Sie sind auf Seite 1von 5

REPORTS

13. J. A. Cardin et al., Nat. Protoc. 5, 247 (2010). 21. K. Mizuseki, A. Sirota, E. Pastalkova, G. Buzski, Neuron Research Council grant (GABAcellsAndMemory grant 250047,
14. S. Jinno, T. Kosaka, Brain Res. 945, 219 (2002). 64, 267 (2009). to H.M.) and a German Ministry of Education and Research
15. C. A. Zappone, R. S. Sloviter, J. Comp. Neurol. 441, 22. J. O'Keefe, L. Nadel, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive (BMBF) grant 01GQ1003A to H.M. and E.C.F.
324 (2001). Map (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford, 1978).
16. S. Tolu et al., FASEB J. 24, 723 (2010). 23. M. P. Witter, E. I. Moser, Trends Neurosci. 29, 671 Supporting Online Material
17. P. Germroth, W. K. Schwerdtfeger, E. H. Buhl, Brain Res. (2006). www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/335/6075/1506/DC1
Materials and Methods
494, 187 (1989).
18. E. C. Fuchs et al., Neuron 53, 591 (2007). Acknowledgments: We thank K. Deisseroth for the AAVs, Figs. S1 to S11
References
19. M. J. Gillies et al., J. Physiol. 543, 779 (2002). A. Vogt for help in generating mice, I. Preugschat-Gumprecht
20. G. Buzski, J. J. Chrobak, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, and R. Hinz for their technical assistance, and P. H. Seeburg 28 November 2011; accepted 10 February 2012
504 (1995). for helpful discussions. This work was supported by a European 10.1126/science.1217139

of the threat (17, 18). In one schedule, the pre-


Glucocorticoids Can Induce PTSD-Like sentation of the tone was always followed by the
delivery of the shock (predicting-cue group). In
Memory Impairments in Mice this case, animals identified the tone as the main
predictor of the threat, showing conditioned fear
Nadia Kaouane,1,2,3 Yves Porte,1,2* Monique Valle,2,3 Laurent Brayda-Bruno,1,2,3 when re-exposed to the cue alone but not to the
Nicole Mons,1,2 Ludovic Calandreau,4 Aline Marighetto,1,2,3 context (Fig. 1A). During the second schedule,
Pier Vincenzo Piazza,2,3 Aline Desmedt1,2,3 the tone presentation was never followed by the

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on October 29, 2016


delivery of the shock (predicting-context group).
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is characterized by a hypermnesia of the trauma and by In this case, animals identified the conditioning
a memory impairment that decreases the ability to restrict fear to the appropriate context. context as the correct predictor of the threat,
Infusion of glucocorticoids in the hippocampus after fear conditioning induces PTSD-like memory showing conditioned fear when re-exposed to the
impairments and an altered pattern of neural activation in the hippocampal-amygdalar circuit. context alone but not to the cue (Fig. 1A). As
Mice become unable to identify the context as the correct predictor of the threat and show expected, in both conditions fear responses to the
fear responses to a discrete cue not predicting the threat in normal conditions. These data correct predictor progressively increased as a
demonstrate PTSD-like memory impairments in rodents and identify a potential pathophysiological function of the threat intensity.
mechanism of this condition. Corticosterone (10 ng per side) was infused
into the dorsal hippocampus immediately after
ituations surrounding threatening events are Animal models of traumatic memories and conditioning with three footshock intensities (0.3,

S better remembered than the ones accom-


panying neutral ones. This hypermnesia for
environments and cues predicting threats is im-
PTSD principally focus on the quantitative aspect
of fear memories: an increased and persistent fear
response (4). In contrast, the critical landmark of
0.5, and 0.8 mA). In the predicting-cue group,
corticosterone did not significantly modify fear
responses (Fig. 1B). This is not surprising because
portant for adaptation because it increases the PTSDthe inability of the subject to restrict fear the hippocampus is necessary for contextual con-
probability of survival in uncontrolled environ- responses to the appropriate predictorsis large- ditioning but is dispensable for cue conditioning
ments. In humans, the exposure to threatening sit- ly neglected. As a consequence, the biological (19, 20). In animals in which the context predicted
uations can also result in memory impairments mechanisms of pathological PTSD-like memo- the threat (predicting-context group), as expected
culminating in severe pathological states such as ries remain largely unknown. (6, 7) corticosterone administered after the lowest
posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (1). In this To address this issue, we analyzed whether shock intensity (0.3 mA) enhanced conditioned
case, a hypermnesia for the core traumatic event PTSD-like memory impairments can be observed fear to the context (Fig. 1B). However, when cor-
is associated with a memory deficit for peritrau- in mice. We therefore injected the stress hor- ticosterone followed the highest shock intensity
matic contextual cues (2, 3), impairing the capacity mone corticosterone, the major glucocorticoid in (0.8 mA) PTSD-like memory impairments ap-
of the subject to identify the correct predictors rodents, into the hippocampus of mice submitted peared. Animals did not show fear in response to
of the threat and restrict fear to the correct place to a fearful situation. Glucocorticoids increase in the correct predictor of the threat, the context, but
and/or to the correct cues. This deficit contributes response to stress, enhance stress-related memo- in response to the tone (Fig. 1B), which is nor-
to the intrusive recollectionre-experiencing the ries (58), and have been involved in the patho- mally not a relevant predictor of the threat for
fear response in safe situationsthat character- physiology of PTSD (9, 10). The hippocampus them. Animals infused with corticosterone also
izes PTSD. plays an important role in memory (11), is the showed a fear response to a previously unexperi-
major brain target of glucocorticoids (12, 13), enced cue (2 kHz tone) at some extent similar to
and seems impaired in PTSD (14, 15). the one (1 kHz tone) experienced during con-
1
CNRS UMR 5228, Centre de Neurosciences Intgratives et We then developed a behavioral model that ditioning (Fig. 1C), but not in response to a com-
Cognitives, 33405 Talence, France. 2Department of Life Science, allows evaluating the ability of the individuals to pletely different cue (white noise). In contrast, the
Universit de Bordeaux, 33077 Bordeaux, France. 3INSERM
U862, Neurocentre Magendie, 146 rue Leo Saignat, 33077
restrict fear responses to the appropriate predictor fear response in the predicting-cue group was ex-
Bordeaux, France. 4Institut National de la Recherche Agrono- of the threatening stimulus. Mice were first sub- clusively restricted to the conditioning cue. In con-
mique (INRA) Centre de Tours Nouzilly, Physiologie de la mitted to a threatening situation, the delivery of clusion, hippocampal corticosterone infusions
Reproduction et des Comportements, CNRS UMR 6175, INRA an electric footshock, when exposed to a discrete impaired the ability of the subject to restrict fear
UMR 85, Universit de ToursHaras Nationaux, 37380
cue (a tone) in a specific context (conditioning responses to the appropriate predicting cues.
Nouzilly, France.
cage). Twenty-four hours after this conditioning In physiological conditions, corticosterone in-
*Present address: Institut des Maladies Neurodgnratives,
CNRS UMR 5293, Universits Bordeaux 1 et 2, Avenue des procedure, animals were re-exposed first to the cue creases systemically in response to stress. We then
Facults, 33405 Talence, France. alone in a familiar and safe environment and then analyzed whether the exposure to a second stress,
Present address: Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives et to the conditioning context without the cue (16). the restraint in a cylinder for 20 min, could also
Intgratives dAquitaine, CNRS UMR 5287, Universits Bor- We used two distinct conditioning schedules induce PTSD-like memory impairments. Similar
deaux 1 et 2, Avenue des Facults, 33405 Talence, France.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
that contained exactly the same nature and quan- to intrahippocampal corticosterone infusions,
To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: aline. tities of environmental stimuli but, differing in when the second stress followed the low shock
desmedt@inserm.fr (A.D.); pier-vincenzo.piazza@inserm.fr (P.V.P.) their associations, identified different predictors intensity (0.3 mA) conditioned fear to the context

1510 23 MARCH 2012 VOL 335 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org


REPORTS
increased (Fig. 2A). However, when the second effects of stress are attributable to stress-induced (from 0.75 to 2.5 mg/kg) that encompasses the
stress followed the high shock intensity (0.8 mA) corticosterone secretion. Thus, systemic (intraperi- increase in corticosterone induced by several types
PTSD-like memory impairments appeared. These toneal) corticosterone injections, in a range of doses of stress (fig. S1B), increased fear responses to

Fig. 1. Corticosterone infusion in the hippocampus induces


PTSD-like memory impairments. (A) Increasing shock intensity
progressively produced higher fear responses to the right
predictor of the threat [all F > 4.67, P < 0.01]: in the predicting-
cue groups to the cue alone (left) and in the predicting-context
groups to the context alone (right). (B) In the context-predicting
group, corticosterone (CORT) infusions in the hippocampus in-
creased fear response to the context for a low-intensity threat
(0.3 mA). After a high-intensity threat (0.8 mA), corticosterone
induced PTSD-like memory impairments, suppressing the re-
sponse to the correct predictor (the context) and increasing the
fear response for the wrong predictor (the cue). (C) Corticoster-
one increased the response to a cue similar (2 kHz tone) to the
one used for conditioning (1 kHz cue) but not to a very different
cue (white noise). *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, group comparisons; P <

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on October 29, 2016


0.05, P < 0.01 versus 0.3 mA; #P < 0.05, aCSF versus CORT;
P < 0.01, P < 0.001 versus 1 kHz cue [Fishers partial least-
squares difference (PLSD)].

Fig. 2. Stress and systemic corticosterone injec-


tion induce PTSD-like memory impairments in the
predicting-context group. (A) A second stress after
conditioning with a low-intensity threat (0.3 mA
shock) increased fear responses to the context.
Stress after a high-intensity threat (0.8 mA shock)
induced PTSD-like memory impairments, suppressing
the response to the correct predictor (the context)
and increasing the fear response for the wrong pre-
dictor (the cue). (B) Corticosterone (CORT) levels in
the plasma and dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) were
higher in the high-threat group and further in-
creased after exposure to stress. (C) Intraperitoneal
(i.p.) corticosterone injection after conditioning in-
creased fear responses to the context for a low-intensity
threat and induced PTSD-like memory impairments for
a high-intensity threat. Ba, basal conditions; *P < 0.05,
**P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001, versus control or 0 mg/kg
CORT or low-intensity threat; #P < 0.05, ###P < 0.001,
versus high-intensity threat (Fishers PLSD).

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 335 23 MARCH 2012 1511


REPORTS
Fig. 3. PTSD-like memories pus can generate PTSD-like memory impair-
were associated with an al- ments in mice. Animals in the predicting-context
tered neural activity within group infused with corticosterone after condi-
the hippocampal-amygdalar tioning with the highest shock intensity did not
circuit. In the control predicting- show conditioned fear in response to the con-
context group, c-Fos expres- ditioning context, the correct predictor of the threat.
sion was high in the dentate These animals showed instead a fear response to
gyrus (DG) of the hippocam- salient cues that did not predict the threat in nor-
pus and low in the right amyg- mal conditions: a tone that was part of the stress-
dala [lateral (LA), basolateral ful experience but was never associated with shock
(BLA), and central (CEA) nuclei].
delivery, and even a tone resembling the previous
The opposite pattern was ob-
one but never experienced before. This impair-
served in the predicting-cue
group. Corticosterone (CORT) ment is very similar to the one observed in PTSD
infusion in the predicting- patients in which contextual peritraumatic cues
context group decreased c-Fos are often forgotten, whereas salient but irrelevant
expression in the dorsal CA1 ones are strongly remembered. These salient cues,
and the ventral DG, whereas and others more or less similar to them, can then
it increased c-Fos expression induce a strong fear response in contexts different
in the amygdala. *P < 0.05, from the traumatic one (1, 2).

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on October 29, 2016


**P < 0.01, Predicting-cue ver- PTSD-like memory impairments were asso-
sus Predicting-context groups; ciated with a switch in neural activation within
#
P < 0.05, ##P < 0.01, ###P < the hippocampal-amygdalar circuit. In controls,
0.001, aCSF versus CORT the identification of the context as the right pre-
(Students t test). dictor of the threat was associated with low amyg-
dalar activation and high activation in the DG,
whereas the opposite pattern was observed in
animals identifying the cue as the correct pre-
dictor. After corticosterone infusions, when the
animals in the predicting-context group identified
the cue instead of the context as the predictor of
the context when injected after conditioning with activation of these circuits, measured by the ex- the threat, the activation of the right amygdala
a low threat (0.3 mA). In contrast, corticosterone, pression of the c-Fos protein, was analyzed 90 increased, whereas it decreased in the ventral DG
in the same range of concentrations, induced min after intrahippocampal infusion of either and the dorsal CA1 of the hippocampus. This
PTSD-like memory impairments when injected corticosterone (10 ng per side) or vehicle [arti- hippocampal subregion selectivity is consistent
after conditioning with an intense threat (0.8 mA) ficial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF)] in animals in with both the critical role of the dorsal CA1 in the
(Fig. 2C). These differences between groups were the predicting-context group conditioned with the processing of contextual information and with
maintained over the entire dose-response func- high shock intensity (0.8 mA). Because animals the high sensitivity of the ventral DG to stress
tion of corticosterone, which included quite high in the predicting-context group infused with cor- (24, 25). The observed alterations in neural acti-
doses (5 and 10 mg/kg) encompassing the entire ticosterone behaved similarly to control animals vation are also very similar to those observed in
range of corticosterone behavioral effects. Plas- in the predicting-cue group, the latest group also PTSD patients, showing an impaired hippocam-
ma and hippocampal corticosterone levels were was studied. In vehicle-infused mice, the correct pal function, which is sometimes associated with
higher in animals conditioned with the high identification of the context as predictor of the decreased hippocampal volume (14), and an amyg-
shock intensity than in the ones conditioned with shock (predicting-context group) was associated dalar hyperactivation in response to trauma-related
the low shock intensity (Fig. 2B). Stress or cor- with high levels of c-Fos expression in the dentate cues (26), in particular in the right hemisphere
ticosterone injections increased corticosterone gyrus (DG) of the hippocampus and low levels of (27, 28). The relevance of an imbalance between
above the levels observed in the high shock in- c-Fos expression in the amygdala. At the oppo- the activity of the right and the left amygdala is
tensity group, but differences between groups site, the identification of the cue as the correct also supported by the development of PTSD in a
were suppressed (Fig. 2B), and this occurred predictor (predicting-cue group) was associated patient in which the left amygdala was surgically
even when the ED100 dose for the behavioral with low c-Fos levels in the DG and high c-Fos ablated (29).
effects of corticosterone injections was studied levels in the amygdala (Fig. 3). No significant These results provide solid evidence for the
(S1A). These results indicate that glucocorticoids differences between the two groups were ob- existence of PTSD-like memory impairments
induce PTSD-like memory impairments in a state- served in CA1 and CA3 (Fig. 3 and fig. S2). As in rodents and identify one of the potential patho-
dependent manner. Corticosterone levels need to previously observed (23), in the amygdala the re- physiological mechanisms of this condition.
go above a certain threshold, and the threat needs sponse was completely lateralized and present Understanding the molecular mechanisms
to reach a certain intensity, for PTSD-like memory only in the right hemisphere (Fig. 3 and fig. S2). through which high levels of glucocorticoids in
impairments to appear. These findings extend to Intrahippocampal corticosterone infusions in the the hippocampus can modify neural activation in
PTSD statedependent effects of glucocorticoids predicting-context group decreased c-Fos expres- the hippocampal-amygdalar circuit could open
already described for the ability of these hormones sion in the dorsal CA1 and the ventral DG, whereas new avenues for the development of innovative
to enhance the sensitivity to drugs of abuse (21), it increased c-Fos expression in the right amyg- treatments of PTSD.
suggesting that this could be a general mechanism dala. Thus, the pattern of c-Fos expression in the
in the pathophysiology of stress-related disorders. predicting-context group infused with corticoster- References and Notes
During acquisition of fear conditioning, the one approximated the one observed in control ani- 1. APA, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders IVText Revision (American Psychiatric Press,
selection of the best predictor of the shock (tone mals for which the correct predictor was the cue. Washington, DC, 2000).
versus context) requires different activations of During conditioning with an intense threat, an 2. B. M. Elzinga, J. D. Bremner, J. Affect. Disord. 70,
the hippocampal-amygdalar circuit (17, 22). The increase in glucocorticoid level in the hippocam- 1 (2002).

1512 23 MARCH 2012 VOL 335 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org


REPORTS
3. B. Layton, R. Krikorian, J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 17. L. Calandreau et al., J. Neurosci. 26, 13556 (2006). Acknowledgments: We thank F. Naneix for helpful discussions
14, 254 (2002). 18. A. Desmedt, R. Garcia, R. Jaffard, J. Neurosci. 18, and C. Dupuy, A. Faugere, A. Grel, and F. Roug-Pont for
4. S. Tronel, C. M. Alberini, Biol. Psychiatry 62, 33 (2007). 480 (1998). technical help. N.K., A.D., and P.V.P. conceived and
5. J. L. McGaugh, B. Roozendaal, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 19. R. G. Phillips, J. E. LeDoux, Behav. Neurosci. 106, designed the experiments. N.K., M.V., N.M., Y.P., L.B.B.,
205 (2002). 274 (1992). L.C., and A.D. performed the experiments. N.K., M.V.,
6. J. M. Revest et al., Nat. Neurosci. 8, 664 (2005). 20. J. J. Kim, M. S. Fanselow, Science 256, 675 (1992). and A.D. analyzed the data. N.K., A.M., A.D., and P.V.P.
7. J. M. Revest et al., Mol. Psychiatry 15, 1125, 1140 21. P. V. Piazza, M. Le Moal, Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 25, wrote the manuscript. This work was supported by Centre
(2010). 359 (1997). National de la Recherche Scientifique, Institut National
8. C. Sandi, M. T. Pinelo-Nava, Neural Plast. 2007, 78970 22. P. Trifilieff, L. Calandreau, C. Herry, N. Mons, J. Micheau, de la Sant et de la Recherche Mdicale, Fondation pour la
(2007). Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 88, 424 (2007). Recherche sur le Cerveau, Conseil Rgional dAquitaine,
9. M. van Zuiden et al., Biol. Psychiatry 71, 309 (2011). 23. A. P. Scicli, G. D. Petrovich, L. W. Swanson, R. F. Thompson, Ministre de lEnseignement suprieur et de la Recherche,
10. R. Yehuda, J. Clin. Psychiatry 62 (suppl. 17), 41 (2001). Behav. Neurosci. 118, 5 (2004). and University of Bordeaux.
11. L. R. Squire, Psychol. Rev. 99, 195 (1992). 24. S. Brummelte, L. A. Galea, Neuroscience 168, 680
12. B. S. McEwen, R. M. Sapolsky, Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 5, (2010).
Supporting Online Material
205 (1995). 25. M. R. Hunsaker, R. P. Kesner, Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/science.1207615/DC1
13. E. R. de Kloet, E. Vreugdenhil, M. S. Oitzl, M. Jols, 89, 61 (2008).
Materials and Methods
Endocr. Rev. 19, 269 (1998). 26. I. Liberzon et al., Biol. Psychiatry 45, 817 (1999).
Figs. S1 and S2
14. J. D. Bremner, J. H. Krystal, S. M. Southwick, D. S. Charney, 27. S. L. Rauch et al., Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 53, 380
References
J. Trauma. Stress 8, 527 (1995). (1996).
15. M. W. Gilbertson et al., Nat. Neurosci. 5, 1242 (2002). 28. L. M. Shin et al., Am. J. Psychiatry 156, 575 (1999). 28 April 2011; accepted 9 February 2012
16. Materials and methods are available as supporting 29. S. D. Smith, B. Abou-Khalil, D. H. Zald, J. Abnorm. Published online 23 February 2012;
material on Science Online. Psychol. 117, 479 (2008). 10.1126/science.1207615

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on October 29, 2016


can be used to control the specific time window
Generation of a Synthetic Memory Trace in which active neurons are genetically tagged
with hM3Dq by modulating tTA-driven transcrip-
Aleena R. Garner,1,2 David C. Rowland,3 Sang Youl Hwang,1 Karsten Baumgaertel,1 tion (11, 13). To test the kinetics of CNO-based
Bryan L. Roth,4 Cliff Kentros,3 Mark Mayford1,2* neural activation in these animals, we performed
in vivo recording in the hippocampus of anesthe-
We investigated the effect of activating a competing, artificially generated, neural representation tized animals. We found an increase in neuronal
on encoding of contextual fear memory in mice. We used a c-fosbased transgenic approach activity that reached a maximum intensity be-
to introduce the hM3Dq DREADD receptor (designer receptor exclusively activated by designer drug) tween 30 and 40 min after CNO injection (Fig.
into neurons naturally activated by sensory experience. Neural activity could then be specifically 1D). To examine the increase in neural activity
and inducibly increased in the hM3Dq-expressing neurons by an exogenous ligand. When an more broadly, we used endogenous c-fos expres-
ensemble of neurons for one context (ctxA) was artificially activated during conditioning in a sion as an indicator of neural activity (Fig. 1, E
distinct second context (ctxB), mice formed a hybrid memory representation. Reactivation of the and F). Relative to controls, we found significant
artificially stimulated network within the conditioning context was required for retrieval of the increases (by a factor of 2 to 20) in c-fos
memory, and the memory was specific for the spatial pattern of neurons artificially activated labeling across multiple brain regions in CNO-
during learning. Similar stimulation impaired recall when not part of the initial conditioning. injected hM3Dqfos transgenic mice (table S1).
Labeling for c-fos was found in both hM3Dq-
irect electrical stimulation can be used to ural sensory stimuli (710). How does this inter- positive and hM3Dq-negative neurons, with 91 T

D define functional domains in the brain,


elicit stereotyped behavioral responses,
drive self-stimulation behavior, and serve as con-
nally generated activity influence the formation
of a new memory representation?
To investigate this question, we used trans-
2% of hM3Dq-positive neurons in CA1 colabeled
with c-fos (fig. S2).
In standard contextual fear conditioning, an-
ditioned or unconditioned stimuli in conditioning genic mice (Fig. 1A) in which the hM3Dq receptor imals develop a memory for the conditioning
paradigms (14). This type of stimulation has is expressed in an activity-dependent manner by chamber in which they receive a footshock. The
typically been focal, using either microelectrodes a c-fos promoterdriven tTA transgene (hM3Dqfos ability to form the context association is depen-
or, more recently, genetically encoded mediators mice) (11, 12). hM3Dq is a Gq-coupled receptor dent on the hippocampus, which participates in
of neural excitability such as channelrhodopsin that responds specifically to clozapine-N-oxide encoding a representation of the environment
(5, 6). Although such discrete, temporally coor- (CNO) and produces strong depolarization and (14, 15). To test the effects of competing circuit
dinated, focal stimulation can drive behavior, we spiking in pyramidal neurons (12). Transgenic activation on the formation of a memory trace,
know much less about the effects of stimulating mice exposed to a particular environmental stim- we designed the fear-conditioning protocol out-
broadly distributed neural networks. The mam- ulus will express hM3Dq in those neurons that are lined in Fig. 2A. On day 1, hM3Dqfos mice were
malian cortex displays substantial nonrandom, sufficiently active to induce the c-fos promoter, exposed to a novel context (ctxA) in order to
spontaneous neural activity that is internally gen- and this naturally occurring neural ensemble can drive expression of the hM3Dq transgene into
erated rather than arising from sensory inputs, be subsequently reactivated artificially in the trans- neurons activated in that context. On day 2, mice
and this activity influences the processing of nat- genic mice by delivery of CNO. Artificial activi- were injected with Dox to inhibit further hM3Dq
ty induced in this manner will retain the spatial receptor expression and with CNO to stimulate
1
Department of Cell Biology and Dorris Neuroscience Center, character of the neural ensemble, but will not activity in the spatial pattern of neurons that
The Scripps Research Institute, 10550 North Torrey Pines Road,
preserve the temporal dynamics achieved by expressed the receptor. The mice were then fear-
La Jolla, CA 92037, USA. 2Neurosciences Graduate Program,
University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La natural stimuli. conditioned in a distinct context (ctxB), and 24
Jolla, CA 92037, USA. 3Department of Psychology, Institute of The expression of hM3Dq is widely distrib- hours later, memory performance was tested in
Neuroscience, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403, USA.
4
uted in the brain of hM3Dqfos double transgenic the absence and presence of CNO. Thus, we fired
Departments of Pharmacology, Chemical Biology, and Medic- mice in the absence of doxycycline (Dox), the neurons active in ctxA while the mice were
inal Chemistry and Program in Neuroscience, University of
North Carolina Chapel Hill Medical School, Chapel Hill, NC enabling tTA-driven transcription (Fig. 1, B fear-conditioned in ctxB.
27599, USA. and C). Within a given brain area, expression is We anticipated three potential outcomes. The
*To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: limited to a fraction of excitatory neurons that are strong synthetic activation of ctxA neurons could
mmayford@scripps.edu sufficiently active to drive the c-fos promoter. Dox be dominant and serve as a conditioned stimulus

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 335 23 MARCH 2012 1513


Glucocorticoids Can Induce PTSD-Like Memory Impairments
in Mice
Nadia Kaouane, Yves Porte, Monique Valle, Laurent
Brayda-Bruno, Nicole Mons, Ludovic Calandreau, Aline
Marighetto, Pier Vincenzo Piazza and Aline Desmedt (February 23,
2012)
Science 335 (6075), 1510-1513. [doi: 10.1126/science.1207615]
originally published online February 23, 2012

Downloaded from http://science.sciencemag.org/ on October 29, 2016


Editor's Summary

Remembering Stressful Events


Situations surrounding emotional events are better remembered than others that accompany
neutral events. However, in severe pathological states such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD),
exposure to threatening situations can also result in memory impairment. In this case, a hypermnesia for
a salient trauma-related cue is associated with loss of memory for important aspects of the traumatic
event. The memory for the core traumatic event is enhanced, but the capacity to place it in the right
place and in response to the right cues is reduced. Kaouane et al. (p. 1510, published online 23
February) associated a high-intensity threat with the infusion of corticosterone in the hippocampus to
induce PTSD-like memory impairments in mice. The animals became unable to identify the threat
context as the right predictor of the threat, and they showed a fear response for discrete salient cues
normally identified as safe. The neural activation patterns in the amygdala and hippocampal regions of
these mice were similar to those observed in human PTSD.

This copy is for your personal, non-commercial use only.

Article Tools Visit the online version of this article to access the personalization and
article tools:
http://science.sciencemag.org/content/335/6075/1510

Permissions Obtain information about reproducing this article:


http://www.sciencemag.org/about/permissions.dtl

Science (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published weekly, except the last week
in December, by the American Association for the Advancement of Science, 1200 New York
Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. Copyright 2016 by the American Association for the
Advancement of Science; all rights reserved. The title Science is a registered trademark of AAAS.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen