Sie sind auf Seite 1von 27

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at:


https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232570396

Internal working models in


attachment relationships: Elaborating
a central construct in attachment
theory

Chapter January 2008

CITATIONS READS

185 2,906

2 authors, including:

Inge Bretherton
University of WisconsinMadison
113 PUBLICATIONS 7,303 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Mary Ainsworth's construct of maternal sensitivity as realted to infant- and child-


mother attachment View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Inge Bretherton on 23 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


From Handbook of Attachment, Second Edition: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications.
Edited by Jude Cassidy and Phillip R. Shaver.
Copyright 2008 by The Guilford Press. All rights reserved.

Chapter 5

Internal Working Models

.
in Attachment Relationships

s
es
Elaborating a Central Construct in Attachment Theory

Pr
Inge Bretherton

rd
Kristine A. Munholland

lfo
ui
G
e
Th

Bowlbys attachment theory (1969/1982, 1973, the course of development, infants sensorimotor
1980) accorded a central role in adaptive human affective internal working models become in-
09

development to supportive interpersonal relation- creasingly complex and mentally manipulable,


ships. From the cradle to the grave, he argued, an enabling not only simple short-term predictions
individuals mental health is intimately tied to but also reflection on current, past, and future re-
20

relationships with attachment figures who afford lationships by means of internal simulation (Bowl-
emotional support and physical protection: by, 1988). We deliberately emphasize the term
internal simulation to indicate that Bowlby

For not only young children, it is now clear, but employed it before it was adopted by philosophers
human beings of all ages are found to be at their hap-
(e.g., Goldman, 1991) and neuroscientists (e.g.,
piest and to be able to deploy their talents to best
ht

advantage when they are confident that, standing be- Adolphs, 2006; Gallese, 2005).
hind them, there are one or more trusted persons who Bowlbys proposals about the formation, de-
ig

will come to their aid should difficulties arise. The velopment, function, and intergenerational trans-
person trusted provides a secure base from which his mission of internal working models of self and
yr

(or her) companion can operate. (1973, p.359) attachment figures are scattered across the three
volumes of his seminal trilogy Attachment and Loss
op

How well attachment relationships can ful- (1969/1982, 1973, 1980) and his book A Secure
fill these safe-haven and secure-base functions, Base (1988). To make these ideas more accessible
C

however, turns not only on attachment partners as a whole, we summarize them in the first section
actual behaviors, but on the translation of their of this chapter. In the second section, we discuss
interaction patterns into relationship represen- possible elaborations and extensions of the work-
tationsor, as Bowlby termed them, internal ing-model construct by drawing on the literature
working models. Internal working models of self from neuroscience and memory development. In
and other in attachment relationships, Bowlby the third section, we turn to studies conducted
claimed, help members of an attachment dyad by attachment researchers. Specifically, we focus
(parent and child, or adult couple) to anticipate, on the extent to which findings obtained with
interpret, and guide interactions with partners. In representational measures of attachment suitable
102
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 103

for adults and children support, extend, and raise models to mentally run off alternative courses of
new questions about Bowlbys conceptualization of action and evaluate their likely outcome. Unfor-
working models. We conclude with suggestions for tunately, Craiks early death cut short any further
future research. work on this topic.
Conceding that the notion of internal
working models might seem fanciful to research-
Bowlby and the Psychoanalytic ers steeped in extreme behaviorism, Bowlby
InnerWorld (1969/1982) contended: The notion that brains

.
do in fact provide more or less elaborate models

s
That Bowlby emphasized the function of represen- that can be made to conduct, as it were, small

es
tation in the conduct of interpersonal relation- scale experiments in the head, is one that appeals
ships is not surprising. As a member of the Brit- to anyone concerned to understand the complex-
ish Psycho-Analytic Society, he was familiar with ity of behavior, and especially human behavior

Pr
Freuds (1940/1963) definition of the inner world (pp. 8081). Bowlby rejected related terms such
in An Outline of Psychoanalysis, which uncannily as cognitive map, because the word conjures
prefigured his own notion about the simulation up merely a static representation of topography

rd
and guidance function of internal working mod- (p. 80). The term internal working model, in
els: contrast, implies a representational system that al-

lfo
lows us, for example, to imagine interactions and
The yield brought to light by scientific work from our conversations with others, based on our previous

ui
primary sense perceptions will consist of an insight
experiences with them:
into connections and dependent relations which are G
present in the external world, which can somehow
be reliably reproduced or reflected in the internal world Every situation we meet with in life is construed in
of our thought and a knowledge of which enables us to terms of the representational models we have of the
e
understand something in the external world, to foresee it world about us and of ourselves. Information reach-
and possibly to alter it. (p.85; emphasis added) ing us through our sense organs is selected and in-
Th

terpreted in terms of those models, its significance


for us and those we care for is evaluated in terms of
When Freuds comments about representation
them, and plans of action executed with those mod-
were posthumously published (he died in 1939), els in mind. On how we interpret and evaluate each
09

most mainstream academic psychologists sub- situation, moreover, turns also how we feel. (Bowlby,
scribed to behaviorism and considered the study of 1980, p.229)
mental processes to be unscientific. Thirty years
20

later, when Bowlby undertook the task of rework- This quotation shows that Bowlby regarded
ing Freudian theory in light of new evidence (e.g., internal working models as a general construct,
1969/1982, 1973), academic psychology was still not one limited to attachment. It was with respect

largely dominated by behaviorist precepts. To gar- to relationship representations (of self and other
ner helpful theoretical ideas and empirical findings in attachment relationships), however, that he
ht

about the role of representation in attachment re- most extensively discussed mental model building,
lationships, Bowlby therefore cast a wider, inter- model use, and model revision, and it is on this
ig

disciplinary net. The result was not a fully worked- aspect that we focus in this chapter.
out theory, but a promising conceptual framework
yr

to be filled in by others. Starting, we may suppose, towards the end of his first
Bowlbys (1969/1982) conception of repre- year, and probably especially actively during his sec-
op

sentation as mental model building was inspired by ond and third when he acquires the powerful and ex-
the writings of an eminent biologist (Young, 1964) traordinary gift of language, a child is busy construct-
C

who had borrowed the idea from a slim volume on ing working models of how the physical world may
The Nature of Understanding written by Kenneth be expected to behave, how his mother and other
Craik (1943). Craik was a young, philosophically significant persons may be expected to behave, how
he himself may be expected to behave, and how each
trained psychologist and brilliant pioneer in what
interacts with the other. Within the framework of
would later be called artificial intelligence. Tak- these working models he evaluates his situation and
ing an evolutionary approach, he proposed that makes his plans. And within the framework of the
organisms that could generate internal working working models of his mother and himself he evalu-
models of the environment would considerably ates special aspects of his situation and makes his at-
improve their chances of survival by using these tachment plans. (1969/1982, p.354)
104 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

In our review of Bowlbys notions about in- with father and mother, Bowlby speculated that
ternal working models, we highlight several issues during the early years, the model of self interact-
that have caused misunderstandings in the lit- ing with mother may be more influential because
erature: (1) whether attachment working models the mother is likely to be the childs principal care-
are to be understood as relationship-specific rep- giver (p.129).
resentations or as general strategies of relating;
(2) how to understand the stability and change
Continuity of Security in the Face
of working models; (3) to what degree and under
ofDevelopmental Change

.
which circumstances internal working models are

s
consciously accessible and subject to defensive In the attachment literature, the label secure is

es
processes; and (4) how to conceptualize the pro- used regardless of age to describe an individuals
cesses in the intergenerational transmission of at- trust that a protective, supportive figure will be
tachment working models. emotionally available and responsive in case of

Pr
need. This general definition obscures develop-
mental changes that take place in how individuals
Relationship Specificity
experience and understand security.

rd
Bowlby (1969/1982, 1988, p.129) postulated that Bowlby (1980, 1988) repeatedly stressed that
a childs attachment working models are based on a continuously secure attachment relationship

lfo
real-life experiences of day-to-day interactions requires an infants embryonic working models
with his parents, and are therefore relationship- of self and attachment figure(s) to be updated in

ui
specific. Moreover, because they are constructed step with communicative, social, and cognitive
in interpersonal relationships, models of self and competencies that develop in childhood and ado-
G
attachment figure(s) are perforce mutually con- lescence. The same holds for a parents working
firming (e.g., parent as loving/protective and self models of the child and of the self as attachment
e
as loved/secure): figure (see George & Solomon, Chapter 35, this
volume).
Th

In the working model of the world that anyone builds Bowlby drew particular attention to young
a key feature is his notion of who his attachment childrens developing understanding that their
figures are, where they may be found, and how they attachment figures have separate (non-child-
09

may be expected to respond. Similarly, in the working focused) goals. This cognitive advance permits
model of the self that anyone builds a key feature is secure attachment relationships to become goal-
his notion of how acceptable or unacceptable he him-
corrected partnerships (1969/1982, p. 355) in
20

self is in the eyes of his attachment figures. On the


structure of these complementary models are based which there is give-and-take on both sides and in
that persons forecasts of how accessible and respon- which inevitable conflicts can be resolved through
sive his attachment figures are likely to be should he reciprocal adjustment of goals (hence the term

turn to them for support. In terms of the theory now goal correction; see Marvin & Britner, Chapter
advanced, it is on the structure of those models that 12, this volume). Pointing to mothers role in this
ht

depends, also, whether he feels confident that his at- development, Bowlby cited a study by Light (1979)
tachment figures are in general readily available or showing that mothers who referred to feelings and
ig

whether he is more or less afraid that they will not intentions while discussing the motherchild re-
be availableoccasionally, frequently or most of the lationship had children with better perspective-
yr

time. (1973, p.203)


taking skills (Solomon & George, Chapter 18, this
volume). Bowlby also speculated that 2-year-
op

In short, the complementary models of self and


olds abilities to talk meaningfully of their own and
parents represent both sides of the relationship.
others mental states, reported by Bretherton and
Bowlby also advanced the idea that internal
C

Beeghly (1982), might be more advanced in chil-


working models of self with specific attachment
dren whose mothers treat them sensitively (2nd
figures in infancy and early childhood increasing-
edition of Attachment, 1982, p.370).
ly become a property of the child himself (1988,
Although working models can and must be
p.127)a process that would be more complex for
updated as children develop, Bowlby (1969/1982)
a child with two very different relationship-specific
mentioned several processes that ensure their rela-
attachment working models. Acknowledging that
tive stability. First, habitual interaction patterns
little is known about the relative influence on per-
bias perceptionsan idea borrowed from Piagets
sonality development of the childs relationship
(1952) concept of assimilation. Thus a childs
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 105

confidence in an attachment figures emotional We stress, however, that Bowlby (1980) regarded
availability is not likely to be shattered by occa- his proposals in this area as particularly tentative,
sional lapses in a caregivers sensitivity. Second, conceding that there is clearly a long way to go
two individuals working models (and hence ex- before the theory sketched is within sight of doing
pectations) are involved in a relationship; hence justice to the wide range of defensive phenomena
when one partner tries out new behaviors, the other met with clinically (p.44).
may resist, and attempt to return the relationship Using as his primary sources Dixon (1971),
to the old pattern. Third, frequently repeated in- Erdelyi (1974), and Norman (1976), Bowlby

.
teraction patterns have a tendency to become in- (1980, p.65) explained repression as the system-

s
creasingly automatized, making lesser demands atic exclusion from further processing of certain

es
on attention, but hindering the conscious revision information of significance to the individual for
of working models. long periods or permanently. To justify this view
This being said, in secure relationships these he cites extensive evidence that incoming infor-

Pr
normal stabilizing processes give way to revisions mation is always subjected to many stages of un-
of the working models as a child (or an adult) conscious analysis and synthesis before becoming
realizes that the current models no longer yield conscious. At each stage, information most rel-

rd
adequate predictions. In Bowlbys (1988) own evant to current goals is selectively retained and
words, As a securely attached child grows older sharpened, whereas less salient information is dis-

lfo
and his parents treat him differently, a gradual up- carded. What distinguishes defensive from other
dating of models occurs. This means that, though types of selective exclusion is the goal: to prevent

ui
there is always a time-lag, his currently operative an individual from becoming aware of events or
models continue to be reasonably good simula- thoughts that would be unbearable if they were
G
tions of himself and his parents in interaction accepted as true. Whereas defensive exclusion
(p.130). may be temporarily adaptive, however, it is likely
e
Aside from developmental revisions of inter- to become maladaptive if maintained in the long
nal working models in attachment relationships, run or when circumstances change.
Th

Bowlby (1973) also considered what we here call To demonstrate the effect of defensive ex-
their affective discontinuity. Relying on evi- clusion on attachment working models, Bowlby
dence from clinical case studies of children with (1973, 1980) reanalyzed published case studies of
09

severe emotional problems, Bowlby (1973, 1980, children and adults with severe emotional prob-
1988) argued that defensive changes in a childs lems, but contended that the proposed processes
working models are likely when parental behav- also apply in less troubled relationships. Two situ-
20

iors, such as threats of abandonment, undercut ations struck Bowlby as particularly likely instiga-
trust in the parent as an attachment figure. Con- tors of defensive exclusion in young children: (1)
versely, when a familys stressful circumstances intense arousal of attachment behavior that is per-

improve, or effective support by others becomes sistently rejected, ignored, ridiculed, or punished
available, a previously rejecting or neglectful par- by a parent; and (2) knowing something about the
ht

ent may become able to respond more sensitively parent that the parent does not wish the child to
to his or her childs attachment needs. However, know about and would punish him for accepting
ig

once defensive aspects of working-model organi- as true (1980, p.73). Examples of the latter are
zation are established in an insecure attachment sexual abuse by a parent who denies that such be-
yr

relationship, such positive reconstructions can havior occurred or witnessing a fathers suicide but
become quite difficult. As we discuss in the next being told by the mother that the death was due
op

subsection, representational processes take a con- to a heart attack. A common response by children
siderably less straightforward course when previ- faced with such representational conflict is to de-
C

ously secure relationships become insecure. velop two conflicting sets of working models of self
with parent. One set, based on the childs adverse
experience with the parent, is defensively exclud-
Consciousness and Working Model Organization
ed, whereas the other set, reflecting what the par-
inLight of Defensive Processes
ent wants the child to believe, remains consciously
The advent of psychologys cognitive revolution accessible.
provided Bowlby with new tools for developing an Bowlby provided two reasons why children
alternative theoretical approach to Freuds ideas commonly accept the parental version of what
about the dynamic unconscious and repression. happened. First, it may be more frightening for a
106 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

child to see his or her attachment figure as non- because one of two incompatible sets of working
caring than to view the self as bad. Second, a models (self with parent) is defensively excluded.
parent may have strongly forbidden the child to The notion of segregated principal systems, in
question the meaning of what occurred and may contrast, involves multiple dissociated selves each
have threatened severe punishment if the child with access to a different organized working model.
divulges the parents behavior to others. In this The distinction seems important, but Bowlby did
context, Bowlby (1973) pointed out that many not pursue it in subsequent writings. While revis-
emotionally troubled adults in therapy still seem to iting his earlier ideas in A Secure Base (1988), he

.
harbor defensively excluded working models that linked defensive exclusion to a spectrum of re-

s
were developed in the early years on fairly simple lated syndromes within their commoner and less

es
lines (p.205) and that conflict with much more severe forms tend to be diagnosed as narcissism or
sophisticated, radically different conscious mod- false self and in their more severe forms may be
els dating from a later period. Here he appeared labeled a fugue, a psychosis, or a case of multiple

Pr
to propose that incompatible working models can personality (p. 113). Likewise, he uses defen-
develop sequentially as well as concurrently, and sive exclusion as an explanation for personality-
that the defensively excluded model, even though splitting (p.114), but segregated principal systems

rd
it has not been updated, reveals itself in an indi- are no longer mentioned.
viduals adult behavior. Finally, pertaining both to defensively ex-

lfo
Alongside defensive exclusion, Bowlby cluded working models and working models se-
(1980, citing research on hypnosis by Hilgard, questered in segregated selves, Bowlby proposed

ui
1974) proposed a second process, termed segre- that contradictory working models that are derived
gation of (principal) systems, that seems more from different sources (own experience or parental
G
akin to dissociation than repression. Principal input) may also be differently encoded. Drawing
system denotes a self capable of self-perception on Tulvings (1972) distinction between epi-
e
and agency. Segregated multiple selves are said to sodic (autobiographical) and semantic memory
be cognitively walled off from each other, each (general knowledge base), he suggested that chil-
Th

self having its own sectionalized memory store drens conscious representations of what parents
(or working models). These selves can alternate in or others misleadingly told them may be stored as
consciousness, but generally only one self is domi- general propositions in the semantic memory sys-
09

nant at any one time whereas the other (includ- tem while the childs own (defensively excluded
ing its working models) is in a state of complete or segregated) memories of traumatic attachment
or partial deactivation (see pp.5960, 345349). experiences might be stored analogically in the
20

Bowlby defined a deactivated principal system as episodic memory system. This, Bowlby noted,
an organized one and no less self-consistent than might explain why patients often give starkly con-
is the system with free access to action and con- flicting accounts of their parents during therapy

sciousness (p.347). He contended that when it sessions. Some provide general descriptions of par-
takes control of behavior the segregated system is ents as highly admirable, but then supply detailed
ht

capable of framing plans ... albeit in clumsy and contradictory anecdotes of how the parents had
ineffective ways (p.348). As an example, Bowlby actually behaved or what they had actually said.
ig

presented the case of an adolescent who, most of Others make uniformly adverse generalizations
the time, was fully aware that her mother had died about parents, but their detailed memories portray
yr

when she was 3 years old but who had no conscious a more favorable image (1980, p.62).
memory of grieving. Yet occasionally this girl en- Whereas Bowlby frequently attributed a
op

tered fugue states during which she disappeared childs construction of conscious, but highly dis-
from her home in what appeared to be a search torted attachment working models to misleading
C

for her mother. Bowlby also discussed a patient parental input, his discussion of the consequences
with two conflicting selves that were consciously of persistent defensive exclusion is also pertinent
accessible at the same time. Rather than being de- to this topic. The consequences he had in mind
activated, one of the selves was merely kept secret include most of the phenomena described as de-
from other people. He likened this condition to fenses in the psychoanalytic literature, but Bowlby
Freuds concept of split in the ego. (1980, pp. 6469) preferred to group them into
The notion of defensive exclusion involves two broad categories: (1) deactivation of behav-
one self that is protected from conscious conflict ioral systems and (2) cognitive disconnection.
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 107

In regard to the first category, Bowlby stressed The more details one comes to know about the events
that behavioral systems, when deprived of their re- in a childs life, and about what he has been told,
quired input (be the source external or internal), will what he has overheard and what he has observed
become partially or completely deactivated. If the but is not supposed to know, the more clearly can his
ideas about the world and what may happen in the
deactivated systems are those controlling attach-
future be seen as perfectly reasonable constructions.
ment, attachment-related behaviors, thoughts, and (1979, p.23)
feelings will cease to occur or to be experienced
(1980, p. 66), resulting in emotional detachment.
Communication and

.
Should fragments of relevant information seep

s
through, system deactivation will be only partial, IntergenerationalTransmission

es
and fragments of attachment behavior or affect may Bowlby (1973, 1988) envisioned two processes
become conscious in the form of moods, memories, through which internal working models of secure
or dreams. The activities, feelings, and thoughts

Pr
and insecure attachment relations may be com-
normally governed by deactivated system(s) will municated from parent to child: (1) the quality of
be replaced by other activities that tend to absorb interaction, and (2) open discussion of emotion and
an undue portion of a persons attention. Bowlby

rd
relationships. Like other psychoanalysts (e.g., Stern,
contended that there is virtually no activity that 1985), Bowlby focused extensively on the role of de-
cannot be used for this diversionary purpose.

lfo
liberate parental miscommunications in disorganizing
In regard to the second set of consequences, or confusing childrens internal working models, but
Bowlby proposed that systematic and persistent de- he also acknowledged that parents perform a positive

ui
fensive exclusion may result in three distinct ways role in helping a child construct and revise working
of cognitive disconnection between an individu- models through emotionally open dialogue:
G
als affective and behavioral responses and the oth-
erwise anxiety-provoking interpersonal situations Thus the family experience of those who grow up
e
that caused them. When this occurs, the discon- anxious and fearful is found to be characterized not
nected feelings or behaviors are likely to receive a only by uncertainty about parental support but often
Th

less threatening but erroneous explanation. First, also by covert yet strongly distorting parental pres-
individuals may misidentify the situation that is sures: pressure on the child, for example, to act as
caregiver for a parent; or to adopt, and thereby to con-
responsible for their negative affect. For example,
09

firm, a parents false modelsof self, of child and of their


a child may unconsciously fear that his mother relationship. Similarly the family experience of those
might abandon him if he does not stay home with who grow up to become relatively stable and self-
her, but consciously he may refuse to attend school
20

reliant is characterized not only by unfailing paren-


because he is afraid of his teachers criticism. Sec- tal support when called upon but also by a steady yet
ond, individuals may direct their negative feelings timely encouragement toward increasing autonomy,
away from the person who aroused them, and in- and by the frank communication by parents of working

stead direct them toward an irrelevant person or modelsof themselves, of child and of othersthat are
themselves. For example, an adult or child may not only tolerably valid but are open to be questioned and
ht

defensively exclude his or her own unassuaged revised. (1973, pp.322323; emphasis added)
attachment needs, and instead direct compulsive
ig

caregiving behaviors toward a rejecting or helpless Bowlby went on to explain that the experience of
attachment figure. Third, an individual may react open communication in the family of origin may
yr

to a painful interpersonal situation by turning away foster parents ability to engage in emotionally
from it and instead become morbidly preoccupied open communication with their own children:
op

with psychological and physiological aspects of his


Because in all these respects children tend unwitting-
or her own suffering. ly to identify with parents and therefore adopt, when
C

Bowlby rejected psychoanalytic labels for they become parents, the same patterns of behavior
defense mechanisms (e.g., projection, projective towards their children that they themselves have ex-
identification, denial, or displacement), because perienced during their own childhood, patterns of in-
he feared that their use might prevent therapists teraction are transmitted, more or less faithfully, from
from adequately exploring patients real-life expe- one generation to another. (1973, p.323)
riences. For the same reason, he urged great cau-
tion when using terms like fantasy and magical These statements are noteworthy for two reasons:
thinking: (1) They lay out the processes whereby attach-
108 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

ment working models may be transmitted from continuation of open communication. Bowlby
parents to children through behavioral and emo- also argued that frank discussion of emotions and
tional interactions; and (2) they stress that chil- other mental states encourages the development
dren are most likely to develop adaptive, revisable of social cognition. In insecure relationships, he
attachment working models when parents encour- noted, clear communication becomes more diffi-
age exploration of the inner world by modeling cult as partners conscious and unconscious work-
emotionally open (frank) verbal communication ing models become less consistent with each other
about relationships. because of defensive processes.

.
Throughout his writings, Bowlby drew a sharp Many of Bowlbys intuitions about working

s
distinction between attachment working models as models have proven to be remarkably prophetic.

es
they develop in secure and insecure relationships, Since his last publication on attachment in 1988,
most likely out of a desire for clearer exposition of numerous studies of neural processes and memory
unfamiliar ideas. It is important to stress, however, development have provided findings that help to

Pr
that he saw these contrasting descriptions not as a flesh out and elaborate his proposals about the
dichotomy, but as two ends of a continuum: working-model construct. We review a selection
of these contributions in the next section.

rd
Between the groups of people with extremes of either
good or bad experience lie groups of people with an

lfo
almost infinite range of intermediate sorts of experi- Selected Perspectives from the Fields
ence. ... For example, some may have learnt that an
attachment figure responds in a comforting way only
ofNeuroscience and Memory Development

ui
when coaxed to do so. They grow up expecting that Internal Working Models and Meaning Making
all such figures have to be coaxed. Others may have
learnt during childhood that the wished for response
G
As our quotations from Bowlbys work reveal, he
can be obtained only if certain rules are kept. Provid- did not intend the concept of working models to
e
ed the rules and the sanctions are mild and predict- be construed in terms of dispassionate mappings
able, a person can still come to believe that support of an objective reality. Rather, he regarded emo-
Th

will always be available. (1973, pp.208209) tional appraisals and goal setting as integral aspects
of representationa viewpoint that has much in
We encourage readers to keep this point in mind common with the work of social psychologists
09

throughout the remainder of this chapter. Lewin (1933) and Heider (1958), with whose the-
ories Bowlby was apparently unfamiliar.
Influenced by Gestalt psychology and with
20

Summary
little concern for the hegemony of behaviorism,
In formulating propositions about the function Lewin and Heider wrote extensively about repre-
of working models in attachment relationships, sentation in terms of personal and interpersonal

Bowlby systematically sought validation and sup- meaning making, proposing ideas that are useful
port from outside the psychoanalytic domain. Be- in thinking about working models. According to
ht

fore the cognitive revolution took hold in psy- Lewin (1933), humans construe their psychologi-
chology, he adopted Craiks (1943) enormously cal environment or life space in terms of the ac-
ig

powerful concept of representation as model build- tions it is seen to invite, repel, permit, or prohibit
ing and model use in the service of predicting and in the context of current goals and competencies.
yr

guiding interpersonal behavior. He then wedded Hence an infant who is likely to perceive a stair-
this idea to Piagets (1951, 1952) theory of sen- case as an impediment to reaching his or her dis-
op

sorimotor development, representation, and per- appearing mother may, a year or so later, perceive
spective taking. Based on information-processing the same staircase as a means of joining her. Re-
studies, he contrasted representational processes
C

lated ideas have appeared in James and Eleanor


in secure and insecure attachment relationships, Gibsons concept of affordances (see E. Gibson,
but acknowledged many gradations in between. 1982).
Viewing representation and interpersonal com- Heider (1958) extended Lewins (1933) con-
munication as mutually reinforcing, he proposed cept of psychological life space to the interpersonal
that emotionally open dialogue with responsive context, stressing that when we react to others, we
attachment figures facilitates an individuals con- do not usually perceive their actions as meaning-
struction of well-functioning, revisable internal less movement patterns that have to be laboriously
working models, which in their turn foster the interpreted. Rather, we understand others behav-
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 109

iors (and we construct working models) in terms These findings naturally raised the question
of how they make us feel, and what we believe our whether humans, too, possess mirror systems that
interaction partners are intending, thinking, per- enable the mental simulation of others actions,
ceiving and feeling. goals, and emotions. The discovery that newborns
Very much in accord with Lewins and are able to imitate mouth opening, tongue protru-
Heiders ideas, neuroimaging studies performed sion, lip pursing, finger movements, and even some
during the last decade have revealed a great deal facial expressions (e.g., Meltzoff & Moore, 1977)
about the brain as a meaning-making organ. These had suggested that humans may begin extrauter-

.
discoveries provide a new understanding of the ine life with a rudimentary mirror neuron system

s
human capacity to envision others as psychological already in place. Neuroimaging studies of human

es
beings who can evaluate and decide on a course of action, emotion, and other aspects of mentaliz-
actionall highly relevant to the working-model ing (thinking about mental states) confirmed and
construct. extended Galleses (2005) notion of embodied

Pr
simulation by using such techniques as positron
emission tomography (PET) and functional mag-
A Model in the Brain
netic resonance imaging (fMRI). Results to date

rd
Since Bowlby (1969/1982) incorporated Craiks are largely confined to adults, because brain imag-
term internal working model into attachment ing is not appropriate for children unless medically

lfo
theory, it was independently rediscovered by cog- indicated, but the findings have important devel-
nitive scientist Johnson-Laird (1983). Through opmental implications for the conceptualization

ui
his influence, it has been adopted by neuroscien- of working models that we discuss later.
tists (e.g., Adolphs, 2003; Gallese, 2005) whose G Specific sites in humans premotor cortex,
work is surprisingly consistent with Bowlbys no- corresponding to the correct location in the pre-
tion that the brain constructs working models of motor body map or homunculus, do indeed show
e
self, attachment figures, and the environment. We increased activity when study participants view
begin this review with macaque studies, but devote video clips of other humans performing mouth,
Th

most of it to insights relevant to working models hand, foot, and face movements (Buccino et al.,
that can be gleaned from neuroimaging of human 2001). Moreover, imagining or recalling an action
brains. (ones own or that of another person) induces acti-
09

vation in the same premotor sites as action execu-


tion and observation (Decety, Chaminade, Grzes,
Mirror Neuron Systems
& Meltzoff, 2002), suggesting that embodied
20

While recording from single cells in monkeys representations form the basis for simulating ones
premotor cortex, Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Gallese, and own and others behavior.
Fogassi (1996) serendipitously discovered mir- Findings for emotions are similar. The ante-

ror neurons. These neurons are triggered when rior insula, which receives input from all parts of
a monkey performs a goal-directed hand action the autonomic nervous system (Damasio, 2003),
ht

(e.g., picking up a peanut), but also when it merely responds in the same way when study participants
observes a conspecific or even a human perform a sniff a foul-smelling substance and when they ob-
ig

similar action. What matters, is whether the ob- serve video clips of human faces displaying disgust
served action is goal-directed, not whether a par- expressions (Wicker et al., 2003). The authors
yr

ticular hand shape or hand orientation is adopted. interpretation of the disgust findings is captured
Other neurons (termed canonical) fire when a in the pithy title of their article: Both of Us
op

monkey looks at an object that requires a particu- Disgusted in My Insula. Corresponding findings
lar hand shape for pickup, as well as when it actu- have been obtained for the psychological aspects
C

ally uses that same hand shape to grasp the object. of physical pain and the pain of social rejection
One might say that the monkeys brain creates an (Eisenberger & Lieberman, 2004).
embodied awareness of the objects pickuppable In addition, brain regions involved in the
affordance. Based on these and related findings, experience of personal and vicarious emotions
Gallese (2005) proposed that premotor mirror are recruited during more complex mentalizing
neurons and other neurons coupling perception functions. Ruby and Decety (2004), for example,
and action enable monkeys to understand their observed the same levels of amygdala activation
peers intentional (meaningful) actions in space whether participants imagined their own or their
through a process of embodied simulation. mothers feelings in a variety of embarrassing situ-
110 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

ations. However, and possibly in the service of course of mutually responsive social interactions.
distinguishing the two perspectives, participants Rather than having to explain how it is even pos-
right somatosensory cortex was engaged only when sible for humans to comprehend others feelings at
they considered their own feelings, whereas their an experiential level, the new question concerns
temporoparietal junction (TPJ) responded only the conditions that suppress the natural capacity
while they took their own mothers perspective. for empathy and mentalizing implied in studies of
Also relevant to attachment working mod- embodied simulation.
els, several somatotopically organized face- and The neuroimaging findings reviewed here are

.
body-responsive sites in the temporal and prefron- compatible with an emerging functional view of

s
tal cortices are recruited when individuals inter- memory as constructive and reconstructive rather

es
pret and imagine social interactions. The superior than reproductive, allowing individuals to reex-
temporal sulcus (STS) becomes activated during perience past and preexperience future episodes
the perception of biological motion (eye gaze di- (see review by Schacter & Addis, 2007). Schacter

Pr
rection; facial expressions; purposeful movement and Addis (2007) point out that because the fu-
of lips and mouth during speech, as well as hands, ture is rarely an exact repetition of the past, simu-
arms, and legs; see review by Haxby, Hoffman, & lation of future scenarios requires a system that can

rd
Gobbini, 2000). However, the same region is also draw on the past in a manner that flexibly extracts
recruited during more complex mentalizing func- and recombines elements of previous experiences.

lfo
tions, as when individuals imagine themselves as They cite evidence of considerable overlap be-
the protagonist in a story (Vogeley et al., 2001), tween the neural processes involved in recalling

ui
solve false-belief tasks and moral dilemmas (see the past and imagining the future, as well as in
review by Frith & Frith, 2003), or judge the inten- adopting others perspectives.
G
tionality of others actions in context (Saxe, Xiao,
Kovacs, Perrett, & Kanwisher, 2004). A comple-
Neuroimaging Studies and the Mental Operation
e
mentary function, individual face identification,
ofWorking Models
enabling differential emotional responses to at-
Th

tachment figures, is supported by the fusiform face A stated function of working models is to evaluate
area (FFA) in the inferior temporal cortex (e.g., the potential effectiveness of alternative courses
Kanwisher & Yovel, 2006). Patients with focused of action. Neuroimaging studies are beginning
09

lesions in the FFA cannot recognize the faces of to show that many sites in the prefrontal cortex
familiar individuals or their own reflections in a (PFC) are involved in aspects of this task. These
mirror (prosopagnosia; Damasio, 1999). PFC sites form an interconnected network, but
20

These and other neuroimaging studies suggest they also receive signals from and transmit signals
that human brains are built for intersubjectivity, to many other brain areas (particularly those re-
or the ability to understand other people and to viewed above). In collaboration with other corti-

imagine interactions with them through embodied cal and subcortical regions, they are involved in
(and experiential) simulation. Brain systems previ- the top-down regulation of emotional and so-
ht

ously believed to support self-related appraisals are cial responses, whereas automatic behaviors such
also recruited when people vicariously feel oth- as orienting to unexpected movement and other
ig

ers actions and emotionsnot only while observ- behaviors that have become automated through
ing or interacting with them, but while imagining practice are no longer subject to control by the
yr

interactions with them. That this capacity begins PFC (Miller & Cohen, 2001).
early is strongly suggested, not only by the evidence Demonstrating the enormous role of emo-
op

for neonatal imitation, but by Trevarthen and tions in so-called executive functions, the orbit-
Hubleys (1978) studies of primary and secondary al (pre)frontal cortex (OFC) guides the processing
C

intersubjectivity, including the emergence before of learned and unlearned sensory and psychologi-
the end of the first year of a capacity for shared at- cal rewards, including the reward value of specific
tention. Studies of intentional gestural communi- faces (Rolls, 1996). Relevant to attachment, Bar-
cation at about 9 months of age likewise appear to tels and Zeki (2004) found that face-responsive
indicate a rudimentary understanding than minds OFC neurons (along with other regions related to
can be interfaced (Bretherton & Bates, 1979). the brains reward circuitry) responded consider-
In good enough attachment relationships, we ably more strongly when mothers viewed the faces
argue, a childs experiential understanding of oth- of their own children as compared to faces of same-
ers actions and emotions develops naturally in the age acquainted children. At the same time, areas
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 111

associated with more analytic social judgments perhaps in the service of comparative appraisal
(e.g., the STS and amygdala) were suppressed. before engaging in impulsive action, the output of
Important for conceptualizing working-model the OFC reward network is transmitted to the pri-
change and adaptive decision making, the OFC mary motor cortex only indirectly via the DLPFC
plays a vital role in overriding habitual responses, (Rolls, 1996).
thereby facilitating adaptive new behaviors if a In short, neuroimaging studies shed new
particular stimulus or behavioral strategy ceases light on the proposed capacity to run off aspects
to be rewarding. Patients with circumscribed OFC of working models to generate and evaluate al-

.
damage who lack this capacity for flexible evalu- ternative plans of action. This capacity involves

s
ation (perhaps also the associated gut feelings) the simultaneous activation and collaboration of

es
tend to make poor decisions, whether in their day- many specialized brain sites, and is consistent with
to-day lives or an experimental economic game a view of memory that includes emotion as an in-
(Damasio, 1999). They make these maladaptive tegral and necessary aspect of meaning making,

Pr
decisions even though they can describe the adap- representation, and adaptive decisions. That many
tive choices that they could or should make in- of the cortical sites involved in guiding, planning,
stead. Infants with this kind of prefrontal damage and imagining individual behavior and social in-

rd
suffer even more severely. Not only do they fail to teractions are organized in terms of body maps
express (and seemingly fail to experience) social (i.e., somatotopically) is presumably not a coinci-

lfo
emotions of sympathy, embarrassment, or guilt; dence, and may facilitate the integration of related
they are also unable to develop an understanding information via a mysterious process neuroscien-

ui
of social rules and rule violations. In such cases, tists call binding. Not only are these neuroim-
the brain is not developmentally plastic enough aging findings consonant with Bowlbys thinking
G
to make up for the damage (Anderson, Bechara, about the function of working models in planning,
Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1999). and with the approaches to memory reviewed by
e
A second prefrontal region, the anterior Schacter and Addis (2007); they also accord well
cingulate cortex (ACC), is involved in signal- with Nelsons (1996) theoretical reformulation of
Th

ing information-processing conflicts (Botvinick, memory development, to which we turn next.


Carter, Braver, Barch, & Cohen, 2001). Its activa-
tion during top-down processing of mental conflict
Developmental Memory Research
09

was studied by Singer and colleagues (2006), who


demonstrated that distrust affects empathic reac- As mentioned, Bowlby proposed that the gener-
tions. Viewing a photograph of someone in a situ- alizations of mother, father, and self enshrined in
20

ation likely to cause pain (a finger pinched by a what I am terming working models or representa-
closing door) normally recruits both amygdala and tional models will be stored semantically (1980,
ACC regions, but these activations are substan- p. 62). However, Bretherton (1985) contended

tially diminished if the pictured person previously that studies of adults memory for scripts (Schank
behaved in a deceptive rather than a trustworthy & Abelson, 1977) and childrens event represen-
ht

manner toward the study participant. tations (e.g., Nelson & Gruendel, 1981) provide a
A third prefrontal area with functions rel- better fit with Bowlbys view that working models
ig

evant to working model operation is the dorsolat- of specific attachment figures allow an individual
eral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which has been to mentally simulate habitual interaction patterns
yr

identified as important for working memory. This with these figures, and thus to anticipate the likely
region contains neurons that can remain acti- course of ongoing and future interactions with
op

vated, or hold representations online for tens of them. Nelsons (1996, 2005) revised conceptual-
seconds after a stimulus has been removed (Fuster, ization of experiential event memory makes this
C

1997). A review by Miller and Cohen (2001) sug- fit even clearer.
gests that this region, as part of working memory For Nelson (1996), as for Bowlby (1969/1982,
function, is involved in the comparison, manip- 1980), the everyday function of representation is to
ulation, and integration of rules for goal or task guide actions in the present and anticipate future
achievement, rather than the recall of unique epi- events in light of what has generally happened in
sodes. In another review, Krawczyk (2002) argued the past. Based on infant research, Nelson posited
that the DLFPC is critical for making decisions that (nonverbal) event memory is the basic experi-
under conditions of uncertainty that require eval- ential memory system that develops first, and that
uation of multiple sources of information. Also, generic event representations (GERs) provide the
112 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

initial building blocks for a childs world model. 1990, 1991, 2005), especially in light of neurosci-
Events are defined as whole scenes unfolding over ence findings on embodied simulation. Second,
time that involve people and/or other animates whereas the event schemas proposed by Nelson
acting over time and in particular places (Nelson, may allow more than one person to fill the agent
2005, p.360). and recipient slots in an event representation,
Nelson (1996) considered event memory to a childs generic representation of, say, self with
be primary, in contrast to Schacter and Tulving father would be specific to the father and would
(1994), who subsumed event memory under the not allow other caregivers or other adults as sub-

.
semantic memory system. Nelson (p.154) argued stitutes. Third, an internal working model of self

s
that semantic memory (general knowledge, un- with a specific other would be unworkable if con-

es
dated and unlocalized) is constructed by repro- ceptualized as a mere collection of GERs. A better
cessing the constituents of generic event memory. approach would be to regard working models as
For example, children may group particular food an organized, multilayered, hierarchical network

Pr
items into one semantic category because the or web of GERs with different levels of general-
items fit the same slot in a generic event, such ity (Bretherton, 1985, 1991). In such a network,
as lunch at the day care center. Nelson also pro- habitual, experience-near, relationship-specific

rd
posed that GERs help to organize autobiographical scenarios would serve as inputs to higher-order
memories. Finally, she explained that GERs are not general event categories.

lfo
to be confused with procedural memory, at least Elaborating on these ideas in terms of attach-
as defined by Sherry and Schacter (1987). Proce- ment theory, we propose that an infants repeated

ui
dural memory is said to operate outside awareness, experience-near attachment scenario with his or
whereas event memory is a form of representation her father could consist of feeling and express-
G
that involves a degree of conscious awareness ing distress, being picked up and cuddled against
(Nelson, 1996, p.62). the fathers chest, and being soothed by comfort-
e
Although Nelson initially assumed that early ing sounds, followed by feelings of relaxation (see
(experiential, nonverbal) event memory is always Stern, 1985). Such a nonverbal, experiential GER
Th

GER- or script-like, she changed her mind after would be relationship-specific because of the par-
studies documented that very young children can ticular way in which the father has cuddled or
display memory for unique events through exter- comforted the child, and therefore might allow
09

nal reenactments weeks to months later (e.g., Bauer the infant to mentally simulate the fathers an-
& Wewerka, 1995), which Nelson (1996, follow- ticipated behavior in similar contexts. In addition,
ing Donald, 1991), called mimetic. However, as such a script, along with other father scenarios,
20

they approach the end of their second year and could form the basis of a more general but still
if their efforts are supported by caregivers, many relationship-specific (nonverbal) event category,
children already begin to translate experiential verbally expressed as When I feel bad/sad, Dad

event representations into language and gain helps me feel better. This in turn might be em-
a rudimentary ability to translate others verbal bedded in (or nested under) what H. S. Waters
ht

narratives back into their own meaningful event and Waters (2006) have called a secure-base
representations, oras Damasio (1999) termed script in relation to the father, verbally abbrevi-
ig

themnonverbal narratives. Fuller mastery of ated as When I need help, Dad is usually there for
these processes extends over the preschool years me. It would represent basic trust not only in the
yr

and beyond (Nelson, 1996). A fascinating account fathers ability to provide emotional support and
of a young childs solitary verbal rehearsal of daily protection, but also in his willingness and avail-
op

routines before falling asleep was published by ability to do so. As noted above, it is at this general
Nelson in 1989. level that Bowlby most often wrote about working
C

Nelsons (1996) theory is highly consonant models, but without access to underlying experi-
with the notion of attachment working models, ence-near GERs, such trust could not, in our view,
once a few assumptions are added. First, even come into being or be sustained. Trait adjectives
though childrens nonverbal and verbal event such as trustworthy or loving are but stand-ins
knowledge has most often been studied in relation for general event categories that are meaningful
to emotionally neutral scripts (e.g., lunch at the only because of underlying experience-near GERs
day care center), Nelsons definition of GERs can (see also E. Waters, Crowell, Elliott, Corcoran,
easily be extended to include affects and inten- & Treboux, 2002). The developmental process
tions of agents and recipients (e.g., Bretherton, whereby relationship-specific working models of
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 113

self with several attachment figures and of other of others emotions and intentions is fostered by
familial and extrafamilial relationships become in- parental responses conveying to the infant that
tegrated into a reasonably coherent general model he or she has been understood. Independently,
of the self is only beginning to become a focus of psychoanalysts have claimed that appropriate pa-
attachment research, although the social-cognitive rental mirroring of infants emotions enables the
literature provides guiding ideas (see Bretherton, development of a reflective/reflected understand-
1990; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2004; Thompson, ing that is likely the foundation for a secure sense
Laible, & Ontai, 2003). of self (e.g., Fonagy, Gergely, & Target, Chapter

.
Space limitations preclude a detailed discus- 33, this volume).

s
sion of experiential (verbal and nonverbal) event Also relevant is Meads work on the social or-

es
memory in relation to defensive processes (for igins of thought, conceptualized as inner conversa-
an attempt, see Bretherton, 1991), but we briefly tions with imagined others or the self. For young
alert readers to the usefulness of attribution theory children, Mead suggested, these inner conversa-

Pr
in this regard. Given that information in every- tions should be viewed as dramatic, involving
day life (as opposed to experimental situations) is mental reenactments of conversations between
usually incomplete and interpretable in multiple child and parent. Later, the inner stage changes

rd
ways, defensive manipulation of causal informa- into the forum and workshop of thought. The
tion could be highly effective in working-model features and intonations of the dramatis personae

lfo
change. A switch of causal attributions from ex- fade out and the emphasis falls upon inner speech,
ternal and transient (an individual behaves in a though thought can always return to the personal

ui
certain way because of situational factors) to inter- mode (Mead, 1913, p. 377). Applied to attach-
nal and stable (the individuals behavior is caused ment, these ideas suggest that young childrens
G
by his or her personality) is likely to have drastic thoughts may in some instances consist of hold-
relationship consequences when it concerns an at- ing inner conversations with parents. For example,
e
tachment figures unreliable behavior (see Collins, to allay fears, they may repeat to themselves re-
1996, for an example based on a study of young membered parental reassurances that the monster
Th

adults). under the bed is only a shadow. However, outer


reassuring conversations must precede the ability
to conduct them internally.
ParentChild Memory Talk
09

Despite Bowlbys (1973) proposals about


Bowlbys (1973) proposition that working models the importance of parentchild dialogue in the
of self develop as a result of interactions and dia- co-construction of attachment working models,
20

logues with attachment figures was shared by psy- Meads (1934) and Vygotskys (1978) theories
choanalysts in the object relations tradition, but about the social self were what inspired the pio-
it also parallels Meads (1934) and other symbolic neering research. Studies of conversations about

interactionists notions about the social self. Ac- past and future events by parents and their young
cording to Mead, children learn the meaning of children (e.g., Fivush & Fromhoff, 1988) revealed
ht

their gestures through the responses others make that some mothers (termed elaborative) wel-
to them, but Bowlbys claims are somewhat differ- comed their childrens additions to the dialogue,
ig

ent. Because evolutionary processes are believed even if minimal, and expanded on their childrens
to have prepared infants to expect appropriate and contributions through affirmations and questions.
yr

caring parental responses to attachment signals, Other mothers, with a low-elaborative or re-
parental ignoring or deliberate misinterpretation petitive style, tended to brush aside what their
op

of emotional signals would not render an infants children had to say if it seemed irrelevant to the
communication meaningless, but rather would conversational topic and to reiterate the same
C

convey rejection. If pervasive and consistent, such question without waiting for an answer. Overall,
rejection is likely to lead to an internal working they seemed more interested in obtaining what
model of self stated verbally as My needs [or I my- they regarded as the correct answer than in co-
self] dont count (Bretherton, 1990). Seen in this constructing a collaborative account.
way, meanings derived from attachment interac- These descriptions reminded Bretherton
tions hold tremendous emotional significance for (1993) strongly of Ainsworth, Bell, and Staytons
the childs developing model of self. We surmise (1974) definitions of the sensitive and insensitive
that the adaptive development of neural mirror maternal behaviors during nonverbal interac-
systems that underlie experiential understanding tions at home that predict infantmother attach-
114 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

ment security. Correlations between assessments opment and validation of representational attach-
of childmother attachment and maternal remi- ment measures.
niscing style have indeed emerged (e.g., Fivush
& Vasudeva, 2002; Laible & Thompson, 2000).
Particularly revealing were findings from a longi- Attachment Research Relevant
tudinal study of motherchild memory talk at 19, toWorkingModels
25, 32, 40, and 51 months (Newcombe & Reese,
2004). Attachment security at 19 months was as- Until the mid-1980s, attachment research-

.
sessed by mothers with the E. Waters (1995) At- ers focused almost exclusively on the quality of

s
tachment Q-Set (AQS), and AQS scores were infantparent attachment patterns in the Strange

es
dichotomized to form two groups of children. Situation (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall,
Mothers of the secure group used more evaluative 1978)either to predict developmental outcomes,
language (internal-state words, intensifiers, affect or to examine precursors of secure and insecure

Pr
modifiers, and emphasis) at each successive age, attachment classifications. Bowlbys (1969/1982,
whereas the opposite held for mothers of the in- 1973, 1980) notions about attachment working
secure group. Furthermore, during all five sessions, models were largely ignored. This changed after

rd
children in the secure group used more evaluative Main, Kaplan, and Cassidy (1985) introduced sev-
language than their insecure peers, and maternal eral instruments for evaluating attachment at the

lfo
and child evaluative language scores in secure (but level of representation.
not insecure) dyads became correlated, beginning Most influential among these was the Adult

ui
at 25 months. In a related study of short-term rem- Attachment Interview (AAI; George, Kaplan, &
iniscing, Etzion-Carasso and Oppenheim (2000) Main, 1984, 1996), an hour-long, semistructured
G
found that mothers of boys who had been secure interview about adults childhood attachment ex-
as infants engaged in open and responsive com- periences and their impact on personality develop-
e
munication after a brief separation, and that their ment (see Hesse, Chapter 25, this volume). Also
children reciprocated with free expression of their important was Kaplans (see Main et al., 1985)
Th

needs and concerns. revised version of Klagsbrun and Bowlbys (1976)


In summary, Nelsons (1996) theory of experi- Separation Anxiety Test (SAT), a semiprojective
ential and verbal event representation not only fits instrument for eliciting attachment narratives
09

well with the neuroimaging studies of embodied from young children.


simulation, but is also highly consonant with the That this work had an almost immediate im-
notion that attachment working models are based pact on developmental and clinical attachment
20

on experiential memories of repeated interpersonal research is attributable to two findings. First, rep-
experiences in the context of specific attachment resentational features of parents AAIs seemed to
relationships. With a few additional assumptions, parallel their infants behavioral attachment pat-

Nelsons theory allows us to conceptualize how a terns in the Strange Situation, suggesting inter-
childs working models of specific relationships generational transmission. Second, Strange Situ-
ht

might also come to serve as input for more general ation classifications in infancy predicted mental
attachment representations and even worldviews. aspects of security assessed with the SAT at age
ig

Studies of memory talk additionally indicate that 6, revealing developmental continuity in attach-
children in more secure dyads tend to adopt their ment patterns from the behavioral-experiential to
yr

mothers mentalizing reminiscing stylenot only the verbal-representational level.


affecting how the children remember specific ex- Several replication studies and the creation of
op

periences, but helping them to incorporate an un- additional instruments by other researchers quick-
derstanding of mental states into their conscious, ly followed, including at least four AAI-like inter-
C

verbally articulable working models of self with views about the current parentchild attachment
mother and others. relationship, adaptations of the AAI to younger
The findings on embodied simulation, expe- ages, and several projective attachment measures
riential event memory, and mentalizing parent for children and adults (see the review by Solomon
child conversations discussed in this section are & George, Chapter 18, this volume). During this
beginning to make limited inroads into repre- period, a methodological tradition evolved within
sentational attachment research, but as the next the field whereby new representational attach-
section demonstrates, their influence is still quite ment measures were recognized only if they were
small. The major emphasis has been on the devel- successfully validated against classifications or
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 115

scales used to code the Strange Situation and the several possibly quite different attachment rela-
AAI. Although enormously fruitful in many ways, tionships and topics. The additional unresolved/
this emphasis on refining assessment and coding disorganized AAI designation applies only to por-
methods had the somewhat unfortunate side effect tions of the interview that concern experiences of
that theoretical links with Bowlbys notions about loss and abuse (see Hesse, Chapter 25, this vol-
attachment working models were often overlooked ume), whereas the remainder of the transcript can
or not clearly drawn. Our goal in this section is to generally be assigned to one of the three organized
make up for this neglect by examining research classifications. Elaborating on previous work (e.g.,

.
conducted with the AAI in light of relationship- Bretherton, 1985, 2005; Bretherton & Munhol-

s
specific and generalized attachment working land, 1999), we argue for a complementary inter-

es
models, their intergenerational transmission, and pretation of AAI classifications in terms of gen-
longitudinal consistency. Next, we consider fur- eral and relationship-specific attachment working
ther insights offered by the SAT and a selection models as affected by defensive processes (Bowlby,

Pr
of other semiprojective attachment measures for 1980).
children and adults. With apologies to colleagues, The hallmarks of secure-autonomous AAI
our review must often refer to other chapters for transcripts are coherence, emotional openness,

rd
specific citations and related findings. and valuing of attachment relationships, but co-
Not included, although important to the herence is not restricted to positive depictions

lfo
conceptualization of attachment working models, of an interviewees childhood attachments. That
are findings based on paper-and-pencil self-report some secure-autonomous adults described very

ui
measures of attachment styles pioneered by Hazan difficult relationships with their own parents led
and Shaver (1987) and used in hundreds of studies GBowlby (1988, p. 135) to suggest that they had
conducted primarily by personality and social psy- achieved a freely accessible and coherent organi-
chologists. As elucidated by Crowell, Fraley, and zation of attachment-relevant information (work-
e
Shaver (Chapter 26, this volume), these measures ing models) by reprocessing, and thus coming to
seem to tap somewhat different constructs than terms with, their unhappy childhood memories.
Th

the narrative assessments created by developmen- The term earned-secure, coined for these par-
tal psychologists do. For a detailed consideration ents, was based on the unstated assumption that
of the large literature on attachment styles and AAI coherence reflects the reworking of formerly
09

their correlates, see J. Feeney (Chapter 21, this incoherent attachment working models.
volume) and Mikulincer and Shaver (Chapter 23, Questions about this interpretation arose
this volume). from findings of the Minnesota Study of Risk and
20

Adaptation from Birth to Adulthood. In that


study, about half of the young adult participants
The AAI in Working-Model Perspective
with AAIs considered earned-secure were found,

Although the AAI focuses on an interviewees on consultation of earlier records, to have received
family-of-origin attachment experiences, the AAI supportive caregiving as toddlers and children
ht

classification procedures developed by Main and (Roisman, Padrn, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2002). The
Goldwyn (1984; Main, Goldwyn, & Hesse, 2003) ensuing debate about a more stringent definition
ig

do not purport to assess working models of an inter- and meaning of earned security has not yet been
viewees past or ongoing attachment relationships. resolved. Whatever its outcome, parents AAI co-
yr

Main and colleagues (1985) initially attempted herence remains the best predictor of their infants
an explanation of these classifications in terms attachment security. AAI coherence is also strong-
op

of working models, but Main (1995, 1999) later ly correlated with the security of young adults cur-
explicitly distanced herself from this approach. In- rent relationships with close friends or romantic
C

stead, she proposed the term state of mind with partners, whether assessed observationally or with
respect to attachment to denote an individuals AAI-like interviews (e.g., Grossmann, Gross-
habitual or organized manner of attention regu- mann, & Kindler, 2005). AAI evaluations even
lation when confronted with attachment cues, forecast an individuals cooperative behavior in a
whether in the course of interviews, interpersonal laboratory task with strangers (Roisman, 2006).
interactions, or intrapersonal reflection. Thus Given consistent evidence that secure-
AAI classifications represent a single predominant autonomous AAI classifications predict an adults
state of mind (secure-autonomous, dismissing, supportive and quite stable behavior within at-
or preoccupied) that pervades the discussion of tachment relationships, as well as the concomitant
116 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

ability to discuss these relationships coherently als will not usually be aware that their relationship-
and openly (see Hesse, Chapter 25, this volume), specific working models are activated (operating)
we interpret the secure-autonomous AAI status as unless their partners behave in completely unex-
an indicator that a person has developed a general pected ways. This does not mean, however, that
working model of a secure self in close relation- individuals cannot recall valid aspects of these
ships. Drawing on our earlier discussion of event models when reflecting on close relationships. In
memories and scripts, we contend that such a gen- clinical practice, Bowlby (1988, p. 149) suggest-
eralized model of the relational self must be rooted ed that whenever plenty of consistent detail is

.
in past working models of at least some specific at- given, individuals memories of childhood should

s
tachment relationships within which an individual be considered as reasonable approximations of

es
has learned to communicate attachment feelings, the truth. We contend that this is also a fair pro-
thoughts, and behaviors with relative open- visional assumption for secure-autonomous AAIs,
ness and honesty. Research suggests that secure although the word truth should be understood

Pr
parentchild relationships play the most influen- to refer an adults current constructions (see also
tial role, but that the quality of subsequent close Main, 1991).
relationships (including those with peers, teach- The proposed generalized working model of

rd
ers, and friends) is also important (e.g., Simpson, a confident relational self as a property of the per-
Collins, Tran, & Haydon, 2007; Sroufe, Egeland, son (to use Bowlbys [1988] phrase) should not be

lfo
Carlson, & Collins, 2005). In at least some cases, regarded as a trait, however, because its operation
a secure relational self may develop despite early in a developing attachment relationship depends

ui
attachment insecurity, as long as the individual on how and whether trust and commitment are re-
has had an opportunity to learn that experiences ciprocated (see also Sroufe et al., 2005). Some in-
G
of rejecting or inconsistent caregiving do not de- dividuals with secure-autonomous AAIs respond
fine his or her self-worth. We note, however, that with low coherence to an AAI-like interview
e
the psychological pathway whereby an individual about their relationship to a romantic partner to
can develop a secure relational self in the context whom they are engaged, but these individuals tend
Th

of both secure and insecure, or consistently inse- to leave the relationship rather than marrying the
cure, early relationships is at present not well un- partner (Crowell & Waters, 2005). A capacity for
derstood. Moreover, as shown by Sroufe and col- secure relationships indicated by the AAI, it ap-
09

leagues (2005), early attachment experiences are pears, affects the likelihood but does not guaran-
never fully erased. tee that attempted attachment relationships will
We further propose that a general model be secure or coherently represented.
20

of the relational self does not replace existing Secure-autonomous interviewees not only
relationship-specific working models or pre- provide coherent and emotionally open AAI nar-
clude the development of specific working mod- ratives, but also tend to discuss relationships re-

els in new relationships at any age. For example, flectively and empathically. Both during the AAI
secure-autonomous parents are likely to construct itself (Fonagy, Steele, Moran, Steele, & Higgitt,
ht

a coherent, well-organized, relationship-specific 1993) and during an AAI-like interview about


working models of their infant and of themselves the parenttoddler relationship (Slade, Grienen-
ig

as caregivers to this infant. Without relationship- berger, Bernbach, Levy, & Locker, 2005), adults
specific working models, a person could neither re- with secure-autonomous AAI status often describe
yr

flect on past nor imagine future interactions with a themselves and close others (parents or children)
particular attachment partner, except in the most in terms of needs, wishes, beliefs, regrets, and val-
op

general way. In line with the embodied simula- ues. Fonagy and Target (1997, p. 683) hypothe-
tions described in our review of neuroimaging sized that such mentalizing also enables parents
C

studies, we argue that individuals who recall or to empathize with their infant as a mental being
imagine relationships with particular others en- with feelings and needs, and thus to appropriately
gage in mental enactments of emotional experi- mirror (reflect back) the infants internal states so
ences drawn or inferred from relationship-specific that he now knows what he is feeling. Revealing
working models. this mentalizing tendency in their speech before
In this context, we wish to dispute the oft- their infants are verbal, mothers of secure, but not
stated notion that working models are invariably insecure, infants appropriately label and comment
unconscious or rely entirely on procedural or im- on their infants emotions during observed inter-
plicit memories. During live interactions, individu- actions (Meins et al., 2002). Moreover, once able
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 117

to participate in verbal dialogue, a secure child ries. This is consistent with Bowlbys (1980) no-
tends to acquire from the mother a mentalizing tion that defensive exclusion, followed by partial
reminiscing style (see the earlier review) that may or complete deactivation of the systems mediating
help him or her construct a verbalizable model of attachment, results in conflicting conscious and
the mother as an individual with her own goals, unconscious working models of self with attach-
emotions, and intentions; such a model is im- ment figures. Dismissiveness is coded when the
portant for a goal-corrected partnership (Bowlby, respondents recall negative attachment episodes
1969/1982). An understanding of others in men- while answering direct questions about separa-

.
tal terms, accompanied by emotional openness tion, rejection, and other untoward situations, but

s
and considerateness, is also useful in repairing the render them emotionally harmless by discount-

es
unavoidable conflicts and misunderstandings that ing their affective importance and influence. We
arise between parents and children or adolescents hypothesize that what are being regulated or sup-
and later between adult mates (see integrative re- pressed in such cases are affective components of

Pr
views by Cassidy, 2001; Kobak & Duemmler, 1994; otherwise relatively accessible working models
and Fonagy et al., Chapter 33, this volume). that might create anxiety if they became fully con-
In contrast to the coherent, fresh, lively, and scious. We return to this issue in our discussion of

rd
believable accounts of childhood attachments that semiprojective measures.
characterize secure-autonomous AAIs, narratives Regarding physiological aspects of such rep-

lfo
classified into one of the three insecure statuses resentational conflict, Dozier and Kobak (1992)
(dismissing, preoccupied, and unresolved/disorga- discovered that AAI dismissiveness (but not ide-

ui
nized) exhibit various inconsistencies and confu- alization) was accompanied by significantly ele-
sions. The evaluation of these transcripts requires vated electrodermal responses, usually considered
G
close analysis of the interview content in terms of indicative of emotional inhibition, conflict, and
its relative consistency, relevance, and connota- deception. Roisman (2007) observed similar auto-
e
tive meanings, supporting Bowlbys proposal that nomic reactivity in married adults with high AAI
working models acquired in insecure relationships deactivating scores (related to dismissiveness) who
Th

develop in a less straightforward manner than were engaged in problem-solving discussions with
those indicative of security. their spouses, suggesting that these adults have
Although the intergenerational match be- developed a defensively excluded working model
09

tween specific subtypes of parental AAI insecurity of self with spouse that conflicts with their con-
and infant Strange Situation classifications is lower scious models. We suspect that autonomic inhibi-
for insecurity than for security (van IJzendoorn, tion may also be demonstrated in parents who ig-
20

1995), the number of parents with dismissing AAI nore their infants distress and reject bids for close
status whose infants are classified as avoidant is bodily contact. By contrast, avoidant infants, who
substantial. Main (1999, p.862) explained this in- turn or move away from their parents on reunion

tergenerational link in terms of a parents singular in the Strange Situation, exhibit physiological
overall classifiable state of mind, held in place arousal (elevated heart rate) rather than inhibi-
ht

by minimizing attention to attachment cues. The tion (Spangler & Grossmann, 1993). Although
adults implicit goal is to maintain a false sense of behaviorally avoidant, such infants may not yet
ig

felt security, whether by evading AAI questions or have learned to inhibit physiological arousal when
by rejecting a distressed infants bids for physical expecting that the desired parental reassurance
yr

contact (Main, Hesse, & Kaplan, 2005, p. 282). will not forthcoming.
Although we consider this emphasis on regula- Because of space limitations, we omit dis-
op

tory processes valuable, we suggest that processes cussion of the preoccupied AAI designation, but
beyond attention regulation should be probed. A recommend more attention to its disproportionate
C

working-model perspective on dismissing AAIs co-occurrence with the unresolved classification


emphasizes defensive exclusion and conflicting (see below). As a consequence, purely preoccu-
working models. pied AAI transcripts are rare (Main et al., 2005).
Two somewhat different defensive strate- They are also not systematically associated with
gies (idealization and dismissiveness) characterize infantparent ambivalence in the Strange Situa-
most dismissing AAIs. Idealization is coded when tion (van IJzendoorn, 1995).
respondents provide glowing adjectives about The third insecure AAI classification (un-
their relationships with parents in childhood, but resolved/disorganized) is given when discussions
are unable or unwilling to recall relevant memo- of loss or abuse contain subtle cognitive and dis-
118 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

course lapses (e.g., inappropriately detailed nar- ing models of self as good and bad, arguing that
ratives, use of inappropriate speech registers, and that the negative episodes in these AAIs were too
irrational thinking). Regardless of secondary AAI encompassing to be dealt with by lack of memory
status (based on the overall transcript), a substan- ... so the difficulties are presented as matter of
tial number of parents with these classifications fact or even as having a certain entertainment
tend to have infants who exhibit behavioral dis- or shock value (p. 20). Supporting the validity
organization in the Strange Situation (see Wein- of the hostile-helpless status were findings of its
field, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, Chapter 4, this strong associations with infant disorganization at

.
volume). 18 months.

s
AAI lapses associated with discussions of un- Overall, AAI findings suggest that parents

es
resolved trauma remind us of dissociative phenom- induct their infants into a way of relating that is
ena that Bowlby (1980) conceptualized as segre- consistent with their own secure or conflicted/
gated selves (or principal systems), capable of defensive models of self in relationships. Devel-

Pr
alternation in consciousness without possibility opmental continuity from nonverbal behavioral
of intercommunication, and therefore liable to and emotional attachment patterns with mother
cause inexplicable, erratic behavior. Note, how- in infancy to adult representational AAI status has

rd
ever, that Bowlbys examples concerned very se- been established in several longitudinal studies of
vere disorders (e.g., fugues) and that he did not middle-class families, but is greater for security

lfo
clarify specific triggers for the proposed alterna- than for specific subtypes of insecurity (e.g., Main
tions. Main (1999) has explained the unresolved et al., 2005; Waters, Merrick, Treboux, Crowell, &

ui
state of mind in terms of the temporary collapse or Albersheim, 2000). In particular, infants classified
disorganization of attention when confronted with as disorganized with their mothers in infancy fre-
G
reminders of traumatic attachment experiences, quently produce dismissing rather than unresolved
whether during the AAI or while interacting with AAIs in adulthood (e.g., Main et al., 2005). How-
e
an infant. Again, deeper probing for explanations ever, in a German longitudinal study, later AAI
beyond attention regulation seems necessary, but status was not predictable from infant Strange
Th

given the subtle and temporary nature of AAI dis- Situations, but was correlated with attachment
organization, we also do not find an explanation in measures in childhood (Grossmann et al., 2005).
terms of segregated principal systems or selves and Concerning which of several caregivers AAI
09

their working models fully satisfactory. status is more influential in a childs general model
In their work with clinical and high-risk of the relational self, Main and colleagues (2005)
groups, Lyons-Ruth, Yellin, Melnick, and Atwood reported longitudinal links of infantmother, but
20

(2005) felt the need for a disorganized AAI cat- not infantfather, attachment with later AAI sta-
egory that, unlike the unresolved status, could tus. Grossmann, Grossmann, Kindler, and Zim-
be assessed on an interview-wide basis. Foremost mermann (see Chapter 36, this volume), on the

among indicators of a pervasively unintegrated other hand, have found that both mother- and
state of mind was frank discussion of a helpless father-related attachment-relevant assessments
ht

and/or hostile primary caregiver in childhood beyond infancy contribute unique and joint vari-
with whom the interviewee currently identified, ance to the prediction of young adult AAIs.
ig

implicitly or explicitly. Associated indicators Finally, in a study of disadvantaged families


were frequent laughter following descriptions of exposed to numerous life stresses, many individuals
yr

highly negative childhood events; numerous refer- who were secure with their mothers as infants pro-
ences to fear, but not in relation to trauma; and duced insecure (usually dismissing) AAIs as young
op

reports of severe abuse (resolved or unresolved). adults. However, in that study relationship quality
After independent reconsideration with this new with peers and teachers added considerable addi-
C

coding system, many AAI transcripts of high-risk tional variance to the prediction of young adult
women were reclassified with the new designation AAI status, as did the motherchild relationship
hostile-helpless, including half of those previ- quality at age 13 (Sroufe et al., 2005). An impor-
ously deemed secure-autonomous in view of the tant next step will be to go beyond correlational
openness and coherence with which these moth- explanations to gain an expanded understanding
ers had discussed highly negative family-of-origin of why specific relational patterns (underpinned
experiences. Lyons-Ruth and colleagues interpret by coherent or conflicting working models) are
the reclassified hostile-helpless AAI status in intergenerationally transmitted in many dyads
terms of alternating split (or segregated) work- but not in others, and why some remain stable in
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 119

unstable circumstances whereas others undergo Young childrens responses to attachment is-
gradual or sudden longitudinal affective change sues presented in SAT pictures and ASCT stems
from security to insecurity, or from one pattern of are taken as indicators of security if they are re-
insecurity to another. solved constructively and coherently. Coherent
stories about noncaring parents are very rare.
Avoidance of attachment topics in both the
Other Representational Attachment SAT and ASCT can be conveyed by refusing to ac-
Measures in Working Model Perspective knowledge the attachment issue presented in the

.
picture or story stem (claiming that a troublesome
Semiprojective Measures for Young Children

s
event, such as a fall, did not take place). Alter-

es
We now consider additional insights that semi- natively, children may completely sidestep attach-
projective measures may contribute to the work- ment topics (by talking about the protagonists ap-
ing-model perspective on attachment. We focus pearance) or generate attachment-avoidant story

Pr
first on two child assessments, the SAT (Main et content, wherein child protagonists do not seek,
al., 1985) and the Attachment Story Completion and their parents do not provide, comfort in dis-
Task (ASCT; Bretherton & Ridgeway, 1990). tressing situations.

rd
Following Bowlbys (1973) precept that Most striking are highly unusual, if not bi-
attachment working models are rooted in real- zarre, stories told by some children in response to

lfo
life experiences, researchers assume that young SAT pictures (the child figure is locked in a closet)
children draw on aspects of these models when and ASCT stems (the family car drives off a cliff

ui
responding to semiprojective attachment tasks. and everyone dies). During the SAT, such stories
In the SAT, children are shown six pictures of were produced by kindergarten children who, as
G
parentchild separations, both mild (saying good- infants, had been disorganized with their mothers
night) and severe (leaving for 2 weeks). They are in the Strange Situation (e.g., Main et al., 2005).
e
then asked what the pictured child would feel Upon reunion with their mothers after a 1-hour
and do in each situation. In the ASCT, an inter- separation at age 6, these children exhibited not
Th

viewer invites children to show me and tell me disorganized but controlling behavior. Reminiscent
what happens next after enacting and narrating a of a defensive response that Bowlby (1980, p.223)
variety of attachment-related story beginnings or called inverting the parentchild relationship,
09

stems with small family figures and simple props they either issued peremptory commands or acted
(an accidental mishap, a painful fall, a monster in toward their mothers in an oversolicitous fashion.
the bedroom, a parentchild separation and re- Here, behavioral (organized) and representational
20

union). Whereas the SAT was intended for some- (disorganized) assessments reflected different fac-
what older preschoolers, the enactive format of ets of attachment working models that together
the ASCT was designed for children as young as 3 provide a more comprehensive, but not fully un-

years. However, with minor adaptations, both in- derstood, picture.


struments have been effectively used with children Following Bowlbys (1980) contention that
ht

up to 8 or 9 years of age. unrealistic fantasies may become comprehen-


The semiprojective format enables children sible when viewed in context, we suggest closer
ig

to create imaginary responses not available during examination of seemingly bizarre attachment
interviews about everyday life. Even though real- narratives. Examples from an ASCT study of pre-
yr

life events are sometimes incorporated, story con- schoolers from postdivorce families (Bretherton &
tent cannot be assumed to represent literal replays Page, 2004) appeared very meaningful when con-
op

of reality, and attachment narratives could easily strued as metaphorical portrayals of overwhelm-
be written off as pure fantasy. However, a (by ing emotions about family situations (e.g., a child
C

now considerable) number of studies in the United protagonist becomes a ghost who is trapped under
States and elsewhere have documented significant furniture by a parent, or a family is uncontrollably
longitudinal and concurrent correlations of SAT battered and tossed about by a tornado). These
and ASCT evaluations with earlier and concur- children also enacted stories that appeared to por-
rent attachment measures (e.g., the Strange Situ- tray a hoped-for future (e.g., Daddy will live with
ation, the AQS, naturalistic home observations). us now) or a feared future (e.g., the children are
One study (Main et al., 2005) also reported signifi- abandoned and have to take care of themselves).
cant longitudinal correlations of the SAT with the Their narratives recall proposals by Parkes (1971)
AAI in terms of the secureinsecure dichotomy. that relinquishing working models of an antici-
120 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

pated future is a difficult aspect of all loss experi- The assumption underlying this task is that
encesa topic that calls for more systematic in- individuals who have acquired generalized secure-
vestigation in adults as well. In our study, the loss base beliefs or working models in past relationships
concerned parents no longer living together. Many will see the secure base script in the word list.
children tried to undo this fact in their narratives, In contrast, adults who do not know or do not
while some focused more on a feared future. have access to this script will produce narratives
Not yet sufficiently understood within these that only partially conform or do not conform to a
assessments are gender differences in childrens secure-base script (H. S. Waters & Waters, 2005,

.
responsesespecially evident in the ASCT, p.190), because of inconsistent or insufficient at-

s
with boys often portraying more aggression and tachment support in the past. This seemingly sim-

es
less prosocial behavior than girls (e.g., Page & ple idea has been remarkably productive.
Bretherton, 2003)although the main results Secure-base ratings of narratives created in
in most studies have held even when child gen- response to the (usually) four prompt lists were

Pr
der was statistically controlled for. In addition, very highly intercorrelated. Because some lists
story completions become more coherent with included words suggesting parentchild dyads
age, making assessment potentially problematic if while others referred to couples, these findings

rd
the same coherence criteria are applied to groups support the hypothesis that the task reflects a gen-
with a wide age range (for a more detailed con- eralized working model. Testretest correlations

lfo
sideration of the validity and limitations of these over approximately 1 year were highly significant
and other semiprojective attachment instruments (rs above .50), suggesting considerable stabil-

ui
for children, see Solomon & George, Chapter 18, ity (Vaughn et al., 2006). Significant associations
this volume). Finally, we note that only one rep- between mothers secure-base script ratings and
G
resentational classification or rating is given to AAI coherence scales have already been reported
each childs narratives, even though the pictures in several U.S. and international studies (e.g.,
e
and stories include two parents, and the observa- Vaughn et al., 2007), showing that this working-
tional attachment measures with the two parents model-based approach is compatible with AAI se-
Th

may differ in security. For this reason, we would curity. We suggest, however, that the Secure Base
not call a child who produces secure stories se- Scriptedness instrument, because it does not assess
curely attached. types of insecurity, is most likely to be useful in
09

nonclinical groups.
The Adult Attachment Projective (AAP;
Two Semiprojective Measures for Adults
George & West, 2004) was influenced by the SAT
20

Both of the instruments we consider here were and a coding system that Solomon, George, and
developed after the semiprojective child measures De Jong (1995) developed for their adaptation of
and were, in part, inspired by them. Whereas the the original ASCT. In contrast to the Secure Base

first measure provides ratings of security, the sec- Scriptedness task, the AAP was specifically creat-
ond is based on classifications and places stress on ed to line up with the Main and Goldwyn (1984;
ht

defensive processes. Main et al., 2003) AAI categories (secure, dismiss-


The Secure Base Scriptedness measure (H. ing, preoccupied, and unresolved). However, un-
ig

S. Waters & Rodrigues-Doolabh, 2004) emerged like that of the AAI, the AAP coding system is
from the idea that childrens ASCT narratives closely tied to Bowlbys (1980) theory of defensive
yr

could be rated in terms of their resemblance to a processes. Another difference is that the AAP re-
secure-base script (see reanalysis by H. S. Waters, quires the participant to tell stories about pictured
op

Rodrigues, & Ridgeway [1998] of transcripts from scenarios, whereas the AAI asks for childhood
the Bretherton et al. [1990] study). Adults are memories. Nevertheless, the developers of the
C

not asked to complete attachment stories, but to measure found, in a sample of 144 adults of diverse
generate narratives by using words from carefully ages and backgrounds, substantial AAPAAI con-
constructed prompt lists. The words can be used cordance (kappa = .84; C. George, personal com-
in any order, but each list, if read in sequence, sug- munication, January 7, 2007).
gests a specific instantiation of a more general or Participants are asked to tell stories about
prototypical secure-base script (i.e., pleasant inter- eight somewhat ambiguous line drawings that de-
action interrupted by a troublesome event that is pict adultchild dyads, adult couples, and solitary
resolved by appeal to and effective intervention by figures in a variety of situations likely to elicit at-
an attachment figure). tachment distress (e.g., separation, solitude, fear,
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 121

injury, and death). A secure stance is indicated by ents to whom they are insecurely attached. Given
coherent and constructively resolved narratives in that the same result was obtained for adults, a
which the distressed protagonists either receive preferable explanation may be that individuals
care or (in response to alone pictures) rely on who have experienced or are experiencing secure
inner resources, such as reflecting on and exploring relationships will create positive hypothetical at-
feelings. The latter responses are coded as indica- tachment scenarios based on their generalized ex-
tive of an internalized secure base. Dismissing pectations or working models, even though their
AAP narratives emphasize relational disconnec- actual relationships are likely to be uncaring from

.
tion and avoidance of attachment topics, inter- time to time.

s
preted in terms of Bowlbys (1980) notion of at- Second, we consider the notion of an in-

es
tachment system deactivation. Preoccupied AAP ternalized secure base, proposed by George and
stories also focus on relational disconnection, but West (2004) in conjunction with AAP narratives
the protagonists are more passive, and their waver- to alone pictures. In explaining this construct,

Pr
ing story lines are very hard to follow. In line with George and West argue that maintenance of prox-
Bowlby, George and West view preoccupied stories imity to attachment figures in adulthood becomes
as evidence for cognitive disconnection of affect an almost exclusively internal process related to

rd
from its source. Finally, plots of unresolved AAP internalized attachment figures. They do not
narratives contain themes of helplessness, loss of specify whether they are referring to representa-

lfo
control, violence, or isolation; the authors ascribe tions of caregiving figures from childhood, current
these to the breakdown of severe repression, refer- attachment figures, or generalized working models

ui
ring to Bowlbys (1980) notion of segregated (prin- not linked to a specific figure. Their construct re-
cipal) systems, but not the link to dissociation. We sembles, but is not equivalent to, Mains (1999,
G
suspect that the AAP may be particularly suitable p.862) statement that secure-autonomous adults
for use with clinical groups, as shown in a neu- feel secure in themselves. Elaborating on this
e
roimaging AAP study of women with borderline explanation, Main and colleagues (2005, p.268)
personality disorder by Buchheim and colleagues posited that in theory, an adult with all attach-
Th

(in press). ment figures deceased and no close relationship


available could still be secure-autonomous and
raise secure offspring. Although we recognize
Summary and Comments
09

the value of an internalized secure base and of


Aware that our selective review cannot compre- autonomy (and similar proposals by Mikulincer
hensively cover (or even touch on) all important & Shaver, 2004), we believe that Bowlby would
20

theoretical issues and empirical work on attach- not accord this much power to states of mind
ment at the representational level, we conclude or generalized attachment working models in the
this section by contrasting and comparing the complete absence of actual support. Moreover,

findings obtained with the AAI and semiprojec- children, by Main and colleagues definition, can-
tive measures. This comparison has yielded an in- not be autonomous, because they need the physi-
ht

teresting set of similarities and differences relevant cal presence of an attachment figure. This obscures
to, but also raising questions about, the conceptu- the fact that even young children can express a
ig

alization of attachment working models. degree of autonomy in their attachment relation-


First, whether produced in response to ques- ships as they gain the ability to take their parents
yr

tions about childhood experiences or hypothetical perspective in goal-corrected partnerships, and as


attachment situations, coherent narratives about they become able to spend more time apart from
op

attachment relationships by children and adults their attachment figures, trusting that these figures
are indicators of attachment-related security as- will be available when needed.
C

sessed with observational attachment measures). Third, with respect to attachment avoidance,
However, coherent narratives about lived expe- the AAI and the semiprojective measures for chil-
riences in the AAI describe both positive and dren and adults elicit both dissimilar and similar
negative attachment relationships, whereas co- defensive strategies. Idealization of attachment fig-
herence and constructive content usually go hand ures, considered one of the hallmarks of dismissing
in hand in childrens and adults narratives about AAIs, is not seen during the SAT, ASCT, or AAP.
hypothetical situations. We expected this link in This raises the possibility that AAI idealization
childrens stories, because earned security seems may be due to dismissing individuals concerns
improbable when children are still living with par- about self-presentation or self-disclosure in a face-
122 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

to-face interview about their own experiences avoidant, ambivalent, or disorganized) becomes
perhaps indicating a form of self-deception, an at- most predominant in early childhood, even when
tempt at positive self-presentation, or merely a way an individual is exposed to both secure and inse-
of trying to head off further inquiries. Main and cure attachment relationships at home, remains to
colleagues (2005) have suggested that the validity be clarified. In addition, it is evident that different
of the AAI does not depend on how the reasons methods of assessment reveal different aspects of
underlying the idealization strategy are explained, an individuals attachment working models, espe-
but an examination of this issue is important for cially with respect to dismissiveness and disorga-

.
theory. In contrast, devaluation and discounting nization. These findings require integration into a

s
of attachment feelings are common to the AAI, complete and coherent picture.

es
ASCT, and AAP; during the SAT, however, chil- Finally, our review suggests that the narrative
dren who were avoidant with their mothers in in- assessments reviewed here may be effective as in-
fancy labeled the pictured child as sad about the dicators of general working models because they

Pr
separation, but unable to act. elicit descriptions of attachment interactions in
Fourth, the plots of disorganized SAT and specific relationship contexts rather than asking
ASCT narratives and unresolved AAP narratives individuals to rate general statements such as my

rd
contain many portrayals of helplessness, loss of mother loves me. Perhaps one can most fully ac-
control, violence, or abandonment. The readi- cess information about differences in the organiza-

lfo
ness with which some children and adults produce tion of participants attachment working models
such narratives raises the question of why indices by asking them to describe remembered or imag-

ui
of disorganization during the AAI are more subtle ined (embodied and felt) interactions in specific
and fleeting, yet are so striking in projective assess- attachment relationships.
G
ments for children and adults. These findings make
us wonder whether individuals who tell these nar-
Concluding Statement
e
ratives also have daydreams that are pervasively
filled with chaotic fantasies, possibly surfacing
Th

from dissociated working models. This would To make further progress in elaborating and clari-
make their inner lives much more disturbing than fying the working-model construct and to foster
is apparent from the more fleeting indicators of at- new discoveries in representational attachment
09

tachment disorganization seen during the AAI. research, we strongly urge close collaboration with
More generally, we conclude that assess- cutting-edge researchers who study memory, nar-
ments of specific attachment relationships or sets ratives, and storytelling in adults and children. We
20

of relationships by means of interviews, projective need to gain a deeper understanding of the devel-
tests, or observations never yield pure insights opment and operation of the collaborating brain
about specific and general working models because systems that Bowlby (1969/1982) subsumed under

of bidirectional influences between them (see also the label attachment behavioral system, and to
Sroufe et al., 2005). Despite strong evidence that which he later referred as systems-mediating at-
ht

the AAI reflects a general stance (and associated tachment (1980). These systems influence repre-
working models), the inferences about this stance sentational and experiential processes, and are in
ig

must rely on descriptions of specific relationships turn influenced by them. In addition, new devel-
and may provide insights into these particular re- opments in clinical and social psychology as well
yr

lationships as well. In addition, the developmen- as neuroscience may offer useful constructs and
tal phase at which it becomes appropriate to speak findings to expand and refine ideas about the op-
op

of a generalized attachment working model, and eration of defensive processes, as urged by Bowlby
ways to conceptualize the complexity of such a (1980); they may also help us discover more about
C

model at different stages of development, are not the many factors that make for working-model
settled. Even by the end of early childhood, secu- change, whether in a secure or insecure direction,
rity ratings of attachment stories predict higher and whether gradual or abrupt (see Mikulincer &
ratings for general self-worth and the quality of re- Shaver, Chapter 23, this volume). Mining the rich
lations with peers and teachers in preschool (e.g., AAI texts for the specific adaptive or maladaptive
Cassidy, 1988; Gullon-Rivera, 2008). At that age, ways in which interviewees process relationship
childrens attachment stories may already be relat- information may be helpful as well.
ed to a general view of peers as more or less trust- Finally, we feel compelled to voice concerns
worthy (e.g., Suess, Grossmann, & Sroufe, 1992). about what may be becoming an excessively mo-
On the other hand, why one approach (trusting, nadic (one-person) emphasis on the role of attach-
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 123

ment representations, whether these are concep- Adolphs, R. (2006). How do we know the minds of oth-
tualized as working models or as states of mind. ers?: Domain-specificity, simulation, and enactive so-
Evaluations of interview-based and semiprojective cial cognition. Brain Research, 1079, 2535.
attachment measures are currently used to explain Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. (1974).
Infantmother attachment and social development:
an individuals mental health, emotion regulation,
Socialization as a product of reciprocal responsive-
and other desirable and important outcomes. Even ness to signals. In P. M. Richards (Ed.), The integration
when interactions between both members of a dyad of a child into a social world (pp.99135). Cambridge,
are observed or both members are interviewed, re- UK: Cambridge University Press.

.
searchers most often evaluate the responses of each Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall,

s
separately, rather than examining relationship S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study

es
functioning when partners attachment working of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
models mesh well or diverge. Similarly, proposals Anderson, S., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., &
that secure-autonomous individuals are secure Damasio, A. (1999). Impairment of social and moral

Pr
within themselves (Main et al., 2005) or rely behavior related to early damage in the prefrontal
cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 10321037.
most of the time on an internalized secure base
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004).The neural correlates
(George & West, 2004) may lead to an exagger-

rd
of maternal and romantic love. NeuroImage, 21,
ated notion about the extent to which attachment 11551166.
representations per se, in the absence of actual sup-

lfo
Bauer, P. J., & Wewerka, S. S. (1995). One- to two-year-
portive relationships, enable self-reliance. olds recall of events: The more impressed, the more
We recommend an increased focus on dy- expressed. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,

ui
adic (two-person) approaches. The incoherent, 59, 475496.
conflicted representational processes observed Botvinick, M. M., Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch,
G
in insecure relationships can be characterized as D. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitor-
self-protective and therefore adaptive from a mo- ing and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108,
624652.
e
nadic point of view (e.g., defensive exclusion of
Bowlby, J. (1969/1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. At-
anxiety-provoking attachment-related thoughts
Th

tachment. New York: Basic Books.


and feelings). However, the associated behaviors Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separa-
turn out to be maladaptive for constructive com- tion: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.
munication in ongoing attachment relationships. Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional
09

By contrast, coherent and open representational bonds. London: Tavistock/Routledge.


processes in secure relationships not only encour- Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss: Sad-
age self-reliance and autonomy, but also foster re- ness and depression. New York: Basic Books.
20

lationships that are likely to be more satisfying to Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic
both members of an attachment dyad. Books.
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect

and prospect. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.),


Acknowledgments Growing points of attachment theory and research.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
ht

We thank the following colleagues with whom we en- ment, 50(12), Serial No. 209, 335.
gaged in stimulating exchanges of ideas during the writ- Bretherton, I. (1990). Open communication and inter-
ig

ing of this chapter (in alphabetical order): Carol George, nal working models: Their role in the development of
Roger Kobak, Karlen Lyons-Ruth, Glenn Roisman, L. attachment relationships. In R. A. Thompson (Ed.),
yr

Alan Sroufe, and Everett Waters. We are also grateful Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Vol. 36. Socioe-
for the enormously helpful feedback we received from motional development (pp.59113). Lincoln: Univer-
op

Jude Cassidy and Phillip Shaver. Finally, Inge Brether- sity of Nebraska Press.
ton wishes to convey her appreciation to Stein Brten Bretherton, I. (1991). Pouring new wine into old bot-
tles: The social self as internal working model. In M.
C

for inspiring her to link John Bowlbys ideas about inter-


nal working models with insights from neuroscience at Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on
the 2004 Theory Forum Symposium of the Norwegian Child Psychology: Vol. 23. Self processes in development
Academy of Science and Letters, Oslo. (pp.141). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bretherton, I. (1993). From dialogue to representation:
The intergenerational construction of self in rela-
References tionships. In C. A. Nelson (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia
on Child Development: Vol. 26. Memory and affect in
Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience of human development (pp.237263). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
social behavior. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 4, Bretherton, I. (2005). In pursuit of the internal working
165177. model construct and its relevance to attachment re-
124 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

lationships. In K. E. Grossmann, K. Grossmann, & E. mann, & E. Waters (Eds.), Attachment from infancy to
Waters (Eds.), Attachment from infancy to adulthood: adulthood: The major longitudinal studies (pp.223244).
The major longitudinal studies (pp.1347). New York: New York: Guilford Press.
Guilford Press. Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens. New
Bretherton, I., & Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of York: Harcourt Brace.
intentional communication. In I. C. Ugiris (Ed.), Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow,
Social interaction and communication during infancy and the feeling brain. Orlando, FL: Harcourt.
(pp.81100). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Decety, J., Chaminade, T., Grzes, J., & Meltzoff, A. N.
Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about in- (2002). A PET exploration of the neural mechanisms

.
ternal states: The acquisition of an explicit theory of involved in reciprocal imitation. NeuroImage, 15,

s
mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906921. 265272.

es
Bretherton, I., & Munholland, K. A. (1999). Internal Dixon, N. F. (1971). Subliminal perception: The nature of
working models in attachment relationships: A con- a controversy. London: McGraw-Hill.
struct revisited. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cam-

Pr
Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
applications (pp.89111). New York: Guilford Press. Dozier, M., & Kobak, R. R. (1992). Psychophysiology in
Bretherton, I., & Page, T. (2004). Shared or conflict- Adult Attachment Interviews: Converging evidence

rd
ing working models?: Relationships in postdivorce for de-activating strategies. Child Development, 63,
families seen through the eyes of mothers and their 14731480.

lfo
preschool children. Development and Psychopathology, Eisenberger, N. E., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why
16, 551575. rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for
Bretherton, I., & Ridgeway, D. (1990). An Attach- physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,

ui
ment Story Completion Task to assess young chil- 8, 294300.
drens internal working models of child and parents Erdelyi, M. H. (1974). A new Look at the New Look:
G
in the attachment relationship. In M. T. Greenberg, Perceptual defense and vigilance. Psychological Re-
D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment view, 81, 125.
in the preschool years: Theory, research, and interven- Etzion-Carasso, A., & Oppenheim, D. (2000). Open
e
tion (pp. 300308). Chicago: University of Chicago motherpreschooler communication: Relations with
Th

Press. early secure attachment. Attachment and Human De-


Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., & Cassidy, J. (1990). As- velopment, 2, 347370.
sessing internal working models of the attachment Fivush, R., & Fromhoff, F. (1988). Style and structure in
relationship: An Attachment Story Completion Task motherchild conversations about the past. Discourse
09

for 3-year-olds. In D. Cicchetti, M. Greenberg, & E. Processes, 11, 337355.


M. Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Fivush, R., & Vasudeva, A. (2002). Remembering to
Theory, research, and intervention (pp.272308). Chi- relate: Socioemotional correlates of motherchild
20

cago: University of Chicago Press. reminiscing. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3,


Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., Fo- 7390.
gassi, L., Gallese, V., et al. (2001). Action observa- Fonagy, P., Steele, M., Moran, G., Steele, H., & Hig-

tion activates premotor and parietal areas in a soma- gitt, M. D. (1993). Measuring the ghost in the
totopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of nursery: Anempirical study of the relation between
Neuroscience, 13, 400404. parents mental representation of childhood experi-
ht

Buchheim, A., Erk, S., George, C., Kchele, H., Kircher, ences and their infants security of attachment. Jour-
T., Martius, P., et al. (in press). Neural correlates of nal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 41,
ig

attachment trauma in borderline personality disorder: 957989.


A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Psy- Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflec-
yr

chiatry Research. tive function: Their role in self-organization. Devel-


Cassidy, J. (1988). Childmother attachment and the opment and Psychopathology, 9, 679700.
op

self in six-year-olds. Child Development, 59, 121134. Freud, S. (1963). An outline of psychoanalysis (J. Strachey,
Cassidy, J. (2001). Truth, lies, and intimacy: An attach- Trans). New York: Norton. (Original work published
ment perspective. Attachment and Human Develop- 1940)
C

ment, 3, 121155. Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and the
Collins, N. L. (1996). Working models of attachment: neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Trans-
Implications for explanation, emotion, and behav- actions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 358,
ior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 459473.
810832. Fuster, J. M. (1997). The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy,
Craik, K. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. New
UK: Cambridge University Press. York: Raven Press.
Crowell, J., & Waters, H. (2005). Attachment repre- Gallese, V. (2005). Embodied simulation: From neurons
sentations, secure-base behavior, and the evolution to phenomenal experience. Phenomenology and the
of adult relationships. In K. E. Grossmann, K. Gross- Cognitive Sciences, 4, 2348.
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 125

George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1984). Adult At- tachment processes in adulthood (pp. 121149). Lon-
tachment Interview protocol. Unpublished manuscript, don: Jessica Kingsley.
University of California at Berkeley. Krawczyk, D. C. (2002). Contributions of the prefrontal
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1985). Adult At- cortex to the neural basis of human decision making.
tachment Interview protocol (2nd ed.). Unpublished Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 631664.
manuscript, University of California at Berkeley. Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. (2000). Motherchild
George, C., Kaplan, N., & Main, M. (1996). Adult At- discourse, attachment security, shared positive affect
tachment Interview protocol (3rd ed.). Unpublished and early conscience development. Child Develop-
manuscript, University of California at Berkeley. ment, 71, 14241440.

.
George, C., & West, M. (2004). The Adult Attachment Lewin, K. (1933). Environmental forces. In C. Murchi-

s
Projective: Measuring individual differences in at- son (Ed.), A handbook of child psychology (2nd ed.,

es
tachment security using projective methodology. In pp. 590625). Worcester, MA: Clark University
M. Hersen (Series Ed.) & M. Hilsenroth & D. Segal Press.
(Vol. Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of psychological as- Light, P. (1979). The development of social sensitivity.

Pr
sessment: Vol. 2. Personality assessment (pp.431447). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Lyons-Ruth, K., Yellin, C., Melnick, S., & Atwood, G.
Gibson, E. J. (1982). The concept of affordances in de- (2005). Expanding the concept of unresolved mental

rd
velopment: The renascence of functionalism. In W. states: Hostile-helpless states of mind on the Adult
A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychol- Attachment Interview are associated with disrupted

lfo
ogy: Vol. 15. The concept of development (pp.5581). motherinfant communication and infant disorgani-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. zation. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 123.
Goldman, A. I. (1991). In defense of simulation. Mind Main, M. (1991). Metacognitive knowledge, metacog-

ui
and Language, 7, 104119. nitive monitoring, and singular (coherent) versus
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., & Kindler, H. (2005). G multiple (incoherent) models of attachment. In C.
Early care and the roots of attachment and partner- M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.),
ship representations. In K. E. Grossmann, K. Gross- Attachment across the life cycle (pp.127159). London:
mann, & E. Waters (Eds.), Attachment from infancy to Routledge.
e
adulthood: The major longitudinal studies (pp.98136). Main, M. (1995). Recent studies in attachment. In
Th

New York: Guilford Press. S. Goldberg, R. Muir, & J. Kerr (Eds.), Attachment
Gullon-Rivera, A. (2008). Examining Puerto Rican theory: Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives
childrens self-representation as reflected in the Attach- (pp.407474). Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
ment Story Completion Task: Linkages with global self- Main, M. (1999). Epilogue. Attachment theory: Eigh-
09

worth, motherchild relationship, and social competence. teen points with suggestions for future studies. In
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of at-
WisconsinMadison. tachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications
20

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). (pp.845887). New York: Guilford Press.
The distributed human neural system for face percep- Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Adult attachment scor-
tion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 223233. ing and classification system. Unpublished manuscript,

Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1987). Romantic love con- University of California at Berkeley.
ceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of Per- Main, M., Goldwyn, R., & Hesse, E. (2003). Adult at-
sonality and Social Psychology, 52, 511524. tachment scoring and classification system. Unpublished
ht

Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. manuscript, University of California at Berkeley.
New York: Wiley. Main, M., Hesse, E., & Kaplan, N. (2005). Predict-
ig

Hilgard, E. (1974). Toward a neo-dissociation the- ability of attachment behavior and representational
ory: Multiple cognitive controls in human func- processes at 1, 6 and 19 years of age. In K. E. Gross-
yr

tioning. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 17, mann, K. Grossmann, & E. Waters (Eds.), Attachment
301316. from infancy to adulthood: The major longitudinal studies
op

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, (pp.245304). New York: Guilford Press.
MA: Harvard University Press. Main, M., Kaplan, K., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in
Kanwisher, N., & Yovel, G. (2006). The fusiform face infancy, childhood, and adulthood: A move to the
C

area: A cortical region specialized for the perception level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters
of faces. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and re-
of Long, Series B, 361, 21092128. search. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
Klagsbrun, M., & Bowlby, J. (1976). Responses to separa- Development, 50(12, Serial No. 209), 66104.
tion from parents: A clinical test for young children. Mead, G. H. (1913). The social self. Journal of Philoso-
British Journal of Projective Psychology, 21, 721. phy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 10, 374380.
Kobak, R., & Duemmler, S. (1994). Attachment and Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago:
conversation: A discourse analysis of goal-directed University of Chicago Press.
partnerships. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Gupta, M.
(Eds.), Advances in personal relationships: Vol. 5. At- D., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-
126 I. OVERVIEW OF ATTACHMENT THEORY

indedness and attachment security as predictors of


m Roisman, G., Padrn, E., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B.
theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73, (2002). Earned attachment status in retrospect and
17151726. prospect. Child Development, 73, 12041219.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of Rolls, E. T. (1996). The orbitofrontal cortex. Philosophic
facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Sci- Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B,
ence, 198, 7598. 351, 14331444.
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Security-based Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel ver-
self-representations in adulthood: Contents and pro- sus how do you think she would feel?: A neuroimag-
cesses. In W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult ing study of perspective-taking with social emotions.

.
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical implications Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 988999.

s
(pp.159195). New York: Guilford Press. Saxe, R., Xiao, D.-K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D. I., & Kan-

es
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative wisher, N. (2004). A region of right posterior superior
theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional ac-
Neuroscience, 24, 167202. tions. Neuropsychologia, 42, 14351446.

Pr
Nelson, K. (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive
MA: Harvard University Press. neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Trans-

rd
Emergence of the mediated mind. New York: Cambridge actions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 362,
University Press. 773786.

lfo
Nelson, K. (2005). Evolution and the development of Schacter, D. L., & Tulving, E. (1994). What are the
human memory systems. In S. Ellis & D. Bjorklund memory systems of 1994? In D. Schachter & E. Tulv-
(Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary psychol- ing (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 138). Cambridge,

ui
ogy and child development (pp. 319345). New York: MA: MIT Press.
Guilford Press. Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans,
G
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
representations: Basic building blocks of cogni- Sherry, D. F., & Schacter, D. L. (1987). The evolution
tive development. In M. E. Lamb & A. Brown of multiple memory systems. Psychological Review, 94,
e
(Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (Vol. 1, 439454.
Th

pp.131158). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Simpson, J. A., Collins, W. A., Tran, S., & Haydon,
Newcombe, R., & Reese, E. (2004). Evaluations and K. C. (2007). Attachment and the experience and
orientations in motherchild narratives as a function expression of emotions in romantic relationships: A
of attachment security: A longitudinal investigation. developmental perspective. Journal of Personality and
09

International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28, Social Psychology, 92, 355367.


230245. Singer, T., Seymour, B., ODoherty, J., Stephan, K. E.,
Norman, D. A. (1976). Memory and attention: Introduc- Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural
20

tion to human information processing (2nd ed.). New responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of
York: Wiley. others. Nature, 439, 466469.
Page, T., & Bretherton, I. (2003). Gender differences in Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., &

stories of violence and caring by preschool children Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning,
in post-divorce families: Implications for social com- attachment, and the transmission gap: A prelimi-
petence. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 20, nary study. Attachment and Human Development, 7,
ht

485504. 283298.
Parkes, C. M. (1971). Psychosocial transitions: A field Solomon, J., George, C., & De Jong, A. (1995). Chil-
ig

for study. Social Sciences and Medicine, 5, 11101115. dren classified as controlling at age six: Evidence of
Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams, and imitation. New York: disorganized representational strategies at home and
yr

Norton. school. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 447464.


Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. Spangler, G., & Grossmann, K. E. (1993). Biobehavioral
op

New York: Norton. organization in securely and insecurely attached in-


Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. fants. Child Development, 64, 14391450.
(1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A.
C

actions. Brain Research: Cognitive Brain Research, 3, (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota
131141. Study of Risk and Adaptation from Birth to Adulthood.
Roisman, G. (2006).The role of adult attachment secu- New York: Guilford Press.
rity in non-romantic, non-attachment-related first Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant.
interactions between same-sex strangers. Attachment New York: Basic Books.
and Human Development, 8, 341352. Suess, G., Grossmann, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992).
Roisman, G. (2007). The psychophysiology of adult Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on
attachment relationships: Autonomic reactivity in quality of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to in-
marital and premarital interactions. Developmental dividual organization of self. International Journal of
Psychology, 43, 3953. Behavioural Development, 15, 4365.
5. Internal Working Models in Attachment Relationships 127

Thompson, R. A., Laible, D. J., & Ontai, L. L. (2003). Waters, E. (1995). The Attachment Q-Set (Version
Early understanding of emotion, morality, and the 3.0). In E. Waters, B. E. Vaughn, G. Posada, & K.
self: Developing a working model. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Kondo-Ikemura (Eds.), Caregiving, cultural and
Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, cognitive perspectives on secure base behavior and
pp.137171). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. working models: New growing points of attachment
Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary inter- theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Re-
subjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of mean- search in Child Development, 60(23, Serial No. 244),
ing in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture, 234246.
and symbol (pp.183229). London: Academic Press. Waters, E., Crowell, J., Elliott, M., Corcoran, D., &

.
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In Treboux, D. (2002). Bowlbys secure base theory

s
E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of and the social/personality psychology of attachment

es
memory (pp.381403). New York: Academic Press. styles: Work(s) in progress. Attachment and Human
van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1995). Adult attachment rep- Development, 4, 230242.
resentations, parental responsiveness, and infant at- Waters, E., Merrick, S., Treboux, D., Crowell, J., & Al-

Pr
tachment: A meta-analysis on the predictive validity bersheim, L. (2000). Attachment security in infancy
of the Adult Attachment Interview. Psychological Bul- and early adulthood: A 20-year longitudinal study.
letin, 117, 387403. Child Development, 71, 684689.

rd
Vaughn, B., Coppola, G., Verssimo, M., Monteiro, L., Waters, H. S., Rodrigues, L. M., & Ridgeway, D. (1998).
Santos, A. J., Posada, G., et al. (2007). The quality of Cognitive underpinnings of narrative attachment as-

lfo
maternal secure base scripts predicts childrens secure sessment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71,
base behavior at home in three socio-cultural groups. 211234.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31, Waters, H. S., & Rodrigues-Doolabh, L. (2004). Manual

ui
6576. for decoding secure base narratives. Unpublished manu-
Vaughn, B. E., Verssimo, M., Coppola, G., Bost, K. K., G script, State University of New York at Stony Brook.
Shin, N., McBridge, B., et al. (2006). Maternal at- Waters, H. S., & Waters, E. (2006). The attachment
tachment script representations: Longitudinal stabil- working models concept: Among other things, we
ity and associations with stylistic features of maternal build script-like representations of secure base ex-
e
narratives. Attachment and Human Development, 8, periences. Attachment and Human Development, 8,
Th

199208. 185197.
Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., Herrmann, S., Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese,
Happe, F., Falkai, P., et al. (2001). Mind reading: V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my
Neural mechanisms of theory of mind and self- insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling
09

perspective. NeuroImage, 14, 170181. disgust, Neuron, 3, 655664.


Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Young, J. Z. (1964). A model for the brain. London: Ox-
Harvard University Press. ford University Press.
20

ht
ig
yr
op
C

Copyright 2008 The Guilford Press. All rights reserved under International Guilford Publications
Copyright Convention. No part of this text may be reproduced, transmitted, 72 Spring Street
downloaded, or stored in or introduced into any information storage or New York, NY 10012
retrieval system, in any form or by any means, whether electronic or mechan- 212-431-9800
ical, now known or hereinafter invented, without the written permission of The 800-365-7006
Guilford Press. www.guilford.com/p/cassidy

View publication stats

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen