Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

504105

research-article2014
CDPXXX10.1177/0963721413504105Reis, CarothersGender Differences

Current Directions in Psychological

Black and White or Shades of Gray: Science


2014, Vol. 23(1) 1926
The Author(s) 2014
Are Gender Differences Categorical or Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Dimensional? DOI: 10.1177/0963721413504105


cdps.sagepub.com

Harry T. Reis1 and Bobbi J. Carothers2


1
Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester,
and 2Center for Public Health Systems Science, Washington University in St. Louis

Abstract
Sex differences are a perpetually popular topic among scholars and lay audiences, but no research has addressed the
underlying structure of these differences. Many people assume that sex differences in social behavior are categorical
that these differences represent fundamental distinctions between two distinct categories (taxa) of humans. Contrasted
with this view is the idea that sex differences are dimensionalthat differences between men and women indicate
nothing more than relative positions along overlapping continuous dimensions. We used taxometric methods to
examine whether a variety of well-established sex differences are indicative of taxa or dimensions. The evidence clearly
supported the latter. Thus, for the psychological constructs that we examined, there is little support for believing that
sex differences are anything more than individual differences that vary in magnitude from one attribute to another.

Keywords
sex differences, taxometric methods, essentialism

Ask anyone to tell you what men and women are like and Men Are From Mars, Women Are From Venus (Gray,
they likely can rattle off a long list of attributes. Men are 1992), which has sold more than 50 million copies world-
strong, aggressive, and good at building things, for exam- wide, exemplify this tendency. An alternate perspective,
ple, whereas women are caring, emotional, and have good which we call the dimensional approach, suggests that
verbal skills. Even children as young as 3.5 years old are gender differences are simply the average position of men
able to associate men and women with gender-stereotypic and women measured along continuous dimensions.
traits (Reis & Wright, 1982). Documented differences in the Our research was designed to compare these two
behavior, traits, or physiology of men and women are positions on a series of behaviors and personal attributes
abundant: Once peripheral to scientific psychology, sex that have been widely studied by researchers interested
differencesand to a lesser extent, similaritiesare now a in sex differences. In this regard, Hyde (2005, 2007)
popular research topic, incorporated regularly into jour- reviewed sex differences in 46 meta-analyses spanning
nals, textbooks, and curricula (Eagly, Eaton, Rose, Riger, & diverse cognitive, social, and personality domains and
McHugh, 2012). For example, in the decade from 2003 to concluded that men and women are substantially more
2012, PsycINFO reported more than 30,000 articles pub- similar than they are different. Although valuable in its
lished under the index term human sex differences.1 Small own right, this sort of review is limited to an examination
wonder, then, that sex differences are widely recognized of the degree of overlap in the score distributions of men
by laypersons and scholars alike. and women and, thus, the results obtained cannot reveal
Underlying observed differences are implicit assump- whether observed differences indicate the existence of a
tions regarding what these differences mean about the
nature of women and men. The extensive array of sex
Corresponding Author:
differences sometimes leads observers to conclude that Harry T. Reis, Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in
men and women represent two fundamentally distinct Psychology, University of Rochester, Box 270266, Rochester, NY 14627
categories of humans, or taxa. Popular books, such as E-mail: reis@psych.rochester.edu

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
20 Reis, Carothers

taxona classification that distinguishes one group of


people from another group of people in a nonarbitrary,
fundamental manner (Ruscio & Ruscio, 2008; Waller &
Correlation 1
Meehl, 1998). Researchers performing taxometric analy- Men
ses ask whether the underlying (latent, unobserved) sit-
uation is a single distribution or composed of two or

Height
more groups (each with its own distribution) (Meehl, Correlation 2
2004, p. 39). In other words, and as explained later, taxo- Women
metric analysis is concerned less with the magnitude of Correlation for
differences than with the pattern (correlation) of differ- Full Sample
ences across several variables examined simultaneously.
We used this analytic tool to empirically determine
whether, when it comes to psychological attributes, dif- Hair Length
ferences between men and women are better conceptu-
alized as fundamentally distinct or as relative positions Fig. 1. Schematic showing within-groups versus between-groups
along a single dimension. (or full sample) correlation patterns underlying taxometric analysis.
Adapted from Multivariate Taxometric Procedures: Distinguishing
Types From Continua (p. 13, Fig. 3.1) by N. G. Waller and P. E. Meehl,
Dimensional and Taxonic Models in 1998, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Copyright 1998 by Sage Publications.

Sex-Difference Research
The appeal of a categorical approach is not limited to lay well-documented plasticity and deep intricacy (Eliot,
audiences. Some evolutionarily oriented researchers, for 2011, p. 897) of the human brain has been used to explain
example, have treated mens and womens social behav- why reliable sex differences in brain structure and func-
ior as categorically distinct, thereby reflecting the diver- tion may have relatively small behavioral impact (Halpern,
gent adaptive tasks imposed by reproductive differences 2012).
and the effects of these differences on humans inherited
biological, cognitive, and behavioral makeup (e.g., Buss,
What Is a Taxon?
1995; Geary, 2010). Another well-known example is the
tend-and-befriend model, which posits that men and Before we describe our work, it is important to explain
women respond to stress in a fundamentally different what a taxon is and is not. Members of a taxon have a
manner (Taylor et al., 2000) that is linked to the influence greater likelihood of possessing traits that are character-
of the neuropeptide oxytocin. Indeed, tacit acknowledg- istic of that taxon than nonmembers do. Thus, knowing
ment of the possibility that mens and womens behavior that a person has two X chromosomes makes it more
may be categorically distinct often leads researchers to likely that this person will develop breasts, ovulate, have
search their data for gender differences, if not analyzing little facial hair, and exhibit other female physical charac-
mens and womens data separately. teristics than if this person has an X and a Y chromo-
Dimensionalist assumptions characterize researchers some. In contrast, although students who receive an A in
who believe that sex differences represent the relative a course are in a different category than are students who
position of women and men along continuous dimen- receive a B, the difference is not a taxon because the
sions; these dimensions may be grounded in personality underlying dimension (student performance) is continu-
traits (e.g., agency and communion; Spence & Helmreich, ous. Knowing on which side of the cutoff a student falls
1978), temperament (Brody, 2000), hormonal influences on one variable would be relatively uninformative about
( Jordan-Young, 2010), or life experiences. For example, which side of the cutoff a student falls on other attributes
by describing how most societies stratify boys and girls (Waller & Meehl, 1998).
socialization experiences on the basis of sex, and how Figure 1 graphically illustrates how the presence of a
these differential experiences shape the development of taxon influences data. When a taxon exists, variables
traits and competencies, researchers have suggested, tend to have restricted range and are uncorrelated within
implicitly or explicitly, that there exists a single continu- each group. For instance, height and hair length are
ous dimension along which individuals can be shifted probably uncorrelated within each sex (as the flat regres-
through experience (e.g., Wood & Eagly, 2012). The sion lines indicate). However, combining groups creates
dimensionalist position also appears in social neurosci- a correlation, which is often large, as the diagonal regres-
ence (notwithstanding the fact that neural sex differences sion line indicates, because cases within each group tend
are frequently, and often misleadingly, interpreted as to be similarly extremethat is, they fall on either side of
support for categorical models). In this case, the the midpoint of the distribution. In the absence of a

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
Gender Differences 21

taxon, the groups are not similarly extreme and, there- Haslam, & Ruscio, 2006, for a detailed discussion of the
fore, combining groups does not produce an inflated cor- criteria for selecting and grouping items). We first sought
relation. Thus, the between-groups and within-groups to validate our approach using two domains in which we
correlation patterns of multiple (three or more) variables expected to find clear evidence of a taxon based on sex:
assessed in the same individuals determines whether physical strength and anthropometric measurements. We
data are indicative of a taxon or a dimension. reasoned that these domains would be taxonic because
men and women differ physically in obvious ways.
Individuals who tend to be extreme on one variable
Examining Taxa and Dimensions in should also tend to be extreme on other variables in that
Social Behavior data set. For physical strength, we used three variables
Three statistical tests are commonly used to infer the from the 1998 to 2002 National Collegiate Athletic
existence of a taxon: MAMBAC (mean above minus Association outdoor track heptathlon and decathlon
below a cut), MAXEIG (maximum eigenvalue), and championships: distances in the long jump, high jump,
L-Mode (latent mode). Table 1 describes 7 representative and javelin throw. For anthropometric measurements, we
examples of the 22 analyses we conducted; the full set is used several measurements from the Centers for Disease
reported in Carothers and Reis (2013). For each analysis, Control and Preventions National Health and Nutrition
we used the actual properties of each data set (e.g., skew, Examination Survey III: height, shoulder breadth, and
number of indicators, number of participants, correla- waist-to-hip ratio. Some variables were combined into
tions among indicators) to create simulations that depict composites to satisfy the mathematical requirements of
what the data would look like if in fact a taxonic or a our analyses. In each data set, a clear taxon based on sex
dimensional structure existed. As is customary in taxo- was obtained.
metric analysis, we then visually compared the curve Figure 2 shows the results of our analysis of the data
generated by the actual data with these two simulations on physical strength. For all three methods (mean above
to determine which one represents a more plausible minus below a cut, maximum eigenvalue, and latent
underlying structure. Because none of these tests is mode), the data better resembled the taxonic simulation
definitive and interpretation is largely visual, consistency than the dimensional simulation. Very similar patterns
across multiple methods is used to confirm ones conclu- were obtained for the anthropometric measurements.
sions (Waller & Meehl, 1998).2 Thus, we concluded that this type of analysis is suitable
We selected data sets for our analyses on the basis of for identifying a sex-based taxon if it exists.
standard recommendations in the literature (Waller & Our substantive analyses were run on a series of 22
Meehl, 1998). Generally speaking, relatively large data discrete data sets in the published literature, which com-
sets having at least three moderately correlated and con- prised 122 indicators and 13,301 individuals from 13
ceptually linked variables are needed (see Ruscio, unique studies. These samples were relatively diverse;

Table 1. Representative Variable Sets for Analyses of the Latent Structure of Psychosocial Attributes Related to Sex

Analysis Variable set


Sexual attitude and behavior Appeal of multiple partners; appeal of sex with a stranger; attitude
toward sex without love; frequency of orgasm; frequency of
thinking about sex; frequency of masturbation
Mate selectivity Importance of financial prospects and status; desire for children;
dependable character; importance of good looks; emotional
stability
Sociosexual orientation Number of partners expected; number of one-night stands;
fantasizing about someone other than partner; comfort with sex
without love; comfort with casual sex; require closeness for sex
Social intimacy Provisions of guidance, reassurance of worth, nurturance, and social
integration in close relationships
Empathy Fantasy, Empathic Concern, Perspective Taking, and Personal Distress
subscales of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (Davis, 1980)
Masculinity/femininity/care orientation Masculinity (agentic traits); femininity (communal traits); caring for
others
Inclination toward science Enjoyment and interest in science; ease of learning science;
environmental awareness

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
22 Reis, Carothers

a
Categorical Comparison Data Dimensional Comparison Data

0.10 0.12 0.14 0.16

0.10 0.12 0.14 0.16


Mean Difference

Mean Difference
20 40 60 80 100 120 140 20 40 60 80 100 120 140
50 Cuts 50 Cuts

b Categorical Comparison Data Dimensional Comparison Data


0.6

0.6
Eigenvalue

Eigenvalue
0.4

0.4
0.2

0.2
0.0

0.0

1.0 0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.0 0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0
25 Windows 25 Windows

c
Categorical Comparison Data Dimensional Comparison Data
0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4

0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4


Density

Density

3 2 1 0 1 2 3 3 2 1 0 1 2 3
Factor Scores Factor Scores
Fig. 2. Results of analysis of data on physical strength. Results are shown separately for mean above
minus below a cut (a), maximum eigenvalue (b), and latent mode (c) statistical tests. Solid lines rep-
resent the minimum and maximum for the simulated data. Shaded bands represent the middle 50% of
the comparison (simulated) data points. For more information on the interpretation of these graphs,
see Ruscio, Haslam, and Ruscio (2006). Adapted from Figure 3 presented in Carothers and Reis (2013).

some were composed of American college students, taxometric methods, the samples were large, ranging
some were composed of adolescents, and others were from a few hundred to several thousand participants.
based on probability samples of American adults. All of Together, the data sets spanned a diverse range of topics
the data sets involved self-reported attitudes, preferences, that are widely thought to differ by sex and are some-
or behaviors, and all of the analyses included three to six times described in taxonic terms.
variables. In some cases, the variables were based on Results of these analyses can be summarized suc-
single items, whereas in other cases, we used composites cinctly: In nearly every case, the data unambiguously
created from highly correlated items. As required by resembled a dimensional model better than a taxonic

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
Gender Differences 23

model. In other words, the data showed little or no ten- conflict. Nevertheless, every time people invoke biology
dency toward the existence of a taxon and, instead, were to explain gender differences, they further strengthen the
consistently better matched to an underlying dimensional view that women and men are different human kinds
structure. To illustrate this conclusion, and because sex (Prentice & Miller, 2007, p. 205), a view that is inconsis-
differences in interest in the disciplines of science, tech- tent with our findings.
nology, engineering, and mathematics is a timely topic, Although essentialist thinking is often justified by
we provide results from our analysis of inclinations appealing to biological causation, it is important to rec-
toward science. These data were taken from a sample of ognize that on the basis of our findings, we are not argu-
American 15-year-olds (2,660 girls and 2,645 boys) who ing a position with regard to questions about biological
participated in the Programme for International Student or sociocultural causation. One can just as well be essen-
Assessment (a program run by the Organisation for tialist for sociocultural reasons (Bohan, 1993; Rangel &
Economic Co-operation and Development), which sur- Keller, 2011). To the extent that social-cultural processes
veys interest in science and scientific careers. As Figure 3 lead males and females to be treated differently, and to
shows, all three analyses were appreciably closer to their be segregated into different activities during develop-
dimensional simulations than to their taxonic simulations, ment, it is reasonable to expect that a taxon might
a result confirmed by their strong dimensional fit indices. emerge. Moreover, biological models can be dimensional,
Thus, the evidence points clearly to the conclusion that such as when a particular hormone relates incrementally
orientations toward science are dimensional and not to a particular behavior. Broadly speaking, our results are
taxonic. consistent with those biosocial models that view biologi-
cal and sociocultural influences as essentially continuous
and interactive, thereby encouraging individuals along
Implications and Conclusions developmental pathways that promote the development
Taken together, our results indicate that at least for the of varied propensities and dispositions (e.g., Wood &
psychological attributes that we studied, gender differ- Eagly, 2012).
ences should be considered as having an underlying This reasoning suggests an interesting and important
dimensional structure and not a taxonic structure. What caveat to our results, namely, our reliance on data col-
this means, in simpler terms, is that although there are lected in the past three decades from North American
average differences between men and women, these dif- adolescents and adults. This data set comprises people
ferences do not support the idea that men are like this, who live in a time and a place in which gender-based
women are like that. Had a taxon been identified, it prescriptions on behavior, both culturally and historically
would have meant that knowing that a person possesses speaking, are relatively weak, which suggests that indi-
male (or female) characteristics on one attribute would viduals who populate this culture may have become
imply that the person also possesses other male (or detaxonified. It is plausible that a gender taxon might
female) attributes. The fact that our results instead sup- be identified were some psychological archeologist to
ported a dimensional conceptualization indicates that unearth an appropriate data set from an earlier era. This
this inference is unwarranted; these sex differences are seems unlikely, however, given that sex-difference
better understood as individual differences that vary in research was far less common before feminism led scien-
magnitude from one attribute to another rather than as a tific psychology to pay attention to women and womens
suite of common differences that follow from a persons experiences (Eagly et al., 2012). More realistically, the
sex. Of course, it is still reasonable to talk about sex dif- modern-day world contains many cultures in which men
ferences. However, these differences should be consid- and women are treated much more differently than in the
ered in terms of overlapping continuous distributions relatively egalitarian culture of the current samples. Most
rather than natural or discrete categories. cultures divide at least some activities by sex, but the
If differences between men and women reflect varia- degree varies considerably from one culture to the next.
tions along a continuum, there is little reason to focus on We would expect such divisions of activity and labor to
explanations for categorical distinctions, much less to have broad and important consequences for the kinds of
reify them in essentialist terms. Yet this sort of thinking is psychological characteristics that we studied, as Eagly
common, and not just in the Mars-Venus community. For and Woods (1999) social-role theory has established.
example, as Fine (2010) explained, the influence of neu- Taxometric procedures provide a useful method for
rosexismthe tendency to justify differential treatment of exploring these consequences.
men and women by citing differences in neural anatomy Why do some people construe the social behavior of
or functioncan be seen in educational and employ- men and women in categorical terms? The tendency to
ment disparities, in arguments about same-sex schools think about the social world categorically is universal
and institutions, and in many types of family and marital (Medin, 1989); as Fiske (2010) observed, We love

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
24 Reis, Carothers

a
Categorical Comparison Data Dimensional Comparison Data

0.18

0.18
Mean Difference

Mean Difference
0.14

0.14
0.10

0.10
1000 3000 5000 1000 3000 5000
50 Cuts 50 Cuts
b
Categorical Comparison Data Dimensional Comparison Data
0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4

0.0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4


Eigenvalue

Eigenvalue

1.0 0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.0 0.5 0.0 0.5 1.0
25 Windows 25 Windows
c Categorical Comparison Data Dimensional Comparison Data
0.4

0.4
0.3

0.3
Density

Density
0.2

0.2
0.1

0.1
0.0

0.0

4 2 0 2 4 2 0 2
Factor Scores Factor Scores
Fig. 3. Results of analysis of data on science inclination (average of enjoyment, value, activity frequency,
interest in learning, interest in science career, ease of science tasks and learning, and environmental
awareness). Results are shown separately for mean above minus below a cut (a), maximum eigenvalue
(b), and latent mode (c) statistical tests. Solid lines represent the minimum and maximum for the simu-
lated data. Shaded bands represent the middle 50% of the comparison (simulated) data points. For more
information on the interpretation of these graphs, see Ruscio, Haslam, and Ruscio (2006). Adapted from
Figure A17 in the Supplemental Material section of Carothers and Reis (2013).

dichotomies (p. 689). Belief in a sex taxon provides an rooted in cultural tradition, and effectively account for
epistemic rationale for stereotyping on the basis of sex. behavioral differencesdescriptors that apply well to
To be sure, there are good reasons to rely on a persons sex. Indeed, social perceivers are often fairly accurate in
sex for forming first impressions and for organizing the their assessment of differences between the sexes, as
abundant information that our complex social world pro- Wood and Eagly (2012) noted. Rather than suggest that
vides. Social cognition favors categories that are readily social perceivers are inaccurate, the findings presented
observable, are discrete, have sharp boundaries, are here focus on potentially erroneous or unexamined
involuntary and mostly immutable, describe differences understandings of the structure and consistency of these

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
Gender Differences 25

differences. Of course, some differences between men Notes


and women are taxonicphysical strength and anthro- 1. To determine the emphasis of current work, we randomly
pometric measurements were identified as such in our selected 100 articles from the PsycINFO search. The first author
workand future empirical research could provide fur- classified these articles according to their predominant focus.
ther insight into other differences indicative of taxa. The The results were as follows: sex differences, 72; sex similari-
present research focused on only a select set of psycho- ties, 7; both sex differences and sex similarities, 8; contextual
logical characteristics, all assessed by self-report, and it is moderation of sex differences, 3; and unclear or irrelevant, 10.
possible that other characteristics or behavioral measures Of course, publication practices may bias the literature toward
would reveal categorical differences. Identification of the significant differences rather than nonsignificant effects.
latent structure of psychological constructs could be a 2. We also computed comparison curve fit indices (Ruscio
et al., 2006), which allowed us to determine whether the curve
valuable step in theory development (Ruscio & Ruscio,
generated by the actual data was closer to its taxonic or its
2008). dimensional simulation. These results were consistent with the
In conclusion, we hope that these and other innova- visual analyses in supporting the dimensional solution over the
tive methods help shine empirical light on fundamental taxonic solution.
questions about the nature of women and men. At
least with regard to the properties that we studied, References
rather than representing distinct categories of humans,
Bohan, J. S. (1993). Essentialism, constructionism, and feminist
women and men are cut from the same psychological psychology. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 17, 521.
cloth. Brody, L. R. (2000). The socialization of gender differences in
emotional expression: Display rules, infant temperament,
Recommended Reading and differentiation. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and
Carothers, B. J., & Reis, H. T. (2013). (See References). A fuller emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 2447). New
account of the research on which the present article is York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
based. Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differences: Origins
Eliot, L. (2009). Pink brain, blue brain: How small differences through sexual selection. American Psychologist, 50,
grow into troublesome gapsand what we can do about 164168. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.3.164
it. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt. A reader- Carothers, B. J., & Reis, H. T. (2013). Men and women are from
friendly discussion of neuroscientific research on sex Earth: Examining the latent structure of gender. Journal of
differences. Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 385407.
Fine, C. (2010). (See References). A thought-provoking, well- Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual
reasoned account of how the findings of sex-difference differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents
research are often misinterpreted. in Psychology, 10, 85.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). (See References). Written by a scholar who Eagly, A. H., Eaton, A., Rose, S. M., Riger, S., & McHugh,
has conducted many meta-analyses of sex differences, a M. C. (2012). Feminism and psychology: Analysis of a
thoughtful consideration of the evidence for sex differences half-century of research on women and gender. American
and sex similarities. Psychologist, 67, 211230.
Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of the Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences
behavior of women and men: Implications for the origins in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social
of sex differences. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699727. A roles. American Psychologist, 54, 408423.
review of what is arguably the most comprehensive theory Eliot, L. (2011). The trouble with sex differences. Neuron, 72,
of the origin of sex differences in behavior. 895898.
Fine, C. (2010). Delusions of gender. New York, NY: W. W.
Norton and Co.
Author Contributions Fiske, S. T. (2010). Venus and Mars or down to earth:
Both authors developed the study concept jointly. B. J. Carothers Stereotypes and realities of gender differences. Perspectives
collected and analyzed the data under the supervision of H. T. on Psychological Science, 5, 688692.
Reis. H. T. Reis drafted the manuscript, which was reviewed Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, female: The evolution of human
and approved by B. J. Carothers. sex differences (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association.
Acknowledgments Gray, J. (1992). Men are from Mars, women are from Venus.
New York, NY: HarperCollins.
We gratefully acknowledge the many colleagues who shared
Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex differences in cognitive abilities
their data sets with us. We also thank John Ruscio for sharing
(4th ed.). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
his expertise on taxometric methods.
Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American
Psychologist, 60, 581592. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Hyde, J. S. (2007). New directions in the study of gender simi-
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with larities and differences. Current Directions in Psychological
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Science, 16, 259263. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00516.x

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015
26 Reis, Carothers

Jordan-Young, R. M. (2010). Brainstorm: The flaws in the science Ruscio, J., & Ruscio, A. M. (2008). Categories and dimensions:
of sex differences. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Advancing psychological science through the study of latent
Medin, D. L. (1989). Concepts and conceptual structure. structure. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,
American Psychologist, 44, 14691481. doi:10.1037/0003- 203207. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00575.x
066X.44.12.1469 Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femi-
Meehl, P. E. (2004). Whats in a taxon? Journal of Abnormal ninity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates and ante-
Psychology, 113, 3943. cedents. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Psychological essentialism Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung,
of human categories. Current Directions in Psychological R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses
Science, 16, 202206. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00504.x to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight.
Rangel, U., & Keller, J. (2011). Essentialism goes social: Belief in Psychological Review, 107, 411429. doi:10.1037/0033-
social determinism as a component of psychological essen- 295X.107.3.411
tialism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, Waller, N. G., & Meehl, P. E. (1998). Multivariate taxometric
10561078. doi:10.1037/a0022401 procedures: Distinguishing types from continua. Thousand
Reis, H. T., & Wright, S. (1982). Knowledge of sex-role stereo- Oaks, CA: Sage.
types in children aged three to five. Sex Roles, 8, 10491056. Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2012). Biosocial construction of sex
Ruscio, J., Haslam, N., & Ruscio, A. M. (2006). Introduction differences and similarities in behavior. In M. Zanna & J. M.
to the taxometric method: A practical guide. Mahwah, NJ: Olson (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Erlbaum. (Vol. 46, pp. 55123). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Downloaded from cdp.sagepub.com at SUNY HEALTH SCIENCE CENTER on March 18, 2015

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen