Sie sind auf Seite 1von 5

Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 202206

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Perception of human body modication


Silke Wohlrab a, Bernhard Fink a,*, Peter M. Kappeler a, Gayle Brewer b
a
Department of Sociobiology/Anthropology, University of Gttingen, Berliner Strasse 28, D-37073 Gttingen, Germany
b
Department of Psychology, University of Central Lancashire, Preston, United Kingdom

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The enhancement of physical attractiveness through body modications, such as tattoos is evident in a
Received 28 August 2008 wide range of cultures and has recently become popular also in Westernized societies. Evolutionary psy-
Received in revised form 23 September chologists have suggested that these invasive body modications could possibly act as handicap signals
2008
in sexual selection. However, knowledge about the actual signalling quality of body modication and its
Accepted 30 September 2008
perception is still scarce. In this present study a sample of 278 men and women rated images of tattooed
and non-tattooed virtual human characters for perceived aggression, attractiveness, dominance, health,
masculinity (male gures), and femininity (female gures). Tattooed male characters were perceived
Keywords:
Body modication
as more dominant, and tattooed female characters as less healthy compared with their non-tattooed
Tattoo counterparts. Female raters were more likely to perceive tattooed men as healthy than male raters.
Quality signal We discuss these results in view of a potential biological signalling function of tattoos.
Perception 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Mate choice
Competition

1. Introduction quality individuals may be able to better afford such invasive body
modication.
In recent years, the practice of tattooing has experienced a It is possible that the signalling function of tattoos is strongest
renaissance (Gilbert, 2001), becoming increasingly popular across amongst men. Despite recent cultural trends, tattoos have histori-
all age and social classes (Wohlrab, Fink, & Kappeler, 2005). How- cally been more common amongst men than women and the high-
ever, tattoos are not a recent phenomenon (Dorfer, Moser, Bahr, est rates of tattooing are found amongst men aged between 25 and
et al., 1999) and the origins of this practice can be found in a range 34 years (Stirn, Hinz, & Brahler, 2006). The signalling potential of
of ancient cultures and tribal societies (Stirn, 2001). The present tattoos is further highlighted by the fact that men are less likely
paper investigates the signalling function of tattoos and the type than women (52% and 11%, respectively) to have a tattoo that
of information conveyed. In particular, we consider whether there can be covered by clothes (Laumann & Derick, 2006) and are more
is a biological signalling quality associated with this type of body likely to obtain multiple tattoos (Horne, Knox, Zusman, & Zusman,
modication centred on physical attributes or rather personality 2007).
and related behaviour. Singh and Bronstad (1997) conducted a cross-cultural study,
A number of health risks are known to be associated with tat- investigating the prevalence of body modications in cultures with
tooing, also in Western societies. These risks include severe bacte- different pathogen loads. Seven species of pathogens were in-
rial infections (Stirn, 2003), transmission of blood borne diseases cluded, with each region coded on a three-tier scale, according to
such as HIV (Kral et al., 2001) and hepatitis (Goldberg & Anderson, the regional severity of each pathogen. Pathogen load predicted fe-
2004; Mariano et al., 2004), as well as allergic reactions to carcin- male body modication (stomach scarication), irrespective of
ogenic tattooing colours (Tsuruta, Sowa, Higashi, Kobayashi, & confounding factors such as polygyny, famine or social class strat-
Ishii, 2004) or the increase of concentrations of hazardous pig- ication. Singh and Bronstad (1997) concluded that the body mod-
ments in skin (Engel et al., 2008). Consequently, an individuals ication could act as a handicap signal (i.e., a feature that indicates
ability to successfully acquire and maintain a tattoo could depend an individuals reproductive quality as its production or mainte-
on their overall physical tness and immunocompetence. The risks nance is physiologically costly and only affordable by high quality
associated with tattoos (and the fact that the results of these com- mates individuals; Zahavi, 1975), because the modications are
plications are typically visible to potential mates) suggest that high generally believed to enhance attractiveness and attractiveness
indicates mate quality in the sense of pathogen resistance.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +49 551 39 9344; fax: +49 551 39 7299. If tattoos act as handicap signals, resulting in greater body
E-mail address: bernhard.nk@ieee.org (B. Fink). modication by high quality mates, we would expect tattooed

0191-8869/$ - see front matter 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.031
S. Wohlrab et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 202206 203

characters to be rated as healthier and more physically attractive pated in the study. Participants predominantly undergraduate
than non-tattooed characters. Based on the aforementioned sex college students were recruited at the University of Gttingen,
differences in body modication, the positive impact of body mod- Germany.
ication is expected to be stronger in male compared to female
characters. 2.2. Stimuli
A number of non-physical traits also inuence the desirability
of a potential mate. In particular, men that are perceived as more Three male and three female virtual human characters were
dominant (Neave, Laing, Fink, & Manning, 2003; Townsend, generated using the software Poser 6 (e-frontier, Scotts Valley,
1993) or masculine (Johnston, Hagel, Franklin, Fink, & Grammer, USA). Six characters were generated, three males and three fe-
2001), are regarded as more attractive by women perhaps because males. Each gure was displayed with and without a tattoo. All
dominant men usually achieve higher social status due to their characters wore bathing suits (bikini or swimming trunk) and were
greater ability to provide or defend the resources necessary for presented in a slightly rotated front view (see Fig. 1). Tattoos were
reproduction (Geary, Vigil, & Byrd-Craven, 2004) or because mas- placed on the most typical location, i.e. the arm and the calf for
culinity signals testosterone, one cue of physical tness (Penton- stimuli of both sexes, as reported in Wohlrab et al. (2005). Since
Voak & Chen, 2004; Thornhill & Gangestad, 1999). sex-specic variations in the location of tattoos appear (Table 1;
There are a number of reasons to suggest that possession of a Wohlrab et al., 2005; Wohlrab, Stahl, Rammsayer, & Kappeler,
tattoo may serve to signal dominance or masculinity amongst 2007), further stimuli placed the tattoo on the chest of the male
men. Firstly, men are more likely than women to obtain a tattoo and on the abdomen of the female. For both male and female char-
in order to demonstrate group membership (Horne et al., 2007), acters, the most common category of tattoo (tribals) was chosen
with considerably higher levels of tattoo prevalence in environ- (Wohlrab et al., 2005, 2007). Typical tribal tattoos encompass bold,
ments that promote intra-sex competition. For example there is a black, silhouette style designs and are characterized by symmetri-
high prevalence of tattoos in military personnel (Armstrong, Mur- cally arranged rolling lines, but are otherwise free of apparent
phy, Sallee, & Watson, 2000; Laumann & Derick, 2006) and a large content.
proportion of individuals obtain a tattoo in prison (Hellard, Aitken,
& Hocking, 2007). Secondly, tattoos are associated with a number 2.3. Experimental setup
of male-typical behaviours such as increased sensation seeking
(Stirn et al., 2006) and risk behaviours (Carroll, Riffenburgh, Rob- Experiments were created using MediaLab Research Software
erts, & Myhre, 2002). Therefore the possession of a tattoo may sug- (Empirisoft Inc., New York), with each experiment containing one
gest that a man is more likely to possess a dominant personality or male and one female stimulus. Stimuli were either both tattooed
engage in typically masculine behaviours. or both not tattooed, resulting in three experiments with tattooed
However, whilst some display of dominance and masculinity stimuli and three with natural stimuli (Table 1). Experiments were
are desirable in a mate, extreme dominance (which may be ex- presented in randomized order on a 17-inch at screen (Iiyama
pressed as aggressive behaviour (Mazur & Booth, 1998) can nega- ProLite E 4815) with a resolution of 1280  1024 pixels.
tively inuence reproductive success (Mueller & Mazur, 1998) and Each participant was randomly assigned to one of the six exper-
may be perceived as undesirable in potential mates. Therefore iments and was given standardized instructions prior to data col-
whilst tattooed male characters may be rated as more dominant lection. All participants were requested to rate each gure on
and masculine than non-tattooed characters, they are not expected perceived aggression, attractiveness, dominance, health, and mas-
to appear more aggressive. culinity or femininity (for male or female stimuli, respectively) on a
Whilst men are more likely to obtain a tattoo in order to display 7-point Likert scale (from 1 = not to 7 = very). Attributes were
group identity, womens motivations for obtaining a tattoo are presented in random order.
more likely to focus on decoration of the self or enhancement of
personal beauty (Horne et al., 2007). The focus on appearance 2.4. Data analyses
and nding that dominance is not a desirable trait in female part-
ners suggest that women do not obtain tattoos to signal the same Inter-rater reliability was tested using Cronbachs alpha of
personality and behavioural traits as men. Increasing attractive- internal consistency for each attribute. Values ranged from 0.77
ness may be the primary goal for women obtaining tattoos. One as- (healthiness) to 0.97 (attractiveness). Group comparisons were
pect of physical appearance, the femininity of a woman, is highly conducted between tattooed and non-tattooed male as well as fe-
valued by men because it indicates her fecundity (Grammer, Fink, male gures for all attributes, using multiple ANOVA with the
Mller, & Thornhill, 2003; Law-Smith et al., 2006; Schaefer et al., attributes aggression, attractiveness, dominance, health and mas-
2006). Consequently, we consider that tattooed female characters culinity (male gures) femininity (female gures) as dependent
may be rated as both more attractive and more feminine than variables and stimulus condition (tattooed vs. non-tattooed) and
non-tattooed female characters. sex of the rater as factors.
Therefore, the present study investigated peoples perception of
tattoos by conducting a rating study in which bodies of virtual g-
ures with and without tattoos were rated on attributes that relate 3. Results
to male and female mate choice criteria. These attributes included
physical attractiveness, health, dominance, physical aggression Overall, stimulus condition (i.e. tattooed vs. non-tattooed)
and masculinity or femininity (for men and women, respectively). (F10,265 = 2.49; p < 0.01) as well as the sex of the rater
(F10,265 = 3.60; p < 0.001) signicantly inuenced the perception
of the stimulus gures. Moreover, there was also a signicant
2. Materials and methods interaction effect of tattoo * sex of rater (F10,265 = 1.97; p < 0.05).
For stimulus condition, between-subject effects were found for fe-
2.1. Participants male health (F1,274 = 4.60; p < 0.05) and male dominance
(F1,274 = 8.70; p < 0.01), such that tattooed female gures were
A total of 278 individuals, 145 men and 133 women between rated less healthy and tattooed male gures more dominant
the ages of 18 and 39 years (mean age = 23.8, SD = 2.97), partici- (Table 2).
204 S. Wohlrab et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 202206

Fig. 1. One male and one female stimulus gure, each with and without tattoo.

Table 1 Table 3
Experimental setup. Means and standard error of mean (SEM) of all ratings as a function of the sex of the
rater (n = 278).
Experiment Stimulus Characters Men Women N
Attribute Stimulus sex Males Females
1 Tattooed Female 1, tattoo on arm 24 21 45
Male 1, tattoo on arm Mean SEM Mean SEM
2 Tattooed Female 2, tattoo on calf 22 22 44
Aggression Male 3.38 0.13 3.21 0.14
Male 2, tattoo on calf
Female 3.49 0.14 3.81 0.15
3 Tattooed Female 3, tattoo on abdomen 23 22 45
Attractiveness Male 2.27 0.12 2.15 0.12
Male 3, tattoo on chest
Female 5.00 0.14 4.27 0.15
4 No tattoo Female 1 25 23 48
Dominance Male 3.39 0.13 3.32 0.14
Male 1
Female 4.61 0.12 5.17 0.12
5 No tattoo Female 2 26 22 48
Femininity Female 5.78 0.11 5.52 0.11
Male 2
Health Male 4.38 0.12 4.43 0.12
6 No tattoo Female 3 25 23 48
Female 5.01 0.12 4.83 0.12
Male 3
Masculinity Male 3.95 0.12 3.52 0.12

The sex of the rater signicantly inuenced attractiveness and


dominance ratings (Table 3), such that men rated female gures We predicted that if tattoos act as quality signals in human mate
as more attractive (F1,274 = 12.85; p < 0.001) and women rated fe- choice, women should rate tattooed men as healthier, more attrac-
male gures more dominant (F1,274 = 11.00; p < 0.001). Further- tive, more dominant and more masculine, but not more aggressive
more, male gures were rated more masculine by men (Table 3; than non-tattooed men, as it is known that women have some
F1,274 = 6.57; p < 0.05). A signicant interaction was only found preference for men who maintain and express these attributes
for male health (F1,274 = 5.62; p < 0.05) such that females consid- (Buss, 1989). Similarly, men were predicted to rate tattooed wo-
ered male gures with tattoos healthier whereas men gave higher men as healthier, more attractive and more feminine than non-tat-
health ratings to male gures without tattoos. tooed women.
Women rated tattooed men as healthier than non-tattooed
4. Discussion men, supporting the suggestion that body modication could act
as a handicap signal (Singh & Bronstad, 1997). Men typically dis-
The results provide mixed support for the original hypothesis, play tattoos in places that cannot easily be covered with clothes
suggesting that tattoos have a mate quality signalling function. (Laumann & Derick, 2006). Therefore, men that suffer visible reac-
tions to the tattoo (Tsuruta et al., 2004), develop a bacterial infec-
tion (Stirn, 2003), or relapse from a pre-existing condition such as
Table 2 psoriasis or the herpes simplex virus (Jacob, 2002) clearly signal
Mean and standard error of mean (SEM) of all ratings as a function of the prevalence
of a tattoo (n = 278).
their lowered physical quality to potential mates. High quality
males however, are able to successfully acquire and display a tat-
Attribute Stimulus sex Tattooed Non-tattooed too, suggesting that this form of body modication may represent
Mean SEM Mean SEM an honest signal of physical tness. The nding that men rate tat-
Aggression Male 3.44 0.14 3.15 0.13 tooed men as less healthy than their non-tattooed counterparts is
Female 3.62 0.14 3.68 0.14 more difcult to explain, but could represent a form of derogation
Attractiveness Male 2.19 0.12 2.23 0.12 and intra-sex competition (Buss & Deeden, 1990).
Female 4.51 0.15 4.76 0.14
The same signalling function was not apparent for female g-
Dominance Male 3.64 0.14 3.07 0.13
Female 5.02 0.12 4.76 0.12 ures, with tattooed characters rated as less healthy than non-tat-
Femininity Female 5.62 0.11 5.67 0.11 tooed characters. It is possible that ratings of tattooed stimuli
Health Male 4.39 0.12 4.42 0.12 might have been inuenced by traditional stigmatizations. For
Female 4.73 0.12 5.10 0.12
example women may be perceived as physically weaker and less
Masculinity Male 3.62 0.12 3.85 0.12
capable of resisting the health risks associated with tattooing.
S. Wohlrab et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 202206 205

Alternatively, women that obtain a tattoo may be rated as more possession of an own tattoo, which is likely to bear an inuence
likely to engage in a range of behaviours such as promiscuous on the perception of tattoos.
sex or drinking alcohol (Swami & Furnham, 2007) associated with In conclusion, the research investigated the potential signalling
poor health or desirability as a mate. The fact that women are more function of tattoos, however mixed support was found for the ori-
likely than men to obtain a tattoo in order to enhance personal ginal evolutionary hypotheses. Specically, female but not male
beauty (Horne et al., 2007) would suggest that women are unaware raters perceived tattooed men as healthier than non-tattooed
of the negative connotations associated with female tattoos. No men. Similar results were not found with regards to female body
relationships were found between tattoo status and attractiveness modication; tattooed women were perceived as less healthy than
or femininity for female characters. non-tattooed gures. However these results did not translate to
Our study found that both women and men rated tattooed ratings of attractiveness. Tattooed male gures were also rated as
male gures as more dominant than non-tattooed gures. Wo- more dominant than non-tattooed gures. Therefore we suggest
men value increased dominance because dominant men may that the possession of a tattoo (for men) may signal both biological
achieve a higher social status, earn more money, and thus have and behavioural traits. The relevance of social or biological factors
more possibilities to invest in offspring (Buss & Barnes, 1986; in altering perception could not be claried in this study and
Sadalla, Kenrick, & Vershure, 1987). Men observe the dominance should be the subject of future research. One approach would be
of a same-sex rival in order to assess his strength as a competitor. a comparison of biological traits (incl. faces) between tattooed
Individuals that successfully display high levels of dominance to and non-tattooed individuals. If biological factors predominantly
other men reduce the level of intra-sex competition, which may inuence dominance ratings we would expect differences in tes-
allow their access to desired mates to continue relatively unchal- tosterone levels between tattooed and non-tattooed individuals
lenged. This increases the opportunities for successful mating and (Dabbs & Dabbs, 2000). If no differences in testosterone levels oc-
reduces the risk of injury or loss of status. Therefore the acquisi- cur, then culturally biased inuences might predominate.
tion of a tattoo may serve to display a mans increased domi-
nance, serving to attract female partners and deter potential Acknowledgements
same-sex competitors.
Women typically prefer dominant men during their fertile This project was supported, in part, by grant KA 1082/13-1
phase and for short-term relationships, while they favour sociable funded by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and awarded
partners that are willing-to-invest outside the fertile window to P.M.K and B.F. B.F. is currently funded by an Emmy-Noether Fel-
(Gangestad, Thornhill, & Garver, 2002; Johnston et al., 2001; Pen- lowship of the German Science Foundation (DFG: FI 1450-4-1).
ton-Voak et al., 1999) as well as for long-term relationships
(Scheib, 2001). The fact that womens preference for a dominant References
partner changes cyclically and is inuenced by the type of relation-
Armstrong, M. L., Murphy, K. P., Sallee, A., & Watson, M. G. (2000). Tattooed army
ship sought may perhaps explain the fact that the possession of a
soldiers: Examining the incidence, behavior and risk. Military Medicine, 165,
tattoo inuenced dominance ratings but not the perception of 135141.
attractiveness for male stimuli in the present study. Thus further Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary
research could explore the role of the menstrual cycle and sexual hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 149.
Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection. Journal of
strategy selection. Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 559570.
The study investigated the signalling function of tattoos, provid- Buss, D. M., & Deeden, L. A. (1990). Derogation of competitors. Journal of Social and
ing a preliminary analysis of the information conveyed by a body Personal Relationships, 7(3), 395422.
Buston, P. M., & Emlen, S. T. (2003). Cognitive processes underlying human mate
modication of this type. The results provide a signicant contri- choice. The relationship between self-perception and mate preference in
bution to the area, with much of the previous research investigat- Western society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100,
ing the wider subject of body modication, making no distinction 88058810.
Cardenas, R. A., & Harris, L. J. (2005). Symmetrical decorations enhance the
between tattoos and body piercing. The use of articial stimuli al- attractiveness of faces and abstract designs. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27,
lowed the researchers to control additional parameters such as 118.
waist-to-hip ratio (Singh, 1993), shoulder width (Horvath, 1981) Carroll, S. T., Riffenburgh, R. H., Roberts, T. A., & Myhre, E. B. (2002). Tattoos and
body piercings as indicators of adolescent risk-taking behaviors. Pediatrics,
and facial features (Fink & Penton-Voak, 2002) that are known to
109(6), 10211027.
inuence attractiveness ratings. This degree of control is essential Dabbs, J. M., & Dabbs, M. G. (2000). Heroes, rogues, and lovers testosterone and
for relatively unchartered areas of research or the application of behavior. McGraw Hill.
a new perspective. Based on these ndings, a number of new ave- Dorfer, L., Moser, M., Bahr, F., et al. (1999). A medical report from the stone age?
Lancet, 354, 10231025.
nues of research are available that may vary these parameters. For Engel, E., Santarelli, F., Vasold, R., Maisch, T., Ulrich, H., Prantl, L., et al. (2008).
example, the tattoos were placed on common locations (Wohlrab Modern tattoos cause high concentrations of hazardous pigments in skin.
et al., 2005) for greater ecological validity. Typical locations com- Contact Dermatitis, 58(4), 228233.
Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. (2002). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness.
prise the lower back for women as well as the calf, causing some Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 154158.
tattoos to be arranged symmetrically, but others asymmetrically. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver, C. E. (2002). Changes in womens sexual
As both the symmetry of natural features (Fink & Penton-Voak, interests and their partners mate-retention tactics across the menstrual cycle:
Evidence for shifting conicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
2002) and articial decorations (Cardenas & Harris, 2005) inuence London, Series B, 269, 975982.
their attractiveness, future research could examine the role of sym- Geary, D. C., Vigil, J., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2004). Evolution of human mate choice. The
metry in gure perception. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 2742.
Gilbert, S. (2001). Tattoo history: A source book. Juno Books.
Further research could also investigate whether the possession Goldberg, D., & Anderson, E. (2004). Hepatitis C: Who is at risk and how do we
of a tattoo by the raters inuences their perception of others with a identify them? Journal of Viral Hepatitis, 11, 1218.
similar form of body modication. It has been suggested that mate Grammer, K., Fink, B., Mller, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (2003). Darwinian aesthetics:
Sexual Selection and the biology of beauty. Biological Reviews, 78, 385407.
choice in Western societies follow a like-attracts-like decision
Hellard, M. E., Aitken, C. K., & Hocking, J. S. (2007). Tattooing in prisons not such a
rule rather than the opposites-attract or a reproductive-poten- pretty picture. American Journal of Infection Control, 35(7), 477480.
tial-attract rule. The like-attracts-like decision rule states that Horne, J., Knox, D., Zusman, D., & Zusman, M. E. (2007). Tattoos and piercings:
partners are chosen based on a preference for similarity on several Attitudes, behaviors and interpretations of college students. College Student
Journal, 41, 4.
characteristics, one of which is physical appearance (Buston & Em- Horvath, T. (1981). Physical attractiveness: The inuence of selected torso
len, 2003). Thus future research should examine the role of the parameters. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 10, 2124.
206 S. Wohlrab et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 46 (2009) 202206

Jacob, C. I. (2002). Tattoo-associated dermatoses: A case report and review of the Scheib, J. E. (2001). Context-specic mate choice criteria: Womens trade-offs in the
literature. Dermatological Surgery, 28, 962965. contexts of long-term and extra-pair mating. Personal Relationships, 8, 371389.
Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive signicance of female physical attractiveness: Role of
attractiveness: Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 293307.
Human Behavior, 22, 251267. Singh, D., & Bronstad, P. M. (1997). Sex differences in the anatomical locations of
Kral, A. H., Bluthenthal, R. N., Lorvick, J., Gee, L., Bacchetti, P., & Edlin, B. R. (2001). human body scarication and tattooing as a function of pathogen prevalence.
Sexual transmission of HIV-1 among injection drug users in San Francisco, USA: Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 403416.
Risk-factor analysis. The Lancet, 357, 13971401. Stirn, A. (2001). Vom Initiationsritual zur geschmckten Haut. Tatowierung im
Laumann, A. E., & Derick, A. J. (2006). Tattoos and body piercings in the United Spiegel vom Stammestraditionen und neuem Kunstverstandnis. Psychotherapie
States: A national data set. Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 55, und Sozwissenschaft, 3(4), 283305.
413421. Stirn, A. (2003). Body piercing: Medical consequences and psychological
Law-Smith, M. J., Perrett, D. I., Jones, B. C., Cornwell, R. E., Moore, F. R., Feinberg, D. motivations. The Lancet, 361, 12051215.
R., et al. (2006). Facial appearance is a cue to oestrogen levels in women. Stirn, A., Hinz, A., & Brahler, E. (2006). Prevalence of tattooing and body piercing in
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 273, 135140. Germany and perception of health, mental disorders, and sensation seeking
Mariano, A., Mele, A., Tosti, M. E., Parlato, A., Gallo, G., Ragni, P., et al. (2004). Role of among tattooed and body-pierced individuals. Journal of Psychosomatic
beauty treatment in the spread of parenterally transmitted hepatitis viruses in Research, 60, 531534.
Italy. Journal of Medical Virology, 74, 216220. Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2007). Unattractive, promiscuous and heavy drinkers:
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Perceptions of women with tattoos. Body Image, 4, 343352.
Brain Sciences, 21, 353363. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). Facial attractiveness. Trends in Cognitive
Mueller, U., & Mazur, A. (1998). Reproductive constraints on dominance Sciences, 3, 452460.
competition in male Homo sapiens. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 387396. Townsend, J. (1993). Gender differences in mate preferences among law students.
Neave, N., Laing, S., Fink, B., & Manning, J. T. (2003). Second to fourth digit ratio, Journal of Psychology, 127, 507528.
testosterone, and perceived male dominance. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Tsuruta, D., Sowa, J., Higashi, N., Kobayashi, H., & Ishii, M. (2004). A red tattoo and a
London, Series B, 270, 21672172. swordsh supper. The Lancet, 364, 730.
Penton-Voak, I. S., & Chen, J. Y. (2004). High salivary testosterone is linked to Wohlrab, S., Fink, B., & Kappeler, P. M. (2005). Menschlicher Krperschmuck aus
masculine male facial appearance in humans. Evolution and Human Behaviour, evolutionrer Perspektive Diversitt und Funktionen von Ttowierungen,
25, 229241. Piercings und Skarizierungen. Mitteilungen der Anthropologischen Gesellschaft
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D., Burt, M., Kobayashi, T., & Murray, L. K. in Wien, 134(135), 110.
(1999). Female preferences for males change cyclically. Nature, 399, 741742. Wohlrab, S., Stahl, J., Rammsayer, T., & Kappeler, P. M. (2007). Differences in
Sadalla, E., Kenrick, D., & Vershure, B. (1987). Dominance and heterosexual personality characteristics between body modied and non-modied
attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 730738. individuals and possible evolutionary implications. European Journal of
Schaefer, K., Fink, B., Grammer, K., Mitteroecker, P., Gunz, P., & Bookstein, F. L. Personality, 21, 931951.
(2006). Female appearance: Facial and bodily attractiveness as shape. Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical
Psychology Science, 48, 187204. Biology, 53, 205213.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen