Sie sind auf Seite 1von 12

Journal of

Experimental Psychology
VOL. 70, No. 1 JULY 1965

STEEPNESS OF APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE GRADIENTS


IN HUMANS AS A FUNCTION OF EXPERIENCE:
THEORY AND EXPERIMENT *
SEYMOUR EPSTEIN AND WALTER D. FENZ 2
University of Massachusetts

33 experienced and 33 novice parachutists rated their approach and


avoidance feelings at different points in time preceding and following a
parachute jump. For the novice parachutists, self-rated avoidance
increased up to a point shortly before the jump, and then decreased.
For the experienced parachutists, self-rated avoidance increased up to
the morning of the jump, decreased to the jump, and increased after the
jump. It was concluded that with experience the point of greatest
anxiety is displaced backward in time. To account for this phenomenon,
a miniature theory was presented which assumed that with continuous
exposure to threat, 2 developments take place, a heightening of the
gradient of anxiety and the development of a gradient of inhibition of
anxiety, the latter having the steeper slope.

Sport parachuting provides an un- ers, who appear to become increas-


usual opportunity for investigating ingly eager to jump as the time of the
approach-avoidance conflict in hu- anticipated jump approaches. This
mans under intensely ego-involving would suggest that for experienced
circumstances. Observation of in- jumpers the gradient of approach is
experienced parachutists suggests that steeper than that of avoidance. The
for them the gradient of avoidance is present study attempts to verify the
steeper than the gradient of approach. above observations by obtaining rat-
The enthusiasm exhibited some time ings of fear of, and desire to jump as
before the jump is seen to gradually a function of the sequence of events
wane and is replaced on the day of the leading up to, and following, a jump.
jump with fear and uncertainty. Such
is not the case with experienced jump- METHOD
1
This paper was presented, in part, at the The 5s consisted of 33 inexperienced and 33
American Psychological Association Conven- experienced sport parachutists. The former
tion in 1963. The study was supported by had made between 1 and S jumps, and the
Grant MH-01293 from the National Institute latter over 100 jumps. Shortly after a jump,
of Mental Health, United States Public 5s were asked to rate the strength of their
Health Service. approach and avoidance feelings at each of
2
Now at the University of Waterloo. the following points in time: (1) last week,
SEYMOUR EPSTEIN AND WALTER D. FENZ

(2) last night, (3) this morning, (4) upon weakest approach and assign it a value of 1.
reaching the airfield, (5) waiting to board the Having defined the range of their responses,
aircraft, (6) getting strapped, (7) boarding they were told to rate each of the other points
the aircraft, (8) during ascent in the plane, in a relative manner. They were instructed
(9) at the "ready" signal, (10) stepping out- to use the same number more than once to
side onto the step above the wheel of the designate points of equal intensity. Following
plane, (11) waiting for the signal to jump, ratings of approach, ratings of avoidance were
(12) freefalling, (13) after the chute has made in a corresponding manner. It should
opened, (14) immediately after landing. Ap- be noted that the procedure for rating that
proach was defined as "looking forward to the was employed provides information only on
jump, wanting to go ahead, being thrilled the relative strength of approach or avoidance
at the prospect of jumping"; avoidance as at the different points in time, and provides
"wanting to turn back and call the jump off, no basis for inferring absolute magnitude.
questioning why you ever let yourself get into
jumping, fear."
The 5s were told to try to keep their rat- RESULTS
ings of approach and avoidance as independ- Novice Parachutists
ent from each other as possible. To this end
it was pointed out that they could feel both In Fig. 1 it can be seen that self-
frightened and intrigued with jumping at the
same time, and that it was important in ratings of approach and avoidance are,
rating approach to consider only the attri- to a large extent, inverted replicas of
butes of approach, and to do the same for each other. Apparently, it was not
avoidance.
Following this, 5s were instructed to select possible for 5s to keep the two ratings
the point of strongest approach and assign it independent. Self-ratings of approach
a value of 10, and then to select the point of are relatively high at Point 1, 1 wk.

INEXPERIENCED PARACHUTISTS

MAXIMAL
AVOIDANCE
GOAL ..
ACT

JUMP SEQUENCE
FIG. 1. Mean self-ratings of approach and avoidance for inexperienced parachutists
as a function of a sequence of events leading to and following a jump.
HUMAN APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE GRADIENTS

before the jump, and fall off until Figure 2 presents curves for self-
shortly before the jump, after which rated avoidance and for self-rated
they again rise. The converse holds approach, the latter with the scale
for self-rated avoidance.3 It is par- reversed, and referred to as "inverse
ticularly noteworthy that the point approach." An F value of 41.39,
of maximum avoidance corresponds to significant at the .001 level, is obtained
the ready signal at Point 9 and not to for the differences among the means
the goal act at Point 12, which is the of the pooled ratings of avoidance and
moment of greatest danger, immedi- inverse approach as a function of the
ately preceding the opening of the time dimension (see Table 1). The
chute. failure of the 5s X T interaction to
An analysis of variance of the data reach significance indicates that the
represented in Fig. 1 would result in a curve form is consistent among indi-
spuriously high interaction between viduals except for deviations within
the time dimension and the approach chance expectancy. Of the 33 novice
and avoidance ratings, because the jumpers who took part in the experi-
curves are largely equivalent but ment, 30 rate approach as greater a
opposite forms of each other. An week before the jump than during the
analysis of variance of each curve free fall, and corresponding results are
separately, on the other hand, would obtained for self-ratings of avoidance.
reduce reliability by eliminating half Examination of the curves of avoid-
the data. A solution is provided by ance and inverse approach in Fig. 2
reversing one of the scales. Not only reveals that they differ from each
does this permit both sets of ratings to other in a systematic manner. From
be pooled, but it allows for a determi-
nation of whether there is an inter- pooled across 5s, are almost perfectly in-
action between the two scales and the versely related.
time dimension. If such is the case, It is important to keep in mind when
evaluating the results of the analyses of
it establishes a degree of independence variance that it is the pooled gradients of
between the two kinds of ratings.4 avoidance and inverse approach rather than
the difference between them, i.e., the inter-
3
The observation that self-ratings of action, that is of primary interest. This
avoidance are greater than of approach at the is because self-ratings of approach and
time of the jump may appear to present a avoidance do not yield independent gradients
problem, as the parachutist, after all, does of approach and avoidance, but are, by and
jump. The problem is more apparent than large, two ways of reporting the same feeling,
real. It will be recalled that the scales of which, as will be seen later, can be viewed as
approach and avoidance yielded relative a single net effect derived from independent
ratings on a 10-point scale within each hypothetical gradients of approach and
dimension, thereby requiring as many ratings avoidance. The major conclusions of the
of approach to be higher than avoidance as study would not be altered if only one of the
the reverse. Furthermore, as has been sets of self-ratings were retained. This is not
pointed out, the two kinds of ratings repre- to say that differences between the curves of
sent, for the most part, two ways of stating self-rated avoidance and inverse approach are
the same information. Accordingly, absolute of no importance, but to caution the reader
differences between the two sets of ratings against assuming that they are the major
cannot be taken at face value. source of inference about the relative steep-
4
If some degree of independence is found, ness of the hypothetical gradients. Such an
it does not, of course, invalidate the assump- error would be an easy one to make, as the
tion that self-ratings of approach and avoid- forms of the curves of avoidance and inverse
ance represent, to a predominant degree, a approach in Fig. 2 are similar in appearance
single feeling state. Inspection of Fig. 1 and 2 to goal gradients of approach and avoidance
makes it apparent that the two dimensions, as represented by Miller (1959).
SEYMOUR EPSTEIN AND WALTER D. FENZ

INEXPERIENCED PARACHUTISTS

MAXIMAL
AVOIDANCE

INVERSE APPROACH

5 6 7 8 9 10 12 13 14

JUMP SEQUENCE
FIG. 2. Mean self-ratings of avoidance and inverse approach for inexperienced parachutists
as a function of a sequence of events leading to and following a jump.

1 wk. before the jump (Point 1) to approach. From the ready signal on,
the ready signal (Point 9), the the decrease in self-rated avoidance is
increase in self-rated avoidance is greater than the decrease in inverse
greater than the increase in inverse approach. The avoidance ratings,
thus, tend to vary more sharply as a
TABLE 1 function of the time dimension than
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE FOR INEXPERIENCED the approach ratings. This con-
PARACHUTISTS ON SELF-RATINGS OF clusion is supported by a significant
AVOIDANCE AND INVERSE
APPROACH A X T interaction in Table 1.
Separate analyses of variance for
Source df MS F the ascending and descending por-
tions of the curve in Fig. 2 indicate
Between 5s 32 14.56 3.40***
Total within 5s 891 that in both cases the pooled gradients
Avoidance vs. inverse of avoidance and inverse approach are
approach (A) 1 23.71 1.85 significantly related to the time di-
5s X A 32 12.84" 3.13***
Time sequence (T) 13 252.04 41.39*** mension at the .01 level. While
5s XT 416 6.09> 1.49 inspection reveals that avoidance
AXT 13 7.23 1.77*
5s X A X T 416 4.07 tends to be steeper than approach for
both portions of the curve, the inter-
Error term for MSs above it up to the next error
term. The J?s X A X T Interaction was used to
action between kind of rating and
evaluate all other error terms.
*p <.OS.
time dimension is significant only for
***p <.001. the ascending portion.
HUMAN APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE GRADIENTS

Experienced Parachutists early in the sequence. This phenom-


Figure 3, like Fig. 1, is divided at enon is associated with a shift in the
two points, one corresponding to the peak of self-rated avoidance for the
point of greatest avoidance, and the experienced jumpers to an earlier
other to the goal act and point of point on the time dimension than for
greatest objective danger. There is an the inexperienced jumpers. It is note-
increase in self-rated avoidance (or worthy that after the goal act, the
conversely, a decrease in self-rated experienced jumpers report an in-
approach) up to the morning of the crease in avoidance.
jump, followed by a more protracted Figure 4 presents the curves of self-
decrease in self-rated avoidance (and ratings of avoidance and inverse
a corresponding increase in self-rated approach for experienced parachutists.
approach) up to the goal act, and An F value of 65.58, significant at the
finally an increase in self-rated avoid- .0001 level (see Table 2), is obtained
ance (or decrease in self-rated ap- for the combined ratings as a function
proach) after the goal act. Opposite of the time dimension. Twenty-nine
to the findings for novice parachutists, out of 33 experienced jumpers report
self-rated approach is higher (and that approach is greater during the
self-rated avoidance correspondingly free fall than on the morning of the
lower) at the time of the jump than jump, and corresponding results are

EXPERIENCED PARACHUTISTS

MAXIMAL GOAL ACT


AVOIDANCE

CO

rr
I
u_
-I
UJ
CO

5 6 7 8 9 10 12 13 14

JUMP SEQUENCE
FIG. 3. Mean self-ratings of approach and avoidance for experienced parachutists
as a function of a sequence of events leading to and following a jump.
SEYMOUR EPSTEIN AND WALTER D. FENZ

EXPERIENCED PARACHUTISTS

MAXIMAL GOAL
AVOIDANCE ACT

7 8 9 10 13 14

SEQUENCE
FIG. 4. Mean self-ratings of avoidance and inverse approach for experienced parachutists
as a function of a sequence of events leading to and following a jump.

obtained for self-ratings of avoidance. 5s X T interaction. The interaction


Individual differences in curve form between the time dimension and the
do not approach significance, as two kinds of ratings is significant at
indicated by an F value of 0.93 for the the .01 level, indicating a degree of
independence between approach and
TABLE 2 avoidance ratings.
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE FOR EXPERIENCED Inspection of Fig. 4 indicates that
PARACHUTISTS ON SELF-RATINGS OF in all three periods, the self-ratings of
AVOIDANCE AND INVERSE avoidance produce steeper gradients
APPROACH
than inverse approach. An analysis
Source df MS F of variance confined to the data pre-
ceding the peak of self-rated avoid-
Between 5s 32 12.03 27.12*** ance indicates that the pooled ratings
Total within 5s 891
Avoidance vs. inverse of avoidance and inverse approach
approach (A) 1 24.03 1.65 vary reliably over the time dimension,
5s X A 32 14.54" 3.29***
Time sequence (T) 13 265.14a 63.58*** and that the slope of the gradient of
5s X T 416 4.17 .93 avoidance is reliably steeper than the
AXT 13 13.92 a 3.14***
5s X A X T 416 4.43 slope of the gradient of inverse ap-
proach. An analysis of the period
Error term for MSs above it up to the next error from the peak of self-rated avoidance
term. The 5s X A X T interaction was used to
evaluate all other error terms. to the goal act produces corresponding
* t < .05.
***? <,001, results, although here both gradients
HUMAN APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE GRADIENTS

are falling. Analysis of the postaction source of variance 5s X T/G) do not


period indicates that while the rise approach significance, attesting to a
in the pooled ratings of avoidance and high degree of uniformity in the form
inverse approach as a function of the of the curve when experience is held
time dimension is highly reliable, the constant.
two curves are not reliably different
from each other. Nevertheless, as was DISCUSSION
the case for inexperienced jumpers, It was found that self-ratings of ap-
there is a tendency for avoidance ra- proach and avoidance, rather than rep-
tings in all periods to vary more sharply resenting independent processes, were
as a function of time than inverse ap- largely inverted replicas of each other.
proach ratings, and, therefore, than As it is necessary to assume separate and
approach ratings. opposed processes for conflict to exist,
and as there is no question but that
Table 3 summarizes the results of an parachuting provides an acute conflict
analysis of variance in which inex- for the novice parachutist, it is necessary
perienced and experienced parachut- to infer hypothetical gradients of ap-
ists are both included. The main proach and avoidance of which the self-
effect for groups is not significant, ratings are a net effect. According to
indicating that they do not differ in Lewin's (1946) and Miller's (1959)
rating style. The interaction between models of conflict, independent gradients
groups and the time dimension yields of approach and avoidance should both
an F value of 87.06, which indicates rise as a function of decreasing time to
the goal, with the avoidance gradient
that the difference in the curve form rising more steeply than the approach
for the two groups is highly reliable. gradient. The obtained results for the
At the same time, individual differ- novice parachutists, with self-rated ap-
ences within groups in curve form (see proach (or self-rated avoidance) treated
as a net effect, are in accord with such an
TABLE 3 assumption, as in order for net approach
ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE FOR INEXPERIENCED
to be high at a time remote from the
AND EXPERIENCED PARACHUTISTS ON jump and low at a time close to the
SELF-RATINGS OF AVOIDANCE jump, the hypothetical gradient of
AND INVERSE APPROACH avoidance would have to be steeper than
the hypothetical gradient of approach.6
Source if MS F The hypothetical gradients as presented
in Fig. 5 do not intersect, as the para-
Total between 5s 65 chutist does jump, and it is necessary
Groups (G) 1 161.91 12.17***
Ss/G 64 13.30" 3.13*** to represent approach as greater than
Total within 5s 1782 avoidance at the goal. Figure 6 presents
Avoidance vs. in- the net differences as derived to scale
verse approach
(A) 1 47.73 3.48 from Fig. 5. It is apparent that with
GXA 1 .001 6
5s X A/G 64 13.69" 3.22*** The reasoning here is no different from
Time sequence (T) 13 70.58 13.76*** that used to infer a steeper gradient of
GXT 13 446.60 87.06*** avoidance than approach when it is noted in
AXT 13 10.87 2.56* a conflict situation that a child (Lewin, 1946)
5s X T/G 832 5.13" 1.21
GX AXT 13 10.28 2.42* or rat (Miller, 1959) makes stronger approach
5s X A X T/G 832 4.25" responses at a distance than when close to a
goal. If we may take the liberty of anthropo-
Error term for mean squares above It up to the morphizing for a moment, Miller's rats, could
next error term. The 5s X A X T/G interaction was they report their feelings in a manner con-
used to evaluate all other error terms. sistent with their behavior, would reproduce
* p < .05.
*** p < .001. the data of our parachutists.
SEYMOUR EPSTEIN AND WALTER D. FENZ

remote time from the jump. Thus, for


them, the data might appear to suggest
that the hypothetical gradient of ap-
proach is steeper than the hypothetical
a: gradient of avoidance. Such a state of
o
u_ affairs would have obvious adaptive
o advantages, as anxiety would auto-
matically alert the parachutist well in
z advance of the potentially dangerous
111
cc event, arid be relatively low at the time
of the jump. However, there are several
reasons for favoring an alternate inter-
NEARNESS TO GOAL pretation which assumes that the hy-
pothetical gradient of avoidance is
FIG. 5. Hypothetical gradients of ap- steeper than that of approach, and adds
proach and avoidance as a function of time
to the goal in a conflict situation where the further assumptions. One difficulty with
goal is reached in the absence of external the first explanation is that it cannot
restraint.6 account for the highly reliable early rise
in self-rated avoidance (or corresponding
increasing nearness to the goal, net drop in self-rated approach) for the ex-
strength of approach steadily diminishes. perienced parachutists before the more
While the theoretical formulation rep- protracted decline (see Fig. 3 and 4) to
resented by Fig. 5 and 6 adequately the goal point. Secondly, the assump-
accounts for the general finding of a tion that the hypothetical gradient of
decrease in self-ratings of approach as approach is steeper than that of avoid-
the goal act is approached, it fails to ance is inconsistent with the findings
account for the decrease in the avoidance on self-rated avoidance and inverse ap-
ratings shortly before rather than im- proach. In three out of five comparisons
mediately after the goal point. This self-ratings of avoidance were reliably
phenomenon requires additional as- steeper than of inverse approach, and in
sumptions, which can best be presented the other two cases the results were in the
in the discussion of the experienced same direction. Thus, while it was con-
parachutists. cluded that the two ratings measure
For the experienced parachutists, self- predominantly one feeling, to the small
ratings of approach are greater (or extent that an independent component
conversely, self-ratings of avoidance is involved it supports the conclusion
weaker) at the goal point than at a more
6
It will be noted that the ordinate is
labeled as drive, rather than as strength of
response or response disposition, as Miller
labels it. Drive is defined as a directed force,
with separate measurable components of
direction and activation, the latter measurable APPROACH!
by physiological indexes of arousal, such as AVOIDANCE^
the GSR. Drive, as so denned, is more closely
related to Hull's (1943) excitatory potential I
a:
than to his concept of drive, and is similar to a.
o.
Lewin's (1946) concept of vector. To derive
total activation from hypothetical gradients
of approach and avoidance, the two gradients -*-
are added disregarding algebraic sign. To NEARNESS TO GOAL
derive net approach strength the gradients
are added considering algebraic sign. (For FIG. 6. Net approach strength as derived
a more detailed presentation of the theoretical from hypothetical gradients of approach and
approach, see Epstein, 1962.) avoidance in Fig. 5. (Units are arbitrary.)
HUMAN APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE GRADIENTS

that the hypothetical gradient of avoid- gradients develop, one based upon the
ance is steeper than that of approach. generalization of conditioned anxiety,
Thirdly, the first explanation is of no use and the other upon learned inhibition of
in accounting for the finding that the anxiety, the latter with steeper slope (see
peak of self-ratings of avoidance for Epstein, 1962, for a more detailed pres-
inexperienced jumpers precedes rather entation of the theory). In order to
than coincides with the goal point, while apply the theory to the present data, it
the alternate theory is able to do so, is only necessary to substitute a time
using the same set of assumptions that dimension for a cue dimension and rat-
account for the yet earlier peaking of the ings of fear and avoidance for a physio-
self-ratings of avoidance for experienced logical measure. The finding that the
jumpers. Finally, the second theory has peak of self-rated avoidance for novice
greater integrating power than the first parachutists precedes the goal act by
in accounting for findings reported by a small amount can be explained by
others. considering that the novices have had
The considerably earlier peak of self- some experience, in training, in identify-
rated avoidance for the experienced than ing with other jumpers, and in having
the inexperienced parachutists is con- made one to five jumps. Thus, the same
sistent with findings in an earlier study process which produces a marked effect
of parachutists (Epstein, 1962; Epstein in the experienced jumpers, is presumed
& Fenz, 1962) which used superficially to produce a limited effect in the novices.
different techniques, but of conceptual It should be noted that the theoretical
relevance to those in the present formulation as presented above does not
study. In the earlier work, GSR and follow Hull's (1943) or Miller's (1959)
reaction time were investigated as a views on conflict. One of the major
function of a cue dimension of words differences is that we are postulating two
related to parachuting embedded in a levels of inhibition, one consisting of a
word-association test. On a control day, hypothetical avoidance gradient which
2 wk. before a jump, experienced and inhibits a hypothetical approach gra-
novice parachutists could not be dis- dient, and the other of an inhibitory
tinguished from each other, both produc- gradient which inhibits the hypothetical
ing monotonic gradients of similar slope avoidance gradient. Thus, a concept of
on both dependent variables. On the day a hierarchy of inhibition is introduced
of a jump, however, experienced para- in place of the single-factor inhibition
chutists produced inverted v-shaped theory of Hull and Miller. Such a con-
curves, while novices simply produced ceptualization is necessary to account
steeper monotonic gradients. With in- for the appearance of inverted V-shaped
creasing experience, the peak of the curves of avoidance in place of monotonic
inverted v was found to advance toward gradients. It is assumed that inhibitory
the remote end of the dimension. Tested gradients, as higher-order responses, are
repeatedly after different amounts of steeper than the gradients they inhibit,
experience, all individuals demonstrated and that this relationship serves to free
the same developmental pattern, al- higher-order thought processes from pri-
though they differed in their rate of mary stimulus generalization. This for-
peaking back. As an example of an mulation differs from Miller's in that
individual developmental pattern, Fig. 7 Miller assumes a dichotomy between
is presented. A theoretical explanation higher- and lower-order processes, while
was offered which assumed that the the present position assumes a continuum
down-turn on the inverted v is the based upon a chaining of inhibitory
consequence of an inhibitory process that gradients of increasing slope.
serves to keep anxiety within nondisrup- It may be questioned as to whether
tive limits. The progressive peaking it is indeed possible to inhibit anxiety.
back was accounted for by assuming that While the mechanism is not evident and
with repeated exposure to threat, two deserves investigation in its own right,
4Or 80

CO
o " s v jt2 nd Jump ^
X -5 th Jump g
3O -
s ft)
o
o: w
o -19th Jump a
20

z;
QL. a
8 I.O -

5O

Neutral Low Medium High Neutral Low Medium High


w
STIMULUS DIMENSION - STIMULUS DIMENSION 2,
N
FIG. 7. GSR curves for a single 5 obtained at three levels of experience. ("Deviation score" refers to a transformation which controls
for individual lability and mean reactivity as evaluated by responses to control stimuli Prom Epstein, 1962].)
HUMAN APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE GRADIENTS 11

it is assumed that the inhibition of adaptive mechanism in nonhuman as


anxiety is similar to the inhibition of well as human animals. The phenom-
other autonomic responses which can enon is remarkably similar to Pavlov's
often be delayed until an appropriate (Pavlov, 1927, 1928, 1941) "paradoxical
time and place for their expression are phase." Pavlov noted that when his
available. Such responses include vomit- dogs were subjected to extreme states of
ing, voiding, crying, and fainting from stress over a prolonged period, a highly
physical exhaustion. Very likely what is orderly sequence of changes took place
involved is the production of responses in their conditioned response hierarchies.
incompatible with the inhibited response, Following a period of total inhibition,
i.e., distracting responses, or the in- a reverse gradient appeared, with the
hibition of goal-oriented responses, in- animal responding most strongly to stim-
cluding those involved in perception, uli that it had originally responded to
thought, and imagination. Whatever least strongly, and least strongly to stim-
the mechanism, there is considerable evi- uli that it had originally responded to
dence that anxiety can be inhibited. most strongly. Pavlov referred to this
Thus, in the present study, the increase complete reversal as the "ultraparadox-
in self-rated avoidance following the goal ical phase." Following this, there oc-
act for the experienced, but not the curred a gradual shift along the stimulus
novice, parachutists is consistent with dimension of the point of strongest
the assumption that inhibited anxiety response until the original gradient was
was released when inhibition no longer restored. With repeated exposure to the
served a purpose. In this respect it will same source of stress, the sequence un-
be recalled that the early peak of self- folded itself more and more rapidly
rated avoidance for the experienced and finally disappeared. The Pavlovian
jumpers was interpreted as resulting from phenomenon, as described above, is
a protracted period of inhibition before opposite in developmental direction to
the goal act. A similar release of in- that observed in our parachutists. How-
hibited anxiety was noted in a study by ever, this is theoretically to be expected,
Basowitz, Persky, Korchin, and Grinker as Pavlov attributed the above sequence
(1955), where it was found that army of responses to the gradual dissipation
paratroopers produced an unanticipated of an inhibitory state, whereas in the
rise in anxiety some time after a stressful theoretical formulation presented for the
training period. In a study in progress parachutists, the peaking back was
on dreams we are finding that parachut- assumed to be associated with increas-
ists almost never report an anxiety dream ing inhibition. Under conditions experi-
immediately before a jump, but do so mentally designed to increase inhibition,
some time after the jump. In investiga- e.g., drowsiness and certain drug states,
tions of parachutists' reactions to word- Pavlov was able to demonstrate a peak-
association (Epstein & Fenz, 1962) and ing back of the generalization gradient
TAT-like tests (Fenz & Epstein, 1962) in the same direction as that found for
containing built-in stimulus dimensions, our parachutists.
it was found that novice parachutists The shift in the peak of anxiety toward
on the day of a jump failed to perceive the remote end of the dimension, whether
anxiety-related words and explicitly a cue dimension or a time dimension, has
denied fear of parachuting in stories to important implications for both adaptive
pictures of parachuting. At the same and maladaptive behavior. On the
time, they showed an increase in anxiety adaptive side, it can serve to alert the
responses to pictures unrelated to para- person or animal in sufficient time for
chuting, suggesting displacement of in- defensive reactions to be brought effec-
hibited anxiety. tively into play. Secondly, the inhibi-
There is reason to believe that the tory reaction responsible for the inverted
backward shift in the peak of avoidance v serves to keep anxiety within relatively
with experience represents a fundamental low and nondisruptive overall levels. It
12 SEYMOUR EPSTEIN AND WALTER D. FENZ

is presumably by this mechanism that BOND, D. D. The love and fear of flying.
the experienced parachutist is able to New York: International Univer. Press,
keep from experiencing the intense anxi- 1952.
ety that the inexperienced parachutist EPSTEIN, S. The measurement of drive and
must cope with. An interesting negative conflict in humans: Theory and experiment.
derivation related to this last advantage In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium
on motivation: 1962. Lincoln: Univer.
is that individuals who have successfully Nebraska Press, 1962. Pp. 281-321.
coped with stress over a prolonged period, EPSTEIN, S., & FENZ, W. D. Theory and
if their controls ever were to break down, experiment on the measurement of ap-
should be susceptible to greater panic and proach-avoidance conflict. J. abnorm. soc.
disorganization than inexperienced in- Psychol., 1962, 64, 97-112.
dividuals. Such a derivation is actually FENZ, W. D., & EPSTEIN, S. Measurement of
supported by observations of wartime approach-avoidance conflict along a stim-
pilots (Bond, 1952). A further implica- ulus-dimension by a thematic apperception
tion of the displacement of the peak of test. /. Pers., 1962, 30, 613-632.
the inverted v is that while it should HULL, C. L. Principles of behavior. New
normally be adaptive in alerting the indi- York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1943.
vidual to an area of potential danger, LEWIN, K. Behavior and development as a
should the conflict become unconscious, function of the total situation. In L.
the displacement of the anxiety from its Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psy-
chology. New York: Wiley, 1946.
point of origin would result in what
MILLER, N. E. Liberalization of basic S-R
would appear to be meaningless anxiety, concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior,
and the greater the time since the motivation, and social learning. In S. Koch
conflict, the more remote would the (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science.
relationship become between the original Vol. 2. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.
conflict and the cues that elicit the PAVLOV, I. P. Conditioned reflexes. (Trans,
anxiety. Space does not permit a full by G. V. Anrep) London: Oxford Univer.
development of the implications of the Press, 1927.
inverted v-shaped curve, which will have PAVLOV, I. P. Lectures on conditioned reflexes.
to be reserved for a future theoretical (Trans, by W. H. Gantt) New York:
paper. International, 1928.
REFERENCES PAVLOV, I. P. Conditioned reflexes and
psychiatry. (Trans, by W. H. Gantt) New
BASOWITZ, H., PERSKY, H., KORCHIN, S. J., York: International, 1941.
& GRINKER, R. R. Anxiety and stress.
New York: McGraw-Hill, 1955. (Received April 17, 1964)

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen