Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat.

44 (2013) 179e185

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Journal of Behavior Therapy and


Experimental Psychiatry
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbtep

Unconscious thought reduces intrusion development: A replication and extension


Julie Krans a, *, Drte Janecko b, Maarten W. Bos c, d
a
University of New South Wales,1 School of Psychology, Sydney NSW 2052, Australia
b
Radboud University Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands
c
Harvard Business School,1 Negotiation, Organizations & Markets, Boston, USA
d
Harvard Medical School,1 Department of Psychiatry, Center for Sleep and Cognition, Boston, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Background and Objectives: Intrusive images after a traumatic event, a hallmark feature of post-traumatic
Received 16 January 2012 stress disorder, are suggested to develop because the trauma memory is disorganized and not integrated
Received in revised form into autobiographical memory. Unconscious Thought Theory predicts that information can be concep-
24 July 2012
tually organized after a period of unconscious thought (UT), more so than after conscious thought (CT).
Accepted 31 August 2012
We aimed to test the hypothesis that UT decreases intrusions and increases conceptual organization in
memory.
Keywords:
Methods: Participants were shown a stressful lm and were required to perform an UT task, a CT task, or
Intrusions
Trauma lm paradigm
a distraction task. Intrusions of the lm, intrusion qualities, and sequence memory were measured
Unconscious Thought Theory afterwards.
PTSD Results: We conrmed our hypothesis that UT (versus CT or mere distraction) leads to fewer intrusions,
thereby replicating earlier research. Contrary to prediction, we found no difference between the
conditions on sequence memory. In addition, conscious thought appeared to increase intrusion nowness
and arousal.
Limitations: The analogue design and healthy participant sample prevent from generalizing results to
other populations. Intrusion frequency and qualities were assessed immediately after the lm thereby
prohibiting us from drawing conclusions about any long-term effects.
Conclusions: Engaging in unconscious thought after a stressful lm can reduce intrusion frequency. This
has potential implications for clinical interventions to prevent initial stress symptoms. The underlying
mechanism remains unclear for now and provides an avenue for future research.
2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction manipulation immediately after an analogue traumatic event


specically targeted at intrusions.
Intrusive images are a hallmark feature of posttraumatic stress Intrusive images are suggested to develop because the trauma
disorder (PTSD; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) and are memory is disorganized and not readily integrated into autobio-
increasingly recognized as being a signicant symptom of other graphical memory (Brewin, Dalgleish, & Joseph, 1996; Ehlers &
disorders, such as depression (Newby & Moulds, 2011). Effective Clark, 2000). A memory that is disorganized may be fragmented
treatment options are available for chronic PTSD. However, there is and not adhering to chronological order. This can make it difcult
a lack of successful interventions that can effectively prevent the for the trauma survivor to make sense of what happened and can
initial development of symptoms by acting immediately after the lead to recurring intrusive images that have a sense of nowness
trauma (National Institute for Clinical Excellence, 2005; Sijbrandij (Ehlers & Clark, 2000). It can thus be hypothesized that a better
et al., 2007). The present study tested a theory-driven organized memory would not be intrusive as often, and would not
be as vivid, experienced as being here-and-now, and arousing.
There is previous research aimed at developing evidence-based
interventions to prevent initial post-traumatic stress symptoms.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: 61 (2) 93858249; fax: 61 (2) 93853641. Several studies found an ameliorating effect of performing
E-mail address: j.krans@unsw.edu.au (J. Krans).
1 a visuospatial task after viewing a trauma lm compared to
Data collection was conducted at the Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud
University Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Authors JK and MWB have moved during a verbal task or a no task control condition (Deeprose, Zhang,
the process of writing this article. DeJong, Dalgleish, & Holmes, 2012; Holmes, James, Coode-Bate, &

0005-7916/$ e see front matter 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2012.08.004
180 J. Krans et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 179e185

Deeprose, 2009; Holmes, James, Kilford, & Deeprose, 2010). A recently supported with experimental evidence (e.g., Bos,
cognitive training towards more positive self-efcacy and meta- Dijksterhuis, & Van Baaren, 2008, 2011).
cognitive beliefs about stress symptoms after a trauma lm was Most UTT research has focused on decision-making. The
shown to reduce intrusion development compared to a training common nding is that unconscious thought leads to better deci-
towards more negative self-efcacy beliefs (Woud, Holmes, sions (Bos et al., 2008; Dijksterhuis, 2004; Dijksterhuis, Bos,
Postma, Dalgleish, & Mackintosh, 2011). Other studies have Nordgren, & Van Baaren, 2006; Dijksterhuis & Meurs, 2006; Ham,
shown that worrying or ruminating after a distressing lm Van den Bos, & Van Doorn, 2009; Lerouge, 2009), although some
increased intrusions (Butler, Wells, & Dewick, 1995; Wells & research reports failures to replicate these effects (Acker, 2008;
Papageorgiou, 1995) although others have found no signicant Calvillo & Penaloza, 2009; Gonzalez-Vallejo, Lassiter, Bellezza, &
effects (Ehring, Szeimies, & Schaffrick, 2009; Williams & Moulds, Lindberg, 2008; Newell, Wong, Cheung, & Rakow, 2009; Payne,
2007). Samper, Bettman, & Luce, 2008; Thorsteinson & Withrow, 2009;
Few studies have directly targeted the hypothesis that intru- Waroquier, Marchiori, Klein, & Cleeremans, 2009). However,
sions develop from a lack of memory organization although this is a recent meta-analysis including more than 80 studies indicated
an important claim made by models of PTSD and intrusive memory that, overall, there is a benet for unconscious thought in decision-
(Brewin et al., 1996; Ehlers & Clark, 2000). Zetsche, Ehring, and making although the overall effect size is small (for more infor-
Ehlers (2009) presented participants with a distressing lm after mation, please see Strick et al., 2011).
which they were assigned to a rumination, memory organization or Importantly for the research at hand, several experiments have
distraction condition. Intrusions were measured during a rst shown that unconscious thought can aid in organizing, clustering
session and reported in a one-week diary following the lm. and restructuring previously unorganized information in a mean-
Overall, the experimental conditions did not have a signicant ingful way (Bos et al., 2008; Dijksterhuis, 2004). For example, in the
effect on intrusion development, although, when split by gender, experiment of Dijksterhuis (2004, Experiment 5; see also Bos et al.,
men in the rumination condition reported more intrusions in the 2008; Experiment 2), participants read 18 sentences describing
rst session than men in the memory organization condition. Men behaviors (for instance: Jeroen usually gets high grades in school)
(but not women) in the memory organization condition, in turn, that related to three overarching traits (intelligence, idealism and
reported fewer intrusions than men in the distraction condition. extraversion) of a hypothetical person, presented in random order.
Despite the unexpected gender effect, this gives some credence to They were then instructed to recall these behaviors. Sequence
the idea that memory organization has an inuence on intrusion memory scores showed that participants in the unconscious
frequency. thought condition, more so than in the conscious thought condi-
In another study using an analogue trauma design, participants tion, recalled the behaviors in the order according to the over-
were shown a lm with trafc accidents, and then received a cued- arching traits rather than in the order in which they were
recognition memory test about the lm. The items were presented presented. That is, memory appeared to be more conceptually
in chronological order (Krans, Nring, Becker, & Holmes, 2009). organized after unconscious thought than after conscious thought.
Participants then reported the recurrence of intrusive images of the Unconscious thought theoretically has the potential to organize
lm in a diary during the following week. After one week they traumatic information in a more conceptual way than dealing with
returned and completed a cued-recall test for the lm. The results many overwhelming details of the event by thinking it over
showed that intrusion frequency was lower for the part of the lm consciously. Having a more structured memory of an event may
for which they had received the recognition memory test. reduce the negative effects of trauma (Ehlers & Clark, 2000).
Furthermore, distress associated with the intrusive images showed Although there have been efforts at nding conscious strategies to
a trend towards being lower for that part of the lm. This suggests aid the integration of information in memory (e.g. Michael & Ehlers,
that imposing a chronological structure to a traumatic event could 2007), this requires one to consciously revisit the negative events.
reduce the occurrence of intrusions and possibly distress associated Unconscious thought potentially offers a way to aid the integration
with the intrusions. of information in memory without consciously focusing on the
The studies described above are part of a recent surge in efforts negative event. Krans and Bos (2012) tested whether unconscious
to discover effective experimental interventions to prevent the thought after a trauma lm would be successful at reducing
initial development of intrusions. There is, however, still a lack of intrusion frequency. Participants were shown a negative lm clip
studies targeting memory organization as a possible benecial known for inducing intrusive images and were then given the
factor. Interestingly, organization of new information in memory instruction to think about the lm before a memory test (conscious
has been described by Unconscious Thought Theory (UTT; thought condition), or performed an unrelated cognitively
Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006). UTT distinguishes between demanding task, which prevented conscious processing. Of the
conscious and unconscious thought, where conscious thought participants who received this distraction task, half were told that
processes are dened as schematic and top-down thinking about the experiment was over (mere distraction condition), whereas the
an object or task using conscious attention allocated to that task or other half were told that a memory test of the lm would be pre-
object. It is thought to be of limited capacity, meaning that only few sented afterwards (unconscious thought condition). Comparing the
chunks of information can be handled simultaneously (Dijksterhuis latter two conditions shows that unconscious thought is a goal-
& Nordgren, 2006). Unconscious thought processes, on the other dependent process.2 After the memory test (presented in all
hand, are dened as thought processes that operate while conditions), participants reported their intrusive images of the lm
conscious attention is directed elsewhere. For example, if people during an intrusion provocation task. This involved presenting the
are faced with a problem that requires a large amount of infor- participants with still frames from the lm that did not depict
mation to be processed, they are often advised to sleep on it (Bos, anything negative. After this presentation, participants monitored
Dijksterhuis, & Van Baaren, 2011; Dijksterhuis, 2004; Dijksterhuis,
Bos, Nordgren, & van Baaren, 2006a, 2006b, 2006c). This mode of
thought is suggested to be of high capacity and using a bottom-up 2
When participants are told the task is over, they no longer have a goal to
approach (Bos & Dijksterhuis, 2011). Importantly, unconscious process information, whereas telling the participants that the experiment will
thought is assumed to be a goal-dependent process, which distin- continue after the distraction task introduces the goal to unconsciously process the
guishes it from a mere distraction effect. This assumption has been information (Bos et al., 2008).
J. Krans et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 179e185 181

their thoughts for 2 min and pressed a button every time they week after the experiment. Complete data were collected from 78
experienced an intrusive image of the lm. Intrusion frequency was participants.3
signicantly lower in the unconscious thought condition than in
both other conditions. However, the free recall memory task that 2.2. Negative lm clip
was designed to measure memory organization for the lm did not
show any differences between the groups. Participants viewed an 8.42 min scene from the lm Irrversible
To summarize, unconscious thought decreased intrusion by Gaspar No (2002). The lm clip shows a violent murder in
frequency as expected, but the proposed mechanism by which this a nightclub. Participants viewed the lm on a PC with a 17 inch
was achieved remained unclear. The particular memory task may monitor and listened to the sound through headphones.
not have been sensitive enough to pick up differences in memory
organization between the three conditions. Another limitation in 2.3. Experimental conditions
the Krans and Bos (2012) study was that only intrusion frequency
was assessed. In the present study, we aimed to (a) replicate the In the conscious thought condition (CT), participants were
effect of unconscious thought, (b) measure qualities of the intrusive instructed to think about the lm for 4 min and were informed that
images to compare between conditions, and (c) measure memory they would receive a memory test afterwards. In the unconscious
organization with a different measure. thought condition (UT), participants were also informed about the
In order to address our research goals, participants were shown memory test but they were instructed to rst complete a 2-back
a negative lm clip and were then randomly assigned to one of task for 4 min (Jonides et al., 1997; Kirchner, 1958; Krans & Bos,
three conditions as in Krans and Bos (2012): conscious thought 2012). In the 2-back task, participants see a series of numbers
(thinking about the lm before a memory test), unconscious presented on the screen in rapid succession (1 s presentation time,
thought (performing a cognitively demanding task before the 0.5 s interval). Whenever the number they saw on the screen was
memory test), or mere distraction (performing the task whilst the same as two numbers before, participants were required to
being unaware of the upcoming memory test). Participants press the spacebar on the computer. This task was chosen as this is
completed a structured memory test and intrusion frequency, a highly demanding task leaving very little opportunity for
vividness, nowness and arousal were measured. conscious thinking about the lm. Participants in the mere
Based on models of PTSD (e.g., Ehlers & Clark, 2000), UTT distraction condition (MD) were informed that the 2-back task was
(Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006), we predicted that (a) intrusion the end of the experiment and that they would not be questioned
frequency would be lowest in the unconscious thought condition, about the lm.
with no differences between the two other conditions, and (b) that
memory organization would be higher in the unconscious thought 2.4. Randomization, manipulation, compliance, and demand
condition than in the other two conditions. In relation to the
intrusion qualities (vividness, nowness, and arousal), we predicted Individual differences in trait anxiety were measured with the
that (c) all would be lower in the unconscious thought condition Dutch trait version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-T; Van
than in the other two conditions. This prediction was based on der Ploeg, 1980). This questionnaire contains 20 items to which
models from the PTSD (e.g., Ehlers & Clark, 2000) and memory participants respond on a scale from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost
literature (Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000) proposing that- by always). Testeretest reliability (0.75) and internal consistency
denition- memory representations that are processed on a more (a 0.85) are good (Van der Ploeg, 1980).
conceptual level will be less vivid, arousing and here-and-now than To measure the emotional impact of the lm, a mood ques-
memory representations at the lowest sensory levels (i.e., tionnaire (MoodQ; Holmes, Brewin, & Hennessy, 2004; Krans,
intrusions). Nring, Speckens, & Becker, 2011) and the Dutch state version of
the State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI-S; Spielberger, Gorsuch,
Lushene, Vagg, & Jacobs, 1983; Van der Ploeg, 1980) were used.
2. Method
The MoodQ consists of ve items about current happiness, fear,
horror, depression and anger to which participants respond on
Approval for this study was obtained from the Ethical
a scale from 0 (not at all) to 10 (extremely). The STAI-S contains 20
Committee of the Behavioural Science Institute (BSI) of the Rad-
items and participants respond on a scale from 1 (not at all) to 4
boud University Nijmegen (number ECG 06042009), where data
(very much so). Testeretest reliability for the STAI-S has been re-
was collected.
ported at a 0.25, internal consistency at a 0.85 (Van der Ploeg,
1980).
2.1. Participants Participants rated their attention for the lm on an 11-point
scale from 0 (not at all) to 10 (completely). In addition, partici-
Participants were undergraduate students from the Radboud pants in all conditions were asked to indicate how much they had
University Nijmegen. They were recruited through an online thought about the lm during the experimental task (continuous
participation system and were rewarded with course credit. For scale from not at all to very much). Participants in the UT and MD
ethical reasons, participants were informed beforehand of the conditions were additionally asked whether their thinking of the
exclusion criteria, that they would be watching a negative lm clip, lm was deliberate or spontaneous. Lastly, participants indicated
and that they could terminate the study at any time without whether they expected their task to increase, decrease or have no
consequence for their credit. Exclusion criteria were as follows: effect on their memory and intrusions of the lm.
PTSD (current and lifetime), major depression (current and life-
time), psychotic episodes (current and lifetime), panic attacks 2.5. Experimental measures
(current), panic disorder (current), intoxication during or shortly
prior to testing, history of fainting, and having to take exams in the 2.5.1. Intrusion frequency
The intrusion provocation procedure (Lang, Moulds, & Holmes,
2009; Michael, Ehlers, Halligan, & Clark, 2005) was used to elicit
3
Data from 18 additional participants was lost due to a technical error. intrusions from the lm clip. Participants were presented with ten
182 J. Krans et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 179e185

still pictures from the lm representing neutral scenes that conditions. Demographic information was obtained and partici-
preceded the negative events in the lm. The pictures were pre- pants continued on the STAI-T, STAI-S, and MoodQ, after which they
sented as a slideshow with a presentation time of 4 s per picture. were shown the lm. They were instructed to completely attend to
Intrusion frequency was measured with an adapted version of the the lm as if they were a bystander and not to look away or
widely used Intrusion subscale of the Impact of Events scale (IES; disengage in any way. After the lm, the experimental instructions
Horowitz, Wilner, & Alvarez, 1980). This subscale has a good (CT, UT, MD) were given randomly with the restriction that an
internal consistency (a 0.86) and good testeretest reliability identical number of participants were allocated to each condition.
(a 0.89) (Sundin & Horowitz, 2002). The 8 items were reworded The recall task was then administered, followed by the STAI-S and
to refer to the lm and participants responded on a scale from MoodQ. Participants then completed the intrusion provocation
0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Participants were instructed to rate task, the IQ, compliance and demand questions, and IES Intrusion
the items in terms of their appropriateness during the course of the subscale. The interval between the lm and the IES administration
present study (rather than during the last seven days as in the was approximately 12 min, depending on individual speed. Finally,
original version). they were thanked for their participation and debriefed.

2.5.2. Intrusion characteristics 3. Results


Intrusion characteristics were measured with an adapted
version of the Intrusion Questionnaire (IQ; Hackmann, Ehlers, 3.1. Outliers and statistical approach
Clark, & Speckens, 2004; Michael et al., 2005). Four items
assessed the mean vividness of the intrusions that participants had Outliers were detected according to the procedure described by
experienced during the provocation task. Participants responded Tabachnick and Fidell (1996). Across and within groups, Mahala-
on a 1 (vague and dim) to 4 (perfectly clear and real) scale, origi- nobis distances were calculated to detect multivariate outliers, but
nally derived from the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire none were found. Univariate outliers were checked across and
(VVIQ; Marks, 1973). Four items assessed the experience of within groups and three were detected. These were changed into
nowness of the intrusions, i.e., a sense of nowness accompanied by the rst unit that was not a statistical outlier based on three stan-
the original emotions as during encoding of the lm. Five items dard deviations from the mean.
measured physical and emotional arousal accompanying the Directional hypotheses were tested one-tailed using Helmert
intrusions (based on the Hyperarousal subscale of the IES-Revised; contrasts. Wherever one-tailed tests were used, this is mentioned
Weiss & Marmar, 1996). These items were answered using a 1 explicitly. In case of non-signicant or opposite effects two-tailed
(completely disagree) to 4 (completely agree) scale. values are reported. The level of signicance was an a of 0.05 for
all tests; multiple comparisons were Bonferroni corrected.
2.5.3. Memory organization
In the Krans and Bos (2012) study, participants were asked to 3.2. Compliance and demand
say out loud everything they remembered about the lm. This was
audio-recorded and later scored for sequence memory. However, In order to be included in the analysis, participants in the CT
no differences were found between the experimental conditions. condition were required to think about the lm at least half of the
Upon further inspection of the data it appeared that only the rst, time during the conscious thought task (Krans & Bos, 2012). Eight
last, and most salient events from the lm were mentioned by participants were excluded from the dataset because they did not
almost all participants with big gaps in between. In the present meet this criterion. To be included in the analysis, participants in
study therefore a structured recall task was used in order to direct the UT and MD conditions were not allowed to have thought about
participants towards recalling all events they could. Participants the lm deliberately during the 2-back task (Krans & Bos, 2012).
were instructed to list all the events in the lm within given rows Based on this criterion, 11 participants from the UT condition and
(one event per row). An example of an event was given to provide eight participants from the MD condition were excluded from
an illustration of the amount of detail that was expected: If you had analysis. The nal dataset included 51 participants; 17 in each
watched a lm clip about a trafc accident, the events could, for condition.
example, be: a car is driving through a red light e a child is waiting to Overall, attention for the lm was rated as high, M 8.82,
cross at the trafc light e the medical staff carry a woman into the SD 1.35; with no signicant difference between conditions
ambulance. First, the order of events as it was recorded in the (p 0.76). Performance on the 2-back task was comparable
participants list was coded in comparison to the order of events in between the UT and MD condition (p 0.93), M 109.15, SD 5.94.
a previously constructed master list (Krans & Bos, 2012). Next, Participants intrusion/sequence memory scores did not depend on
a sequence memory score was calculated by computing a Spearman whether they expected their experimental task to increase,
correlation between the participants list and the master list (see decrease or not affect their intrusions/memory of the lm; for
Jelinek, Randjbar, Seifert, Kellner, & Moritz, 2009; Kindt & van den intrusions: F(2, 48) 0.52, p 0.60, or for sequence memory: F(2,
Hout, 2003; Kindt, van den Hout, & Buck, 2005; Krans & Bos, 2012; 48) 0.73, p 0.49.
Wegner, Quillian, & Houston, 1996).
3.3. Randomization check
2.6. Procedure
There was no signicant difference in participant age between
The total experiment took approximately 40 min depending on conditions (p 0.23), M 19.76, SD 1.69. Distribution of men and
individual speed. All measures were presented on a PC using women was comparable across conditions, p 0.21; with 47
Macromedia Authorware software (version 7.01). Potential partic- women and 4 men in total. There was no signicant difference in
ipants were rst screened for exclusion criteria. If they could trait anxiety (STAI-T) between conditions (p 0.98), overall
continue in the study they received written information and signed M 35.71, SD 7.96. The conditions were comparable on pre-lm
an informed consent form. The experiment started with instruc- state anxiety (STAI-S; p 0.71), overall M 40.29, SD 5.30; and
tions on the 2-back task and practice trials. To avoid an experi- pre-lm negative mood (MoodQ; p 0.35), overall M 12.92,
mental confound, these instructions were provided in all SD 4.87.
J. Krans et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 179e185 183

3.4. Manipulation check 4. Discussion

Change in state anxiety from pre- to post lm was tested with In the present study, we aimed to (a) replicate the effect of
a mixed model repeated measures ANOVA, with time (pre-lm, unconscious thought on intrusion frequency, (b) extend earlier
post-lm) as the within-subjects factor, condition (CT, UT, MD) as research by including intrusion qualities, and (c) test the organi-
the between-subjects factor, and state anxiety (STAI-S scores) as zation of the trauma lm in memory with a structured recall test.
the dependent variable. There was a predicted signicant effect of The main nding was that participants who were given an
time, F(1, 48) 145.60, p < 0.001, f 1.74, indicating an overall unconscious thought instruction after viewing the trauma lm
signicant increase in state anxiety from pre- to post-lm. The reported a reduction in intrusive experiences compared to partic-
main effect of condition was not signicant, F(2, 48) 1.62, p 0.21. ipants who were instructed to think about the lm deliberately, or
There was a signicant time  condition interaction, F(2, participants in the distraction control condition. This nding
48) 5.48, p 0.01, f 0.48. Pairwise comparisons of the anxiety replicates our earlier work (Krans & Bos, 2012) and supports the
change scores (post minus pre) showed that the increase in state goal-dependent asset of unconscious thought as opposed to it
anxiety was signicantly larger in the CT than in the UT condition, being a mere distraction effect (Bos et al., 2008, 2011). This time, we
p 0.01, and MD, p 0.03; whereas the latter two conditions did included a widely used and well validated measure of intrusive
not differ signicantly from each other, p > 0.99. experience, namely the Impact of Event Scale (Horowitz et al.,
The same pattern emerged for negative mood (MoodQ): there 1980). Thus, the effect of unconscious thought reducing intrusion
was a signicant main effect of time (pre- to post-lm), F(1, frequency has now been found using two difference intrusion
48) 247.25, p < 0.001, f 2.27; but no signicant main effect of measures, which increases the validity of the effect.
condition (CT, UT, MD), F(2, 48) 0.93, p 0.40. The Contrary to expectations, we did not nd an effect of uncon-
time  condition interaction effect was signicant, F(2, 48) 3.51, scious thought on intrusion qualities. If anything, it appeared that
p 0.04, f 0.38, though pairwise comparisons on the change consciously thinking about the lm increased intrusion nowness
scores of negative mood (post-lm minus pre-lm) did not reach and arousal. This is in line with the nding that state anxiety and
signicance. The descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. negative mood increased to a larger extend in the conscious
thought condition than in the other two conditions. Although this
3.5. Experimental effects was not in line with our expectations, this opens up an interesting
possibility that can be addressed in future studies. Conscious
3.5.1. Intrusion frequency and quality thought is better at encoding details than at processing the
As predicted, Helmert contrasts showed that scores on the conceptual meaning of new information (Dijksterhuis & Nordgren,
intrusion subscale of the IES were signicantly lower in the UT 2006). Intrusive images, by denition, consist mostly of sensory
condition compared to the other two conditions, p < 0.05 (one- impressions rather than more abstract representations. Thus, more
tailed). There was no signicant difference between the CT and MD conscious thought may have resulted in more arousing and here-
conditions, p 0.45 (two-tailed). and-now intrusions because of enhanced sensory encoding.
Contrary to prediction, Helmert contrasts showed no signicant As in Krans and Bos (2012), we were unable to nd differences in
difference between the UT condition and the CT and MD condition sequence memory between the three conditions despite using
on intrusion vividness, nowness or arousal, (all ps > 0.12, two- a structured recall test to measure memory organization for the
tailed). However, there was a signicant difference between the lm. According to PTSD models (Brewin et al., 1996; Ehlers & Clark,
CT and MD condition on nowness and arousal (all ps  0.02, two- 2000), intrusions occur when the trauma information is disorga-
tailed). Follow-up contrasts (two-tailed) showed a signicantly nized and not well integrated into memory. As unconscious
higher feeling of nowness in the CT than in the UT and MD, p 0.02, thought is suggested to be superior in organizing information in
and a higher arousal level in the CT condition compared to the MD a conceptual way (Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006) this should have
condition, p 0.05, but not compared to the UT condition, p .18. been detected in both the intrusion and recall measure. One
possibility is that the recall measure that was used here was still
3.5.2. Sequence memory insensitive to differences in sequence memory. Another possibility
Contrary to prediction, Helmert contrasts showed no signicant is that sequence memory is not a good measure of the conceptu-
difference on sequence memory between the UT condition and the alization of the lm information. For example, asking about the
CT and MD conditions (p 0.82, two-tailed). There was no signif- meaning or general gist of the lm may have resulted in differences
icant difference between the CT and MD conditions either (p 0.88, between groups. According to UTT, unconscious thought would
two-tailed). lead to a more global assessment of meaning (e.g., vengeance)

Table 1
Means and standard deviations for control and experimental variables within and across conditions.

Conscious thought Unconscious thought Mere Distraction Total

M SD M SD M SD M SD
STAI-T 36.06 9.58 35.53 6.72 35.53 7.80 35.71 7.96
STAI-S Pre-lm 39.59 3.78 40.18 6.26 41.12 5.75 40.29 5.30
Post-lm 55.76 5.54 49.24 7.64 50.88 5.98 51.96 6.91
MoodQ Pre-lm 11.82 3.88 12.71 4.83 14.24 5.70 12.92 4.87
Post-lm 34.88 5.84 29.59 9.36 30.24 7.01 31.57 7.68
Attention 9.00 1.37 8.82 1.29 8.65 1.46 8.82 1.35
Sequence memory 0.81 0.37 0.77 0.37 0.79 0.36 0.79 0.36
Intrusion vividness (IQ) 5.06 3.31 3.94 2.28 3.29 2.59 4.10 2.80
Intrusion nowness (IQ) 9.29 1.57 7.47 1.88 7.41 2.24 8.06 2.07
Intrusion arousal (IQ) 12.12 2.26 10.59 2.81 10.12 1.73 10.94 2.42
Post-lm intrusions (IES) 9.29 6.47 5.87 3.63 7.88 5.73 7.68 5.49
Post-lm avoidance (IES) 10.33 7.89 9.59 5.50 11.59 4.49 10.50 6.06
184 J. Krans et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 179e185

whereas conscious thought should lead to more detailed informa- Acknowledgements


tion (e.g., a man assaults another man in a bar). This is another
testable hypothesis that can instigate future research. We would like to thank Gosia Fietz, Imke Veenstra, and Karin
Another necessary avenue for future research is to test the effect of van Roosmalen for their help with data collection. The data was
unconscious thought in comparison to different forms of conscious collected at the Radboud University Nijmegen in the Netherlands
thought. In the rumination literature, for example, a distinction is with support of the Behavioural Science Institute.
made between abstract and concrete thought (Watkins, 2008).
Whereas abstract thought is hypothesized to increase intrusions
References
through ruminative processes, concrete thought may work to coun-
teract cognitive avoidance and should enhance emotional processing Acker, F. (2008). New ndings on unconscious versus conscious thought in decision
(Borkovec, Alcaine, & Behar, 2004). Several studies have compared making: additional empirical data and meta-analysis. Judgment and Decision
Making, 3, 292e303.
these forms of conscious thought but none included an unconscious
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
thought condition (e.g., Williams & Moulds, 2007; Zetsche et al., disorders (4th ed. Text Revision). Washington, D.C.: APA.
2009). The present study only included a general conscious Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O. M., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance theory of worry and
thought condition thus this comparison still open for investigation. generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G. Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.),
Generalized anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp. 77e108).
Our study has several limitations. First, the sample consisted of New York, NY: Guilford.
healthy students and a lm was used as an analogue traumatic event. Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., & Van Baaren, R. B. (2008). On the goal-dependency
Therefore, the results may not generalize to other populations or of unconscious thought. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1114e
1120.
real-life events such as a trauma. The implications of this study are Bos, M. W., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). Unconscious thought works bottom-up and
limited to the phenomenon of intrusions and do not inform about conscious thought works top-down when forming an impression. Social
other PTSD symptoms or, for example, biological markers of trauma. Cognition, 29, 727e737.
Bos, M. W., Dijksterhuis, A., & Van Baaren, R. B. (2011). The benets of sleeping on
Second, we did not include a natural group that was not instructed things: unconscious thought leads to automatic weighting. Journal of Consumer
to think in a certain way about the trauma lm or was immediately Psychology, 21(1), 4e8.
distracted. Theoretically, it is possible that all three conditions had Brewin, C. R., Dalgleish, T., & Joseph, S. (1996). A dual representation theory of post-
traumatic stress disorder. Psychological Review, 103, 670e686.
an effect on intrusion development compared to a more natural state Butler, G., Wells, A., & Dewick, H. (1995). Differential effects of worry and imagery
of processing. It can also not be ascertained how other natural after exposure to a stressful stimulus: a pilot study. Behavioural and Cognitive
behaviors, such as avoidance, rumination, or spontaneous conscious Psychotherapy, 23, 45e56.
Calvillo, D. P., & Penaloza, A. (2009). Are complex decisions better left to the
or unconscious thought processes would interact with our manip-
unconscious? Further failed replications of the deliberation-without-attention
ulations. However, it is difcult to come up with a natural condition effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 509e517.
that is matched on all methodological aspects, e.g., the 4 min delay to Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobio-
recall that is present in the three experimental conditions of this graphical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107,
261e288.
study. Third, intrusive experiences were measured immediately Deeprose, C., Zhang, S., DeJong, H., Dalgleish, T., & Holmes, E. A. (2012). Imagery in
after the lm. Future studies are needed that test the longer-term the aftermath of viewing a traumatic lm: using cognitive tasks to modulate
effect of the different modes of thought. Additionally, the measure the development of involuntary memory. Journal of Behavior Therapy Experi-
mental Psychiatry, 43, 758e764.
used (IES Intrusion scale) is retrospective in nature and is therefore Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: the merits of unconscious thought in
sensitive to memory biases. Next, as in Krans and Bos (2012) a fair preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social
number of participants had to be excluded because they did not Psychology, 87, 586e598.
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006a). On making
fulll the task criteria. However, there is no easy solution for this the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005e
problem as instructing participants not to think about the lm could 1007.
result in unwanted suppression effects (Krans & Bos, 2012; Wegner, Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006b). Complex
choices better made unconsciously? Science, 313, 760e761.
1994). Most trauma victims likely try not to think about their trauma,
Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006c). Making
and engage in distracting activities. A strength of the unconscious choices without deliberation. Science, 312, 1472.
thought approach is that, in fact, participants are not required to Dijksterhuis, A., & Meurs, T. (2006). Where creativity resides: the generative power
of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 135e146.
think about the trauma. Instead, it is made explicit that the event
Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought.
will be revisited at some time in the future. This is clearly different Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95e109.
from any existing situation so far. Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2000). A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 319e345.
Ehring, T., Szeimies, A. K., & Schaffrick, C. (2009). An experimental analogue study
4.1. Conclusions intro the role of abstract thinking in trauma-related rumination. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 47, 285e293.
To conclude, the present study replicated the effect that uncon- Gonzalez-Vallejo, C., Lassiter, D. G., Bellezza, L. S., & Lindberg, M. J. (2008). Save
angels perhaps: a critical examination of unconscious thought theory and the
scious thought after a trauma lm reduces intrusion frequency. We deliberation-without-attention effect. Review of General Psychology, 12, 282e
extend previous ndings by having a widely used measure for intru- 296.
sions (the IES) and by incorporating a measure of intrusion quality. As Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., Clark, D. M., & Speckens, A. (2004). Characteristics of
intrusive memories in PTSD and their response to treatment. Journal of Trau-
UTT is a very recent theory, several questions about the underlying matic Stress, 17, 231e240.
mechanisms of the effects remain unanswered here. We suggested Ham, J., Van den Bos, K., & Van Doorn, E. A. (2009). Lady justice thinks uncon-
several areas of future research that can help extend our knowledge in sciously: unconscious thought can lead to more accurate justice judgments.
Social Cognition, 27, 509e521.
this eld. Additionally, if future research can conrm the effect of
Holmes, E. A., Brewin, C. R., & Hennessy, R. G. (2004). Trauma lms, information
unconscious thought on intrusion frequency when more time has processing, and intrusive memory development. Journal of Experimental
passed between the trauma and the intervention, then results of the Psychology: General, 133, 3e22.
Holmes, E. A., James, E. L., Coode-Bate, T., & Deeprose, C. (2009). Can playing the
presented study may proof to be clinically promising as well.
computer game Tetris reduce the build-up of ashbacks for trauma? A
proposal from cognitive science. PLoS ONE, 4(1), e4153.
Funding Holmes, E. A., James, E. L., Kilford, E. J., & Deeprose, C. (2010). Key steps in devel-
oping a cognitive vaccine against traumatic ashbacks: visuospatial Tetris
versus pub quiz. PLoS ONE, 5(11), e13706.
Julie Krans is currently supported by a Rubicon fellowship from Horowitz, M. J., Wilner, N., & Alvarez, W. (1980). Impact of event scale: a measure of
the Netherlands Organisation for Scientic Research. subjective stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 209e218.
J. Krans et al. / J. Behav. Ther. & Exp. Psychiat. 44 (2013) 179e185 185

Jelinek, L., Randjbar, S., Seifert, D., Kellner, M., & Moritz, S. (2009). The organization Payne, J. W., Samper, A., Bettman, J. R., & Luce, M. F. (2008). Boundary on uncon-
of autobiographical memory and nonautobiographical memory in post- scious thought in complex decision making. Psychological Science, 19, 1118e
traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 288e298. 1123.
Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E. E., Lauber, E. J., Awh, E., Minoshima, S., et al. Sijbrandij, M., Olff, M., Reitsma, J. B., Carlier, I. V. E., de Vries, M. H., & Gersons, B. P. R.
(1997). Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation as (2007). Treatment of acute stress disorder with brief cognitive behavioural
measured by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 462e475. therapy: a randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 82e
Kindt, M., & van den Hout, M. (2003). Dissociation and memory fragmentation: 90.
experimental effects on meta-memory but not on actual memory performance. Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, P. R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs, A. G. (1983).
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 167e178. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists
Kindt, M., van den Hout, M., & Buck, N. (2005). Dissociation related to subjective Press, Inc.
memory fragmentation and intrusions but not to objective memory distur- Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Sjoerdsma, A., van Baaren, R. B., &
bances. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 36, 43e59. Nordgren, L. F. (2011). A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Social
Kirchner, W. K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing Cognition, 29, 738e762.
information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 352e358. Sundin, E. C., & Horowitz, M. J. (2002). Impact of event scale: psychometric prop-
Krans, J., & Bos, M. W. (2012). To think or not to think about trauma? An experi- erties. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 205e209.
mental investigation into unconscious thought and intrusion development. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New
Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, . York: Harper Collins.
Krans, J., Nring, G., Holmes, E. A., & Becker, E. S. (2009). Tell me more: can Thorsteinson, T. J., & Withrow, S. (2009). Does unconscious thought outperform
a memory test reduce analogue traumatic intrusions? Behaviour Research and conscious thought on complex decisions? A further examination. Judgment and
Therapy, 47, 426e430. Decision Making, 4, 235e247.
Krans, J., Nring, G., Speckens, A., & Becker, E. S. (2011). Eyewitness or earwitness: Van der Ploeg, H. M. (1980). Validity of the Zelf-Beoordelings-Vragenlijst. A Dutch
the role of mental imagery in intrusion development. International Journal of version of the Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory. Nederlands Tijdschrift
Cognitive Therapy, 4, 154e164. Voor De Psychologie En Haar Grensgebieden, 35, 243e249.
Lang, T. J., Moulds, M. M., & Holmes, E. A. (2009). Reducing depressive intrusions via Waroquier, L., Marchiori, D., Klein, O., & Cleeremans, A. (2009). Methodological
a computerized cognitive bias modication of appraisals task: developing pitfalls of the unconscious thought paradigm. Judgment and Decision Making, 4,
a cognitive vaccine. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 139e145. 610.
Lerouge, D. (2009). Evaluating the benets of distraction on product evaluations: Watkins, E. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psycho-
the mindset effect. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 367e379. logical Bulletin, 134, 163e206.
Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. British Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101,
Journal of Psychology, 64, 17e24. 34e52.
Michael, T., & Ehlers, A. (2007). Enhanced perceptual priming for neutral stimuli Wegner, D. M., Quillian, F., & Houston, C. E. (1996). Memories out of order: thought
occurring in a traumatic context: two experimental investigations. Behaviour suppression and the disturbance of sequence memory. Journal of Personality and
Research and Therapy, 45, 341e358. Social Psychology, 71, 680e691.
Michael, T., Ehlers, A., Halligan, S. L., & Clark, D. M. (2005). Unwanted memories of Weiss, D. S., & Marmar, C. R. (1996). The impact of event scale-revised. In
assault: what intrusion characteristics are associated with PTSD? Behaviour J. P. Wilson, & T. M. Keane (Eds.), Assessing psychological trauma and PTSD (pp.
Research and Therapy, 43, 613e628. 399e411). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. (2005). Post-traumatic stress disorder Wells, A., & Papageorgiou, C. (1995). Worry and the incubation of intrusive images
(PTSD): The management of PTSD in adults and children in primary and secondary care following stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 579e583.
(No. CG026). London National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. Williams, A. D., & Moulds, M. L. (2007). The impact of ruminative processing on the
Newby, J. M., & Moulds, M. L. (2011). Characteristics of intrusive memories in development of intrusive memories. Behaviour Change, 24, 55e69.
a community sample of depressed, recovered depressed and never-depressed Woud, M. L., Holmes, E. A., Postma, P., Dalgleish, T., & Mackintosh, B. (2011).
individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 234e243. Ameliorating intrusive memories of distressing experiences using computer-
Newell, B. R., Wong, K. Y., Cheung, J. C. H., & Rakow, T. (2009). Think, blink or sleep ized reappraisal training. Emotion, . http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024992.
on it? The impact of modes of thought on complex decision making. Quarterly Zetsche, U., Ehring, T., & Ehlers, A. (2009). The effects of rumination on mood and
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 707e732. intrusive memories after exposure to traumatic material: an experimental
(Director) No, G. (2002). Irreversible [motion picture]. France: Mars Distribution. study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 40, 499e514.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen