Sie sind auf Seite 1von 14

Review Article

Executive Dysfunction
Address correspondence to
Dr Gil D. Rabinovici, University
of California, San Francisco,
Memory and Aging Center,
Nelson Rising Lane, Suite 190, Gil D. Rabinovici, MD; Melanie L. Stephens, PhD;
San Francisco, CA 94143,
Gil.Rabinovici@ucsf.edu. Katherine L. Possin, PhD
Relationship Disclosure:
Dr Rabinovici receives
research support from the ABSTRACT
Alzheimers Association; Avid
Radiopharmaceuticals, Inc; Purpose of Review: Executive functions represent a constellation of cognitive abi-
John Douglas French lities that drive goal-oriented behavior and are critical to the ability to adapt to an
Alzheimers Foundation; the
National Institutes of Health;
ever-changing world. This article provides a clinically oriented approach to classifying,
and the Tau Consortium. localizing, diagnosing, and treating disorders of executive function, which are pervasive
Dr Rabinovici has received in clinical practice.
honoraria for speaking
engagements from the
Recent Findings: Executive functions can be split into four distinct components:
Alzheimers Association, GE working memory, inhibition, set shifting, and fluency. These components may be dif-
Healthcare, and Medscape. ferentially affected in individual patients and act together to guide higher-order cognitive
Dr Stephens reports no
disclosure. Dr Possin receives
constructs such as planning and organization. Specific bedside and neuropsychological
research support from tests can be applied to evaluate components of executive function. While dysexecutive
Capability Maturity Model syndromes were first described in patients with frontal lesions, intact executive func-
Integration, the Hellman
Family Foundation, the National
tioning relies on distributed neural networks that include not only the prefrontal cortex,
Institutes of Health, and but also the parietal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, and cerebellum. Executive dys-
Quest Diagnostics. function arises from injury to any of these regions, their white matter connections, or
Unlabeled Use of neurotransmitter systems. Dysexecutive symptoms therefore occur in most neurode-
Products/Investigational
Use Disclosure: generative diseases and in many other neurologic, psychiatric, and systemic illnesses.
Drs Rabinovici, Stephens, and Management approaches are patient specific and should focus on treatment of the
Possin report no disclosures. underlying cause in parallel with maximizing patient function and safety via occupa-
* 2015, American Academy tional therapy and rehabilitation.
of Neurology.
Summary: Executive dysfunction is extremely common in patients with neurologic
disorders. Diagnosis and treatment hinge on familiarity with the clinical components
and neuroanatomic correlates of these complex, high-order cognitive processes.

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659.

INTRODUCTION originally described in the 1970s based


The term executive functions refers to on patterns of deficits observed in pa-
a constellation of cognitive abilities that tients with frontal lobe lesions.3,4 Execu-
enable and drive adaptive, goal-oriented tive abilities evolve over childhood and
behavior. These include the ability to adolescence, paralleling myelination
generate thought and think flexibly, to and synaptogenesis of the frontal lobes5
update and manipulate information men- and then decline with age in relation to
tally, to inhibit what is irrelevant to current loss of prefrontal function.6 Tasks re-
goals, to self-monitor, and to plan and quiring executive function activate dis-
adjust behavior as appropriate to the pre- tributed neural networks that prominently
sent context.1 Intact executive functions involve the prefrontal cortex, but also
are critical to the ability to adapt to an include the parietal cortex, basal ganglia,
ever-changing world, and deficits in thalamus, and cerebellum.7Y10 Executive
executive functioning lead to dispropor- functions are vulnerable to white mat-
tionate impairment in function and ac- ter injury11 and to perturbations in the
tivities of daily living.2 The cognitive cholinergic, noradrenergic, serotonergic,
construct of executive functions was and dopaminergic neurotransmitter

646 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


KEY POINTS
systems.12,13 As a result, executive func- executive abilities are driven by distinct, h Executive functions
tions are sensitive to a broad array of although related, processes.3,14,15 From include a constellation of
neurologic, psychiatric, and medical con- a clinical perspective, it is useful to split cognitive abilities that
ditions. Disorders of executive function executive functions into specific com- drive goal-oriented
are pervasive in clinical practice, and it ponents that can be differentially affected behavior and allow the
is critical for neurologists to be skillful in individual patients. This article empha- individual to adapt to an
in their assessment and treatment. sizes four components that make distinct ever-changing world.
This article defines the related but dis- clinical contributions: information updat- The concept of executive
sociable components of executive func- ing and monitoring (referred to in this functions first arose from
tioning that can be variably affected in article as working memory), inhibition of deficits seen in patients
with frontal lesions.
individual patients, introduces bedside prepotent responses, mental set shifting,
and neuropsychological tests used in the and fluency.14,15 h Executive functions can
assessment of these components and be split into four distinct
describes their proposed neuroanatomic Working Memory components that can be
differentially affected
correlates. This article provides a clinical Working memory is a limited capacity
in individual patients:
approach to assessing patients with dis- system that enables us to temporarily
working memory,
orders of executive functions, illustrating process, store, and manipulate informa- response inhibition, set
the diversity of clinical presentations and tion in conscious awareness. Examples shifting, and fluency.
the broad differential diagnosis, and con- from everyday life include rehearsing a
h Working memory
cludes with a brief review of current ap- phone number as we prepare to dial
enables us to temporarily
proaches to therapy. and registering a long sentence spoken process, store, and
in conversation as we process its meaning. manipulate information
COMPONENTS OF EXECUTIVE Patients with working memory deficits in conscious awareness,
FUNCTION may report absentmindedness and trou- an example of which is
Since the initial description of a central ble focusing (Case 4-1). Intact working the rehearsal of a phone
executive by Baddeley and Hitch in 1974, memory is critical to higher-level tasks number as we prepare
considerable debate has occurred about such as planning and decision making to dial. Patients with
whether executive function can be ex- as it allows us to actively keep track of working memory
plained by a single mechanism or whether all of the necessary information. An deficits may report
absentmindedness or
trouble focusing.

Case 4-1
A 74-year-old retired college professor was self-referred for memory loss
that he had been experiencing over the previous year. He retired 3 years ago
when his wife fell ill with metastatic breast cancer, and he served as her
primary caregiver until she passed away 9 months before this evaluation.
He noted difficulty staying focused and concentrating. He found himself
reading the same page in a book multiple times without retaining the
information and often lost track of his purpose when entering a room. The
patient reported anhedonia, crying spells, insomnia, and weight loss. His past
medical history was significant for an episode of major depression in
his 30s. On mental status testing he was a good historian, but his affect was
restricted. He became tearful when discussing his wifes death. He scored
30/30 on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and the remainder of
his neurologic examination was normal. On formal neuropsychological
testing, performance was generally in the above-average to superior range.
However, he scored below average on backward digit span (repetition of
numbers in reverse order) and letter fluency (generating as many words as
Continued on page 648

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 647

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Executive Dysfunction

KEY POINT
h Inhibition represents the Continued from page 647
ability to hold back
a predominant, possible beginning with a specific letter in 1 minute). He scored 18/30 on the
automatic, or previously Geriatric Depression Scale,16 a screening questionnaire in which scores over
learned response, which 10 raise concern for clinically significant depression. Brain MRI was normal.
may be inappropriate or The patient was referred to a psychiatrist with a presumptive diagnosis
irrelevant in the present of major depression and was started on a selective serotonin reuptake
context. Patients with inhibitor (SSRI) and cognitive-behavioral therapy. In follow-up 6 months
deficits in cognitive later, he reported that his mood and sleep were much improved. He started
inhibition may appear volunteering at a local museum. He felt that his cognition had improved,
stimulus bound or and repeat cognitive testing confirmed this improvement.
exhibit utilization behavior. Comment. Executive dysfunction can accompany mood disorders as
well as other psychiatric conditions. Patients may misrepresent their
symptoms as related to memory, although the primary problem is in
attention and executive functioning. Screening for depression should be
included as part of the neuropsychological evaluation, as illustrated here
by use of the Geriatric Depression Scale. This patient was at high risk for
depression given his wifes illness and death. The diagnosis of major
depression was made based on both mood and vegetative symptoms
and signs. His functional deficits were out of proportion to his formal
test scores, which is common in depression. Although his cognitive
symptoms responded to treatment for depression, he should continue to
be followed neurologically since late-life depression can represent a
prodrome of a neurodegenerative condition.

early model proposed by Baddeley and including tasks specific for visual infor-
Hitch3 split working memory into a phono- mation, are listed in Table 4-1.
logic loop that maintains auditory and
verbal information and a visuospatial Inhibition
sketchpad that maintains visual infor- Inhibition is the ability to hold back a
mation. This model has gained credence predominant, automatic, or previously
from functional neuroimaging studies learned response that may be inappro-
showing left-lateralized activations when priate or irrelevant in the present context.
performing verbal working memory tasks Certain stimuli are loaded to stimulate
and right lateralized activity in response an automatic behavioral response due to
to visually oriented tasks.17 either familiarity or immediate reward.18
Working memory can be further di- However, adaptive behavior may require
vided into tasks that require simple main- inhibition of the prepotent (ie, automatic
tenance of information (eg, forward digit or habitual) response in order to meet
span, which is a task that requires repeat- the current goal. Failure of cognitive
ing a chain of numbers) and those that inhibition should be distinguished from
require active manipulation of informa- behavioral disinhibition, which, while re-
tion (eg, backward digit span, in which lated, suggests a distinct anatomy and
numbers are repeated in reverse order). differential diagnosis. For more informa-
Either spelling WORLD backward or per- tion on this topic, refer to the article Dys-
forming serial 7s on the Mini-Mental function of Social Cognition and Behavior
State Examination (MMSE) are examples by Bradford C. Dickerson, MD, in this
of working memory tests that require issue of .
information manipulation. Additional ex- Patients who are cognitively disinhi-
amples of working memory paradigms, bited have difficulty actively ignoring

648 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


TABLE 4-1 Neuropsychological Tests of Executive Functions

Executive Brief Description of


Function Domain Relevant Neuropsychological Tests Neuropsychological Test
Working memory Digit Span subtest from the Wechsler Adult Digit Span subtest: repeat a series of
Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) numbers in forward and backward order
Corsi Block-Tapping Test or Spatial Span Corsi Block-Tapping Test and Spatial Span:
from the Wechsler Memory Scale, Third repeat a tapping sequence of up to nine
Edition (WMS-III) blocks in forward and backward order
N-back task and dot counting task from N-back task: indicate whether each of a
the National Institutes of Health (NIH) series of squares is in the same location as
Executive Abilities: Measures of the square presented one or two back
Instruments for Neurobehavioral Dot counting task: count and remember
Evaluation and Research (EXAMINER) the number of dots on a series of displays

Inhibition Flanker task, Continuous Performance Flanker task: indicate the direction the center
Test (CPT), or antisaccade task from the arrow is pointing as quickly as possible, while
NIH EXAMINER ignoring flanking arrows that, on some
trials, point in the opposite direction
CPT: respond to the target image (eg, a
five-pointed star) and withhold responses
to distractor images
Antisaccade task: move eyes in the opposite
direction of moving dots
Stroop test such as the Color-Word Stroop test: name the ink colors of color
Interference Test from the Delis-Kaplan words that are printed in discordant ink
Executive Function System (D-KEFS)
Set shifting Trail Making Test from the D-KEFS or the Trail Making Test: draw lines that connect
Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery numbers and letters in alternating and
ascending order
Wisconsin Card Sorting WCST: sort cards according to changing
Test (WCST) rules and based on examiner feedback
Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shift (IED) Test IED: select which of each pair of stimuli is
from the Cambridge Neuropsychological correct according to changing rules and
Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) based on examiner feedback
Set Shifting test from the NIH EXAMINER Set Shifting test: match objects by color or
shape according to changing rules that are
explicitly stated on the computer screen
Fluency Verbal and Design Fluency tests, (eg, Verbal Fluency test: generate as many
the Controlled Oral Word Association words as possible that begin with a specific
Test (COWAT) or from the D-KEFS) letter or are from a specific category (eg,
animals) in 1 minute
Design Fluency test: generate as many
different designs as possible while applying
a fixed set of rules (eg, using four lines) in
1 minute

irrelevant or even penalizing stimuli. They ing up and using objects they observe for
may appear easily distracted, stimulus no clear purpose), echolalia (involuntarily
bound, and impulsive. In advanced cases, repeating what is heard), or echopraxia
patients exhibit utilization behavior (pick- (involuntarily imitating actions).

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 649

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Executive Dysfunction

KEY POINT
h Set shifting reflects the Cognitive inhibition can be tested at blue, red, and green). In incongruent
ability to modify the bedside with go/no go tasks that trials on the Flanker task, patients must
attention and behavior in require the patient to respond to a certain indicate the direction the central arrow
response to changing stimulus while withholding a response is pointed, ignoring the adjacent arrows
circumstances and if presented with an alternative stimulus. that are pointed in the opposite direc-
demands. Patients with For example, the examiner can instruct tion (Figure 4-1B). Examples of addi-
deficits exhibit the patient to clap once if he or she claps tional tests are provided in Table 4-1.
perseverative behavior twice, but not to clap at all if the examiner
and rigid thinking. claps once.19 In the antisaccade task, the Set Shifting
patient is instructed to look away from an Set shifting reflects the ability to modify
engaging stimulus (eg, the examiners attention and behavior in response to
moving finger).20 Figure 4-1 illustrates changing circumstances and demands.
tests of cognitive inhibition administered Set shifting inherently also relies on work-
during neuropsychological testing. In ing memory (in order to keep in mind
the response inhibition portion of the the current goals) and response inhibition
Stroop test21 (Figure 4-1A), the subjects (in order to ignore a previously relevant
must inhibit the prepotent response to goal or focus of attention), illustrating the
read words and, instead, name the color interdependence of different compo-
of ink that the word is printed in (the nents of executive function. Patients
correct responses in the first row are with deficits in set shifting may report

FIGURE 4-1 Example tasks to assess response inhibition, set shifting, and fluency. A, For the
Stroop test, patients are asked to first read the words ignoring the colors (color
naming), then name the colors ignoring the word (interference). B, For the
Flanker task, patients are asked to identify the direction of the central arrow, with the
adjacent arrows pointing either in the same direction (congruent) or opposite direction
(incongruent). Stroop interference and incongruent trials on the Flanker task are considered
tests of response inhibition. C, Trail making is a classic set shifting task in which patients write
lines alternating in order between numbers and days of the week. D, Design fluency is an
example of a nonverbal fluency task, in which patients are asked to draw as many unique
designs as possible connecting the dots with four lines in 1 minute.

650 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


KEY POINTS
difficulties with multitasking and appear tongue word retrieval deficits (in the h Fluency is the ability
rigid in their thinking. On clinical evalu- absence of a true anomia), lack of initia- to maximize the
ation, they may exhibit perseverative tion or inertia, or disorganization. production of verbal or
thoughts or behaviors. The Luria manual The components of executive function visual information in a
sequencing task, in which patients are act in concert to enable planning and specific time period,
asked to alternate sequentially between organization, which are higher-order cog- examples of which
three hand positions (closed fist, fingers nitive constructs that allow an individual include category fluency
extended parallel to the ground, and to identify, prioritize, and properly seq- (eg, animals), letter
fingers extended perpendicular to the uence the individual steps needed to fluency (eg, D words),
ground), is a useful bedside test of set achieve a goal in an efficient manner and and design fluency.
shifting. Trail-making tasks are often em- adapt as needed to changes along the h The components of
ployed on neuropsychological testing. way.22 The functional consequences of executive function act
In the trail-making example shown in executive dysfunction in daily life include together to enable
Figure 4-1C, patients are asked to draw ineffective planning and disorganization. planning and organization,
which are higher-order
alternating lines connecting numbers and On neuropsychological testing, planning
cognitive constructs
days of the week in ascending order as can be evaluated via more complex tasks
that allow an individual
rapidly and accurately as possible. The (eg, The Tower of London test, which to identify, prioritize, and
number of correct lines, time of comple- requires moving colored beads across properly sequence the
tion, and number of shifting errors are pegs to reproduce a target design in as individual steps needed
recorded. Performance on shifting trials few moves as possible).23 to achieve a goal in
are often compared to sequencing trials an adaptive and
without a shifting component (eg, con- NEUROANATOMY OF EXECUTIVE efficient manner.
necting numbers only). Additional clas- FUNCTIONS h Executive functioning
sic set shifting paradigms are listed The concept of executive function first relies on distributed
in Table 4-1. arose from deficits observed in patients neural networks that
with frontal lobe lesions. With the advent include the prefrontal
Fluency of functional neuroimaging, it has be- cortex, parietal cortex,
Fluency represents the ability to maxi- come clear that executive functioning basal ganglia, thalamus,
mize the production of verbal or visual relies on distributed neural networks that and cerebellum.
information in a specific time period, Executive dysfunction
encompass the prefrontal cortex, but also
arises from injury to
while avoiding repeating responses. The engage the parietal cortex, basal ganglia,
any of these regions,
three most common types of fluency thalamus, and cerebellum. Overlapping their white matter
tasks are category, letter, and design. For activations in these regions are seen in connections, or
category fluency (also known as seman- paradigms that engage working memory, neurotransmitter systems.
tic fluency), subjects are asked to gener- set shifting, response inhibition, fluency,
ate as many words as possible from a and planning (Figure 4-224).7Y10,25 Execu-
specified category (eg, animals or gro- tive dysfunction can, therefore, arise from
ceries). For letter fluency (also referred any neurologic process that involves these
to as phonemic fluency), subjects are regions, their white matter connections,
asked to generate as many words as pos- or neurotransmitter systems (Table 4-2).
sible that start with a specified letter, ex- Executive functioning is also vulnerable
cluding names of people and places or to toxic-metabolic insults that lead to dif-
grammatic variants of previous responses. fuse, bihemispheric dysfunction.
For design fluency, subjects are asked The nature of executive deficits can
to generate as many designs as possible assist with more precise localization. The
while applying a fixed set of rules (eg, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex is engaged
using four lines to connect the dots) during set shifting, planning, and working
(Figure 4-1D). Clinically, deficits on these memory.26 The right prefrontal cortex
measures may correlate with tip-of-the- is specialized for self-monitoring and
Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 651

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Executive Dysfunction

FIGURE 4-2 Neuroimaging correlates of executive functions. A, Left (red ) and right (blue)
hemisphere functional networks that are activated on functional MRI during
executive control tasks are shown on a template brain in neurologic orientation.
B, Gray matter correlates of a composite executive function score derived from the Executive
Abilities: Measures and Instruments for Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research (EXAMINER)
battery identified via voxel-based morphometry. Black clusters represent brain regions in which,
across subjects, higher gray matter volumes were correlated with higher composite executive
function scores. All results are thresholded at PG.001 and corrected for multiple comparisons
using permutation analysis at PG.05.
R = right; L = left; MFG = middle frontal gyrus; SFG = superior frontal gyrus; IFG = inferior
frontal gyrus; MCG = middle cingulate gyrus.

Panel A network templates courtesy of Michael Greicius, MD, and the Functional Imaging in Neuropsychiatric
Disorders Laboratory, Stanford University.
24
Panel B is reprinted with permission from Possin KL, et al, J Int Neuropsychol Soc. B 2013 The International
Neuropsychological Society. journals.cambridge.org/action/displayAbstract?fromPage=online&aid=9135793
&fileId=S1355617713000611.

spatial tasks, whereas left hemisphere right frontal or parietal injury.28 On work-
regions are engaged in verbal process- ing memory tasks, the ventrolateral
ing. For example, greater deficits in verbal prefrontal cortex is active during re-
versus design fluency indicate greater trieval and maintenance of information,
left frontal dysfunction,27 whereas greater while dorsolateral prefrontal regions are
impairment in design fluency indicates activated when either active manipulation

652 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


TABLE 4-2 Differential Diagnosis TABLE 4-2 Continued
of Executive
Dysfunction b Primary Medical Conditions/
Toxic Metabolic
b Neurodegenerative Conditions Medication with
Frontotemporal dementia CNS-depressing effects
Dementia with Lewy bodies Substance abuse/withdrawal
Parkinson disease Sleep disorder (eg, insomnia,
Alzheimer disease sleep apnea)
Corticobasal degeneration Electrolyte abnormality
Progressive supranuclear palsy Hypoglycemia/hyperglycemia
Chronic traumatic encephalopathy Hypothyroidism/hyperthyroidism
Multiple system atrophy Vitamin B12 deficiency
Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis Pulmonary disorder (hypoxia,
b Other Primary Neurologic hypercapnia)
Conditions Congestive heart failure
Traumatic brain injury Chronic kidney disease/uremia
Vascular cognitive impairment End-stage liver disease/
Ischemic stroke hepatic encephalopathy

Intracranial hemorrhage Heavy metal toxicity

Subcortical ischemic vascular Thiamine deficiency


disease (Wernicke-Korsakoff syndrome)

Tumor Genetic metabolic disorder (eg,


Wilson disease, phenylketonuria)
Epilepsy
b Infectious Conditions
Multiple sclerosis
Delirium due to systemic infection
Hydrocephalus (idiopathic or
secondary) HIV/AIDS dementia complex

Tic disorder Neurosyphilis

Metabolic leukodystrophy Meningitis/encephalitis

Radiation-induced CNS Lyme disease


leukoencephalopathy b Developmental Conditions
Inflammatory encephalopathy Attention deficit hyperactivity
b Primary Psychiatric Conditions disorder

Depression Autism spectrum disorder

Anxiety Learning disability/


developmental delay
Bipolar affective disorder
CNS = central nervous system;
Obsessive-compulsive disorder HIV = human immunodeficiency virus;
AIDS = acquired immunodeficiency syndrome.
Schizophrenia

or updating of information is required.10 prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex) may


The preYsupplementary motor area is be particularly critical for assessing shifting
engaged during response selection, reward-punishment contingencies and
whereas the anterior cingulate cortex inhibiting inappropriate responses.31,32
plays a critical role in error detection.29,30 Subcortical structures support executive
Inferior frontal regions (ie, ventrolateral functions via their roles in cortico-basal

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 653

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Executive Dysfunction

KEY POINTS
h Executive control gangliaYthalamocortical circuits.33 The of cognitive impairment, beginning
networks are network engaged in executive control is with a comprehensive history and neu-
impacted by distinct from the salience network, which rologic examination.35 Patients are un-
many neurodegenerative is a network composed of frontal insula, likely to present a chief complaint of
diseases, whereas the anterior cingulate, and ventromedial pre- executive dysfunction and may misrep-
salience network, which frontal cortex, with connections to limbic resent their cognitive problem as mem-
mediates social and and subcortical structures.34 The salience ory loss. Specific questions that capture
emotional behavior, is network mediates decision making re- executive deficits include inquiries about
specifically targeted in lated to social and emotional as well as the patients difficulty with planning or
frontotemporal dementia. autonomic and interoceptive process- organization, problems with multitasking,
h When probing about ing (ie, monitoring the internal state as poor judgment or decisions, impaired
executive functions, reflected by heart and respiratory rates concentration/short attention span, dif-
clinicians should ask or homeostatic needs such as hunger ficulty with problem solving, mental
about the patients and thirst) and is targeted early and rigidity/inflexibility, and impulsivity. When
difficulty with planning testing global cognitive function, the
specifically in behavioral variant front-
or organization,
otemporal dementia. In contrast, in- Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)
problems with
volvement of executive control regions (www.mocatest.org) is more sensitive
multitasking, poor
judgment or decisions, of the prefrontal cortex is characteristic than the MMSE for detecting executive
impaired concentration/ of many neurodegenerative disorders dysfunction (Case 4-3).36 Bedside testing
short attention span, and is not specific for frontotemporal and neuropsychological batteries
difficulty with problem dementia (Case 4-2). For more infor- should test all of the executive compo-
solving, mental rigidity/ mation on this topic, refer to the article nents described above. For individual
inflexibility, and impulsivity. Dysfunction of Social Cognition and tests and reference to commonly used
h The Montreal Cognitive Behavior by Bradford C. Dickerson, batteries, see Table 4-1. The Executive
Assessment is more MD, in this issue of . Abilities: Measures and Instruments for
sensitive to deficits in Neurobehavioral Evaluation and Research
executive function than CLINICAL APPROACH TO PATIENTS (EXAMINER) (examiner.ucsf.edu) was
the Mini-Mental WITH EXECUTIVE DYSFUNCTION designed as an efficient (30-minute) bat-
State Examination. The clinical evaluation of patients with tery to test the spectrum of executive
executive dysfunction should follow functions across a broad range of ages
practice parameters for the assessment and disorders in clinical research and

Case 4-2
A 54-year-old woman sought evaluation after experiencing 2 years of progressive cognitive difficulties.
Her symptoms were first apparent during her work as a high-level executive. She reported difficulty
comprehending complex written material. She made a number of impulsive decisions with poor outcomes, and
she subsequently felt paralyzed when faced with multifaceted problems. She lost her job 1 year ago and has
been unable to get things together to apply for a new position. Her husband, interviewed separately, noted no
inappropriate behavior, loss of empathy, stereotyped behaviors, or dietary changes. Her elemental neurologic
examination was normal. She scored 28/30 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), but on formal
neuropsychological testing she showed moderate impairment on all tests of executive function (including digit
span forward and backward, a modified Trail Making Test, lexical fluency, semantic fluency, design fluency, and
Stroop inhibition), with milder deficits noted in naming and figure copying. On memory testing she showed mild
to moderate impairment in encoding and spontaneous retrieval but intact recognition. Fluorodeoxyglucose
positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) showed marked hypometabolism in bilateral temporoparietal,
dorsolateral, and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Figure 4-3). CSF biomarker studies revealed low amyloid-$42 and
high total and phosphorylated tau levels. The patient was diagnosed with probable Alzheimer disease
dementia, started on a cholinesterase inhibitor, and referred to a social worker to assist with future planning.
Continued on page 655

654 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Continued from page 654
Comment. While Alzheimer disease most commonly presents with episodic memory loss, patients can
present with primary deficits in executive functions, and this is particularly true of patients with an early age
of onset (under age 65). Executive dysfunction can also be the presenting cognitive deficit in vascular
dementia, Lewy body diseases, and frontotemporal dementia, among other neurodegenerative conditions
(Table 4-2). In this patient, preservation of emotional and social function (prominent early features in
frontotemporal dementia) and a normal motor examination pointed to Alzheimer disease as the likely
diagnosis. FDG-PET and CSF biomarkers confirmed the diagnosis. This case also highlights the poor
sensitivity of the MMSE to executive dysfunction and the importance of obtaining neuropsychological
testing to better characterize the cognitive correlates of this individuals devastating functional disability.

FIGURE 4-3 Fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET) of the patient in Case 4-2. Images
displayed in the National Institutes of Health color scale in neurologic orientation. Hypometabolism is
noted in bilateral temporoparietal and dorsal prefrontal cortex. Temporoparietal hypometabolism is
the FDG pattern associated with Alzheimer disease, while additional involvement of the prefrontal cortex may explain
the prominent executive dysfunction experienced by the patient.
L = left; R = right.

therapeutic trials.37 The composite A differential diagnosis for executive


EXAMINER score correlates with dysfunction is shown in Table 4-2, al-
informant-based measures of real-world though a completely comprehensive list
executive functioning and with gray cannot be provided given that executive
matter volumes in the prefrontal cortex dysfunction can accompany most pro-
(Figure 4-2).24 cesses that impact the brain. To address

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 655

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Executive Dysfunction

Case 4-3
A 62-year-old woman presented with progressive cognitive decline. She worked as a nursing assistant for a home
health company, and over the past year, she had been noted to be increasingly inefficient with poor prioritization
of tasks. She had recently been placed on personal leave after she had missed a number of appointments and
on one occasion attempted to administer a treatment to the wrong patient. She acknowledged being easily
distracted and having problems staying on task. She also noted frequent headaches. On mental status testing, she
scored 21/30 on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), losing points on trail making, backward digit
span, vigilance, abstract reasoning, and delayed recall (the latter improving with cuing). Physical examination
revealed a subtle left hemiparesis with left-side hyperreflexia and a mute plantar response. Brain MRI revealed a
dural-based enhancing extraaxial mass along the right anterior clinoid process, suggestive of a meningioma,
with displacement of the right inferior frontal cortex, and surrounding vasogenic edema (Figure 4-4). She was
referred for neurosurgical consultation.
Comment. This patient presented with dysexecutive symptoms, including difficulty with planning,
organization, multitasking, and attention. The neurologist appropriately used the MoCA to screen
global cognitive function, as this test includes several items that directly test executive functions. The subacute
presentation, presence of headaches, and focal motor findings increased suspicion for an underlying mass lesion,
which was confirmed on MRI. The lesion and associated edema were within executive control networks in the
right hemisphere and, thus, were very likely to be responsible for the patients clinical presentation.

FIGURE 4-4 Brain MRI of the patient in Case 4-3. Coronal postcontrast T1-weighted (A) and noncontrast
fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) (B) images show a dural-based enhancing extraaxial mass
along the right anterior clinoid process, suggestive of a meningioma, with displacement of the right
inferior frontal cortex, and surrounding vasogenic edema.
R = right; L = left.

656 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


KEY POINT
the broad differential diagnosis, the cli- versible or treatable conditions. Famil- h Treatment of dysexecutive
nician must characterize the relative in- iarity with the components of executive disorders needs to be
volvement of other cognitive domains, functions, underlying neuroanatomy, and tailored to the individual
motor and psychiatric symptoms and signs, differential diagnosis can help guide the patient, often requiring a
comorbid neuropsychiatric and medical clinician toward an accurate diagnosis combination of
conditions, and medication use. A labo- and optimal treatment plan. disease-specific
ratory evaluation for systemic disorders pharmacologic treatments,
and structural brain imaging is needed to REFERENCES occupational therapy,
1. Jurado MB, Rosselli M. The elusive nature and cognitive rehabilitation.
assess for treatable conditions (Case 4-3). of executive functions: a review of our
In selected cases, CSF or imaging bio- current understanding. Neuropsychol Rev
markers for Alzheimer disease may be 2007;17(3):213Y233. doi:10.1007/
s11065-007-9040-z.
helpful (Case 4-2).
2. Cahn-Weiner DA, Boyle PA, Malloy PF. Tests of
executive function predict instrumental activities
TREATMENT of daily living in community-dwelling older
Treatment of dysexecutive disorders individuals. Appl Neuropsychol 2002;9(3):187Y191.
needs to be tailored to the individual pa- doi:10.1207/S15324826AN0903_8.
tient. The first step is to identify and treat 3. Baddeley AD, Hitch GJ. Working memory. In:
Bower G, ed. Recent advances in learning and
the primary underlying condition. For motivation. London: Academic Press,
example, performance on executive func- 1974:47Y90.
tion tests responds to cholinesterase 4. Stuss DT, Benson DF. The frontal lobes.
inhibitors in Alzheimer disease,38 whereas New York: Raven Press, 1986.
patients with Parkinson disease perform 5. Anderson VA, Anderson P, Northam E,
better on dopamine replacement therapy.39 et al. Development of executive functions
through late childhood and adolescence in
An evaluation by occupational therapy an Australian sample. Dev Neuropsychol
can help clarify the real-world functional 2001;20(1):385Y406. doi:10.1207/
implications in order to ensure patient S15326942DN2001_5.
safety and maximize function.40 Cogni- 6. Buckner RL. Memory and executive function
in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause
tive rehabilitation strategies include decline and reserve factors that compensate.
environmental manipulation (eg, mini- Neuron 2004;44(1):195Y208. doi:10.1016/
mizing distractors and simplifying tasks), j.neuron.2004.09.006.
compensatory techniques (eg, increased 7. Collette F, Van der Linden M, Laureys S,
et al. Exploring the unity and diversity of
use of daily planners or smartphones),
the neural substrates of executive functioning.
and direct interventions (eg, repetitive Hum Brain Mapp 2005;25(4):409Y423.
training to improve a skill).41 Experi- doi:10.1002/hbm.20118.
mental approaches include dopamine 8. Monchi O, Petrides M, Strafella AP, et al.
agonist therapy in patients without Functional role of the basal ganglia in
the planning and execution of actions.
known dopamine deficiency (eg, post- Ann Neurol 2006;59(2):257Y264.
traumatic brain injury and stroke),42 doi:10.1002/ana.20742.
computer-based brain exercises,43 and 9. Wager TD, Jonides J, Reading S. Neuroimaging
transcranial magnetic stimulation.44 studies of shifting attention: a meta-analysis.
Neuroimage 2004;22(4):1679Y1693.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.052.
CONCLUSION
10. Wager TD, Smith EE. Neuroimaging studies
Intact executive functions are critical for of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cogn
adaptive behavior and day-to-day func- Affect Behav Neurosci 2003;3(4):255Y274.
tion and rely on widespread frontal, pa- doi:10.3758/CABN.3.4.255.
rietal, and subcortical brain networks. 11. Kerchner GA, Racine CA, Hale S, et al.
Cognitive processing speed in older adults:
Executive functions are vulnerable to a
relationship with white matter integrity.
broad array of neurologic, psychiatric, and PLoS One 2012;7(11):e50425. doi:10.1371/
medical processes, including many re- journal.pone.0050425.

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 657

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


Executive Dysfunction

12. Barnes JJ, Dean AJ, Nandam LS, et al. 25. Criaud M, Boulinguez P. Have we been
The molecular genetics of executive function: asking the right questions when assessing
role of monoamine system genes. Biol response inhibition in go/no-go tasks with
Psychiatry 2011;69(12):e127Ye143. fMRI? A meta-analysis and critical review.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0050425. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2013;37(1):11Y23.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.003.
13. Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Differential
regulation of fronto-executive function 26. Stuss DT, Alexander MP, Floden D, et al.
by the monoamines and acetylcholine. Fractionation and localization of distinct
Cereb Cortex 2007;17(suppl 1):i151Yi160. frontal lobe processes: evidence from focal
doi:10.1093/cercor/bhm066. lesions in humans. In: Stuss DT, Knight RT,
eds. Principles of frontal lobe function.
14. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, et al. New York: Oxford University Press,
The unity and diversity of executive functions 2002:392Y407.
and their contributions to complex Frontal
Lobe tasks: a latent variable analysis. 27. Baldo JV, Shimamura AP, Delis DC, et al.
Cogn Psychol 2000;41(1):49Y100. Verbal and design fluency in patients with
doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. frontal lobe lesions. J Int Neuropsychol Soc
2001;7(5):586Y596.
15. Fisk JE, Sharp CA. Age-related impairment
in executive functioning: updating, 28. Possin KL, Chester SK, Laluz V, et al.
inhibition, shifting, and access. The frontal-anatomic specificity of design
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004;26(7): fluency repetitions and their diagnostic
874Y890. doi:10.1080/13803390490510680. relevance for behavioral variant
frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol
16. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rolse TL, et al. Soc 2012;18(5):834Y844. doi:10.1017/
Development and validity of a geriatric S1355617712000604.
depression scale: a preliminary report.
J Psychiatr Res 1982Y1983;17(1):37Y49. 29. Lau H, Rogers RD, Passingham RE.
Dissociating response selection and conflict
17. Smith EE, Jonides J, Koeppe RA. Dissociating in the medial frontal surface. Neuroimage
verbal and spatial working memory using 2006;29(2):446Y451. doi:10.1016/
PET. Cereb Cortex 1996;6(1):11Y20. j.neuroimage.2005.07.050.
doi:10.1093/cercor/6.1.11.
30. Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA.
18. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex
among inhibition and interference control to behaviour. Brain 1995;118(pt 1):
functions: a latent-variable analysis. 279Y306. doi:10.1093/brain/118.1.279.
J Exp Psychol Gen 2004;133(1):101Y135.
doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101. 31. Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal
cortex. Brain Cogn 2004;55(1):11Y29.
19. Luria AR. Human brain and psychological
doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X.
processes. New York: Harper Row, 1966.
32. Levy BJ, Wagner AD. Cognitive control
20. Hellmuth J, Mirsky J, Heuer HW, et al.
and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex:
Multicenter validation of a bedside
reflexive reorienting, motor inhibition, and
antisaccade task as a measure of executive
action updating. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2011;1224:
function. Neurology 2012;78(23):1824Y1831.
40Y62. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05958.x.
doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e318258f785.
33. Schmahmann JD, Pandya DN.
21. Golden C. Stroop color and word test:
Disconnection syndromes of basal ganglia,
manual for clinical and experimental uses.
thalamus, and cerebrocerebellar systems.
Chicago, IL: Stoelting, 1978.
Cortex 2008;44(8):1037Y1066.
22. Grafman J. Planning and the brain. In: doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.004.
Miller BL, Cummings JL, editors. The human
34. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF,
frontal lobes: functions and disorders. 2nd ed.
et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks
New York: The Guilford Press, 2007:249Y261.
for salience processing and executive control.
23. Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. J Neurosci 2007;27(9):2349Y2356.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007.
1982;298(1089):199Y209.
35. Knopman DS, DeKosky ST, Cummings JL,
24. Possin KL, LaMarre AK, Wood KA, et al. et al. Practice parameter: diagnosis of
Ecological validity and neuroanatomical dementia (an evidence-based review).
correlates of the NIH EXAMINER executive Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee
composite score. J Int Neuropsychol Soc of the American Academy of Neurology.
2014;20(1):20Y28. doi:10.1017/ Neurology 2001;56(9):1143Y1153.
S1355617713000611. doi:10.1212/WNL.56.9.1143.

658 www.ContinuumJournal.com June 2015

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.


36. Lam B, Middleton LE, Masellis M, 40. Cramm HA, Krupa TM, Missiuna CA, et al.
et al. Criterion and convergent validity of Executive functioning: a scoping review of
the Montreal cognitive assessment with the occupational therapy literature. Can
screening and standardized neuropsychological J Occup Ther 2013;80(3):131Y140.
testing. J Am Geriatr Soc 2013;61(12):
41. DEsposito M, Gazzaley A.
2181Y2185. doi:10.1111/jgs.12541.
Neurorehabilitation of executive function.
37. Kramer JH, Mungas D, Possin KL, et al. In: Textbook of neural repair and
NIH EXAMINER: conceptualization and rehabilitation. Cambridge: Cambridge
development of an executive function University Press, 2005:475Y486.
battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc
2014;20(1):11Y19. doi:10.1017/ 42. McDowell S, Whyte J, DEsposito M. Differential
S1355617713001094. effect of a dopaminergic agonist on
prefrontal function in traumatic brain injury
38. Behl P, Lanctot KL, Streiner DL, et al.
patients. Brain 1998;121(pt 6):1155Y1164.
Cholinesterase inhibitors slow decline in
doi:10.1093/brain/121/6/1155.
executive functions, rather than memory, in
Alzheimers disease: a 1-year observational 43. Anguera JA, Boccanfuso J, Rintoul JL,
study in the Sunnybrook dementia cohort. et al. Video game training enhances
Curr Alzheimer Res 2006;3(2):147Y156. cognitive control in older adults. Nature
doi:10.2174/156720506776383031. 2013;501(7465):97Y101. doi:10.1038/
nature12486.
39. Oswal A, Litvak V, Sauleau P, Brown P.
Beta reactivity, prospective facilitation of 44. Luber B, Lisanby SH. Enhancement of human
executive processing, and its dependence cognitive performance using transcranial
on dopaminergic therapy in Parkinsons magnetic stimulation (TMS). Neuroimage
disease. J Neurosci 2012;32(29):9909Y9916. 2014;85(pt 3):961Y970. doi:10.1016/
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0275-12.2012. j.neuroimage.2013.06.007.

Continuum (Minneap Minn) 2015;21(3):646659 www.ContinuumJournal.com 659

Copyright American Academy of Neurology. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen