [1991-1994], phase 5 [1997-1999], and phase 7
Vocabulary
[2002-2004]). Participants gave written consent to participate
in the study, and the University College London ethics com-
mittee approved the study. Vocabulary was assessed using the Mill Hill Vocabulary Test19
in its multiple format, consisting of a list of 33 stimulus words
ordered by increasing difficulty and 6 response choices.
SMOKING HISTORY

Data on smoking were collected at every phase using questions


Verbal Fluency
about smoking status (current, past, or never), age at which the
participant started smoking, the mean number of cigarettes We used 2 measures of verbal fluencyphonemic and seman-
smoked per day, the number of cigars or cigarillos smoked, and tic.20 Phonemic fluency was assessed via /s/ words and seman-
ounces of tobacco smoked in a pipe or in hand-rolled cigarettes tic fluency via animal words. Subjects were asked to recall in
per week (Figure 1). Ex-smokers were asked the age at which writing as many words beginning with /s/ and as many animal
they had stopped smoking. The smoking history variable was names as they could. One minute was allowed for each test.
created with the following categories: never smoker (those who
never smoked), current smoker at phase 5, long-term ex- COVARIATES
smoker (those who stopped before phase 1), and recent ex-
smoker (stopped smoking between phase 1 and phase 5). Among Sociodemographic Variables
smokers at phase 5, we further used the amount of tobacco
smoked in total grams of tobacco per day (1 cigarette equals 1 g, Sociodemographic variables used were age, sex, marital status
and 1 cigar or cigarillo equals 3 g17) to calculate pack-years of (single, widowed, married or cohabiting, or divorced or sepa-
smoking (the mean daily number of grams of tobacco divided rated), education achievement (none or lower primary school,
by 20 and multiplied by the number of years of smoking). lower secondary school, higher secondary school, university,
or higher university degree), and socioeconomic position (using
COGNITION the British civil service grades of employment of high [admin-
istrative], intermediate [professional or executive], or low [cleri-
Cognitive function was assessed at phase 5 and at phase 7 using cal or support]).
a battery of the following standard tasks:
Health Behaviors
Memory
Health behaviors included the following: (1) alcohol use, as-
Short-term verbal memory was assessed using a 20-word free sessed via questions about the number of alcoholic drinks (mea-
recall test. Participants were presented a list of 20 words (1 syl- sures of spirits, glasses of wine, and pints of beer) consumed in

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1166

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


the past 7 days converted to the number of units of alcohol cially available statistical software (SAS, version 8; SAS Insti-
(1 U=8 g); (2) frequency of fruit and vegetable consumption, evalu- tute Inc, Cary, North Carolina).
ated using the question How often do you eat fresh fruit or veg-
etables? (responses were on an 8-point scale, ranging from sel-
dom or never [1] to 2 times a day [8]); and (3) physical activity, RESULTS
calculated as the sum of the hours of mild, moderate, and vigor-
ous physical activities in response to a 20-item questionnaire on SAMPLE DESCRIPTION AND MISSING DATA
the frequency and duration of participation in walking, cycling,
sports, gardening, housework, and home maintenance.21
Of 10 308 participants at phase 1 (1985-1988), 7830 par-
Health Measures ticipated in at least 1 part of phase 5 (1997-1999), 2204
were nonresponders, and 274 had died (Figure 1). At
Health measures were drawn from phase 5. Coronary heart dis- phase 5, data on cognitive function, smoking history, and
ease prevalence was based on clinically verified events and in- all covariates were available for 5388 respondents. Com-
cluded myocardial infarction and definite angina.22 Stroke and pared with baseline, this group was younger (55.5 vs 56.1
diabetes mellitus were assessed using self-reports of physician years) and comprised fewer women (27.6% vs 33.1%) and
diagnosis. Blood pressure (systolic and diastolic) was mea- fewer participants of low socioeconomic status (14.6%
sured at the phase 5 clinical examination, twice in the sitting vs 22.7%) (P .001). From this population, calculation
position after 5 minutes rest using an automated device (Om-
of cognitive decline (implying participation in cognitive
ron 907; Omron Healthcare Europe, Hoofddorp, the Nether-
lands). The mean of 2 measurements was taken to be the mea- tests at phase 7) was possible for 4659 participants. Miss-
sured blood pressure. Serum cholesterol level was measured ing data were similarly influenced by age, sex, and so-
within 72 hours in serum samples stored at 4C using enzy- cioeconomic position compared with data available for
matic colorimetric methods. analysis of cognitive deficit (n=5388).
To assess whether the smoking-cognition association
is underestimated because of premature mortality among
STATISTICAL ANALYSIS smokers, we examined the association between smoking
status at phase 1 and mortality during the mean (SD) 17.1
The association between smoking status at phase 1 (never
smoker, ex-smoker, and current smoker) and mortality until
(2.3) years of follow-up until phase 7 (Table 1). Cur-
phase 7 was assessed using Cox proportional hazards regres- rent smokers at phase 1 had a higher risk of dying during
sion models. The association between smoking status at phase the follow-up period compared with never smokers after
1 and nonparticipation in cognitive tests at phase 7 was as- adjustment for age, marital status, and socioeconomic po-
sessed using logistic regression analyses. sition among men (hazard ratio [HR], 2.00; 95% confi-
Descriptive analyses as a function of smoking history at phase dence interval [CI], 1.58-2.52) and among women (HR,
5 were performed and tested using 2 analysis for trend for cat- 2.46; 95% CI, 1.80-3.37). Ex-smokers at phase 1 did not
egorical variables and by fitting a linear trend for continuous have a higher risk of death during the follow-up period
variables. We first assessed the association between smoking examined (HR, 1.09; 95% CI, 0.84-1.41 among men and
history and continuous measures of cognition using linear HR, 1.23; 95% CI, 0.84-1.79 among women). Among sur-
mixed-effects models to account for unequal intervals be-
tween the 2 clinical examinations (range, 3.9-7.1 years). The
vivors at phase 7 (n=9625), we examined the association
independent variables were time, smoking history, between smoking status at phase 1 and nonparticipation
timesmoking history since the first cognitive assessment, and in cognitive tests at phase 7. In analyses adjusted for age,
other covariates. The dependent variables were the cognitive marital status, and socioeconomic position, current smok-
measures. Next, we examined the association between smok- ers at phase 1 were more likely to be nonparticipants in
ing history and the dichotomized measures of cognition in lo- cognitive tests among men (odds ratio [OR], 1.32; 95%
gistic regression analysis in which the reference group was those CI, 1.16-1.51) and among women (OR, 1.69; 95% CI, 1.41-
who never smoked. Cognitive scores in the lowest sex- 2.02). To examine the persistence of this association, we
specific quintile of cognitive function at phase 5 were consid- repeated the analysis with smoking history at phase 5 and
ered to represent cognitive deficit, and cognitive scores in the with participation in cognitive tests at phase 7 (n=7221).
lowest sex-specific quintile of change in cognitive function be-
tween phase 5 and phase 7 were considered to represent de-
Greater numbers of male smokers (OR, 1.47; 95% CI, 1.20-
cline. The interval between the 2 measures of cognition has been 1.81) and female smokers (OR, 1.81; 95% CI, 1.35-2.43)
adjusted for in the analyses of change using logistic regres- did not undertake the cognitive tests. Long-term ex-
sion. The analyses were adjusted first for age and sex, then for smokers and recent ex-smokers at phase 5 did not differ
sociodemographic measures (education and age as continu- from never smokers.
ous variables and all others as categorical variables), and fi- Characteristics of individuals included in the analy-
nally for health behaviors (all continuous variables) and health ses on smoking and cognitive deficit at phase 5 are given
measures (all vascular risk factors as continuous variables). in Table 2. The test for trend showed that smoking sta-
Other ways of looking at smoking history (eg, age at start- tus was associated with education achievement, alcohol
ing smoking and time since stopping smoking) were exam- use, socioeconomic status, and fruit and vegetable con-
ined in exploratory analyses but are not presented herein (ex-
cept for analysis using pack-years of smoking for current
sumption (P.001). Prevalences of stroke, diabetes melli-
smokers) because the results are not strikingly different. In ad- tus, and coronary heart disease were not associated with
dition, we undertook post hoc analysis to examine changes in smoking history. Among the vascular risk factors, smok-
health behaviors (use of alcohol and consumption of fruits and ing history was associated only with cholesterol level
vegetables) between phase 1 and phase 7 in the 4 smoking his- (P .001). Cognitive scores at phase 5 as a function of
tory categories. All analyses were performed using commer- health measures are given in Table 3.

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1167

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


Table 1. Association Between Smoking and Mortality and Nonparticipation in Cognitive Tests (2002-2004) a

Smoking Status at Phase 1 (1985-1988)

Hazard Ratio (95% Confidence Interval)

Variable Sex Never Smoker Ex-Smoker Current Smoker b


Association with mortality until phase 7c Male 1 [Reference] 1.09 (0.84-1.41) 2.00 (1.58-2.52)
Female 1 [Reference] 1.23 (0.84-1.79) 2.46 (1.80-3.37)
Association with nonparticipation in Male 1 [Reference] 1.06 (0.93-1.21) 1.32 (1.16-1.51)
cognitive tests at phase 7 d Female 1 [Reference] 1.08 (0.90-1.29) 1.69 (1.41-2.02)

Smoking Status at Phase 5 (1997-1999)

Odds Ratio (95% Confidence Interval)

Variable Sex Never Smoker Long-term Ex-Smoker Recent Ex-Smoker Current Smokerb
Association with nonparticipation Male 1 [Reference] 0.95 (0.79-1.15) 1.04 (0.80-1.35) 1.47 (1.20-1.81)
in cognitive tests at phase 7e Female 1 [Reference] 0.96 (0.74-1.25) 1.23 (0.83-1.82) 1.81 (1.35-2.43)

a Never smoker indicates those who never smoked; current smoker, those who were smoking at phase 5; long-term ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking
before phase 1; and recent ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking between phases 1 and 5.
b P .05.
c Includes 6841 men and 3371 women.
d Includes 6449 men and 3176 women, excluding participants lost to follow-up at phase 7 because of death.
e Includes 5064 men and 2157 women, excluding participants lost to follow-up at phase 7 because of death.

Table 2. Characteristics of the Study Population at Phase 5 (1997-1999) a

Never Smoker b Long-term Ex-Smoker b Recent Ex-Smoker b Current Smoker b P Value


Characteristic (n=2543) (n = 1519) (n = 511) (n = 815) for Trend
Age, mean (SD), y 55.2 (6.0) 56.1 (6.0) 56.2 (6.0) 55.0 (5.7) .24
Female sex, No. (%) 826 (32.5) 387 (25.5) 97 (19.0) 177 (21.7) .001
High socioeconomic status, No. (%) 932 (36.6) 525 (34.6) 180 (35.2) 231 (28.3) .001
University degree or higher, No. (%) 901 (35.4) 434 (28.6) 132 (25.8) 171 (21.0) .001
Married or cohabiting, No. (%) 1908 (75.0) 1217 (80.1) 395 (77.3) 600 (73.6) .87
Alcohol use, mean (SD), U/wk 10.5 (11.9) 15.3 (14.5) 17.1 (15.8) 20.6 (22.1) .001
Physical activity, mean (SD), h/wk 21.9 (15.1) 22.7 (15.0) 23.1 (16.0) 21.5 (15.8) .98
Daily consumption of fruits and vegetables, 1966 (77.3) 1152 (75.8) 373 (73.0) 509 (62.5) .001
No. (%)
Coronary heart disease, No. (%) 143 (5.6) 87 (5.7) 48 (9.4) 48 (5.9) .18
Stroke, No. (%) 18 (0.7) 13 (0.9) 3 (0.6) 8 (1.0) .55
Diabetes mellitus, No. (%) 60 (2.4) 41 (2.7) 14 (2.7) 12 (1.5) .30
Blood pressure, mean (SD), mm Hg
Systolic 121.9 (16.4) 123.6 (16.6) 124.1 (16.9) 122.0 (15.6) .24
Diastolic 77.2 (10.6) 77.8 (10.3) 78.4 (11.2) 76.8 (10.0) .91
Cholesterol level, mean (SD), mg/dL 224 (39) 232 (39) 228 (39) 232 (43) .001

SI conversion factor: To convert cholesterol to millimoles per liter, multiply by 0.0259.


a Includes 3901 men and 1487 women with complete data.
b Never smoker indicates those who never smoked; current smoker, those who were smoking at phase 5; long-term ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking
before phase 1; and recent ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking between phases 1 and 5.

SMOKING HISTORY AND (OR, 1.53; 95% CI, 1.27-1.85), vocabulary (OR, 1.42; 95%
COGNITIVE FUNCTION AT PHASE 5 CI, 1.18-1.70), phonemic fluency (OR, 1.32; 95% CI, 1.09-
1.60), and semantic fluency (OR, 1.30; 95% CI, 1.08-
The fully adjusted mixed-effects model showed that smok- 1.57). In fully adjusted models, the association remained
ing history was associated with memory (P=.01), reason- for memory (OR, 1.37; 95% CI, 1.10-1.73). Compared with
ing (P.001), vocabulary (P.001), phonemic fluency never smokers, long-term ex-smokers were less likely to
(P.001), and semantic fluency (P.001). Table 4 gives have deficits in memory (OR, 0.79; 95% CI, 0.65-0.96),
results of the logistic regression analysis using binary cog- vocabulary (OR, 0.73; 95% CI, 0.60-0.87), phonemic flu-
nitive outcomes; the sex-specific cutoffs used are also given. ency (OR, 0.73; 95% CI, 0.61-0.87), and semantic flu-
In age- and sex-adjusted models, current smokers were ency (OR, 0.75; 95% CI, 0.63-0.89) in fully adjusted mod-
more likely to have cognitive deficits on all tests as fol- els. Recent ex-smokers also had a reduced risk of poor
lows: memory (OR, 1.54; 95% CI, 1.25-1.90), reasoning vocabulary score (OR, 0.65; 95% CI, 0.49-0.85) and of poor

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1168

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


Table 3. Cognitive Function as a Function of Health Measures at Phase 5 a

Mean (SD)

Memory Reasoning Vocabulary Phonemic Fluency Semantic Fluency


Variable (Range, 0-20) (Range, 0-65) (Range, 0-33) (Range, 0-35) (Range, 0-36)
Coronary heart disease
No 7.0 (2.4) 47.3 (10.7) 25.2 (4.3) 17.0 (4.4) 16.6 (4.1)
Yes 6.4 (2.3) 44.4 (12.0) 24.4 (4.9) 16.1 (4.4) 15.4 (4.3)
Stroke
No 6.9 (2.4) 47.1 (10.7) 25.2 (4.3) 17.0 (4.4) 16.5 (4.2)
Yes 6.6 (2.6) 43.8 (10.7) 25.1 (3.8) 14.5 (3.3) 14.9 (3.5)
Diabetes mellitus
No 6.9 (2.4) 47.3 (10.7) 25.2 (4.3) 17.0 (4.4) 16.5 (4.1)
Yes 6.4 (2.3) 41.3 (13.1) 23.1 (5.6) 15.6 (4.6) 15.0 (4.3)
Blood pressure, mm Hg
Systolic
140 7.0 (2.4) 47.5 (10.5) 25.2 (4.2) 17.1 (4.4) 16.6 (4.1)
140 6.6 (2.3) 44.8 (11.9) 24.7 (4.8) 16.1 (4.3) 15.9 (4.2)
Diastolic
90 6.9 (2.4) 47.1 (10.7) 25.2 (4.3) 17.0 (4.4) 16.5 (4.2)
90 6.7 (2.3) 46.9 (11.4) 25.1 (4.5) 16.7 (4.2) 16.4 (4.1)
Cholesterol level, mg/dL
240 7.0 (2.3) 47.4 (10.8) 25.3 (4.5) 17.1 (4.5) 16.8 (4.2)
240 6.9 (2.4) 47.1 (10.8) 25.1 (4.3) 16.9 (4.4) 16.5 (4.2)

SI conversion factor: To convert cholesterol to millimoles per liter, multiply by 0.0259.


a Includes 3901 men and 1487 women with complete data.

Table 4. Odds Ratio of Being in the Lowest Quintile of Cognitive Function at Phase 5 as a Function of Smoking Status (1997-1999) a

Odds Ratio (95% Confidence Interval)

Never Smoker Long-term Ex-Smoker Recent Ex-Smoker Current Smoker


Variable (n=2543) (n = 1519) (n = 511) (n = 815)
Memory (Cutoff, 5)
Adjusted for age and sex 1 [Reference] 0.80 (0.66-0.97) b 1.17 (0.90-1.51) 1.54 (1.25-1.90) b
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.77 (0.63-0.93) b 1.10 (0.85-1.44) 1.33 (1.07-1.65) b
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.79 (0.65-0.96) b 1.12 (0.86-1.47) 1.37 (1.10-1.73) b
Reasoning (Cutoff, 42 in Men and 31 in Women)
Adjusted for age and sex 1 [Reference] 0.96 (0.82-1.14) 0.94 (0.74-1.20) 1.53 (1.27-1.85) b
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.87 (0.73-1.05) 0.81 (0.61-1.06) 1.11 (0.90-1.37)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.91 (0.76-1.10) 0.83 (0.63-1.10) 1.20 (0.96-1.49)
Vocabulary (Cutoff, 24 in Men and 20 in Women)
Adjusted for age and sex 1 [Reference] 0.87 (0.74-1.02) 0.84 (0.66-1.08) 1.42 (1.18-1.70) b
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.72 (0.60-0.86) b 0.67 (0.51-0.88) b 0.97 (0.79-1.19)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.73 (0.60-0.87) b 0.65 (0.49-0.85) b 0.92 (0.74-1.15)
Phonemic Fluency (Cutoff, 14 in Men and 13 in Women)
Adjusted for age and sex 1 [Reference] 0.76 (0.64-0.90) b 1.00 (0.79-1.27) 1.32 (1.09-1.60) b
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.70 (0.59-0.84) b 0.91 (0.71-1.17) 1.04 (0.85-1.28)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.73 (0.61-0.87) b 0.95 (0.74-1.22) 1.10 (0.89-1.35)
Semantic Fluency (Cutoff, 14 in Men and 13 in Women)
Adjusted for age and sex 1 [Reference] 0.80 (0.65-1.05) 0.82 (0.65-1.05) 1.30 (1.08-1.57) b
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.73 (0.61-0.87) b 0.72 (0.56-0.93) b 0.97 (0.80-1.19)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.75 (0.63-0.89) b 0.72 (0.55-0.94) b 0.98 (0.79-1.21)

a Includes subjects with complete data. Never smoker indicates those who never smoked; current smoker, those who were smoking at phase 5; long-term
ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking before phase 1; and recent ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking between phases 1 and 5.
b P .05.

semantic fluency (OR, 0.72; 95% CI, 0.55-0.94). Among pack-years of smoking and cognitive deficit (memory,
current smokers at phase 5, in fully adjusted models there P=.97; reasoning, P=.13; vocabulary, P=.33; phonemic flu-
was no evidence of a dose-response association between ency, P=.25; and semantic fluency, P=.97).

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1169

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


Table 5. Odds Ratio of Being in the Lowest Quintile of Change in Cognitive Function Between Phase 5 (1997-1999)
and Phase 7 (2002-2004)

Odds Ratio (95% Confidence Interval)

Never Smoker Long-term Ex-Smoker Recent Ex-Smoker Current Smoker


Variable (n=2218) (n = 1338) (n = 443) (n = 660)
Memory (Decrease of 1)
Adjusted for age and sex a 1 [Reference] 0.91 (0.78-1.06) 0.95 (0.75-1.21) 1.01 (0.83-1.23)
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.91 (0.78-1.07) 0.95 (0.75-1.21) 1.01 (0.83-1.23)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.91 (0.77-1.06) 0.96 (0.75-1.22) 0.99 (0.80-1.22)
Reasoning (Decrease of 7)
Adjusted for age and sex a 1 [Reference] 0.98 (0.82-1.18) 1.41 (1.10-1.82) b 1.46 (1.18-1.81) b
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 0.97 (0.81-1.16) 1.40 (1.09-1.80) b 1.45 (1.09-1.80) b
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 0.96 (0.80-1.16) 1.38 (1.07-1.77) b 1.40 (1.11-1.75) b
Vocabulary (Decrease of 1)
Adjusted for age and sex a 1 [Reference] 1.04 (0.88-1.23) 1.01 (0.78-1.30) 1.01 (0.81-1.25)
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 1.00 (0.85-1.19) 0.97 (0.75-1.25) 0.93 (0.75-1.25)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 1.01 (0.85-1.20) 0.97 (0.75-1.26) 0.95 (0.75-1.19)
Phonemic Fluency (Decrease of 3)
Adjusted for age and sex a 1 [Reference] 1.00 (0.85-1.19) 1.01 (0.79-1.30) 0.97 (0.79-1.21)
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 1.02 (0.86-1.20) 1.03 (0.80-1.32) 1.00 (0.81-1.24)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 1.00 (0.84-1.18) 1.01 (0.78-1.30) 0.97 (0.78-1.21)
Semantic Fluency (Decrease of 3)
Adjusted for age and sex a 1 [Reference] 1.05 (0.88-1.25) 0.94 (0.72-1.24) 1.08 (0.86-1.35)
Plus sociodemographics 1 [Reference] 1.03 (0.86-1.23) 0.94 (0.72-1.24) 1.09 (0.87-1.37)
Plus health behaviors and health measures 1 [Reference] 1.02 (0.85-1.23) 0.94 (0.71-1.24) 1.09 (0.86-1.38)

a Includes subjects with complete data and adjusted for interval between phase 5 and phase 7. Never smoker indicates those who never smoked; current
smoker, those who were smoking at phase 5; long-term ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking before phase 1; and recent ex-smoker, those who stopped
smoking between phases 1 and 5.
b P .05.

SMOKING HISTORY AND COGNITIVE DECLINE Those who stopped smoking between phase 1 and phase
BETWEEN PHASE 5 AND PHASE 7 5 (recent ex-smokers) had the smallest increase in alco-
hol use between phase 1 and phase 7 (0.82 U/wk of al-
The interaction term timesmoking history in the fully cohol) compared with the other groups (1.46 U/wk of
adjusted mixed-effects model showed that smoking his- alcohol among never smokers). In terms of healthy eat-
tory was associated with cognitive decline in reasoning ing, the percentage of participants consuming at least 1
(P .001) but not with memory (P = .64), vocabulary fruit or vegetable per day increased more among recent
(P=.68), phonemic fluency (P=.63), or semantic fluency ex-smokers than among never smokers. Figure 2 shows
(P=.61); detailed results are available in an appendix from that recent ex-smokers were at the same level of fruit and
the author. Further analysis regarding decline (Table 5) vegetable consumption as current smokers at phase 1,
uses the lowest quintile of change, implying decreases of but by phase 7 they had reached the same level as long-
greater than 1 point for memory and vocabulary, 7 points term ex-smokers and never smokers.
for reasoning, and 3 points for the fluency measures. In
fully adjusted models, current smokers (OR, 1.40; 95% CI,
COMMENT
1.11-1.75) and recent ex-smokers (OR, 1.38; 95% CI, 1.07-
1.77) were more likely to show a decline in reasoning. No
other association was evident. Further adjustment for health This study presents 4 key findings. First, smoking in middle
behaviors at phase 7 did not notably change these results. age is associated with memory deficit and decline in rea-
Among current smokers at phase 5, in fully adjusted mod- soning abilities. Second, long-term ex-smokers are less likely
els there was no dose-response association between pack- to have cognitive deficits in memory, vocabulary, and ver-
years of smoking and cognitive decline (memory, P=.22; bal fluency. Third, giving up smoking in midlife is accom-
reasoning, P=.88; vocabulary, P=.54; phonemic fluency, panied by improvement in other health behaviors. Fourth,
P=.30; and semantic fluency, P=.94). our results based on a large prospective cohort study of
middle-aged British civil servants suggest that the associa-
POST HOC ANALYSIS tion between smoking and cognition, even in late midlife,
could be underestimated because of higher risk of death
This analysis was aimed at the exploration of changes in and nonparticipation in cognitive tests among smokers.
other health behaviors along with change in smoking sta- During the past 20 years, public health messages about
tus (giving up smoking) during the follow-up period. smoking have led to changes in smoking behavior.23-25

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1170

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


Therefore, estimation of the association between smok-
ing and any health outcome needs to assess smoking be- Current Recent Long-term Never
havior over time, to explore whether change in smok- smokers ex-smokers ex-smokers smokers

ing status is accompanied by other changes, and to 90


examine possible underestimation of the association due
80
to premature mortality or greater loss to follow-up among
70
smokers. Our analyses show all 3 aspects to be impor-

% of Participants
tant. Exploration of the association between smoking and 60

dementia among older persons is complicated by the fact 50

that assessment can only be among those who have sur- 40


vived long enough to become demented.2,3 The alterna- 30
tive is to examine cognitive deficit and decline at earlier 20
ages. Cognition in midlife is clinically relevant because 10
research suggests that individuals with mild cognitive im- 0
pairment progress to clinically diagnosed dementia at an 1 2 3 5 7
accelerated rate.6-8 Phase

COMPARISON WITH OTHER STUDIES Figure 2. Participants consuming at least 1 fruit or vegetable per day at the
various phases as a function of smoking history at phase 5. Never smoker
indicates those who never smoked; current smoker, those who were
Studies using global cognitive tests (eg, the Mini-Mental smoking at phase 5; long-term ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking
State Examination) have found smoking to be associ- before phase 1; and recent ex-smoker, those who stopped smoking between
ated with cognitive impairment26-29 and with decline.30 phases 1 and 5.
Smokers have also been reported to have poorer psycho-
motor speed,11 visuospatial performance,12,31 memory,12,31,32 Previous results regarding the association between
reasoning,27 and vocabulary.32 Our results suggest poorer smoking and cognition in ex-smokers are mixed. In the
performance on memory and reasoning. Current evi- European Community Concerted Action Epidemiology
dence does not allow conclusions to be drawn about the of Dementia study,30 ex-smokers and never smokers did
association between smoking and specific cognitive not differ on cognitive impairment. Other studies26,28 have
domains. found the risk of cognitive impairment to be lower among
Few studies9-15 have examined the association be- ex-smokers compared with never smokers, although the
tween smoking and cognition in a middle-aged popula- differences were not significant. Apart from a few stud-
tion, and only 2 studies12,15 reported analysis of cogni- ies,12,28,29 most have looked at ex-smoking status with-
tive decline in this age group. Smoking was found to be out distinguishing between long-term ex-smokers and re-
associated with decline in memory in one study,12 but cent ex-smokers.11,26,27,30,32 In the 1946 British Birth Cohort
no association between the two was found in the other study,12 long-term ex-smokers had better memory and a
study.15 Our results suggest a greater risk of deficit but slower decline in memory compared with never smok-
not of decline in memory among smokers. Findings from ers. In the Honolulu-Asia Aging study,28 long-term ex-
a recent study31 suggest that the effect of smoking on de- smokers did not have a lower risk of cognitive impair-
cline in memory is confined to those older than 75 years. ment than never smokers, and recent ex-smokers had the
Future studies need to replicate these analyses to esti- same increased risk of impairment as current smokers.
mate the age at which smoking-related decline in memory Our results show that long-term ex-smokers were con-
becomes apparent. Our results also show a decline in rea- sistently less likely to have cognitive deficits in vocabu-
soning abilities among recent ex-smokers and among cur- lary and in verbal fluency. Future studies need to delin-
rent smokers. eate the long-term ex-smokers from the recent ex-smokers.
One could expect survival bias because of premature The association between smoking and cognition could
death of smokers to be limited among middle-aged in- be explained by the fact that smoking is a risk factor for
dividuals. Few studies33,34 have measured this bias or the atherosclerotic disease,36 which is related to a higher risk
bias introduced by greater loss to follow-up among smok- of cognitive deficit.37,38 However, we did not find a dose-
ers. In our study, smoking was associated with loss to response association between pack-years of smoking and
follow-up by death and through nonparticipation in cog- cognitive deficit or decline. Some studies26-28 have also
nitive tests. Current smokers at phase 1 were twice as likely reported the lack of a dose-response association, while
to die during the follow-up period, and those who were other studies12,30 have found this effect to be inconsis-
current smokers at phase 1 or at phase 5 were less likely tent. It is possible that the loss of the heavy smokers
to participate in the cognitive tests. These effects due to through death and nonparticipation in cognitive tests bi-
death or owing to nonparticipation in cognitive tests were ases the results using pack-years of smoking. Relative to
not evident among ex-smokers, and their results on the results among ex-smokers, it has been suggested that some
association between smoking and cognition are likely not of the differences in cognitive performance between
to be biased. Therefore, the risk of cognitive deficit and groups defined by their smoking habit may be the con-
decline among current smokers in our analyses may have sequence of self-selection out of the smoking groups.
been underestimated. It is possible that those who are Therefore, smokers with higher cognitive function scores
missing due to death or owing to nonparticipation in cog- would be more likely to quit and to become ex-
nitive tests had higher risk of cognitive deficit.35 smokers.27 This hypothesis is plausible. However, a com-

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1171

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


peting hypothesis is that those who stop smoking also Marmot); and by a Chaire dExcellence Award from the
change other health behaviors and possibly other as- French Ministry of Research and by a European Young
pects of their life as well. In our population, those who Investigator Award from the European Science Founda-
stopped smoking in the 10 years preceding cognitive test- tion (Dr Singh-Manoux). The Whitehall II study was sup-
ing considerably improved their other health behaviors ported by grants from the British Medical Research Coun-
(use of alcohol and consumption of fruits and veg- cil; the British Heart Foundation; the British Health and
etables) compared with others. Safety Executive; the British Department of Health; grant
HL36310 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood In-
STRENGTHS stitute; grant AG13196 from the National Institute on Ag-
ing; grant S06516 from the Agency for Health Care Policy
This study has several strengths. The detailed prospective and Research; and by the John D. and Catherine T.
assessment allowed a precise lifelong smoking history to MacArthur Foundation Research Networks on Success-
be established, and several confounders and explanatory ful Midlife Development and Socioeconomic Status and
variables were included in the analysis. We were able to Health.
examine changes in health behaviors longitudinally. Fur-
thermore, the design of the Whitehall II study allowed us
to assess the underestimation of the association between REFERENCES
smoking and cognition by evaluating the extent of miss-
ing data related to death during the follow-up period and 1. Almeida OP, Hulse GK, Lawrence D, Flicker L. Smoking as a risk factor for Alz-
heimers disease: contrasting evidence from a systematic review of case-
related to nonparticipation in cognitive tests. control and cohort studies. Addiction. 2002;97(1):15-28.
2. Brayne C. Smoking and the brain. BMJ. 2000;320(7242):1087-1088.
LIMITATIONS 3. Kukull WA. The association between smoking and Alzheimers disease: effects
of study design and bias. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49(3):194-199.
First, although the sample covered a wide socioeco- 4. Anstey KJ, von Sanden C, Salim A, OKearney R. Smoking as a risk factor for
dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J
nomic range, the data are from white-collar civil ser- Epidemiol. 2007;166(4):367-378.
vants and cannot be assumed to represent general popu- 5. Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Laatikainen T, Winblad B, Soininen H, Tuomilehto J.
lations. Second, smoking habits were self-reported and Risk score for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among middle aged
may have been underreported. Third, the requirement people: a longitudinal, population-based study. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5(9):735-
to write down answers for tests of verbal fluency may have 741.
6. Chertkow H. Mild cognitive impairment. Curr Opin Neurol. 2002;15(4):401-407.
led to a restriction in response range. Fourth, change be- 7. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment.
tween 2 time points is insufficient to examine intraindi- Arch Neurol. 2001;58(12):1985-1992.
vidual change, and analyses on further waves of data are 8. Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, et al. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-
necessary. stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(3):397-405.
In conclusion, our results show an association be- 9. Cerhan JR, Folsom AR, Mortimer JA, et al; Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities
(ARIC) Study Investigators. Correlates of cognitive function in middle-aged adults.
tween smoking and risk of memory deficit and reason- Gerontology. 1998;44(2):95-105.
ing decline. These results may have been underesti- 10. Jacobsen LK, Krystal JH, Mencl WE, Westerveld M, Frost SJ, Pugh KR. Effects
mated because of premature death and lower participation of smoking and smoking abstinence on cognition in adolescent tobacco smokers.
in cognitive tests among smokers. Stopping smoking in Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(1):56-66.
middle age was associated with improvement in other 11. Kalmijn S, van Boxtel MP, Verschuren MW, Jolles J, Launer LJ. Cigarette smok-
ing and alcohol consumption in relation to cognitive performance in middle age.
health behaviors and with little residual adverse effect Am J Epidemiol. 2002;156(10):936-944.
of smoking on cognition. Public health messages on smok- 12. Richards M, Jarvis MJ, Thompson N, Wadsworth ME. Cigarette smoking and
ing should continue to target smokers at all ages. cognitive decline in midlife: evidence from a prospective birth cohort study. Am
J Public Health. 2003;93(6):994-998.
Accepted for Publication: December 19, 2007. 13. Starr JM, Deary IJ, Fox HC, Whalley LJ. Smoking and cognitive change from age
11 to 66 years: a confirmatory investigation. Addict Behav. 2007;32(1):63-
Correspondence: Severine Sabia, MSc, Institut Na- 68.
tional de la Sante et de la Recherche Medicale Unite 687, 14. Whalley LJ, Fox HC, Deary IJ, Starr JM. Childhood IQ, smoking, and cognitive
IFR69, Hopital Paul Brousse, 16 Avenue Paul Vaillant change from age 11 to 64 years. Addict Behav. 2005;30(1):77-88.
Couturier, Batiment 15/16, 94807 Villejuif CEDEX, France 15. Knopman D, Boland LL, Mosley T, et al; Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities
(severine.sabia@inserm.fr). (ARIC) Study Investigators. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in
middle-aged adults. Neurology. 2001;56(1):42-48.
Author Contributions: Study concept and design: Sabia 16. Marmot MG, Smith GD, Stansfeld S, et al. Health inequalities among British civil
and Singh-Manoux. Acquisition of data: Marmot and Singh- servants: the Whitehall II study. Lancet. 1991;337(8754):1387-1393.
Manoux. Analysis and interpretation of data: Sabia, Dufouil, 17. Bernaards CM, Twisk JW, Snel J, van Mechelin W, Kemper HC. Is calculating
and Singh-Manoux. Drafting of the manuscript: Sabia and pack-years retrospectively a valid method to estimate life-time tobacco smok-
ing? a comparison between prospectively calculated pack-years and retrospec-
Singh-Manoux. Critical revision of the manuscript for im-
tively calculated pack-years. Addiction. 2001;96(11):1653-1661.
portant intellectual content: Sabia, Marmot, Dufouil, and 18. Heim AW. AH4 Group Test of General Intelligence. Windsor, England: NFER
Singh-Manoux. Statistical analysis: Sabia and Dufouil. Ob- Nelson Publishing Co Ltd; 1970.
tained funding: Marmot and Singh-Manoux. Administra- 19. Raven JC. Guide to Using the Mill Hill Vocabulary Test With Progressive Matrices.
tive, technical, and material support: Singh-Manoux. Study London, England: HK Lewis; 1965.
20. BorkowskiJG,BentonAL,SpreenO.Wordfluencyandbraindamage.Neuropsychologia.
supervision: Singh-Manoux.
1967;5:135-140.
Financial Disclosure: None reported. 21. Singh-Manoux A, Hillsdon M, Brunner E, Marmot M. Effects of physical activity
Funding/Support: This study was supported by a Brit- on cognitive functioning in middle age: evidence from the Whitehall II prospec-
ish Medical Research Council research professorship (Dr tive cohort study. Am J Public Health. 2005;95(12):2252-2258.

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1172

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017


22. Ferrie JE, Langenberg C, Shipley MJ, Marmot MG. Birth weight, components of Effect of smoking on global cognitive function in nondemented elderly. Neurology.
height and coronary heart disease: evidence from the Whitehall II study. Int J 2004;62(6):920-924.
Epidemiol. 2006;35(6):1532-1542. 31. Reitz C, Luchsinger J, Tang MX, Mayeux R. Effect of smoking and time on cogni-
23. Lee DJ, Fleming LE, Arheart KL, et al. Smoking rate trends in U.S. occupational tive function in the elderly without dementia. Neurology. 2005;65(6):870-875.
groups: the 1987 to 2004 National Health Interview Survey. J Occup Environ Med. 32. Stewart MC, Deary IJ, Fowkes FG, Price JF. Relationship between lifetime smok-
2007;49(1):75-81. ing, smoking status at older age and human cognitive function. Neuroepidemiology.
24. OConnor RJ, Giovino GA, Kozlowski LT, et al. Changes in nicotine intake and 2006;26(2):83-92.
cigarette use over time in two nationally representative cross-sectional samples 33. Edelstein SL, Kritz-Silverstein D, Barrett-Connor E. Prospective association of
of smokers. Am J Epidemiol. 2006;164(8):750-759. smoking and alcohol use with cognitive function in an elderly cohort. J Womens
25. Cummings KM. Programs and policies to discourage the use of tobacco products. Health. 1998;7(10):1271-1281.
Oncogene. 2002;21(48):7349-7364. 34. Wang HX, Fratiglioni L, Frisoni GB, Viitanen M, Winblad B. Smoking and the oc-
26. Cervilla JA, Prince M, Mann A. Smoking, drinking, and incident cognitive im- currence of Alzheimers disease: cross-sectional and longitudinal data in a popu-
pairment: a cohort community based study included in the Gospel Oak project. lation-based study. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;149(7):640-644.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;68(5):622-626. 35. Tyas SL, Salazar JC, Snowdon DA, et al. Transitions to mild cognitive impair-
27. Elwood PC, Gallacher JE, Hopkinson CA, et al; Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. ments, dementia, and death: findings from the Nun Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;
Smoking, drinking, and other life style factors and cognitive function in men in 165(11):1231-1238.
the Caerphilly cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1999;53(1):9-14. 36. Bolego C, Poli A, Paoletti R. Smoking and gender. Cardiovasc Res. 2002;53(3):
28. Galanis DJ, Petrovitch H, Launer LJ, Harris TB, Foley DJ, White LR. Smoking 568-576.
history in middle age and subsequent cognitive performance in elderly Japanese- 37. Singh-Manoux A, Britton AR, Marmot M. Vascular disease and cognitive function:
American men. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;145(6):507-515. evidence from the Whitehall II study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51(10):1445-1450.
29. Launer LJ, Feskens EJ, Kalmijn S, Kromhout D; Zutphen Elderly Study. Smok- 38. Tatemichi TK, Desmond DW, Stern Y, Paik M, Sano M, Bagiella E. Cognitive im-
ing, drinking, and thinking. Am J Epidemiol. 1996;143(3):219-227. pairment after stroke: frequency, patterns, and relationship to functional abilities.
30. Ott A, Andersen K, Dewey ME, et al; EURODEM Incidence Research Group. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57(2):202-207.

(REPRINTED) ARCH INTERN MED/ VOL 168 (NO. 11), JUNE 9, 2008 WWW.ARCHINTERNMED.COM
1173

2008 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: http://archinte.jamanetwork.com/pdfaccess.ashx?url=/data/journals/intemed/5676/ on 06/17/2017

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen