Sie sind auf Seite 1von 17

Exceptional Children

Vol. 73, No. 4, pp. 417-433.


2007 Council for Exceptional Children.

The Real-World Effectiveness


of Early Teaching Interventions
for Children With Autism
Spectrum Disorder
PHIL REED
LISA A. OSBORNE
Swansea University

MARK CORNESS
Children, Young People, and Families Directorate, Oxford

ABSTRACT: The effectiveness of 3 early teaching interventions (applied behavior analysis [ABA],

special nursery placement, and portage) for children with autism spectrum disorder was studied
in a community-based sample over 10 months. Measures of autism severity as well as intellectual,
educational, and adaptive behavioral function were administered. In contrast to reports in some
previous research (Lovaas, 1987), there was no evidence of recovery from autism. Children in the
ABA condition made greater intellectual and educational gains than children in the portage
program. They also made greater educational gains than students in the nursery program. Fur-
thermore, the nursery program produced larger gains than the portage program in adaptive
functioning.

E ducationally based programs


are often the only form of in-
tervention given to children
with autism spectrum disorder
(ASD) and are often the only
form of support that their families receive (e.g.,
see Howlin & Moore, 1997; Osborne & Reed, in
press, for surveys). Often these interventions are
offered early in the childs development, and
and perceived importance of educational ap-
proaches to the management of ASD, there are
relatively few quantitative investigations of the
comparative efficacy of such programs.
A number of early-teaching interventions
have been suggested as offering benefit to some
children with ASD. Applied behavior analysis
(ABA) approaches have been widely investigated
(e.g., Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, 2002;
there is a perception that interventions offered Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, & Stanislaw,
early are more effective than those offered later 2005; Lovaas, 1987; Smith, Annette, & Wynn,
(Lovaas, 1987). However, despite the ubiquity 2000). In addition, special nursery placements

Exceptional Children 417

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


(see Charman, Howlin, Berry, & Prince, 2004; Howard et al. (2005) compared two groups of
Gabriels, Hill, Pierce, Rogers, & Wehner, 2001), children with ASD undergoing either an intensive
floortime (Greenspan & Wieder, 1997), and program in an autism special school or a less in-
portage (e.g., Smith, 2000) have been offered as tensive program in a local community special ed-
approaches to managing the problems associated ucation classroom. In neither case was there any
with ASD. Some of these latter approaches have change in a variety of assessments of intellectual
the benefit of being reasonably cost effective, but functioning. Lovaas (1987) found no improve-
they suffer from the problem of having a scant ev- ment for children undergoing generic educational
idence base with respect to their effectiveness in interventions over a 3-year period. Charman et al.
promoting the childs intellectual, educational, (2004) found no improvement in adaptive behav-
and social functioning. ior scores as measured by the Vineland Adaptive
The effectiveness of placement in specialist Behaviour Scale (see also Eikeseth et al., 2002).
nursery provision has been studied, often as con- Unfortunately, the usefulness of many of
trol conditions in the assessment of ABA pro- these studies in assessing the impact of special ed-
grams. Such programs are often referred to in this ucational provision on children with ASD is lim-
literature as generic (e.g., Lovaas, 1987) and/or ited by the fact that many of these groups are
eclectic (e.g., Howard et al., 2005) programs. derived from archive records (e.g., Gabriels et al.,
This is typically because such approaches do not 2001; Sheinkopf & Siegel, 1998), rather than
follow a particular brand name approach to ed- being studied contemporaneously with the other
ucation, and they offer a variety of teaching meth- children in the study. Additionally, many of these
ods (e.g., picture exchange systems, activities studies employ different measurements both
drawn from the TEACCH [Treatment and Edu- across participants and at baseline and follow up
cation of Autistic and Related Communication (e.g., Howard et al., 2005; Lovaas, 1987), thus,
Handicapped Children] model, circle time, social
reducing the reliability of the data (see Magiati &
stories) that do not necessarily adhere to a single
Howlin, 2001). There are few studies of special
underlying philosophy, as do ABA approaches.
educational placement that are prospective in na-
The results from these studies of special nursery
ture and that use the same tests at baseline and
placements have not been particularly encourag-
follow up, which was the first aim of the current
ing. Three studies have shown small improve-
study.
ments in intellectual functioning following special
educational placement. Sheinkopf and Siegel
(1998) found a 2-point increase in IQ across a There are few studies of special educational
year in children in special school provision, and
Eikeseth et al. (2002) found a 4-point improve-
placement that are prospective in nature
ment in IQ over a year (using one-to-one tech- and that use the same tests at baseline and
niques such as TEACCH and sensory-integration follow up, which was the first aim of the
therapy). However, there was no improvement in current study.
adaptive behavior (as measured by the Vineland
Adaptive Behaviour Scale). Gabriels et al. (2001),
in a retrospective, 3-year study of children with Portage is an approach offered by numerous
ASD undergoing an eclectic approach to inter- local education authorities in the United King-
vention, found a 5-point increase in IQ over 3 dom for children with ASD. Portage is a home-
years. based, parent-administered early intervention for
In contrast, four studies have found no im- developmental disabilities. Smith (2000) provides
provement in functioning. Shallows and Graup- some data to suggest that portage programs can
ner (1999) found a 7-point decrease in IQ over a have an impact on social development of children
year. The remaining three studies in this latter with autism, and Reed, Osborne, and Gibson
group found no overall change in the standard- (2000) discuss some possible developments and
ized score of children in special educational place- limitations of this approach. However, this ap-
ments with an eclectic approach to intervention. proach, although commonly used in practice, suf-

418 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


fers from a great paucity of empirical evidence re- eralize to interventions as they are typically used
garding its effectiveness. in the parents home, severely compromising the
Much current debate in educational litera- usefulness of the study. This issue has been high-
ture has centered on ABA techniques as an inter- lighted in a number of recent reports of home-
vention for ASD problems (see Connor, 1998; based ABA programs, which have questioned the
Mudford, Martin, Eikeseth, & Bibby, 2001). extent to which the results of clinic-based assess-
There are a number of different ABA approaches ments can be generalized to the type of ABA pro-
that have been outlined in a variety of sources gram that occurs in the community (Mudford et
(e.g., Greer, 1997; Koegel & Koegel, 2006; Lo- al.).
vaas, 1981; Lovaas & Smith, 1989; Sundberg & Although several of these critiques have been
Michael, 2001; Twyman, 1998). In general, these addressed in subsequent research, few, if any,
approaches share a number of common features: studies have addressed them in a single investiga-
(a) the one-to-one teaching of children with tion. For example, Eikeseth et al. (2002), and
autism by adult tutors; (b) a discrete-trial rein- Howard et al. (2005), have used community-
forcement-based method; and (c) an intensive based rather than clinic-based samples, but nei-
regime (up to 40 hr a week, for 3 years, in some ther used the same tests at baseline and at follow
instances). The initial results reported by Lovaas up. In light of this, a second aim of the current
(1987) concerning the effectiveness of such an ap- study was to assess the effectiveness of ABA ap-
proach were remarkable. The children undergoing proaches on a more typical sample of children
this approach made gains of up to 30 IQ points (a with ASD, using the same instruments at baseline
finding noted in some children with ASD under- and follow up.
going special educational programs, see Gabriels
et al., 2001), and just less than half of these chil-
dren appeared to recover, that is, they were not [A] second aim of the current study was
noticeably different from normally developing to assess the effectiveness of ABA approaches
children after 3 years of the intervention (a find-
ing yet to be replicated). on a more typical sample of children with
There have been a number of critiques of this ASD, using the same instruments at
original piece of research (i.e., Lovaas, 1987) that baseline and follow up.
have focused on problems both with the internal
and external validity of the study (e.g., Connor,
1998; Gresham & MacMillan, 1997; Mudford et Thus, there are a number of issues that are
al., 2001). In terms of the internal validity of the clearly unresolved concerning the effectiveness of
Lovass (1987) study, it should be noted that dif- educational interventions for ASD. It appears
ferent IQ tests were used at baseline and at follow critical to be able to assess the effectiveness of the
up to assess the childrens intellectual functioning, ABA approach in community-based, rather than
thus reducing the reliability of the measurement clinic-based, settings with participants more typi-
(Magiati & Howlin, 2001). This is not a unique cally representative of those who present to local
problem with ABA studies (see earlier text). In service providers (i.e., more severe symptoms, less
terms of the threats to the external validity of the linguistically able). It also appears important to
Lovaas (1987) study, the reliance on IQ as a sole utilize a wide range of instruments in the assess-
measure may be questioned, given that IQ is not ment procedure, not only to examine intellectual
necessarily the main problem in autism function- functioning, but also educational functioning,
ing. Second, the sample chosen for the study re- and adaptive behavior. Simultaneously providing
ported by Lovaas (1987) were verbal, relatively evidence on the effectiveness of other interven-
high-functioning participants, who may have per- tions than ABA would not only allow these inter-
formed equally well with any intervention of a ventions to be assessed, but also would allow a
reasonable input (such as some of those noted well-matched alternate-treatment control group
earlier). Finally, the study reported by Lovaas for the ABA studies, a comparison so far missing
(1987) was a clinic-based study and may not gen- (Hohmann & Shear, 2002).

Exceptional Children 419

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


Given the previous considerations, the cur- data; and one for highly discrepant GARS scores
rent study directly compared the impact of exist- at baseline and ceiling effects on follow up).
ing ABA, special nursery placements, and portage Assignment to a group was on the basis of
programs on a variety of aspects of the childrens the intervention being offered to the child in their
abilities. The latter two were selected because spe- particular area. For example, if a child was in an
cial nursery placement is a commonly occurring area that offered a special nursery placement, then
program offered to children with ASD, which has that child was assigned to that group. The areas
received little direct assessment in terms of its ef- involved in the study offered a similar socioeco-
fectiveness. Portage was chosen as, again, it is in- nomic profile, all being in southeast England.
creasingly offered to children with ASD (see Reed Thus, although the allocation to group was not
et al., 2000; Smith, 2000). The portage interven- truly random, the childs characteristics did not
tion also allows comparison of a very intensive influence group assignment, and the groups of-
intervention (ABA) with a less intensive interven- fered a well matched profile (see Results and Table
tion (portage) in a community-based setting. This 2). Ethical approval for the study (University Col-
comparison formed part of the original clinic- lege London Hospital Trust Ethics Committee)
based study conducted by Lovaas (1987), and the was granted based on this understanding.
current comparison allows assessment of the gen- In terms of the estimated sample size
eralization to a community-based sample. required for finding statistical effects, previous
studies have employed between 11 and 19 partici-
pants per group. Given the average effect size in
METHOD these studies of about 1.0, the minimum number
of participants for a study with high power (0.8)
P A R T I C I PA N T S would be approximately 8. Given this, the num-
Participants were selected on the basis of four cri- ber of participants per group (12 to 20) was
teria. They were (a) 2:6 (2 years 6 months) to 4:0 thought to be adequate in this study.
years old, (b) at the start of their first teaching in-
tervention, (c) receiving no other major interven- INTERVENTIONS
tion during the period of the assessment, and (d) Three early educational interventions for autism
they had an independent diagnosis of ASD made were studied (i.e., they were not part of a specially
by specialist pediatricians following initial referral organized trial)applied behavior analysis
from a general medical practitioner. In addition (ABA), special nursery placements, and portage.
to this independent diagnosis, several of the scales These interventions were selected as they repre-
that were administered allowed further indepen- sent some of the most commonly occurring com-
dent assessment of the degree of the autism sever- munity-based interventions, and they provided a
ity (Gilliam Autism Rating Scale [GARS]; broad spectrum of the types of approach currently
Gilliam, 1995) and the degree of behavioral prob- offered in the United Kingdom.
lems (Conners Rating Scale [CRS-R]; Conners, Applied Behavior Analysis. The ABA programs
1997). included in this study were provided by a range of
A total of 53 participants were identified in organizations, and followed one of a number of
conjunction with local authorities in the south- well recognized ABA procedures: (a) UCLA Lo-
east of England during 2002 to 2004. None of vaas-type approaches (see Lovaas, 1981; Lovaas &
the families contacted refused to participate in the Smith, 1989); (b) verbal behavior (see Sundberg
study. Of the participants initially recruited, five & Michael, 2001); or (c) Comprehensive Appli-
were excluded from the study: three for compro- cation of Behavior Analysis to Schooling
mised treatment integrity (i.e., at the follow-up (CABAS; see Greer, 1997). All of these programs
assessment it became apparent that the child had were overseen by appropriately trained supervisors
received both ABA and special nursery interven- (who were either Board Certified Behavior Ana-
tions in two cases, and portage and special nurs- lyst [BCBA] accredited or who had master
ery interventions in one case); one for missing teacher-level CABAS qualifications). The inter-

420 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


TABLE 1
Characteristics of the Three Interventions Studied
Variable ABA Special Nursery Portage

Mean intervention (hr) 30.4 12.7 8.5


Range (hr) 2040 323 215
Interquartile range (hr) 2834 1215 39
One-to-one teaching (hr) 28.3 3.1 6.5
Group teaching (hr) 2.1 9.6 2.0
Tutors (family tutors) 4.4 (1.0) 4.0 (1.0) 4.0 (2.0)
Service providers 5 7 7
Treatment characteristics
Based Home School Home
Teaching One-to-one Group One-to-one
Led by Tutor Tutor Parent
Methods Reward Various Reward
Intensity High Moderate Low

ventions were conducted by tutors trained in ac- small, with about six to eight children in each. All
cordance with the appropriate intervention man- classes were under the supervision of a teacher
uals associated with the ABA approach offered. with postgraduate qualifications in teaching and
All of these programs shared key ABA fea- specialist training in special educational needs. All
tures. All were home-based and offered almost ex- curriculum and practices had been approved by
clusively one-to-one teaching for the child with Ofsted Reports (U.K. government inspection re-
autism, and the intensity (hours per week) of the ports given regularly to all schools). In addition to
interventions were typically quite high (see Table the teacher, each class had two or three learning
1). Typically, a session would last 2 to 3 hr, and support assistants, who would help work with the
would comprise approximately 8 to 14 tasks or children in small groups. Thus, most teaching was
drills per session (depending on the particular conducted in small groups rather than individu-
needs of the child). Typically these tasks would ally (about four times as much group work as in-
last about 5 to 10 min each and would be re- dividual work). The intensity of the intervention
peated until some criterion performance was (hours per week) was moderate compared to the
reached. Each task would be separated by a 5- to ABA group.
10-min break, or down time. The programs used The children attended the nursery for a
an antecedent (question/task), behavior (re- number of 2- to 3-hr sessions per week, depend-
sponse) sometimes prompted if necessary, and ing on the severity of the childs autism (see Table
consequence procedure as outlined in the various 1 for the range of time inputs). Typically, a session
manuals. Reinforcement was usually a tangible would start and end with six to eight children in a
such as food, but could also be praise and activi- group with the teacher at the front. The teacher
ties depending on what was effective with the in- usually guided a song, or other introduction, and
dividual child. No aversive stimuli were used in the children were encouraged to take turns in an-
any of the programs. swering their names or responding, often involv-
Special Nursery Placements. The special nurs- ing doing an individual activity (e.g., picking up
ery placements (Nursery) occurred across several name card, shaking an instrument, etc.), while
school provisions (see Table 1). The classes were the others were encouraged to respond and com-

Exceptional Children 421

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


ment. A key feature was the use of materials and day, and are scheduled when the parent believes
methods appealing to children with ASD, such as the child will be at his or her most receptive.
bright colored visual materials, glitter, water, Thus, the program shares some common ele-
paint, sand, or musical instruments. Much of the ments with more motivationally oriented ap-
schools environment and many of the tasks given proaches such as floortime (Greenspan & Wieder,
to the children were presented in a highly struc- 1997). Typically, the parent will teach the child in
tured method as outlined by the TEACCH a one-to-one situation and will target several skills
methodology (Mesibov, Schopler, & Hearsey, per week. The children are taught new skills
1994). through the use of questions and tasks, prompts,
Thus, the focus was on learning and motiva- and rewards. The skills to be taught are outlined
tion, developing shared attention, and participa- in the portage manuals, and each skill acquired is
tion (with support from up to two or three other used to build larger and more complex behavioral
adults). Other sessions included a free choice of responses. Monitoring and evaluation of progress
activities for the children, with adults focusing occurs at the supervisors visits.
their support on children working toward their Comparison of Approaches. The key character-
learning objectives, all the time encouraging and istics of the different interventions, along with a
prompting social interaction, turn-taking, shared description of their main features (overall hours
attention, and responding. Another session in- per week, hours of one-to-one and group, num-
volved outdoor group activities, such as running ber of tutors involved, etc.) reported by tutors in
across the playground, together in pairs, to prac- pre- and poststudy questionnaires are shown in
tice physical coordination, group participation, Table 1. These questionnaires were given at the
and so forth. Most of the reinforcers were social start of the intervention and at the end and were
(e.g., saying good boy, or clapping, or providing compared to check the degree to which the
the opportunity to play with a desired toy, etc., planned intervention was adhered to over the
but always in the context of the group). course of the program. In all cases, the descrip-
Portage. Portage is a home-based teaching tions of the program given in the postinterven-
program for preschool children with special educa- tion questionnaires were used for the data in
tional needs. The portage program has been exten- Table 1, but it should be noted that in most cases
sively used with children with developmental the postintervention descriptions adhered to those
delay (see Cameron, 1997), but has recently been outlined at the start of the intervention. Similarly,
developed to accommodate children with ASD (see the descriptions of the program given by the su-
Reed et al., 2000; Smith, 2000). This program pervisors and the parents were very similar to one
involves slightly increased time input, and a focus another. To this extent, the data obtained on the
on developing attentional responses, such as joint characteristics of the intervention were verifiable
attention and attention sharing behaviors. The in- from multiple sources, and fidelity to the initial
tervention is a relatively low-intensity program, aims of treatment, insofar as it can be achieved in
with the majority of the work being conducted home-based settings, was reasonable.
one-to-one with the child, although about 25% of Thus, inspection of Table 1 shows that the
the time involves group work (see Table 1). ABA program had the highest time intensity, fol-
The portage program is supervised by a lowed by nursery, and portage. The ABA and
trained portage supervisor, who has graduate-level portage approaches delivered mainly one-to-one
qualifications and who attended training courses teaching, whereas the nursery placements offered
in the delivery of portage for children with ASD. mainly group instruction. All of the programs of-
The program typically follows a manual written fered exposure to a range of adult tutors in addi-
by the portage service provider. The supervisor tion to the teacher/supervisor. It should be noted
visits the parents once every 1 to 2 weeks, and that all of the supervisors/teachers leading the var-
parents and caregivers are shown how to apply ious programs were professionally accredited.
this system during these visits. That is, the ABA supervisors were all BCBA or
The training sessions conducted with the CABAS accredited, the nursery teachers were all
child are brief, usually about 40 to 60 min per certified teachers, and the portage home visitors

422 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


had all been trained and held Portage certificates. McCulloch, 1996) is a battery of tests of cognitive
Each intervention was conducted in accordance abilities that index educational achievement. It is
to either the appropriate manual or in accordance suitable for use with children and adolescents ages
with best teaching practice in special schools. 2 years, 6 months (2:6) to 17 years, 11 months
(17:11). For the current purposes, the Early Years
MEASURES Battery was employed, which is designed for chil-
dren under the age of 6 years. The present use of
Gilliam Autism Rating Scale. The GARS
the test concerned educational achievement, so
(Gilliam, 1995) comprises four subtests, each de-
the Verbal Comprehension, Early Number Con-
scribing behaviors symptomatic of autism (Stereo-
cepts, Picture Matching, and Naming Vocabulary
typed Behaviors, Communication, Social
subscales were used. These scales allow the calcu-
Interaction, and Developmental Disturbances).
lation of a General Cognitive Ability scale (M =
The raw scores from these subscales can be con- 100, SD = 15), which represents early educational
verted into standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15). achievement.
These subscales combine to give an Autism Quo- Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. The
tient, high scores meaning greater autistic severity Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (VAB; Spar-
(M = 100 [average autistic severity], SD = 15). row, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) is a semistructured
The scale is appropriate for persons ages 3 to 22 interview, administered to a parent or other care-
years and is completed by parents or professionals giver of the child. It can be used for children from
in about 10 min. Its internal reliability is 0.96, birth to 5:11 years, making it suitable for the pre-
and it has high criterion validity with the Autism sent cohort. The VAB scales assess childrens day-
Behavior Checklist (0.94). to-day adaptive functioning. Scores from four
Psychoeducational Profile (Revised). The Psy- domains of adaptive behavior were used in the
choeducational Profile Revised (PEP-R; Schopler, present study (Communication, Daily Living
Reichler, Bashford, Lansing, & Marcus, 1990) is a Skills, Socialization, and Motor Skills). The raw
developmental test designed for assessing both the scores can be converted to standard scores, and a
typical strengths and characteristic weaknesses of Composite Overall score can be derived based on
children with autism. The test measures function- sum of scale standard scores (M = 100; SD = 15).
ing in seven developmental domains: Imitation, The internal reliability of the Overall Composite
Perception, Fine and Gross Motor Skills, score is 0.93.
EyeHand Coordination, and Nonverbal and Conners Rating ScalesRevised. The Conners
Verbal Conceptual Ability. The mental age re- Rating ScalesRevised (CRS-R; Conners, 1997)
quired to perform the tests ranges from 1 to 72 assess children (ages 317) for behavioral prob-
months. The test also gives an overall develop- lems and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
mental functioning score that can be converted The short version of the form was used, which
into an overall score (mental age/chronological takes 5 to 10 minutes to complete. The short ver-
age ! 100). The internal reliability of the PEP-R sion of the CRS-R comprises 28 items for the
for children with autism ranges from 0.85 (Per- teacher, using a 0 (Not True at All) to 4 (Very
ception) to 0.98 (Cognitive Verbal Performance), Much True) Likert-type scale. There are four sub-
and it has high criterion validity with other tests scales of the test: (a) Oppositional Behaviorin-
for intelligence, such as the Merrill Palmer Scale dicative of rule breaking, authority problems, and
of Mental Tests (0.85). The PEP-R was used be- ease of anger; (b) Cognitive Problemsindexing
cause its low floor and high ceiling made it appro- inattention, difficulty in organizing work, or con-
priate to administer to the current sample both at centrating for sustained periods; (c) Hyperactiv-
baseline and at follow up. This avoids one major itysuggesting difficulties in sitting still,
criticism of previous work that different tests are restlessness, and impulsivity; and the (d) ADHD
administered at these times reducing the reliabil- indexidentifying children likely to suffer from
ity of the measure (Magiati & Howlin, 2001). ADHD. The range of internal reliability of the
British Abilities Scales II. The British Abilities subscales is from 0.77 to 0.96. Although not used
Scales: Second Edition (BAS II; Elliott, Smith, & primarily for autism per se, this tool has been

Exceptional Children 423

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


identified as important in the assessment of the R), educational functioning (BAS II), and adap-
effects of interventions on behavioral difficulties tive behavioral functioning (VAB). These data
for children with autism (see, Handen, Johnson, were analyzed to ascertain whether the groups
& Lubetsky, 2000). were well matched. The analysis was conducted
by means of a multivariate analysis of covariance
PROCEDURE (MANCOVA) with the three intervention groups
The children were visited by an experienced and as independent variables, the overall baseline
qualified senior educational psychologist (10 years scores for the four scales as dependent variables,
postqualification), who completed the PEP-R and and the participants age as a covariate. To docu-
BAS II assessments. These first set of measures ment further the precise nature of the problems
took about 120 to 180 min to complete, and all experienced by the children at intake, their scores
the measures were completed in a single session. on the four subscales of the GARS, and the four
In addition, the educational psychologist helped subscales of the CRS-R, at baseline, were analyzed
the parent to complete the GARS and VAB mea- separately using univariate analysis of covariance,
sures during this visit. After 9 months, the follow- with group as an independent variable and age at
up measures were taken by the same educational intake as a covariate.
psychologist, who again completed all of the mea- The next series of analyses dealt with the
sures as previously described. At this time, the ed- changes that occurred in the overall functioning
ucational psychologist also helped the parent to scores over the period of the intervention. First,
complete the follow-up GARS and VAB mea- the changes in autistic severity were analyzed to
sures. The educational psychologist had no previ- determine if any of the three interventions had an
ous experience of the children being tested in this impact on the severity of the autistic characteris-
study and was not informed of the childs group tics (separately from changes in intellectual or ed-
status (but may have been able to obtain that in- ucational functioning). Therefore, this analysis
formation by the location of the visit or through was conducted by analysis of covariance (AN-
the parents). COVA), with the three intervention types as the
In addition to these measures, the supervisors independent variable, change in the overall GARS
or teachers delivering the interventions were as a dependent variable, and participants ages at
asked to complete a CRS-R concerning the childs intake as a covariate.
behavior and a questionnaire concerning the na- Following this analysis, the changes in the
ture of the intervention. The supervisors/teachers overall scores for intellectual (PEP-R), educational
were asked to complete these forms at the start of (BAS II), and adaptive behavioral functioning
the intervention (at baseline) and again after 9 (VAB) were analyzed by MANCOVA, with the
months (follow up). These questionnaires were intervention group as an independent variable,
completed at the same time as the baseline and the three overall outcome measures as the depen-
follow-up child assessment. Finally, the parents dent variable, and the participants ages and autis-
were also asked to complete a questionnaire re- tic severities at intake as covariates. The changes
garding the characteristics of the program that in the subscales of these measures across the three
their child had experienced. intervention groups were also analyzed separately
by ANCOVA, using age and autistic severity as
covariates, where the initial overall differences had
RESULTS proved to be statistically significant.
Finally, changes in the behavioral problems
D ATA A N A LY S I S
experienced by the children, as noted by the
The data analysis was divided into a number of CRS-R, were analyzed by means of a MAN-
sections for ease of understanding. Initially, the COVA, with the intervention group as an inde-
baseline scores of the children were analyzed with pendent variable, the four Conners subscales as
respect to the main outcome measures: autistic the dependent variable, and the age and autistic
severity (GARS), intellectual functioning (PEP- severity at intake as covariates.

424 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


TABLE 2
Mean (Standard Deviation) Baseline, Follow Up, and Change Scores for the Three Intervention Groups
Group Scale Baseline Follow Up Change

ABA GARS 90.5 (13.9) 88.5 (15.4) 2.0 (8.4)


PEP-R 55.6 (13.8) 69.2 (19.8) 13.6 (12.7)
BAS II 56.8 (16.6) 74.5 (22.4) 17.8 (15.0)
VAB 58.2 (8.0) 61.1 (10.6) 2.9 (6.5)
Nursery GARS 96.8 (10.4) 97.2 (11.0) 0.5 (9.3)
PEP-R 51.9 (20.1) 62.0 (21.9) 10.2 (13.6)
BAS II 57.8 (12.8) 65.7 (15.5) 7.9 (8.6)
VAB 53.0 (4.6) 56.2 (8.1) 3.3 (6.3)
Portage GARS 91.3 (14.3) 92.8 (14.9) 1.5 (5.9)
PEP-R 53.3 (16.1) 54.8 (19.2) 1.6 (11.2)
BAS II 53.4 (10.9) 60.0 (13.9) 6.6 (9.1)
VAB 58.6 (6.0) 57.2 (7.7) 1.4 (4.0)

Note. Gilliam Autism Rating Scale = GARS; Psychoeducational Profile Revised = PEP-R; British Ability Scales: Second
Edition = BAS II; Vineland Adaptive Behavior = VAB. All standard scores (mean = 100, standard deviation = 15).

B A S E L I N E D ATA specific behavioral problems through the sub-


scales of the GARS and the CRS-R. This allows
The baseline data from the 48 participants who
further documentation of the subject equivalence
completed the study are described in Tables 2, 3,
across the groups.
and 4. Inspection of these baseline data shows
The subscales of the GARS deal with the de-
that the participants in each of the three groups
were well matched in terms of their baseline gree of stereotyped behaviors, communication
scores on all overall measures. In terms of the difficulties, social interaction, and developmental
group composition: the ABA group had 12 par- disturbances shown by the children. All of these
ticipants (11 male and 1 female), with a mean age scales offer standardized scores, with a mean of 10
of 40 months (range 3247 months) at intake; and standard deviation of 3 (with high scores
the special nursery group had 20 participants (18 meaning more problems). The group means for
male and 2 female), with a mean age of 43 stereotyped behaviors at baseline are shown in
months (range 4148 months); and the portage Table 3. Inspection of these data shows that none
group had 16 participants, with a mean age of 38 of the groups were discrepant from one another
months (range 3045 months). A MANCOVA on any of the subscales for the GARS. Separate
was conducted on the baseline scores for the ANCOVAs conducted on the baseline scores
GARS, PEP-R, BAS II, and VAB as dependent across group, with age at intake as a covariate, re-
measures, with intervention group as an indepen- vealed no statistically significant difference for
dent variable, and age as a covariate. The Pillais any scale, all ps > .30.
Trace criterion was adopted as the most conserva- The scores from the baseline CRS-R also
tive of the MANOVA test statistics (Olson, offer a standardized T score (M = 50 and SD =
1979). This analysis revealed no statistically sig- 10, with high scores meaning more problems).
nificant differences between the groups at base- The group means for the four subscales (opposi-
line, Pillais Trace = .244, F(8,84) = 1.46, p > .10. tional behavior, cognitive problems, hyperactivity,
In addition to these overall cognitive and be- and ADHD) are shown in Table 4. Inspection of
havioral functioning measures, it is important to these data shows that none of the groups were dis-
note that the children were also assessed on their crepant from one another on any of the subscales

Exceptional Children 425

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


TABLE 3
Mean (Standard Deviation) Baseline, Follow Up, and Change Scores for Each Subscale for
the Three Intervention Groups
Group and Scale Baseline Follow Up Change

ABA
GARS (M = 10, SD = 3)
Stereotyped behaviors 8.1 (2.8) 7.1 (3.0) 1.0 (1.5)
Communication 1.6 (3.3) 5.4 (4.8) 3.8 (5.5)
Social interaction 7.4 (2.7) 7.0 (2.9) 0.4 (1.6)
Developmental disturbance 9.2 (2.3) 9.2 (2.4) 0 (1.1)
PEP-R (M = 100, SD = 15)
Imitation 45.5 (30.9) 76.5 (24.9) 31.0 (22.8)**
Perception 78.2 (24.6) 99.8 (35.6) 21.6 (20.1)**
Fine motor 71.5 (18.7) 75.3 (19.2) 3.8 (19.5)
Gross motor 60.3 (17.5) 91.1 (31.7) 30.8 (25.0)**
Hand-eye 64.8 (21.7) 77.3 (22.5) 12.4 (16.4)*
Cognitive 40.7 (23.2) 59.3 (22.2) 18.6 (17.4)**
Verbal 41.9 (22.6) 61.7 (24.9) 19.8 (24.5)*
BAS II (M = 50, SD = 10)
Verbal comprehension. 23.3 (6.8) 32.8 (17.6) 9.5 (14.3)*
Picture matching 33.8 (13.3) 42.3 (15.3) 8.6 (8.8)**
Naming 22.4 (8.3) 35.6 (13.5) 13.2 (11.9)**
Early number skills 26.3 (5.3) 34.8 (10.9) 8.6 (9.6)**
VAB (M = 100, SD = 10)
Communication 57.1 (7.6) 64.1 (14.9) 7.0 (11.6)
Daily living skills 61.6 (6.7) 64.3 (10.8) 2.7 (6.2)
Socialization 62.1 (9.3) 66.1 (9.2) 4.0 (7.3)
Motor skills 73.8 (16.6) 71.8 (15.7) 2.0 (11.6)

Special Nursery
GARS (M = 10, SD = 3)
Stereotyped behaviors 9.3 (2.3) 9.6 (3.2) 0.3 (2.1)
Communication 5.9 (5.6) 4.6 (5.3) 1.3 (5.8)
Social interaction 9.1 (2.8) 8.6 (2.9) 0.6 (2.5)
Developmental disturbance 9.6 (1.9) 9.7 (1.9) 0.2 (1.2)
PEP-R (M = 100, SD = 15)
Imitation 52.9 (30.3) 61.0 (30.5) 8.2 (21.2)
Perception 80.4 (45.6) 81.2 (30.0) 0.8 (34.6)
Fine motor 59.9 (21.0) 63.6 (18.2) 3.7 (13.1)
Gross motor 67.2 (28.2) 80.5 (28.9) 13.3 (19.7)**
Hand-eye 66.0 (31.3) 71.0 (25.4) 5.0 (21.9)
Cognitive 42.5 (20.2) 56.3 (24.3) 13.9 (16.0)**
Verbal 43.8 (23.8) 55.3 (26.8) 11.5 (15.3)**
BAS II (M = 50, SD = 10)
Verbal comprehension 23.3 (6.8) 26.0 (7.0) 1.8 (4.1)
Picture matching 33.8 (13.3) 37.1 (11.9) 7.9 (10.9)**
Naming 22.4 (8.4) 29.7 (10.1) 2.7 (4.0)**
Early number skills 26.3 (5.3) 32.1 (9.9) 4.3 (7.6)*

continues

426 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


T A B L E 3 (Continued)

Group and Scale Baseline Follow Up Change

Special Nursery - cont.


VAB (M = 100, SD = 10)
Communication 57.1 (7.6) 58.3 (10.1) 3.1 (6.4)*
Daily living skills 61.6 (6.9) 60.9 (11.0) 3.1 (8.6)
Socialization 62.1 (9.3) 61.6 (9.0) 5.0 (8.8)*
Motor skills 73.8 (16.6) 64.5 (14.0) 3.2 (10.7)

Portage
GARS (M = 10, SD = 3)
Stereotyped behaviors 8.9 (3.1) 8.8 (3.7) 0.1 (2.5)
Communication 0.4 (1.8) 1.6 (3.5) 1.2 (3.1)
Social interaction 8.3 (2.9) 8.1 (2.6) 0.1 (1.1)
Developmental disturbance 9.2 (1.8) 9.6 (2.0) 0.5 (1.5)
PEP-R (M = 100, SD = 15)
Imitation 39.4 (22.1) 49.4 (26.8) 10.1 (23.6)
Perception 74.8 (31.3) 76.8 (26.7) 1.9 (20.8)
Fine motor 65.0 (19.9) 63.9 (21.6) 1.1 (15.1)
Gross motor 68.3 (20.2) 79.3 (36.4) 11.0 (26.0)
Hand-eye 64.7 (28.0) 63.8 (31.1) 0.9 (18.9)
Cognitive 38.3 (17.6) 44.1 (23.4) 5.9 (14.3)
Verbal 42.6 (15.9) 45.6 (18.7) 3.1 (17.7)
BAS II (M = 50, SD = 10)
Verbal comprehension 21.7 (4.3) 22.3 (3.9) 0.7 (3.4)
Picture matching 28.8 (8.0) 35.8 (11.1) 6.9 (7.6)**
Naming 22.3 (6.3) 24.0 (8.5) 1.7 (4.6)
Early number skills 26.1 (7.8) 27.8 (8.8) 1.8 (8.6)
VAB (M = 100, SD = 10)
Communication 57.1 (4.9) 54.8 (4.9) 2.3 (4.1)*
Daily living skills 64.6 (6.2) 63.1 (10.4) 1.5 (6.6)
Socialization 61.0 (5.8) 60.7 (7.3) 0.3 (3.9)
Motor skills 70.9 (11.1) 70.0 (13.7) 0.9 (10.3)

Note. Gilliam Autism Rating Scale = GARS; Psychoeducational Profile Revised = PEP-R; British Ability Scales:
Second Edition = BAS II; Vineland Adaptive Behavior = VAB.
*p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01.

for the CRS-R. Separate ANCOVAs conducted indicated a worsening of the symptoms over time.
on the baseline scores across group, with age at in- Analysis of the difference score between the over-
take as a covariate, revealed no statistically signifi- all GARS score obtained at baseline and follow-
cant difference for any scale, all ps > .10. up revealed very little change over this period for
any of the intervention groups (see Table 2).
CHANGE IN OV E R A L L AU T I S M S E V E R I T Y These difference scores were analyzed by an AN-
The overall autism severity of the participants was COVA, with intervention type (ABA, Nursery,
assessed at baseline and follow up using the and Portage) as a between-subject factor and age
GARS. A change in a negative direction indicated at intake as a covariate. This analysis revealed no
an improvement (i.e., a lessening) in the severity statistically significant difference between the
of autistic symptoms, whereas a positive change groups, F(2,44) = .56, p > .60.

Exceptional Children 427

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


TABLE 4
Mean (Standard Deviation) Baseline, Follow Up, and Change Scores for the Three Intervention Groups
for Connors Scales
Group Scale Baseline Follow Up Change

ABA Oppositional 62.1 (9.9) 61.3 (12.6) 0.8 (10.1)


Cognitive 72.4 (19.4) 61.6 (16.1) 10.4 (26.6)
Hyperactivity 63.2 (8.2) 55.8 (8.8) 7.0 (7.1)
ADHD 64.6 (7.2) 57.8 (10.2) 6.9 (8.0)
Nursery Oppositional 70.4 (13.0) 59.8 (14.1) 10.7 (11.4)
Cognitive 78.9 (10.9) 62.7 (12.7) 16.1 (14.1)
Hyperactivity 68.6 (9.9) 62.0 (13.6) 6.5 (8.5)
ADHD 69.0 (9.3) 62.3 (11.7) 6.7 (6.2)
Portage Oppositional 68.3 (6.1) 58.4 (1.8) 9.9 (6.3)
Cognitive 74.9 (13.6) 74.8 (2.1) 0.4 (13.2)
Hyperactivity 68.4 (6.3) 63.1 (0.6) 4.8 (6.4)
ADHD 66.9 (7.1) 63.3 (0.9) 3.8 (7.1)

CHANGE IN OV E R A L L F U N C T I O N I N G ery, and portage) as the independent variable; and


the participants ages and their autistic severity
Table 2 displays the mean change scores (follow-
up standardized score minus baseline standardized (GARS) at the start of the intervention as covari-
score) for all three intervention types, along with ates. This analysis revealed a statistically signifi-
the mean baseline and follow-up scores. Intellec- cant effect of intervention, Pillais Trace = .30,
tual functioning was measured by the overall F(6,84) = 2.47, p < .05, partial eta2 = .150. Sepa-
PEP-R score ([developmental age/chronological rate univariate analyses of covariance (ANCO-
age] ! 100), educational functioning was mea- VAs) were performed on each of the three
sured by the general cognitive ability score of the dependent scores (PEP-R, BAS II, and VAB),
BAS II, and adaptive behavior change was mea- with intervention as the independent variable,
sured by the composite VAB. and age and GARS as covariates. These analyses
Inspection of Table 2 shows that for intellec- revealed a statistically significant effect of inter-
tual functioning (PEP-R), the ABA and nursery vention type on intellectual functioning (PEP-R),
interventions produced gains of around 10 to 13 F(2,43) = 3.88, p < .05, partial eta2 = .153; edu-
points over the 9-month period, which were cational functioning (BAS II), F(2,43) = 4.52, p <
somewhat higher than the gains produced by the .05, partial eta2 = .174; and adaptive behavior
portage intervention. Educational functioning
(VAB), F(2,43) = 3.80, p < .05, partial eta2 =
(BAS II) showed the biggest gain in the ABA
.150. Tukeys honestly significant difference
group (approximately 18 points), which exceeded
(HSD) tests were conducted, which revealed sta-
the change in the other three interventions (ap-
proximately 58 points). For adaptive behavioral tistically significant pairwise differences between
functioning, the nursery placements produced the the ABA and portage for intellectual functioning
largest gain, followed by ABA, with no gains (PEP-R), p < .05, between ABA and each of the
being shown for portage. other two interventions for educational function-
These data were analyzed by a MANCOVA ing (BAS II), ps < .05. The Tukeys HSD tests for
with the intellectual (PEP-R), educational (BAS the adaptive behavior score (VAB) revealed a sta-
II), and adaptive behavior (VAB) measures as de- tistically significant difference between the nurs-
pendent variables; intervention type (ABA, nurs- ery and portage interventions, p < .05. None of

428 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


the other pairwise differences proved to be statis- for the communication and socialization scales,
tically significant. and the nursery intervention tended to outper-
form the other two interventions marginally on
CHANGES IN THE SUBSCALES OF EACH all the scales, except communication. However,
MEASURE OF FUNCTIONING only this latter scale showed a statistically signifi-
cant difference between the interventions, F(2,39)
The mean change scores (follow-up standard
= 4.95, p < .01, partial eta2 = .191, with the ABA
score minus baseline standard score) for the indi-
intervention outperforming the portage interven-
vidual subscales of each test of functioning (i.e.,
tion, p < .05.
PEP-R, BAS II, and VAB) can be seen in Table 3,
along with the mean baseline and follow-up
CHANGES IN MEASURES OF B E H AV I O R A L
scores. The top panel of Table 3 shows the change D I F F I C U LT I E S
in the standard scores for the subscales of the in-
tellectual functioning (PEP-R) measure. In gen- In addition to the overall intellectual and cogni-
eral, the ABA intervention showed the greatest tive changes, the changes in behavioral problems
change scores of the interventions. Table 3 also as indexed by the CRS-R are shown in Table 4,
shows that there were statistically significant im- along with the group means at baseline and fol-
provements in six of the subscales of the PEP-R low up. In terms of oppositional behavior, nursery
for the ABA intervention (imitation, perception, and portage programs showed more improvement
gross motor skills, handeye coordination, cogni- than the ABA group; whereas for cognitive prob-
tive performance, and cognitive verbal perfor- lems, ABA and nursery showed more change than
mance). There were statistically significant portage. All groups showed similar improvements
changes in three of the subscales for the nursery for hyperactivity and ADHD problems.
intervention (gross motor skills, cognitive perfor- These data were analyzed by a MANCOVA
mance, and cognitive verbal performance). How- with intervention group as a dependent variable,
ever, in only the imitation score was there a and the age and autistic severity of the partici-
statistically significant difference between the in- pants as covariates. This analysis showed a statisti-
terventions, F(2,39) = 3.34, p < .05, partial eta2 = cally significant difference between the groups,
.146. Tukeys HSD tests revealed that ABA pro- Pillais Trace = .521, F(8.82) = 3.61, p < .001,
duce a greater change than nursery on this mea- partial eta 2 = .260. Separate ANCOVAs con-
sure, p < .05, but none of the other pairwise ducted on each subscale revealed significant dif-
comparisons proved statistically significant. ferences on the oppositional problems scale,
For the subscales of the educational function- F(2,43) = 5.22, p < .01, partial eta2 = .195. Here
ing (BAS II) measure, the ABA intervention pro- both the nursery and portage groups showed sig-
duced statistically significant gains in all the nificantly greater improvements than the ABA
subscales. The nursery intervention produced sta- group (Tukeys HSD tests, p < .05). There were
tistically significant gains in all the subscales, ex- also statistically significant differences between
cept verbal comprehension. Across the the groups on the cognitive problem scale,
interventions, ABA produced statistically signifi- F(2,42) = 3.82, p < .05, partial eta 2 = .151.
cantly greater improvements than the other inter- Tukeys HSD tests showed that the nursery group
ventions, these being statistically significant for differed statistically significantly from the portage
both verbal comprehension, F(2,39) = 5.00, p < group, p < .05. There were no group differences
on either of the hyperactivity and ADHD scales,
.01, partial eta2 = .204 and naming vocabulary,
both ps > .20.
F(2,39) = 10.08, p < .001, partial eta2 = .341. For
both measures, follow-up Tukeys HSD tests
showed that the ABA intervention outperformed
DISCUSSION
both the nursery and portage interventions.
For the subscales of the VAB scale there was a The current study examined the effectiveness of
mixed pattern of results. The nursery intervention three early-teaching interventions for children
produced statistically significant improvements with ASD. The primary aims of this study were to

Exceptional Children 429

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


examine the impact of these interventions as they there is no sign of recovery from the symptoms of
occur in the community rather than the clinic, autism.
and to examine their impact on a set of partici- Irrespective of the impact on the autistic
pants not heavily selected on criteria designed to severity of the participants ASD, the ABA pro-
maximize possible outcome efficacy. In this way, gram produced significant gains on both intellec-
it was hoped to produce a study with a high exter- tual and educational functioning, and the nursery
nal validity. In addition, it was hoped to show intervention produced a strong impact on all
some of the factors that contribute to the success three measures of functioning, with the improve-
of these interventions. In summary, the results ments resulting from portage being limited to
shed new light on the real-world effectiveness of only a few measures. The comparison between the
several of the educational interventions. Particu- ABA intervention and the portage intervention in
larly, they partially replicated the effectiveness of the current study offers a chance to replicate the
previous studies of clinic-based ABA programs in original comparison in the Lovaas (1987) study
a community-based sample. They also showed between a high intensity and low intensity treat-
strong gains over a relatively short period of time, ment program, with the same results; the high-in-
and demonstrated the effectiveness of nursery tensity program produced stronger improvements
placements in promoting aspects of intellectual (see also Smith, Eikeseth, Klevstrand, & Lovaas,
and adaptive behavioral functioning. 1997).
The participants in this study tended to have
moderate autistic symptoms, being in the mid
range of the standard scores generated by the [I]t appears that in a typical community-
GARS. The participants intellectual functioning, based intervention, over 9 months, there is
educational functioning, and adaptive behavioral no sign of recovery from the symptoms of
functioning, measured at the start of the interven-
autism.
tion, all showed quite severe impairments; the
mean scores were around 50 on each standardized
instrument, indicating that these participants With respect to the ABA intervention, these
were at the severe end of those taken on previous data show that ABA has a strong impact on intel-
studies of ABA (e.g., Lovaas, 1987, excluded chil- lectual and educational functioning. These gains
dren with IQs lower than 50). To this extent, the in functioning replicate, in part, the effects of pre-
current participants were more representative of vious ABA studies, which were conducted in a
children with ASD who present for intervention clinic, on a home-based sample (Eikeseth et al.,
at local authorities (see Conner, 1998). 2002). The gains are also of a similar size to previ-
The present results indicated that, on the ous longer period studies of ABA (Lovaas, 1987),
basis of the GARS rating of autism symptom but were achieved over a somewhat brief period
severity, none of the interventions produced re- (9 months). In addition, they show that ABA can
covery, as noted in some previous studies (e.g., be effective for children with relatively severe dif-
Lovaas, 1987). This finding corroborates a num- ficulties (see also Smith et al., 2000).
ber of other studies of the effectiveness of ABA Although ABA also showed some impact on
approaches and adds to the current weight of evi- adaptive behavioral functioning, it was the special
dence that this approach will not cure autism. nursery program that had the greatest impact on
However, it should be noted that there are a num- this aspect of functioning (with an impact also on
ber of possible reasons for this discrepancy be- intellectual and educational functioning). This as-
tween this study and that of Lovaas (1987); the pect of the data was borne out by the teachers
length of time of the intervention (9 months vs. ratings of behavioral problems, which showed
36 months), the place of intervention (commu- that nursery placement affected these problems to
nity vs. clinic), and the severity of the participants the greatest extent. Thus, these data show, for the
symptoms (severe vs. milder). Whatever the rea- first time, the impact of special nursery placement
son for this difference, it appears that in a typical on these measures, and offer support for other
community-based intervention, over 9 months, studies that have shown some improvement that

430 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


is due to special nursery placement (Gabriels et state of knowledge of outcome effectiveness in the
al., 2001). They also stand in contrast to the often community this approach seems a reasonable first
reported failures to produce gains in the nursery step.
control groups of ABA intervention studies (see There are a number of caveats to be made
Howard et al., 2005; Lovaas, 1987). It is difficult when interpreting these data. The main caveat
to be precise about the reasons why the current concerns the fact that the groups were not ran-
study produced gains whereas other studies did domly selected for each of the interventions. The
not, although there are some differences that participants received whatever intervention was
might bear subsequent investigation. In the cur- being provided in their locality. This aspect of the
rent study, the special nursery placement was design was a deliberate part of the study, adopted
heavily reliant on group-teaching approaches, to ensure a high external validity. Low external va-
whereas in previous studies (e.g., Howard et al.) lidity has been a criticism of many previous stud-
this has not always been the case. There also re- ies (see Conner, 1998); children with ASD are
mains the possibility that there are individual dif- not randomly allocated to treatment in the real
ferences between the effectiveness of special world. However, despite this concern, inspection
nursery placements and the receptiveness of the of the groups of participants reveals that they
children to the approach. The precise nature of were very evenly matched, suggesting that there
the special nursery placements is, of course, diffi- were no preexisting systematic biases in the par-
cult to determine on a day-to-day basis, and they ticipants across the groups. This should be the
did contain elements of ABA and TEACCH ap- case, as the geographical location of the partici-
proaches. A precise description of special nursery pant, rather than any characteristic, determined
placement, often characterized as an eclectic ap- their allocation to intervention. Additionally, this
proach would be a useful step in this field, as it study was not designed to address exactly what as-
remains a very widely used early intervention for pect of the intervention was responsible for any
children with ASD. Thus, disentangling these in- observed effectiveness. The nature of the interven-
fluences remains a key objective, but it is an ob- tions was characterized by the tutors and parents,
jective that may be very difficult to implement in and some variables, such as time input, were iden-
a natural setting. tified as important. However, further work will be
needed to establish which aspects of these com-
plex interventions are critical in generating intel-
In addition, they show that ABA can be lectual and behavioral change in the participants.
effective for children with relatively severe Additionally, further measures perhaps tailored to
difficulties. particular curriculums would be helpful for as-
sessing the impact of specific early teaching inter-
ventions, although these would limit the
That ABA has a strong impact on intellec- generality of findings to particular locations. (For
tual/educational gains, and special nursery on example, curriculum key stages may well differ
adaptive behaviors, may reflect the difference in between the United States and the United King-
teaching approaches between the two interven- dom, and certainly with other regions.)
tions (one-to-one vs. group). This pattern of data One issue that should be commented on in
might lead to the suggestion that any program this study concerns the fidelity of the treatment
should include both relatively intense one-to-one regimes in the current study. This study was an
work, followed by group work such as occurs in a investigation into how the programs sometimes
special nursery intervention. studied in the clinic fared when placed in the
The current study was primarily concerned community. This is a key issue about the effec-
with determining whether a number of interven- tiveness of such programs, especially ABA (see
tions would produce gains in children with ASD Mudford et al., 2001). As the programs were con-
when they were conducted in the community. It ducted in the home, their day-to-day fidelity was
was not a study designed to answer the question not directly measured. It should be noted that all
of why those interventions worked; given the programs were conducted by trained supervisors

Exceptional Children 431

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


in accordance with the program manuals, which Charman, T., Howlin, P., Berry, B., & Prince, E.
should ensure some treatment fidelity, as in other (2004). Measuring developmental progress of children
studies. However, the lack of direct observation with autism spectrum disorder on school entry using
may present a limitation on the current work. It parent report. Autism, 8, 89100.
should be noted that it is not clear the degree to Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners Rating Scales. Eagan,
which any program has fidelity to the manual in MN: Pearson Assessment.
the face of specific demands of individual chil- Connor, M. (1998). A review of behavioural early in-
dren, and that many programs are individually tervention programmes for children with autism. Edu-
tailored to the child, and therefore vary from in- cational Psychology in Practice, 14, 109117.
dividual-to-individual, and from day-to-day Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, E. (2002).
within individuals. Of course, stronger measures Intensive behavioral treatment at school for 4- to 7-year
of the treatment fidelity would improve the inter- old children with autism. Behavior Modification, 26,
nal validity of the study and would be welcomed 4968.
in future work, but if the characteristics of the Elliott, C. D., Smith, P., & McCulloch, K. (1996).
program conducted in a study with high internal British Ability Scales: Second Edition. London: NFER-
validity do not correspond to the way such a pro- Nelson.
gram would be conducted in the majority of Gabriels, R. L., Hill, D. E., Pierce, R. A., Rogers, S. J.,
cases, the findings would be of limited external & Wehner, B. (2001). Predictors of treatment outcome
value. in young children with autism. Autism, 5, 407429.
Gilliam, J. E. (1995). Gilliam Autism Rating Scale.
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
[T]he current report failed to note any Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1997). Developmental
evidence of recovery from autism produced patterns and outcomes in infants and children with dis-
orders in relating and communicating: A chart review
by any early intervention.
of 200 cases of children with autistic spectrum diag-
noses. Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders,
In summary, the current report failed to note 1, 87141.
any evidence of recovery from autism produced Greer, R. D. (1997). The Comprehensive Application
by any early intervention. In terms of intellectual of Behavior Analysis to Schooling (CABAS[R]). Behav-
functioning, applied behavior analysis and special ior and Social Issues, 7, 5963.
nursery interventions produced gains (of the same Gresham, F. M., & MacMillan, D. L. (1997). Autistic
magnitude as many gains produced by previous recovery? An analysis and critique of the empirical evi-
longer term clinic-based ABA programs). The re- dence on the Early Intervention Project. Behavioral
sults from clinic-based ABA trials were partially Disorders, 22, 185201.
replicated on a community-based sample; specifi- Handen, B. L., Johnson, C. R., & Lubetsky, M.
cally with respect to intellectual and educational (2000). Efficacy of methylphenidate among children
skills. Special nursery placement was also found with autism and symptoms of attention-deficit hyper-
to be effective for improving adaptive behavior activity disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
and educational skills. Of course, the relative im- Disorders, 30, 245255.
portance of educational skills versus adaptive be- Hohmann, A. A., & Shear, M. K. (2002). Community-
havioral skills at preschool for subsequent school based intervention research: Coping with the noise of
functioning and school inclusion needs to be in- real life in study design. American Journal of Psychiatry,
159, 201207.
vestigated.
Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green,
G., & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive
behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young
REFERENCES
children with autism. Research in Developmental Dis-
Cameron, R. J. (1997). Early intervention for young abilities, 26, 359383.
children with developmental delay: The portage ap- Howlin, P., & Moore, A. (1997). Diagnosis in autism:
proach. Child: Care, Health and Development, 23, A survey of over 1200 parents in the U.K. Autism, 1,
1127. 135162.

432 Summer 2007

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016


Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (2006). Pivotal response Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I.
treatments for autism. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. (1997). Intensive behavioral treatment for preschoolers
Lovaas, O. I. (1981). The ME book: Teaching develop- with severe mental retardations and pervasive develop-
mentally disabled children. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. mental disorder. American Journal on Mental Retarda-
tion, 102, 238249.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal
educational and intellectual functioning in young autis- Smith, T. G., Annette, D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000).
tic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- Randomized trial of intensive early intervention for
ogy, 55, 39. children with pervasive developmental disorder. Ameri-
can Journal on Mental Retardation, 105, 269285.
Lovaas, O. I., & Smith, T. (1989). A comprehensive
behavioral theory of autistic children: Paradigm for re- Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984).
search and treatment. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN:
Experimental Psychiatry, 20, 1729. American Guidance Service.
Magiati, I., & Howlin, P. A. (2001). Monitoring the Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The benefits of
progress of preschool children with autism enrolled in Skinners analysis of verbal behavior for children with
early intervention programmes: Problems in cognitive autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698724.
assessment. Autism, 5, 399406. Twyman, J. S. (1998). The Fred S. Keller School. Jour-
Mesibov, G. B., Schopler, E., & Hearsey, K. A. (1994). nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 695701.
Structured teaching. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov
(Eds.), Behavioral issues in autism (pp. 195207). New
York: Plenum Press. ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Mudford, O. C., Martin, N. T., Eikeseth, S., & Bibby,
PHIL REED Professor; and LISA A. OSBORNE,
P. (2001). Parent-managed behavioral treatment for
Senior Research Assistant, Department of Psychol-
preschool children with autism: Some characteristics of
U.K. programs. Research in Developmental Disabilities, ogy, Swansea University, Swansea, U.K. MARK
22, 173182. CORNESS, Principal Educational Psychologist,
Children, Young People, and Families Direc-
Olson, C. L. (1979). Practical considerations in choos-
ing a MANOVA test statistic: A rejoinder to Stevens. torate, Macclesfield House, Oxford, U.K.
Psychological Bulletin, 86, 13501352.
Osborne, L. A., & Reed, P. (in press). Caregivers per-
ceptions of communication with professionals during
Address correspondence to Phil Reed, Depart-
the diagnosis of autism. Autism. ment of Psychology, Swansea University, Single-
ton Park, Swansea, SA2 8PP, U.K. (e-mail:
Reed, P., Osborne, L. A., & Gibson, E. (2000). Portage
as an intervention for autism. International Portage
p.reed@swansea.ac.uk).
Newsletter, 13, 13.
This research was funded by a grant from the
Schopler, E., Reichler, R., Bashford, A., Lansing, M., &
Marcus, L. (1990). Psychoeducational profile (Rev. ed.).
South East Regional Special Educational Needs
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Partnership (SERSEN, U.K.), and we thank them
for their support during the course of this re-
Shallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (1999, May).
Replicating Lovaas treatment and findings: Preliminary
search. Also, we are very grateful to the children,
results. Paper presented at PEACH: Putting Research parents, teachers, and schools who kindly partici-
Into Practice Conference, London. pated in this study. Some of these data were ini-
Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based be- tially discussed at the European Association for
havioral treatment of young children with autism. Jour- Behaviour Analysis Conference held in Parma,
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 1523. Italy, 2003, and at the Behavioural Association of
Smith, C. (2000). The development and evaluation of Ireland Conference held in Galway, Ireland,
additional training for portage workers in addressing the 2004.
needs of children with complex social and communication
difficulties /autistic spectrum disorders. Unpublished doc- Manuscript received June 2006; accepted October
toral dissertation, University of Southampton, U.K. 2006.

Exceptional Children 433

Downloaded from ecx.sagepub.com at Universitas Gadjah Mada on April 13, 2016

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen