Sie sind auf Seite 1von 6

Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 408413

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Cognitive emotion regulation strategies: Gender differences and associations


to worry
Kimberly R. Zlomke a,*, Kathryn S. Hahn b
a
Baylor College of Medicine, Texas Childrens Hospital, Adolescent and Sports Medicine, 6621 Fannin St., CCC1710.00 Houston, TX 77030-2399, USA
b
Millsaps College, Psychology Department, 1701 North State St Jackson, MS 39210, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Research generally supports differences in the prevalence of GAD and reports of excessive worry between
Received 10 December 2008 men and women. Psychosocial theories espouse individual vulnerability factors as correlates of anxiety
Received in revised form 27 October 2009 and in turn related to gender differences. Emotion regulation is one vulnerability factor that has shown
Accepted 3 November 2009
involvement in the development, exacerbation, and/or maintenance of anxiety, although there is insuf-
Available online 14 December 2009
cient evidence of this direct contribution to observed gender differences in anxiety. Using a sample of
1080 young adults, the current study examines the differential use of cognitive emotion regulation strat-
Keywords:
egies between males and females and the subsequent effect on worry. Results of the present study pro-
Cognitive emotion regulation
Worry
vide tentative support for differential cognitive emotion regulation strategies between gender as a
Gender differences vulnerability to increased worry and potentially GAD. Specically, males and females signicantly dif-
Anxiety fered in the endorsement of use of rumination, putting problems into perspective and blaming others
as cognitive emotion regulation strategies.
2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction cial, behavioral, and cognitive processes (Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spin-
hoven, 2001; Garnefski, Legerstee, Kraaij, van den Kommer, &
The extant literature on the gender distribution of the preva- Teerds, 2002; Thompson & Calkins, 1996). Several denitions of
lence of anxiety disorders suggests that baserates for specic disor- emotion regulation ranging from modulation of emotional inten-
ders are higher for women than men. Specically, the baserate of sity and arousal (e.g., Gross & Thompson, 2007) to the ability to
Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) in females is approximately control behavioral responses to negative emotions have been
twice the reported baserate of the disorder in men (Wittchen, examined across the full spectrum of anxiety disorders (e.g., Ams-
Zhao, Kessler, & Eaton, 1994). Biological theories suggest that cer- tadter, 2008; Campbell-Sills, Barlow, Brown, Hofmann, 2006) and
tain biological or genetic characteristics unique to women, such as specically GAD (e.g., Decker, Turk, Hess, & Murray, 2008;
hormones, are key in the development of anxiety symptomatology McLaughlin, Mennin, & Farach, 2007; Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, &
(Seeman, 1997). In contrast, psychosocial theories espouse individ- Fresco, 2005; Salters-Pedneault, Roemer, Tull, Rucker, & Mennin,
ual vulnerability factors as correlates of anxiety and in turn related 2006).
to gender differences (Lewinsohn, Gotlib, Lewinsohn, Seeley, & Al- A subset of these emotion regulation processes include the con-
len, 1998). Vulnerability factors such as cognitive processes (Beck, scious cognitive regulation processes (Garnefski et al., 2001). Cog-
Emery, & Greenberg, 2005; Riskind, 1997), personality (Brandes & nitive emotion regulation can generally be described as the
Bienvenu, 2006; Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994), and emotion reg- management of emotionally arousing information with conscious
ulation (Amstadter, 2008; Martin & Dahlen, 2005) have been cognitive strategies (Thompson, 1991). Such cognitive strategies
shown to be involved in the development, exacerbation, and/or are crucial in the management of threatening or stressful events
maintenance of anxiety, although there is insufcient evidence of by assisting individuals to manage, regulate, and control over emo-
this direct contribution to observed gender differences in anxiety tions (Garnefski et al., 2001).
(Garnefski, Teerds, Kraaij, Legerstee, & van den Kommer, 2004). Recent research has begun to examine differences between
Emotional regulation is conceptualized as a broad construct males and females on the use of specic cognitive emotion regula-
encompassing a number of regulatory processes, including the reg- tion strategies (Garnefski et al., 2004; Martin & Dahlen, 2005).
ulation of the experience of emotion as well as the regulation of the Garnefski et al. (2004) found that males and females reported
underlying features of emotion such as physiological reactivity, so- differential reliance on a number of strategies, with the most
striking differences for rumination, positive refocusing, and
catastrophizing. For all three of these signicant differences, fe-
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 281 979 2372.
E-mail address: zlomke@bcm.edu (K.R. Zlomke).
males reported using such strategies more often. For both males

0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.007
K.R. Zlomke, K.S. Hahn / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 408413 409

and females, it appears the increased use of positive reframing dur- strategies for managing the intake of emotionally arousing infor-
ing stressful situations is related to decreased levels of depression mation. The assessed strategies primarily involve thoughts which
whereas those who engage in rumination or catastrophizing when are used to manage or regulate ones emotions or mood in the face
confronted with stressful situations reported increases in symp- of threatening or stressful life events (Garnefski et al., 2002;
toms of depression. Martin and Dahlen (2005), also reported that Thompson, 1991). The CERQ has demonstrated acceptable internal
females endorsed signicantly higher levels of a number of cogni- consistency and ve-month testretest reliability (Garnefski et al.,
tive coping strategies. 2001). Previous research documents a strong conceptual and
Worry is intricately linked to anxiety in general and to General- empirical factor structure for the CERQ (i.e. Garnefski & Kraaij,
ized Anxiety Disorder (GAD) specically. Excessive worry is the 2007; Garnefski et al., 2001). For the current administration of
core symptom of GAD and has been shown to effectively discrim- the CERQ, the internal consistency was acceptable with a = .91
inate GAD from other anxiety disorders (American Psychiatric for the total CERQ score and subscales ranging from a = .75 to
Association, 2001). Borkovec, Davey, and Tallis (1994) describe a = .86.
worry as the cognitive component of anxiety, often in the form Life stressors were measured with the Life Events Questionnaire
of verbal thoughts as opposed to the cognitive, behavioral, and (LEQ; Norbeck, 1984), a checklist assessing the number and impact
physiological presentation of anxious symptomatology. Borkovec, of various life events. Norbeck (1984) reported adequate internal
Ray, and Stber (1998) also suggest that the cognitive phenomena consistency and testretest reliability of the LEQ. Computed inter-
of worry is distinct from the physiological and behavioral manifes- nal consistency for the current administration of the LEC was in the
tations of anxiety and related disorders. The relationship between acceptable range (total events a = .87). In the present sample,
gender and worry has largely been ignored in the empirical litera- males and females did not differ in the total number of events
ture and the available data is often mixed. In studies in which gen- experienced, but did differ signicantly in the number of life events
der differences in worry were directly examined, women reported experienced in four categories. Males reported experiencing more
signicantly higher levels of excessive worry than their male coun- events related to parenting and nances, whereas females reported
terparts (Dugas, Freeston, Ladouceur, 1997; Dugas, Gosselin, & a greater number of events related to love/marriage and
Ladouceur, 2001; Lewinsohn et al., 1998; McCann, Stewin, & Short, relationships.
1991; Robichaud, Dugas, & Conway, 2003; Stavosky & Borkovec, The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS; Lovibond &
1988). Other research reports mixed results, suggesting no differ- Lovibond, 1995a) is a 42-item self-report that assesses symptoms
ences in worry across males and females (i.e. Brown, Antony, & of depression, anxiety, and stress in adults and adolescents. The
Barlow, 1992; Tallis, Davey, & Bond, 1994). DASS has demonstrated good internal consistency and acceptable
Given the increased rates of GAD in women and excessive worry testretest reliability (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995a, 1995b). Only
as the primary symptom of GAD, worry itself is an important con- the Anxiety and Stress Scales were utilized for the current study.
struct for the understanding of GAD and other anxiety disorders. Internal consistency for the current administration of the DASS
While these are important ndings, a more salient question may was excellent (DASS Anxiety a = .93, DASS Stress a = .94). No differ-
be whether the differences in the use of cognitive coping strategies ence was found between males and females on the Anxiety sub-
are differentially related to anxiety and worry in men and women. scale of the DASS, whereas differences between males and
The identication of discrepancies on the use of cognitive emotion females were recorded on the DASS Stress subscale
regulation strategies by males and females may provide useful (t(1078) = 2.26, p < .05) with females (M = 26.49, SD = 9.26)
information by which to understand the heightened vulnerability reporting signicantly higher levels of physiological symptoms
of women for excessive worry and GAD. The current study exam- commonly associated with anxiety (i.e. difculty relaxing, in-
ines differential use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies creased tension, irritability).
across males and females and their subsequent effects in worry. The Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer, Miller,
Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990) is a 16-item self-report measure of
excessive and uncontrollable worry. The PSWQ possesses high
2. Method
internal consistency and satisfactory testretest reliability (Molina
& Borkovec, 1994). Internal consistency of the PSWQ for the cur-
2.1. Sample
rent administration was excellent (a = .93). In the present sample,
males and females signicantly differed in worry scores
Participants included 1080 young adults recruited from under-
[t(1078) 10.13, p < .001). As expected, women (M = 54.87,
graduate psychology courses at a large southern university. The
SD = 12.18) reported signicantly more symptoms of excessive
sample included 291 males (27%) and 789 females (73%) ranging
worry than males (M = 46.64, SD = 10.91).
from 18 to 28 years old, with a mean age of 20.07 (SD = 1.73). The
majority of the participants were Caucasian (84%). No differences be-
tween males and females were found for age, race, or marital status.
3. Results

2.2. Procedure 3.1. Use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies by males and
females
Participants were part of a large study of worry in college stu-
dents. All participation was conducted through internet adminis- Table 1 presents means and standard deviations for each of the
tered questionnaires. Participants were informed of the purposes cognitive emotion regulation strategies for males and females sep-
of the study and provided consent through the internet adminis- arately. Males and females reported frequent use of the strategies
tered questionnaire. of Positive Reappraisal, Refocus on Planning and Putting into Per-
spective. An ANCOVA was conducted to determine if multivariate
2.3. Instruments differences existed between males and females in the use of cogni-
tive emotion regulation strategies, after controlling for participant
The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ; Gar- age and specic life events experienced. The results indicated that
nefski et al., 2001) is a 36-item measure of cognitive coping strat- there was a signicant overall difference between males and
egies. Nine conceptually derived subscales assess cognitive females (Wilks k = .944; F(9, 1029) = 6.76, p < .001). Follow-up
410 K.R. Zlomke, K.S. Hahn / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 408413

Table 1
Descriptive statistics and univariate F-test for CERQ subscales.

Males Females Effect sizea Univariate F-testsb


M SD M SD F (1, 1037)
Self-Blame 10.37 3.20 10.21 3.54 .05 1.72
Acceptance 12.14 3.25 12.24 3.41 .03 0.03
Rumination 10.92 3.32 11.66 3.43 .22 5.42*
Positive Refocusing 9.78 3.34 10.28 3.43 .15 2.52
Refocus on Planning 12.43 3.31 12.97 3.78 .15 0.84
Positive Reappraisal 13.15 3.62 13.33 3.85 .05 0.03
Putting into Perspective 12.42 3.48 13.13 3.76 .20 5.29*
Catastrophizing 8.78 3.31 8.78 3.50 0 0.01
Other-Blame 8.51 2.78 7.89 2.89 .22 9.78**
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
a
Effect sizes reported as Cohens d.
b
ANCOVA correcting for covariates of age, love, relationships, parenting, and nances. Wilks lambda (with covariates) = 0.94; F (9, 1030) = 6.77, p < .0001.

univariate F-tests revealed that signicant differences were pres- tered into step 2, the regression model was signicant and ex-
ent for a number of strategies (see Table 1). plained a signicant portion of the variance in anxiety (24%),
stress (38%), and worry (27%). The strategies of Self-Blame and
3.2. Relationship between cognitive emotion regulation strategies and catastrophizing were consistent across the three models, in that
anxiety these coping strategies were positively predictive of anxiety, stress,
and worry. This nding suggests that higher levels of these strate-
Hierarchical multiple regressions were performed separately gies will result in greater reports of anxiety and excessive worry as
for males and females to examine the predictive value of cognitive well as the increased physiological symptoms typically associated
coping strategies for anxiety, stress, and worry. All tolerance values with anxiety. Greater levels of Acceptance and Positive Reappraisal
were within the acceptable range. To control for life events, the were related to lower levels of excessive worry for women.
LEQ subscale scores for Love, Relationships, parenting, and nances In order to determine whether cognitive emotion regulation
were entered at step 1, followed by the CERQ subscales in step 2. strategies account for the relationship between gender and worry,
For males, the entered life events were predictive of increased anx- a third hierarchical multiple regression was conducted. Using the
iety and stress but were not signicantly related to reports of wor- PSWQ as the dependent variable, the four subscales of the LEQ pre-
ry (see Table 2). When the cognitive emotion regulation strategies viously examined were entered at step one. In the second step the
were entered into step 2, the regression model was signicant and nine subscales of the CERQ were entered with gender entering the
explained a signicant portion of the variance in anxiety (39%), equation in the third step. The overall regression equation ac-
stress (40%), and worry (28%). For males, increased reports of rumi- counted for a signicant amount of the variance in worry, R = .57,
nation and catastrophizing were related to increased levels of wor- F(14, 1043) = 34.44, p < .001. When entered in the rst step, the life
ry and stress whereas increased use of the strategy of Refocus on events accounted for 2% of the variance in worry, R = .13, F(4,
Planning is related to decreased levels of anxiety, stress, and worry. 1043) = 4.21, p < .005. The cognitive emotion regulation strategies
The same analysis was repeated for females. The entered life accounted for an additional 24% of the variance in worry scores,
events were signicantly predictive of anxiety, stress and excessive R = .51, F(13, 1043) = 27.12, p < .001. In the third step of the equa-
worry. When the cognitive emotion regulation strategies were en- tion, gender was entered as a predictor and accounted for an addi-

Table 2
Relations between cognitive emotion regulation, anxiety, stress, and worry in males and females.

DASS-anxiety DASS-stress PSWQ


Male b Female b Male b Female b Male b Female b
Step 1: covariates
Love .14* .05 .11 .12*** .13 .02
Relationships .13* .02 .18** .02 .07 .03
Parenting .11 .06 .05 .05 .04 .01
Finances .02 .01 .03 .01 .05 .03
Step 1 R2 .07*** .02** .06*** .04*** .002 ns .02**
Step 2: cognitive strategies
Self-Blame .16* .16*** .20** .16*** .10 .15***
Acceptance .08 .06 .08 .07* .08 .11**
Rumination .13 .07 .15** .25*** .34*** .35***
Positive Refocusing .12* .11** .13* .01 .07 .06
Refocus on Planning .25*** .13 .22** .06 .21** .04
Positive Reappraisal .01 .01 .06 .08 .01 .19**
Putting into Perspective .02 .04 .01 .00 .05 .05
Catastrophizing .16* .25*** .20** .23*** .27*** .15***
Other-Blame .28*** .18*** .17* .14*** .01 .05
DR2 .33*** .27*** .34*** .33*** .28*** .27***

ns = nonsignicant result.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.
K.R. Zlomke, K.S. Hahn / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 408413 411

tional 6% of the variance in worry scores above and beyond the emerged that adaptive strategies were related to lower levels of re-
prediction by life events and cognitive strategies. ported anxiety and worry. Interestingly, the pattern of these rela-
tionships differed between men and women for the adaptive
strategies. Whereas a signicant relationship was found between
4. Discussion Refocus on Planning and lower levels of anxiety, stress, and worry
for men, this relationship was not demonstrated for women. The
Although it is widely recognized that women report a dispro- strategy of Refocus on Planning refers to the cognitive part of a
portionate amount of excessive worry and in turn are diagnosed problem-focused coping strategy, namely problem-solving. For
with Generalized Anxiety Disorder twice as often as men, the the males in the current sample, increased reports of thinking
empirical literature on gender differences in worry is sparse. The about how to handle negative situations was signicantly related
current study focused on the comparability of males and females to lower levels of anxiety and worry. Ladouceur, Blais, Freeston,
regarding the reporting of cognitive emotion regulation strategies and Dugas (1998) found that poor problem-solving orientation
and their relationship with worry and anxiety. Results indicate that (i.e., low problem-solving condence and low sense of control over
cognitive emotion regulation strategies do play an important role problem-solving process) but not actual problem-solving skills dif-
in the symptomatology of excessive worry and anxiety for both ferentiated individuals meeting criteria for GAD and a subclincial
men and women, although the reporting of specic strategies did population. Therefore, reliance and condence in problem-solving
differ between the genders. Alternatively, anxiety and worry symp- skills may serve as a protective factor for males and pathological
tomatology may also play an important role in the development worry.
and use of cognitive emotion regulation strategies for both men On the other hand, a negative relation was found between the
and women. In the absence of longitudinal research, the exact nat- strategies of Acceptance and Positive Reappraisal and worry for
ure of any casual relation between cognitive emotion regulation women, suggesting that resigning oneself to outcomes and
strategies and anxiety or worry is unclear. The relative predictive attaching a positive meaning to life events is associated with
value of specic emotion regulation strategies for anxiety and wor- lower levels of worry. Although Martin and Dahlen (2005) re-
ry varied for men and women. ported a positive relation between acceptance and stress, Garnef-
The largest gender differences were found for the strategies of ski et al. (2001) and Carver et al. (1989) suggest that acceptance
rumination, Putting into Perspective, and Other-Blame. Whereas is an adaptive coping strategy. A growing body of literature indi-
women reported utilizing rumination and putting the situation in cates that chronic efforts to control, suppress, or avoid unsolic-
perspective in the face of stress more than men, men reported ited internal experiences and emotions may actually increase
blaming others during stressful situations more than women. In the frequency and severity of emotional experiences (Hayes,
contrast to other ndings, the current study did not reveal a differ- Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999; Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, &
ence between men and women for the strategy of Positive Reap- Strosahl, 1996). Chronic worry has been directly associated with
praisal (Martin & Dahlen, 2005). These ndings are also similar attempts to control and avoid negative emotions in both clinical
to previous research on depression, in that women are more likely and non-clinical samples (Roemer, Salters, Raffa, & Orsillo, 2005).
to focus on the emotional aspects of stressful experiences, discuss Further, Kashdan, Zvolensky, and McLeish (2008) found that
their emotional experiences with others, and engage in rumination anxious arousal and chronic worry were positively correlated
(Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987). Although rumination is traditionally with less acceptance of negative emotions for individuals high
thought of as specic and distinct to depression and worry is most in anxiety sensitivity. Our ndings lend support to mounting lit-
often referred to in the context of anxiety, these traditional theo- erature, indicating acceptance of emotional experiences and posi-
ries have been recently challenged. The repetitive thought feature tive reframing of emotions are related to adaptive psychological
that is common to both worry and rumination has been found to be functioning. However, these ndings indicate that acceptance
predictive of both anxiety and depression (Segerstrom, Tsao, Alden, and positive reframing may be particularly salient for conceptu-
& Craske, 2000) and other authors have reported that worry and alizing and treating females who report experiencing pathologi-
rumination are highly correlated but distinct constructs (Fresco, cal worry and stress. Due to gender role socialization, females
Heimberg, Mennin, & Turk, 2002). may be more likely to use the specic strategies of acceptance
Secondly, although both rumination and catastrophizing were and positive reframing (Garnefski et al., 2004). This is not to
related to the reporting of worry for males and females, a number say that males may not benet from use of or treatment empha-
of differences emerged regarding the extent to which specic cog- sizing the development of acceptance of positive reframing strat-
nitive strategies were predictive of worry. For both males and fe- egies. Further research examining these emotion regulation
males, higher levels of rumination and catastrophizing were strategies could result in improved treatment efcacy for GAD
strongly related to increased stress and excessive worry. For males, and other anxiety disorders.
one additional emotion regulation strategy was related to worry, The literature suggests that several factors may contribute to
Refocus on Planning. Increased reporting of thinking about how increased rates of anxiety and worry in females. For example wo-
to handle a negative event was signicantly related to lower levels men have been found to exhibit increased levels of negative affect
of anxiety, stress, and worry in males. On the other hand, the strat- (Arcus & Kagan, 1995; Lynn & Martin, 1997) as well as increased
egies of Self-Blame, Acceptance, and Positive Reappraisal were re- negative self-focus (Mor & Winquist, 2002) that may lead to the
lated to worry exclusively for females. Carver, Scheier, and development and maintenance of worry and other anxiety-related
Weintraub (1989) reported that accepting that a negative event pathology. More research is necessary to better understand the
has occurred and attaching meaning to a negative event for per- numerous underlying mechanisms that have been posited to inu-
sonal growth are both coping strategies which are positively re- ence gender specic increases or decreases in anxiety and worry.
lated to optimism, self-esteem and negatively related to anxiety.
Specically, the current results conrm recent research ndings 4.1. Limitations
in that Acceptance and Positive Reappraisal were negatively pre-
dictive of excessive worry for females. This study is only a preliminary study of gender differences
An interesting pattern of results pertaining to the relative and the predictive role of the cognitive emotion regulation in wor-
reporting of adaptive versus maladaptive cognitive coping strate- ry. Consequently, several limitations should be noted. This study
gies was found. For the entire sample, a consistent pattern utilized a non-selected sample of university students and results
412 K.R. Zlomke, K.S. Hahn / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 408413

may not generalize to clinical populations of GAD. A measure of Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive
emotion regulation, and emotional problems. Personality and Individual
socio-economic status was not included in the assessment, which
Differences, 30, 13111327.
limits conclusions regarding confounding impact on exposure to Garnefski, N., Legerstee, J., Kraaij, V., van den Kommer, T., & Teerds, J. (2002).
negative life events and coping abilities. The self-report method Cognitive coping strategies and symptoms of depression and anxiety: A
used is subject to biases that could alter the results of the study. comparison between adolescents and adults. Journal of Adolescence, 25,
603611.
Individuals with GAD may not be able to identify or clearly recog- Garnefski, N., Teerds, J., Kraaij, V., Legerstee, J., & van den Kommer, T. (2004).
nize their emotions (Mennin, Heimberg, & Turk, 2002), some par- Cognitive emotion regulation strategies and depressive symptoms:
ticipants in the present study may have had difculty reporting on Differences between males and females. Personality and Individual Differences,
36, 267276.
their internal experience on self-report measures. Future research Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations.
would benet from incorporating objective methodologies such as In James J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). New York, NY:
behavioral observations in addition to self-report measures to bet- Guilford Press.
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy:
ter understand the concept of emotion regulation and gender re- Understanding and treating human suffering. New York: Guilford Press.
lated to increased worry and anxiety. Along such lines as well, the Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. D. (1996).
measures also did not include a measure of social desirability. Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional
approach to diagnosis and treatment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Lastly, the current investigation examined only one subset of Psychology, 64, 11521168.
emotion regulation strategies and many other strategies exist be- Kashdan, T. B., Zvolensky, M. J., & McLeish, A. C. (2008). Anxiety sensitivity and
yond cognitive; including behavioral, social, and physiological affect regulatory strategies: Individual and interactive risk factors for anxiety-
related symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 429440.
processes.
Ladouceur, R., Blais, F., Freeston, M. H., & Dugas, M. J. (1998). Problem solving and
problem orientation in generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,
12, 139152.
5. Conclusion Lewinsohn, P. M., Gotlib Lewinsohn, M., Seeley, J. R., & Allen, N. B. (1998). Gender
differences in anxiety disorders and anxiety symptoms in adolescents. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 107, 109117.
In conclusion, results of the present study provide tentative
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995b). The structure of negative emotional
support for differential cognitive emotion regulation strategies be- states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the
tween gender as a vulnerability to increased worry. Gaining a bet- Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33,
ter understanding of possibly different etiologies of worry is salient 335343.
Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995a). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress
to the conceptualization of GAD and its subsequent treatment. Scales (2nd ed.). Sydney: Psychology Foundation.
While these are interesting initial ndings, further research must Lynn, R., & Martin, T. (1997). Gender differences in extraversion, neuroticism,
be conducted to examine the relations between emotion regula- and psychoticism in 37 nations. Journal of Social Psychology, 137,
369373.
tion, gender, and worry. Martin, R. C., & Dahlen, E. R. (2005). Cognitive emotion regulation in the prediction
of depression, anxiety, stress, and anger. Personality and Individual Differences,
39, 12491260.
References McCann, S. J. W., Stewin, L. L., & Short, R. H. (1991). Sex differences, social
desirability, masculinity, and the tendency to worry. The Journal of Genetic
American Psychiatric Association (2001). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Psychology, 152, 295301.
disorder, 4th ed. text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American McLaughlin, K. A., Mennin, D. S., & Farach, F. J. (2007). The contributory role of worry
Psychiatric Association. in emotion generation and dysregulation in generalized anxiety disorder.
Amstadter, A. (2008). Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 17351752.
Disorders, 22, 211221. Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., & Turk, C. L. (2002). Applying an emotion regulation
Arcus, D., & Kagan, J. (1995). Temperament and craniofacial variation in the rst two framework to integrative approaches to generalized anxiety disorder. Clinical
years. Child Development, 66, 15291540. Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 8590.
Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. L. (2005). Anxiety disorders and phobias. New Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary
York: Basic Books. evidence for an emotion dysregulation model of generalized anxiety disorder.
Borkovec, T. D., Davey, G. C. L., & Tallis, F. (1994). The nature, functions, and origins Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 12811310.
of worry. In G. Davey, F. Tallis, & G. C. L. Davey (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives on Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and
theory, assessment and treatment (pp. 533). Oxford, England: John Wiley & validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and
Sons. Therapy, 28, 487495.
Borkovec, T. D., Ray, W. J., & Stber, J. (1998). Worry: A cognitive phenomenon Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire:
intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral Psychometric properties and associated characteristics. In G. C. L. Davey & F.
processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22(6), 561576. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspective on theory, assessment, and treatment
Brandes, M., & Bienvenu, O. J. (2006). Personality and anxiety disorders: A review. (pp. 265283). Oxford, England: John Wiley and Sons.
Current Psychiatry Reports, 8, 263269. Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-
Brown, T. A., Antony, M. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1992). Psychometric properties of the analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638662.
Penn State Worry Questionnaire in a clinical anxiety disorders sample. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30, 3337. theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 259282.
Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2006). Effects of Norbeck, J. S. (1984). Modication of recent life event questionnaires for use with
suppression and acceptance on emotional responses of individuals with anxiety female respondents. Research in Nursing and Health, 7, 6171.
and mood disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 12511263. Riskind, J. H. (1997). Looming vulnerability to threat: A cognitive paradigm for
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies: A anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 685792.
theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, Robichaud, M., Dugas, M. J., & Conway, M. (2003). Gender differences in
267283. worry and associated cognitive-behavioral variables. Anxiety Disorders, 17,
Clark, L. A., Watson, P., & Mineka, S. (1994). Temperament, personality and the 501516.
mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 103116. Roemer, L., Salters, K., Raffa, S., & Orsillo, S. M. (2005). Fear and avoidance in
Decker, M. L., Turk, C. L., Hess, B., & Murray, C. E. (2008). Emotion regulation among generalized anxiety disorder: A preliminary conceptual model. Cognitive
individuals classied with and without generalized anxiety disorder. Journal of Research and Therapy, 29, 7188.
Anxiety Disorders, 22, 485494. Salters-Pedneault, K., Roemer, L., Tull, M. T., Rucker, L., & Mennin, D. S. (2006).
Dugas, M. J., Freeston, M. H., & Ladouceur, R. (1997). Intolerance of uncertainty and Evidence of broad decits in emotion regulation associated with chronic
problem orientation in worry. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21(6), 593606. worry and generalized anxiety disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30,
Dugas, M. J., Gosselin, P., & Ladouceur, R. (2001). Intolerance of uncertainty and 469480.
worry: Investigating specicity in a nonclinical sample. Cognitive Therapy and Seeman, M. V. (1997). Psychopathology in women and men: Focus on female
Research, 25(5), 551558. hormones. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 16411647.
Fresco, D. M., Heimberg, R. G., Mennin, D. S., & Turk, C. L. (2002). Conrmatory factor Segerstrom, S. C., Tsao, J. C. I., Alden, L. E., & Craske, M. G. (2000). Worry and
analysis of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, rumination: repetitive thoughts as a concomitant and predictor of negative
40, 313332. mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 671688.
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The Cognitive Emotion Questionnaire: Stavosky, J. M., & Borkovec, T. D. (1988). The phenomenon of worry: theory,
Psychometric features and prospective relationships with depression and research, treatment and its implications for women. Women and Therapy, 6,
anxiety in adults. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 23, 141149. 7795.
K.R. Zlomke, K.S. Hahn / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 408413 413

Tallis, F., Davey, G. C. L., & Bond, A. (1994). The Worry Domains Questionnaire. In G. Thompson, R. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1996). The double-edged sword: Emotional
C. L. Davey & F. Tallis (Eds.), Worrying: Perspectives of theory, assessment, and regulation for children at risk. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 163182.
therapy (pp. 285297). New York: Wiley. Wittchen, H. U., Zhao, S., Kessler, R. C., & Eaton, W. W. (1994). DSM-III-R Generalized
Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Anxiety Disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General
Educational Psychology Review, 3, 269307. Psychiatry, 51, 355364.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen