Sie sind auf Seite 1von 8

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jretconser

Understanding perceived retail crowding: A critical review


and research agenda
Ritu Mehta n
Marketing Group, Indian Institute of Management Calcutta, Joka, D.H. Road, Kolkata 700104, India

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Perceived retail crowding is an important, but under-researched construct in marketing research. It has
Received 17 March 2013 been found to inuence a number of retail outcomes. However, the limited empirical studies in this area
Received in revised form have not just produced seemingly contradictory results, but have also used different conceptualizations
7 June 2013
of the construct. This paper provides an explanation of the construct of perceived retail crowding and
Accepted 7 June 2013
Available online 1 July 2013
reviews the empirical studies in the area so as to integrate the knowledge that may help in
understanding of consumer behavior in crowded retail settings. The paper also presents directions for
Keywords: future research and managerial implications.
Perceived crowding & 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Store atmospherics
Empirical research
Emotions
Evaluations
Consumer behavior

1. Introduction been reported that try to explore the phenomenon of perceived


crowding and understand its effects in the retail environment.
Perceived retail crowding, also termed as perceived crowding These studies, however, lack consistency on the use of the term,
or crowding in the literature on retailing is an important element the conceptualization of the construct, and seem to have produced
of store atmospherics (Baker, 1986; Turley and Milliman, 2000). It contradictory results. Lam (2001) and Turley and Milliman (2000)
has attracted attention under social factor or human variable of provide a review of the effects of various atmospheric variables,
store atmospherics (Turley and Milliman, 2000). It has been shown including crowding, on shopping behavior. More than a decade
to inuence a number of retail outcomes such as consumers' has passed since these two reviews got published, and issues in
satisfaction (Eroglu et al., 2005; Machleit et al., 2000), attitude the area of retail crowding have advanced considerably.
towards the store (Mehta et al., 2012; Pan and Siemens, 2011), and The recent developments in the retail space also call for
behavioral responses (Hui and Bateson, 1991; Pan and Siemens, renewed attention in the area of retail crowding. There has been
2011), to name but a few. Aylott and Mitchell (1998) identied a growth in the number of shoppers and consumption especially
crowding and queuing, a function of crowding, as the two major from the emerging economies. Globally, retailers are expanding
stressors for grocery shopping. their businesses in emerging markets to avail the opportunities
The fact that many retailers use a number of strategies for arising from the growing middle class and the rising disposable
crowd management implies that they too acknowledge the fact incomes in these regions. These developments have led to greater
that consumers' perceptions of crowding can inuence their importance to the issue of retail crowding. Another interesting
behavior. Designing the layout of the retail space, specic place- development during the last decade is the proliferation of social
ment of different products, deciding on the number of sales- media and online retailing. This phenomenon could imply that
support personnel and check-out counters are some of the ways consumers feel lonelier now and consumers' preferred level of
adopted by retailers to control the level of consumers' perceived crowding might have changed over the period. All these reasons
crowding stimulated this study and provided motivation to comprehensively
Harrell and Hutt (1976) introduced the concept of perceived review the research in crowding till date.
crowding in the retailing literature. Since then many studies have This paper aims at achieving the following objectives: (1) pro-
viding a conceptual clarication of the construct of perceived retail
crowding, (2) synthesizing the ndings and methodologies of
n
Tel.: +91 33 2467 8300; fax: +91 33 2467 8307. more than three decades of empirical investigation in the area of
E-mail address: ritumehta@iimcal.ac.in crowding in the marketing literature, and delineate the possible

0969-6989/$ - see front matter & 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2013.06.002
R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649 643

sources of discrepancy in the ndings, (3) identifying possible restrictive aspects of spatial limitation become prominent and
avenues for future research. Thus, the paper will serve as a aversive. McGrew (1970) suggested that there are two types of
comprehensive source of information for researchers as well as density: social and spatial. Social density refers to the actual
marketers who are interested in understanding consumer beha- number of people in a given space and spatial density refers to
vior in crowded retail environments. the amount of space per person. Rapoport (1975) used the term
The remainder of the paper is structured as follows. First, the perceived density to refer to the subjective estimate of the number
paper discusses the conceptual background of perceived retail of people, the space available, and the organization of the space.
crowding and differentiates between the terminologies of per- He termed perceived crowding as the subjective experience of
ceived crowding, perceived density, and density that are some- certain density levels. Thus, density is an objective measure,
times used interchangeably in crowding literature. Subsequently, perceived density is subjective and neutral as it does not have
based on the literature review, it discusses the ndings and any positive or negative connotation, while perceived crowding is
comments on the methods adopted in the empirical studies on subjective and evaluative in nature.
perceived retail crowding. Finally, it draws implications for future The above discussion leads us to adopt the following deni-
research as well as practice. tions on the three constructs for the purpose of this article.
Density is a function of number of individuals and amount of
space available. One can examine human density by varying the
2. Perceived retail crowding: the evolution of the concept and actual number of individuals keeping the retail space constant, or
conceptual clarication look at spatial density by varying the space available through the
amount and placement of objects and merchandise keeping the
Perceived crowding in the retailing literature attracted atten- number of individuals constant. There is only one study (Harrell
tion from researchers in the 1970s when super stores and shop- et al., 1980) that uses the construct of density and uses actual
ping centers became increasingly popular. The rise in the number number of shoppers in the store as a measure of density. Alter-
of recreational shoppers during that period (Bellenger et al., 1977), native ways to capture shopper density are number of transactions
along with growing number of working women having limited in a given duration, sales in a given duration, or actual waiting
shopping hours, and cluttered merchandising in stores contributed time at the checkout counter (Harrell and Hutt, 1976).
to the need for inquiry into the concept of crowding in retail Perceived density is the respondents' assessment on the
settings (Harrell and Hutt, 1976). number of shoppers (termed as perceived human density) or
Prior to the introduction of the concept of crowding in retailing space available to the shoppers (termed as perceived spatial
literature, several theories from environmental psychology and social density). The human and spatial aspects of perceived density can
psychology provide insights into the phenomenon of crowding. be captured using statements on respondents' assessment about
absolute number of people in the store or number of people per
2.1. Theories to explain the phenomenon of crowding unit area, and amount of space available per person, respectively
(Hui and Bateson, 1991; Machleit et al., 2000).
The stimulus overload theory suggests that crowding is experi- The third construct, most frequently used in the literature on
enced when environmental stimulation exceeds one's capacity to retail crowding, perceived crowding is a consumer's evaluative
cope (Desor, 1972; Milgram, 1970). This theory emphasizes on assessment of the restrictive aspects of the retail space. It is a
cognitive overload associated with high density. Interference complicated construct because the same density or perceived
theories suggest that crowding is a result of dysfunctionally dense density level may or may not result in same discomfort for all
environment (Stokols et al., 1973; Sundstrom, 1975). When the individuals. A person can feel crowded even in a less dense store
level of density interferes with an individual's activities and and vice versa. It depends on a number of factors, including
achievement of goal, crowding is experienced. Control model personal (Machleit et al., 2000), situational (Eroglu and Machleit,
attributes crowded feeling to loss of perceived control in high- 1990; Machleit et al., 2000), and cultural variables (Pons et al.,
density conditions (Sherrod, 1974). This loss of control can be due 2006; Pons and Laroche, 2007). There has also been an issue with
to excessive unwanted interactions or lack of behavioral freedom the dimensionality of the construct of perceived crowding with
in the environment. Social intrusion perspective views invasion of some researchers treating it as a unidimensional construct (Eroglu
privacy as the cause of feeling crowded (Altman, 1975). Thus, the and Machleit, 1990; Hui and Bateson, 1991), while others as a two-
construct of crowding is generally associated with negative eva- dimensional construct (Eroglu et al., 2005; Harrell et al., 1980;
luation of too many people in the same environment. Whether Machleit et al., 1994; Machleit et al., 2000). Machleit et al. (1994)
these effects are salient or not depends on a number of personal, through a series of laboratory experiment and eld studies
situational, and social variables (Stokols, 1972). identied two dimensions of perceived retail crowding: human
The above stated approaches help to explain the phenomenon and spatial dimensions. The two-dimensional approach is consis-
of crowding in the context of retail settings. tent with the environmental psychology literature that suggests
two types of density: spatial and social density (McGrew, 1970) as
2.2. Conceptual clarication: density, perceived density, discussed earlier in the section. Many of the subsequent studies on
perceived crowding retail crowding recognize the importance to distinguish between
the two dimensions (Eroglu et al., 2005; Machleit et al., 2000, Pan
Most of the empirical work on crowding in retail settings and Siemens, 2011). The operationalization of the two dimensions
makes use of three interrelated terms of density, perceived is discussed next.
density, and perceived crowding. These terms have been used
interchangeably in many of these studies, but it is important to 2.3. Operationalization of perceived retail crowding
differentiate between these concepts. These concepts have been
derived from literature in psychology (Rapoport, 1975; Stokols, Harrell et al. (1980) developed a six-item, seven-point semantic
1972). Stokols (1972) contended that while density is a physical differential scale based on the literature on crowding from
state involving spatial limitation, crowding is an experiential different disciplines. The scale items were: too many shoppers
state that refers to the restrictive nature of the limited space as too few shoppers, restricts movementallows free movement, can
perceived by the individuals. Crowding is experienced when move at my own pacemust move at a pace set by others,
644 R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649

crowdeduncrowded, spaciousconned, gives an open feeling 3.1.2. Consequences of perceived retail crowding
gives a closed feeling. The study by Machleit et al. (1994) devel- There are emotional, cognitive, and behavioral consequences of
oped and validated an alternative seven-point Likert scale with a perceived retail crowding as discussed in the following sub-
few more items added to the Harrell et al. (1980) scale and based sections.
on content validity, recommend the use of two-dimensional Likert
scale over the semantic differential scale. The items on perceived
3.1.2.1. Perceived crowding, emotions, and satisfaction. So far as the
spatial crowing include the following three items: the store
consequences of perceived retail crowding are considered, most of
seemed very spacious, I felt cramped shopping in the store, the
the empirical studies investigate the impact of perceived crowding
store felt conning to shoppers. The items on perceived human
on consumers' satisfaction. Past studies have consistently produced
crowding include the following four items: the store seemed very
negative effects of spatial crowding perceptions on satisfaction
crowded to me, the store was a little too busy, there was not much
(Eroglu et al., 2005; Li et al., 2009; Machleit et al., 2000). However,
trafc in the store, and there were a lot of shoppers in the store.
the effects of human crowding perceptions have produced mixed
Subsequent studies have adopted or adapted the items from these
results. Some studies report negative effects of perceived human
two scales or have added a few items based on the literature in the
crowding on shopping satisfaction (Machleit et al., 1994; Machleit
area of crowding. The next section provides a comprehensive
et al., 2000), while a few others report positive effects (Eroglu et al.,
literature review of retail crowding.
2005; Li et al., 2009; Pons et al., 2006). The relationship between
perceived crowding and shopping satisfaction is affected by a
number of mediating and moderating variables. Emotions mediate
3. Review of empirical studies on perceived retail crowding the effect of crowding on shopping satisfaction (Eroglu et al., 2005; Li
et al., 2009; Machleit et al., 2000). Factors such as expectations of
The following section reviews empirical studies on perceived crowding, personal tolerance for crowding, and store type moderate
retail crowding right from the introduction of the concept in 1976 the crowdingsatisfaction relationship (Machleit et al., 2000).
to 2012. These studies are from marketing and services journals Shopper satisfaction was higher when perceived crowding is
including Journal of Marketing Research, Journal of Consumer short of or meets crowding expectations, and lower when
Research, Journal of Retailing, Journal of Consumer Psychology, perceived crowding exceeds expectations. For individuals with high
Journal of Business Research, Psychology and Marketing, Journal of tolerance of crowding, human crowding did not signicantly affect
Marketing Management, Marketing Letters, Journal of Services shopping satisfaction as it did for those with a low tolerance level for
Marketing, International Journal of Hospitality Management, and crowding. For discount stores, relationship between human
The Service Industries Journal. To be included in the review, the crowding and satisfaction was non-signicant. Pons and Laroche
studies had to meet the following criteria: (1) perceived retail (2007) demonstrated that culture moderates the density-satisfaction
crowding being the dominant subject in the study, (2) the article relationship. A comparison of consumers' reactions to crowd in two
to have appeared in a peer-reviewed journal, and (3) empirical different countries (Canada and Mexico) suggested that high density
studies with quantitative research approach were considered. affected Mexicans less negatively than their Canadian counterparts.
Table 1 provides a comprehensive overview of the literature on Gender was not found to signicantly affect emotions (Mehta et al.,
perceived retail crowding. On the basis of the literature review, 2012) or shopping satisfaction (Machleit et al., 2000), though a
this section discusses the ndings and the methodological pattern of stronger negative effects of crowding on satisfaction was
approaches used in different studies, and identies possible observed for females than for males (Machleit et al., 2000). Store-
reasons for the inconsistent ndings of the previous research. familiarity too was not found to affect crowding-satisfaction
relationship (Machleit et al., 2000; Mehta et al., 2012). Negative
(Eroglu and Machleit, 1990) as well as non-signicant effects have
3.1. Findings of the empirical studies on perceived retail crowding been reported for interaction of time-pressure and crowding on
satisfaction (Machleit et al., 2000).
The earlier studies focus both on the antecedents and con- The effect of perceived crowding on emotions was found to be
sequences of perceived retail crowding, while the latter studies curvilinear in a recent study by Mehta et al. (2012) and the effects
focus mainly on its consequences. were moderated by shoppers' optimal stimulation level with the
effect being stronger for individuals with high optimal stimulation
level than for individuals with low optimal stimulation level.
3.1.1. Antecedents of perceived retail crowding
Earlier studies demonstrate that perceived retail crowding 3.1.2.2. Perceived crowding and evaluations. The effect of perceived
increases with shopper density (Harrell et al., 1980) and perceived crowding on store attitudes and evaluations were found to be
density (Eroglu and Machleit, 1990). Spatial as well as human curvilinear in more recent studies. Medium level of crowding was
density perceptions affect both spatial and human crowding associated with more favorable store attitudes (Pan and Siemens,
perceptions (Machleit et al., 2000). However, there are other 2011) and merchandise evaluations (Mehta et al., 2012). Store
variables that mediate or moderate the relationship between attitudes were assessed through impressions about the store at an
perceived density and perceived crowding. Perceived density aggregate level (unfavorable/favorable, bad/good, unlikable/
affects perceived crowding directly as well as indirectly through likable). Merchandise evaluations were based on perceptions of
perceived control (Hui and Bateson, 1991). Factors such as task merchandise quality (low/high), style (outdated/up-to-date), and
orientation, perceived risk, and time pressure were found to affect selection (in-adequate/adequate). In the context of restaurant,
crowding perceptions under high-density conditions (Eroglu and Tse et al. (2002) found that the level of perceived crowding is
Machleit, 1990). Gender differences were found with men report- positively associated with high-quality food, low price, and good
ing higher crowding perceptions than women under high-density reputation of the restaurant. Similarly, Pan and Siemens (2011)
conditions (Eroglu and Machleit, 1990). Culture inuences per- found linear effects of perceived crowding on store attitudes in
ceived crowding with a dense shopping situation triggering higher case of service setting (salon), but an inverted U-shaped trend for
perceived crowding for Canadian consumers than for Mexicans individuals who were pressed for time. In the context of services,
(Pons and Laroche, 2007). Giebelhausen et al. (2011) demonstrated that in situations where
R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649 645

Table 1
Summary table of past studies on perceived retail crowding.

Study Sample Setting Method Independent Dependent variables Main ndings


Variables

Harrell et al. (1980) 600 shoppers Supermarket Field study Shopper density Perceived crowding, Retail density positively affects crowding
adaptation strategies, perceptions. Perceived crowding
outcomes negatively affects satisfaction. Task
orientation, perceived risk, and time
pressure, intensify crowding perceptions
under high-density conditions.
Eroglu and 112 adults Mall Laboratory Perceived retail Perceived crowding, Retail density positively affects crowding
Machleit (1990) experiment: slides density, motive, time shoppers' satisfaction perceptions. Perceived crowding
constraint, perceived with the retail negatively affects satisfaction. Task
risk, shopping environment orientation, perceived risk, and time
motives, gender pressure, intensify crowding perceptions
under high-density conditions.
Hui and Bateson 115 adults between Bank, bar Laboratory Perceived consumer Perceived control, Perceived control mediates the effects of
(1991) the age 25 and 40 experiment: slides density, choice, perceived crowding, consumer density and consumer choice
service setting pleasure, approach- on pleasantness and behavior.
avoidance
Machleit et al. 76 students, 140 Bookstore, Laboratory Dimensionality of Satisfaction Perceived crowding has two distinct
(1994) bookstore shoppers grocery store, experiment: perceived crowding, human and spatial dimensions that affect
(mainly students), discount store videotapes, eld crowding expectation satisfaction differently. The effect of
232 store shoppers studies human crowding on satisfaction is
moderated by expected level of crowding.
Machleit et al. 722 students (eld Variety of stores Field studies with Perceived crowding, Emotions, satisfaction Perceived crowding negatively affects
(2000) study), 153 non- like mall, take-home expectation about shopping satisfaction. Emotions mediate
students, 231 department, questionnaire, crowding, store-type, the crowding-satisfaction relationship.
students (lab study) grocery store laboratory personal tolerance, Expectation, store-type, and personal
etc. experiment: gender tolerance moderate the crowding-
videotapes satisfaction relationship.
Tse et al. (2002) 300 adults Restaurant Laboratory Perceived crowding Food quality, food Perceived crowding is associated with
experiment: pictures price, reputation of high food quality, low food price, and
restaurant good reputation of the restaurant.
Eroglu et al. (2005) 153 adults, 296 Variety of stores Field studies with Perceived crowding Emotions, shopping Perceived crowding affects shopping
students like grocery take-home value, and value via emotions. Emotions and
department, questionnaire satisfaction shopping value completely mediate the
hypermart, mall spatial crowding-satisfaction relationship.
etc. However, for human crowding there
exists a positive direct effect on shopping
satisfaction.
Pons et al. (2006) Canadian Disco Laboratory Perceived density, Affective evaluation, The density manifestations produce
undergraduates and experiment: personal space, satisfaction positive responses from consumers in
244 Lebanese videotapes privacy, territoriality, crowded hedonic service. Culture affects
undergraduates freedom of perceptions of density manifestations and
movement moderates consumers reactions to a
crowded retail environment.
Pons and Laroche 861 Canadian Bookstore Laboratory Perceived density, Perceived crowding, Consumers perceive and react to crowded
(2007) undergraduates, 862 experiment: density expectation, affective evaluation, situations differently, depending on their
Mexican videotapes culture shopping satisfaction culture. Expectations affect the way
undergraduates consumers evaluate crowded settings.
Mattila and Wirtz 138 shoppers Variety of Field study Perceived crowding, Impulse buying Perceived crowding and employee
(2008) outlets like environmental assistance jointly affect impulse buying.
book and music stimulation, Over stimulation has a positive effect on
store, cosmetics, employee assistance impulse purchases.
clothing store
Li et al. (2009) 554 shoppers Hypermarket Field study Perceived crowding Emotions, Perceived human crowding positively
satisfaction, impacted emotions, while perceived
behavioral outcomes spatial crowding negatively affected
emotions. Retail crowding affects
shopping behaviors through emotions
and satisfaction.
Pan and Siemens 270 undergraduates, Clothing store, Laboratory Perceived crowding, Store attitude, Perceived crowding exhibits an inverted
(2011) 403 faculty, staff and salon experiments: time pressure behavioral intentions U-shaped relationship with shoppers'
students photographs store attitudes and behavioral intentions
in a goods setting. The relationship is
linear in a service setting except under
high pressure.
Mehta et al. (2012) 201 adult shoppers Hypermarket Field study Perceived crowding, Emotions, The effects of perceived crowding on
optimal stimulation evaluations, patronage intention is curvilinear and is
level patronage intention mediated by pleasure and store-
evaluation. The effects of perceived
crowding on emotions and evaluations
appear to be moderated by optimal
stimulation level.

quality is important or wherein the customer is not familiar with 3.1.2.3. Perceived crowding and shopping value assessment. Perceived
the service provider, perceived wait manipulated through the retail crowding negatively affects shopping values and the effects
presence of a crowd increases perceived quality. appear to be moderated by factors such as personal tolerance for
646 R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649

crowding, time spent shopping, shopping intention, and whether Machleit et al. (2000) reports non-signicant effects with the
purchase was made (Eroglu et al., 2005). For individuals with low student sample, and signicant effects with non-student sample.
tolerance for crowding, and with purchase-directed intention of visit, Possible reasons for this conicting nding could be the con-
there were strong effects of human crowding perceptions on both founding variables not accounted for, such as tolerance for
utilitarian and hedonic types of shopping value. For those who spent crowding and expectation of crowding.
less than an hour in the store, there were strong negative correlations
between human and spatial crowding perceptions and both the 3.1.4. Managerial implications of the ndings
types of shopping values. Adaptability to the high-density stimuli Research indicates that perceived crowding is an important yet a
was possibly at play for individuals who spent more time in the store. difcult variable to control for managers. It presents a challenge for
The effect of perceived crowding on shopping value is mediated by retailers as to how to maximize density without triggering the
emotions experienced by the shopper. The emotions and shopping negative effects of perceived crowding (Harrell and Hutt, 1976;
value reactions, in turn, completely or partially mediate the effects of Harrell et al., 1980). Managers should anticipate crowd levels and
spatial and human crowding perceptions on satisfaction respectively consumers shopping patterns under different levels of crowding.
(Eroglu et al., 2005). Accordingly they should make arrangements and devise their product
and promotion strategies (Harrell et al., 1980). Designing architectural
3.1.2.4. Perceived crowding, coping, and behavioral responses. Perceived features of the store and the store environment can help alleviate
crowding also affects consumers' shopping behavior and behavioral feeling of crowding (Eroglu and Machleit, 1990). For example, adjust-
intentions. Shoppers may use a number of adaptation strategies such ments in store atmospheric variables like music and temperature can
as adjustment in shopping time and deviation from shopping plan to help reduce feeling of human crowding. Likewise, arrangement of
cope with perceived crowding (Harrell et al., 1980). Mattila and Wirtz merchandise and layout of the store can mitigate the feeling of spatial
(2008) found that employee assistance could reduce the negative crowding (Machleit et al., 1994). Retailers can have extra staff and
effects of perceived human crowding on unplanned purchases. extra check-out counters to handle the large number of shoppers
Perceived human crowding also affects consumers' store patronage during peak shopping times (Machleit et al., 2000; Mehta et al., 2012).
intentions, but the relationship follows an inverted U-shaped trend Since the negative effects of high consumer density can be minimized
(Mehta et al., 2012, Pan and Siemens, 2011). Consumers' emotional by providing some control to the consumers, retailers can provide
dimension of pleasure, and store evaluation mediate these effects information, a form of cognitive control, to shoppers as to the length
(Mehta et al., 2012). The effect of crowding on behavioral intentions of the queue so that they are prepared for the wait (Hui and Bateson,
like entering the store and recommending the store for a service 1991). Similarly, designing the store so that shoppers have a choice to
setting was not found to be inverted U-shaped, but linear except move around and alter their directions if needed, would provide them
under conditions of time pressure (Pan and Siemens, 2011). behavioral control to mitigate the negative consequences of crowding
(Li et al., 2009). Retailers should manage expectations of density to
reduce consumers' dissatisfaction with the shopping experience (Pons
3.1.3. Summary of the ndings and Laroche, 2007). They should also try to gure out how shoppers
To summarize, the perceptions of human and spatial densities react to the strategies they design to avoid the negative effects of
affect both the dimensions of consumers' perceived crowding. crowding (Machleit et al., 2000). Since shoppers' reactions to different
Individual variables like personal tolerance for crowding and situa- levels of crowding vary with culture, international retail organizations
tional variables such as task orientation, perceived risk, and time should aim at recognizing and understanding the cultural differences
pressure may affect perceived crowding levels. Giving some control in assessment and dealing of crowded retail settings (Pons et al.,
to the consumers can minimize the experience of crowding and its 2006; Pons and Laroche, 2007). Managers should have a dynamic
negative effects. Consumers' emotions, cognitive evaluations of the approach to crowd management so that they are able to maintain the
store, shopping value, and coping mechanisms seem to mediate the optimal level of crowding for their stores (Mehta et al., 2012).
effects of perceived crowding on retail outcomes.
The differential effects of perceived human crowding observed in
3.2. Perceived retail crowding and waiting research1
the empirical studies might be explained in the context of choice of
the retail setting. For instance, negative effects of crowding are more
The previous section presented ndings of studies with per-
prominent in utilitarian settings such as grocery stores (Machleit et al.,
ceived retail crowding as the primary focus of the study. Since
1994), book stores (Pons and Laroche, 2007), and banks (Hui and
crowding is related to wait expectation and waiting time, this
Bateson, 1991). Crowded hedonic settings like discos (Pons et al., 2006)
section establishes the link between crowding literature and
and restaurants (Tse et al., 2002) have produced positive responses
waiting time literature.
from consumers. In neutral settings, the effects were found to be
Grewal et al. (2003) demonstrated that higher perceived
inverted U-shaped (Mehta et al., 2012). A possible explanation for the
consumer density leads to longer wait perception that negatively
positive effects of human crowding found in the study by Eroglu et al.
affects evaluation of the store's atmosphere and patronage inten-
(2005) wherein respondents report their responses after their visit to
tion in a service-intensive store setting. Prior research in waiting
any of the retail outlets like grocery, hypermarket, or departmental
literature too reports negative effects of waiting time on affective
stores could be due to overall moderate level of perceived crowding at
reactions and evaluations (Dube-Rioux et al., 1989; Taylor, 1994).
the time the respondents chose to visit these stores.
Moreover, earlier studies suggest waiting to be associated with
The effects of perceived crowding also appear to be contingent
economic (time and effort) and psychological (stress) cost (Baker
on several other factors as expectations of crowding, personal
et al., 2002; Osuna, 1985).
tolerance for crowding, individual's optimal stimulation level,
A recent study by Giebelhausen et al. (2011) manipulated
store-type, time spent in the store, and shopping intention.
wait by the presence or absence of crowd in different service
Gender has produced non-signicant, or at best inconclusive
settings. The study found that the presence of a wait resulted in
moderating effects on consumer responses. Interactive effects of
signicantly lower purchase intentions for shoppers with conve-
time pressure and crowding are mixed with some studies report-
nience as the primary motivation. However, for shoppers
ing non-signicant effects (Machleit et al., 2000) and others
negative effects (Eroglu and Machleit, 1990; Machleit et al.,
2000) in retail store setting. Interestingly, the same study by 1
We thank the referee for suggesting to add this issue.
R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649 647

motivated by quality the presence of a wait signicantly increased on perceived retail crowding over the years, there are issues that
purchase intentions. Additionally, after accounting for the mediat- still need attention. The implications for research, including
ing effect of quality perceptions, the effect of wait on purchase method and practice are presented below.
intentions was found to be negative. The study highlights the need
to control for variables such as shopping motivation and perceived 4.1. Implications for research: issues
quality in studies on waiting and perceived crowding in order to
capture their true effects. The study also presents evidence that There is sufcient evidence for the mediating role of emotions
there might be curvilinear effects (inverted U-shaped) of wait on on the response to perceived retail crowding. However, the gamut
shopping outcomes as found in recent studies on crowding (Mehta of emotions induced by perceived retail crowding is still not clear.
et al., 2012; Pan and Siemens, 2011). Most studies make use of the pleasure and arousal dimensions
of Mehrabian and Russell's (1974) PAD (PleasureArousal
3.3. Method of the studies Dominance) scale to measure emotions. Machleit and Eroglu
(2000) suggest that the emotional dimension of dominance may
Research in perceived retail crowding makes use of either or both be of critical signicance in studies on crowding where control
of eld and laboratory studies, as seen in Table 1. Field studies have over the retail environment is a concern. Yet most studies do not
the advantage of external and ecological validity. But many of the include the emotional dimension of dominance (e.g., Hui and
other environmental elements that can inuence the studies on Bateson, 1991, Mehta et al., 2012). Other scales, such as PANAS
perceived crowding are not controlled for, and thus the causal nature (Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, Watson et al., 1988) may be
of the relationships is not clear. Laboratory studies use slides or used instead of or in addition to the PAD scale.
videotapes to provide a simulated store environment. These studies The effect of perceived crowding on store image has not been
may suffer from experiential realism and individuals may not behave tested sufciently. A few studies investigate the effect of perceived
the same in actual retail environments. Bateson and Hui (1992) crowding on store attitude (e.g., Pan and Siemens, 2011) and
demonstrated that slides and videotapes used as environmental evaluations (e.g., Mehta et al., 2012), however these studies have
simulation provide ecological validity. Also, experimental design gives looked at the overall impression of the store. The effects of
better control over confounding variables. But the disadvantage is that perceived retail crowding on specic attributes such as percep-
certain dimensions like temperature, smell, and noise are eliminated tions of service-quality and price range, and discount or exclusive
(McClelland and Auslander, 1978). These dimensions go hand in hand image of the store have not been studied. We anticipate that
with the number of shoppers and therefore consumer responses may perceived retail crowding should be negatively related to service-
differ in actual retail setting and laboratory conditions. For example, quality, price perceptions, and exclusive store-image.
Machleit et al. (1994) found in a laboratory simulation that human Additional research is necessary to unravel the mechanisms
crowding has more effect than spatial crowding on store satisfaction. consumers use to cope with the negative effects of perceived retail
Contradictorily, the correlation between human crowding and satis- crowding and the precise role of coping mechanism in the
faction was not found to be signicant in the eld study. The authors relationship between perceived crowding and retail outcomes.
subsequently found that consumers' expectations about the number For example, Harrell et al. (1980) found that certain coping
of shoppers moderates the relationship between human crowding mechanisms like deviation from planned shopping time mediate
and store satisfaction, and since expectation may be missing in the the effects of perceived crowding on satisfaction. However, such a
laboratory conditions it resulted in different ndings on the effects of deviation may not always be possible, especially with growing
human crowding on satisfaction. working class of population. Thus, it would be interesting to know
The eld and laboratory studies both have been conducted with about other coping measures frequently undertaken, whether
student as well as non-student sample. The study by Machleit there is an alteration in behavior with other shoppers, and
et al. (2000) resulted in almost similar responses to perceived whether these have immediate effects or have long term implica-
crowding from student and non-student respondents. For the eld tions as on consumers' store-loyalty and patronage intentions.
studies, intercept surveys with actual shoppers have been carried The interactive effects of perceived density and other atmo-
out (Harrell et al., 1980; Li et al., 2009; Machleit et al., 1994, Mehta spheric variables on perceived crowding and shopper responses
et al., 2012) or the respondents were asked to ll out a retro- have received little attention. Extant research suggests that con-
spective questionnaire after their next shopping trip (Eroglu et al., gruency of different environmental stimuli affects consumers'
2005; Machleit et al., 2000). All eld studies, except for Harrell responses. For instance, Mattila and Wirtz (2001) demonstrated
et al. (1980) have used convenience sampling. that congruency between ambient scent and music in terms of
Finally, studies have been conducted in different settings, their arousing qualities results in enhanced perceptions of the
ranging from a mall to different types of retail stores such as store environment. Spangenberg et al. (2006) found that consu-
grocery stores, departmental stores, and bookstores, as well as mers' evaluations in a Christmas shopping context are more
service settings such as banks, salons, and restaurant bar. As favorable when the Christmas scent is in the presence of Christmas
mentioned in the review section, the effects of perceived crowding music. Likewise, perceived crowding may interact with other
have been found to vary with the type of store (Machleit et al., atmospheric variables to inuence congruency and thereby con-
2000; Pan and Siemens, 2011), thus the ndings should be sumer responses. Baker et al. (1994) found that classical music,
extrapolated to other settings with caution. soft lighting, and green/peach color scheme are associated with
prestige-image store environment. It would be interesting to see
how these prestige-image store elements interact with perceived
4. Conclusions crowding, i.e., whether they help in alleviating the negative
connotations associated with an overcrowded store such as low-
The empirical studies in this review highlight that perceived quality merchandise (Mehta et al., 2012) or whether they lower
retail crowding is a complex process that results from a combina- the evaluations of the overall prestige-image store environment.
tion of many personal, cultural, situational, and environmental The effects of demographic variables and personality traits have
factors. The studies also underline the innumerable ways in which not been sufciently explored in the context of retail crowding. An
perceived crowding can affect consumer behavior and shopping individual's optimal stimulation level (OSL) was found to moderate
outcomes in retail settings. In spite of the growth in the literature the effects of perceived human crowding (Mehta et al., 2012). Studies
648 R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649

have shown that gender, age, education, and employment status are that the results may be distorted because of reliance on memory
correlated with OSL (Raju, 1980, Zuckerman et al., 1978) about the shopping episode (Barrett and Barrett, 2001).
Studies have also demonstrated that crowding affects task perfor- Empirical studies on perceived retail crowding should also
mance of extraverts signicantly more than that of introverts (Khew ensure that there is enough variability in the independent variable
and Brebner, 1985). Thus, demographic and personality variables can before concluding for linear or curvilinear effects. If the entire
play an important role in retail crowding research. range of the variable is not represented, one might nd a linear
Studies in environmental psychology suggest that the architec- relationship simply because of range restriction.
ture of a space can result in feeling of invasion of personal space. Observational method can also be used to enrich the under-
For example, lowered ceiling height (Savinar, 1975) and reduction standing of consumer behavior in crowded environments. For
in lighting (Adams and Zuckerman, 1991) can cause an increase in example, one can make note of the number of items examined,
personal space requirements. Thus, it would be interesting to test time spent viewing the items, shoppers' reliance on pre-planned
the effects of different architectural designs on perceived crowding list of items to be purchased, and the kind of interactions
and shopping behavior. customers have with each other or the sales staff. One can also
Perceived retail crowding may result in waiting time as examine the path undertaken under crowded situationsdo
discussed earlier. We briey investigate the links between the shoppers get attracted to crowds and exhibit bandwagon effect
two issues in Section 3.2, but there is a need to integrate the or do they prefer to avoid it.
ndings further. Also, since crowding implies greater exposure to
other shoppers present in the same environment, crowded envir-
onments are likely to boost customer-to-customer effects that 4.3. Implications for practice
exist in the social inuence research. For instance, Argo et al.
(2008) found that the attractiveness of other shoppers and their Perceived crowding in retail settings, both human and spatial, can
gender affects evaluations of touched products. Thakor et al. inuence consumer behaviors. Since the optimal level of crowding
(2008) found that young consumers' evaluation of the service that maximizes positive shopping outcomes may vary with the store-
was negatively affected in the presence of older consumers. Thus, type, managers should try to gure out the optimal level for their
the role of other shoppers in inuencing consumptions needs to stores. Adjusting the timing of promotions, and extending the
be further investigated in crowded retail settings. operating hours of the store on weekends can help to shift crowds
from peak to non-peak hours. Managers can primarily control
perceived spatial crowding by concentrating on store design and
4.2. Implications for research: method layout. For instance, ceiling height should be considered while
designing the store. Similarly, placement and arrangement of racks,
Field experiments have not been conducted in the existing merchandise, and displays can also be worked upon to eliminate the
feelings of over-crowdedness in the stores. Managers can also take the
studies on perceived retail crowding. Field experiments that would
allow manipulation of the level of perceived crowding through help of other atmospheric cues such as choice of store color and
brightness of light to alleviate the negative effects of perceived
control over number of shoppers or arrangement of merchandise
can be especially useful for studies on crowding. Thereby partici- crowding. Managers should also help consumers to cope with
crowding. Playing music, dispersing the right kind of scent, adjusting
pants' emotions and behaviors can be captured more naturally.
However eld experiments can be more expensive and more time- the temperature of the store, providing seating arrangement, and
having play zone for kids are some of the measures that can help to
consuming than laboratory experiments. Also control over the
environment may be difcult. One important reason for this is the ease out the stress resulting from feeling of crowdedness. The
dispersed placement of various items on promotions and strategically
researcher's ability to control and manipulate conditions shown in
pictures such as height and size of space, details in drawings, an installing televisions can also help to spread out the crowds.
Disney is a good example of crowd management (Borrie, 1999).
elimination of other environmental features.
There is some concern with having a homogenous sample as Visual magnets, such as castles, are placed at the end of thorough-
fares to motivate customers to keep moving on. Disney characters
that of students in studies on crowding as they may exhibit similar
responsiveness to crowding and bias the results. Therefore, it may move in different directions to scatter the crowds. Disney arranges
shows and parades during busy seasons as an additional attraction
be a good idea to supplement the results obtained from student
sample with a more representative group of respondents that to disperse the crowds, and as a compensation for the busy
conditions. It also adjusts the operating hours of the parks
takes into account the age factor.
The eld studies make use of a single survey to measure all depending on the anticipated crowd levels. The parks generally
variables, including the independent, dependent, mediating, and/or open earlier and stay open for longer hours during those times of
moderating variables. A common problem with this type of a design is the year when it witnesses huge number of visitors than when
common method variance that may cause systematic measurement there is a modest level of crowd.
error and inate or deate observed relationships between con- Cross-cultural studies on perceived retail crowding have estab-
structs. When using such a design, common method variance should lished that different cultures perceive and react to crowded environ-
be assessed and accounted for, in case there are biases. The results of ments differently. Depending on the expectations of appropriate
eld studies may also suffer from self-selection bias because of the distance for communication and interactions between strangers
use of convenience sampling. Random sampling may be used to within different cultures, the same space can produce different
improve the generalizability of the results. Another issue with eld reactions from customers. Multi-national retailers should keep these
studies using store-intercept surveys is the timing of the intercept. cultural differences in mind so as to design their stores to meet the
Some studies conduct it before the respondents enter the check stand customers' expectations.
(e.g., Mehta et al., 2012) while a few others carry it out as the
consumers exit the store (e.g., Machleit et al., 1994, Mattila and Wirtz,
2008). When data is collected at the point of exit, the responses to Acknowledgments
perceived crowding can be mingled with other experiences such as
the time one had to spend at the check-out counter, and therefore, We thank the editor and the anonymous reviewers for provid-
should be avoided. Moreover, a concern with retrospective surveys is ing valuable comments and suggestions on this paper.
R. Mehta / Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services 20 (2013) 642649 649

References Machleit, K.A., Eroglu, S.A., Mantel, S.P., 2000. Perceived retail crowding and
shopping satisfaction: what modies this relationship? Journal of Consumer
Psychology 9 (1), 2942.
Adams, L., Zuckerman, D., 1991. The effect of lighting conditions on personal space Mattila, A., Wirtz, J., 2008. The role of environmental stimulation and social factors
requirements. Journal of General Psychology 118 (4), 335340. on impulse purchasing. Journal of Services Marketing 22 (7), 562567.
Altman, I., 1975. The Environment and Social Behaviour: Privacy, Personal Space, Mattila, A.S., Wirtz, J., 2001. Congruency of scent and music as a driver of in-store
Territory and Crowding. Brooks/Cole, Monterey, California. evaluations and behavior. Journal of Retailing 77 (2), 273289.
Argo, J.J., Dahl, D.W., Morales, A.C., 2008. Positive consumer contagion: responses to McClelland, L., Auslander, N., 1978. Perceptions of crowding and pleasantness in
attractive others in a retail context. Journal of Marketing Research 45 (6), public setting. Environment and Behavior 10 (4), 535553.
690701. McGrew, P.L., 1970. Social and spatial density effects of spacing behavior in
Aylott, R., Mitchell, V-M., 1998. An exploratory study of grocery shopping stressors. preschool children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 11 (3), 197205.
International Journal of Retail and Distribution Management 26 (9), 362373. Mehrabian, A., Russell, J.A., 1974. An Approach to Environmental Psychology. MIT
Baker, J., 1986. The role of the environment in marketing services: the consumer Press, Cambridge, MA.
perspective. In: Cepeil, J.A., et al. (Eds.), The Services Challenge: Integrating for Mehta, R., Sharma, N.K., Swami, S., 2012. The impact of perceived crowding on
Competitive Advantage. American Marketing Association, Chicago, IL, pp. 7984. consumers' store patronage intentions: role of optimal stimulation level and
Baker, J., Grewal, D., Parasuraman, A., 1994. The inuence of store environment on shopping motivation. Journal of Marketing Management , http://dx.doi.org/
quality inference and store image. Journal of Academy of Marketing Science 10.1080/0267257X.2012.729075.
22 (4), 328339. Milgram, S., 1970. The experience of living in cities. Science 167, 14611468.
Baker, J., Parasuraman, A., Grewal, D., Voss, G.B., 2002. The inuence of multiple Osuna, E.E., 1985. The psychological cost of waiting. Journal of Mathematical
store environment cues on perceived merchandise value and patronage Psychology 29 (1), 82105.
intentions. Journal of Marketing 66 (2), 120141. Pan, Y., Siemens, J.C., 2011. The differential effects of retail density: an investigation
Barrett, L.F., Barrett, D.J., 2001. An introduction to computerized experience of goods versus service settings. Journal of Business Research 64 (2), 105112.
sampling in psychology. Social Science Computer Review 19 (2), 175185. Pons, F., Laroche, M., 2007. Cross-cultural differences in crowd assessment: the
Bateson, John E.G., Michael K., Hui, 1992. "The Ecological Validity of Photographic differential inuence of density expectations on consumers' shopping experi-
Slides and Videotapes in Simulating the Service Setting,". Journal of Consumer ence in Mexico and Canada. Journal of Business Research 60 (3), 269276.
Research 19, 271284. Pons, F., Laroche, M., Mourali, M., 2006. Consumer reactions to crowded retail
Bellenger, D., Robertson, D.H., Greenberg, B.A., 1977. Shopping center patronage settings: cross-cultural differences between North America and the Middle
motives. Journal of Retailing 53 (2), 2938. East. Psychology and Marketing 23 (7), 555638.
Borrie, W.T., 1999. Disneyland and Disney World: Designing and prescribing the Raju, P.S., 1980. Optimum stimulation level: Its relationship to personality, demo-
recreational experience. Loisir et Socit/Society and Leisure 22 (1), 7182. graphics, and exploratory behaviour. Journal of Consumer Research 7 (3),
Desor, J.A., 1972. Toward a psychological theory of crowding. Journal of Personality 272282.
and Social Psychology 21, 7983. Rapoport, A., 1975. Toward a redenition of density. Environment and Behavior
Dube-Rioux, L., Schmitt, B.H., Leclerc, F., 1989. Consumers' reactions to waiting: 7 (2), 133158.
when delays affect the perception of service quality. Advances in Consumer Savinar, J., 1975. The effect of ceiling height on personal space. ManEnvironmental
Research 16, 5963. Systems 5, 321324.
Eroglu, S.A., Machleit, K.A., 1990. An empirical study of retail crowding: antecedents Sherrod, D., 1974. Crowding, perceived control and behavioral after-effects. Journal
and consequences. Journal of Retailing 66 (2), 201221. of Applied Social Psychology 4, 171186.
Eroglu, S.A., Machleit, K.A., Barr, T.F., 2005. Perceived retail crowding and shopping Spangenberg, E.R., Sprott, D.E., Grohmann, B., Tracy, D.L., 2006. Gender-congruent
satisfaction: the role of shopping values. Journal of Business Research 58 (8), ambient scent inuences on approach and avoidance behaviors in a retail store.
11461153. Journal of Business Research 59 (12), 12811287.
Giebelhausen, M.D., Robinson, S.G., Cronin, J.J., 2011. Worth waiting for: increasing Stokols, D., 1972. On the distinction between density and crowding: some
satisfaction by making consumers wait. Journal of the Advancement of Market- implications for future research. Psychological Review 79 (3), 275277.
ing Science 39 (9), 889905. Stokols, D., Rall, M., Pinner, B., Schopler, J., 1973. Physical, social, and personal
Grewal, D., Baker, J., Levy, M., Voss, G.B., 2003. The effects of wait expectations and antecedents of the perception of crowding. Environment and Behavior 5 (1),
store atmosphere evaluations on patronage intentions in service-intensive 87115.
retail stores. Journal of Retailing 79 (4), 259268. Sundstrom, E., 1975. An experimental study of crowding: Effects of room size,
Harrell, G.D., Hutt, M.D., 1976. Buyer behavior under conditions of crowding: an intrusion, and goalblocking on nonverbal behaviors, self-disclosure, and self-
initial framework. Advances in Consumer Research 3, 3639. reported stress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32 (4), 645654.
Harrell, G.D., Hutt, M.D., Anderson, J.C., 1980. Path analysis of buyer behavior under Taylor, S., 1994. Waiting for services: the relationship between delays and
conditions of crowding. Journal of Marketing Research 17 (1), 4551. evaluations of service. Journal of Marketing 58 (2), 5669.
Hui, M.K., Bateson, J.E.G., 1991. Perceived control and the effects of crowding and Thakor, M.V., Suri, R., Saleh, K., 2008. Effects of service setting and other consumers'
consumer choice on the service experience. Journal of Consumer Research age on the service perceptions of young consumers. Journal of Retailing 84 (2),
18 (2), 174184. 137149.
Khew, K., Brebner, J., 1985. The role of personality in crowding research. Personality Tse, A.C.B., Sin, L., Yim, F.H.K., 2002. How a crowded restaurant affects consumers'
and Individual Differences 6 (5), 641643. attribution behavior. International Journal of Hospitality Management 21 (4),
Lam, S.Y., 2001. The effects of store environment on shopping behaviors: a critical 449454.
review. Advances in Consumer Research 28, 190197. Turley, L.W., Milliman, R.E., 2000. Atmospheric effects on shopping behavior.
Li, J.-G.T., Kim, J.-O., Lee, S.Y., 2009. An empirical examination of perceived retail A review of the experimental evidence. Journal of Business Research 49 (2),
crowding, emotions, and retail outcomes. The Service Industries Journal 29 (5), 193211.
635652. Watson, D., Clark, L.A., Tellegen, A., 1988. Development and validation of brief
Machleit, K.A., Eroglu, S.A., 2000. Describing and measuring emotional response to measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of
shopping experience. Journal of Business Research 49 (2), 101111. Personality and Social Psychology 54 (6), 10631070.
Machleit, K.A., Kellaris, J.J., Eroglu, S.A., 1994. Human and spatial dimensions of Zuckerman, M., Eysenck, S.B.J., Eysenck, H.J., 1978. Sensation seeking in England
crowding perceptions in retail environments: a note on their measurement and and America: cross-cultural, age, and sex comparisons. Journal of Consulting
effect on shoppers' satisfaction. Marketing Letters 5 (2), 183194. and Clinical Psychology 46 (1), 139149.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen