Sie sind auf Seite 1von 7

This article was downloaded by: [Auburn University]

On: 24 September 2013, At: 11:07


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,
37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of Sports Sciences


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjsp20

Maximal strength and cortisol responses to psyching-up


during the squat exercise
a b c
Michael R McGuigan , Jamie Ghiagiarelli & David Tod
a
School of Biomedical and Sports Science, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA, Australia
b
Department of Exercise and Sport Science, University of Wisconsin-La Crosse, La Crosse,
WI, USA
c
Centre for Sport and Exercise Science, University of Wales Aberystwyth, Aberystwyth, UK
d
School of Biomedical and Sports Science, Edith Cowan University, 100 Joondalup Drive,
Joondalup, WA 6027, Australia E-mail:
Published online: 18 Feb 2007.

To cite this article: Michael R McGuigan , Jamie Ghiagiarelli & David Tod (2005) Maximal strength and cortisol responses to
psyching-up during the squat exercise, Journal of Sports Sciences, 23:7, 687-692, DOI: 10.1080/02640410400021401

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02640410400021401

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the Content) contained
in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no
representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and
are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for
any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever
or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of
the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic
reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://
www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
Journal of Sports Sciences, July 2005; 23(7): 687 692

Maximal strength and cortisol responses to psyching-up during the


squat exercise

MICHAEL R. McGUIGAN1, JAMIE GHIAGIARELLI2, & DAVID TOD3


1
School of Biomedical and Sports Science, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA, Australia, 2Department of Exercise and
Sport Science, University of Wisconsin-La Crosse, La Crosse, WI, USA, and 3Centre for Sport and Exercise Science, University
of Wales Aberystwyth, Aberystwyth, UK

(Accepted 23 September 2004)


Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 11:07 24 September 2013

Abstract
We studied the effect of psyching-up on one-repetition maximum (1-RM) performance and salivary cortisol responses
during the squat exercise. Ten men (age 21.6 + 1.4 years; mean + s) and ten women (age 22.4 + 2.8 years) with weight
training experience of 4.5 + 2.0 years participated in this study. One-repetition maximum squats were performed on a Smith
machine during each of two different intervention conditions that were counterbalanced and consisted of a free choice psych-
up and a cognitive distraction. Saliva samples were obtained at the beginning of each test session and immediately after the
nal 1-RM attempt. No signicant difference in 1-RM was identied between psyching-up (104 + 50 kg) and cognitive
distraction (106 + 52 kg). Performing a 1-RM in the squat exercise signicantly increased salivary cortisol concentrations
during both conditions (P 5 0.05). There was no signicant difference in salivary cortisol responses between conditions.
These results suggest that psyching-up does not increase 1-RM performance during the squat exercise in strength-trained
individuals.

Keywords: Mental preparation, performance enhancement, psychology, strength

1988; Tenenbaum, Bar-Eli, Hoffman, & Jablonovs-


Introduction
ki, 1995; Theodorakis et al., 2000). These negative
Psyching-up refers to the use of self-directed results cast doubt on the efcacy of the psych-up
cognitive strategies designed to enhance physical effect when participants are required to divide their
performance (Tod, Iredale, & Gill, 2003). The attention across both a strength movement and a
common belief among many athletes is that the use mental preparation procedure.
of these strategies will enable them to lift heavier It is widely accepted that stress induces activation
loads (Tod et al., 2003). Strategies used by athletes of the hypothalamus pituitary adrenal axis (HPA)
typically include preparatory arousal, imagery, atten- (Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1994). In many
tional focus and positive self-talk (Shelton & studies, the endocrine response of participants to
Mahoney, 1978). Researchers have found that varying types of stress has been tested (Kirschbaum
psyching-up may increase dynamic strength (Elko & Hellhammer, 1994). Acute resistance exercise
& Ostrow, 1992; Gould, Weinberg, & Jackson, 1980; induces a change in plasma cortisol concentrations
Theodorakis, Weinberg, Natsis, Douma, & Kazakas, (Mulligan et al., 1996). Salivary samples provide a
2000; Tynes & McFatter, 1987; Weinberg, Gould, & valid and reliable measure of serum cortisol (Ob-
Jackson, 1980, 1981; Whelan, Epkins, & Meyers, minski & Stupnicki, 1997). There is also evidence
1990), muscular endurance (Caudill & Weinberg, that tter individuals show stronger cortisol re-
1983; Lee, 1990; Weinberg, Jackson, & Seaboune, sponses than less trained people (Luger et al.,
1985) and power (Weinberg et al., 1985), particularly 1987; Marthur, Toriola, & Dada, 1986; Rudolph &
in novice lifters performing simple strength tasks. McAuley, 1998). In addition, low cortisol concen-
Not all researchers have shown a positive effect, trations are related to positive psychological
however (Brody, Hateld, Spalding, Frazer, & constructs, such as high self-efcacy (Rudolph &
Caherty, 2000; Murphy, Woolfolk, & Budney, McAuley, 1995), whereas higher cortisol concentra-

Correspondence: M. R. McGuigan, School of Biomedical and Sports Science, Edith Cowan University, 100 Joondalup Drive, Joondalup, WA 6027, Australia.
E-mail: m.mcguigan@ecu.edu.au
ISSN 0264-0414 print/ISSN 1466-447X online 2005 Taylor & Francis Group Ltd
DOI: 10.1080/02640410400021401
688 M. R. McGuigan et al.

tions are associated with negative affective states, engaged in one of two interventions. The allocation
such as anxiety (Lader, 1983). Psychological stress of the interventions was counterbalanced to avoid
associated with acute exercise may result in increased any order effect and included a free choice psych-up
cortisol secretion because participants are either and a distraction control condition. The participants
unfamiliar with the task or attach importance to the completed the two interventions on separate days.
event (Davis, Gass, & Bassett, 1981; Scavo, Barletta,
Vagiri, & Letizia, 1988). Based on these ndings, we
Participants
hypothesized that a psychological strategy, such as
psyching-up, would elevate the cortisol response to Twenty experienced resistance trained men and
acute exercise, due in part to the extra cognitive women volunteered to participate in the study.
demands involved with integrating psyching-up with There were 10 men (age 21.6 + 1.4 years, body
the movement and activation of the HPA axis. mass 88.5 + 17.1 kg, height 1.78 + 0.07 m) and 10
Cortisol concentration has been suggested to be a women (age 22.4 + 2.8 years, body mass
performance factor in weightlifting because it is 62.4 + 3.9 kg, height 1.65 + 0.06 m; mean + s).
higher in international weightlifters than national The participants had to have been resistance training
weightlifters (Passelergue, Robert, & Lac, 1995). at least three times a week for a year and to be
The results of previous studies have shown that familiar with the squat exercise. They were asked not
Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 11:07 24 September 2013

resistance exercise has a signicant effect on cortisol to take any performance-enhancing substances or
concentrations following exercise (Mulligan et al., perform any exercises that would fatigue the lower
1996; Nindl et al., 2001). Consequently, cortisol has body before each session. The participants were told
been recommended as a marker of the stress of about the experimental procedures and signed
training (Passelergue et al., 1995). informed consent statements in adherence with the
Only a few researchers have recruited well-trained human subjects guidelines of the University of
individuals (Shelton & Mahoney, 1978; Tynes & Wisconsin- La Crosse. To obtain statistical power
McFatter, 1987), including a recent study investigat- of 0.8, using an effect size of 1.23 reported by
ing the bench press exercise, in which psyching-up Meyers, Whelan and Murphy (1996) using the
resulted in greater peak force than cognitive distrac- psyching-up literature, the computer program GPO-
tion and an attention-placebo condition in WER (Faul & Erdfelder, 1992) calculated that a
participants with a minimum of one year of weight sample of 18 individuals was required.
training experience (Tod, Iredale, McGuigan,
Strange, & Gill, in press). Researchers need to
Psych-up conditions
investigate the inuence of self-directed psyching-
up on actual weight lifted during compound resis- Free-choice psych-up. The participants were asked to
tance training exercises, such as the squat, in prepare themselves for a maximal effort using any
individuals with relevant training histories (Tod et method they desired. They were given 20 s to
al., in press). One purpose of this study, therefore, identify the method that they would use, after which
was to examine the inuence of psyching-up on one- they were asked to spend 30 s engaging in their
repetition maximum (1-RM) performance during selected strategy. This experimental approach has
the squat exercise in individuals with at least one year been used previously (Tod et al., in press). The
of resistance training experience. We hypothesized method was developed in consultation with compe-
that psyching-up would lead to increased 1-RM titive powerlifters and time limits were imposed to
performance compared with a distraction control control the experimental procedures, as previous
condition. We also hypothesized that psyching-up research has suggested that the time taken to psych-
would result in a signicant elevation in salivary up does not inuence its effectiveness. Other
cortisol compared with the distraction condition. researchers have found that a free choice psych-up
is an effective strategy (Gould et al., 1980; Shelton &
Mahoney, 1978; Whelan et al., 1990)
Methods
Distraction control. As used in previous research,
Experimental approach to the problem
participants were asked to count backwards in groups
We examined the inuence of self-directed psyching- of seven from 1000 for 30 s (1000, 993. . ., etc.)
up on 1-RM performance and salivary cortisol (Shelton & Mahoney, 1978).
responses during the squat exercise using a within-
subjects repeated-measures design. Men (n = 10)
Test procedures
and women (n = 10) with weight training experience
(4.5 + 2.0 years) performed 1-RM attempts for the The participants reported to the laboratory on three
squat exercise. Before each attempt, the participants separate occasions spaced one week apart. The rst
Psyching-up in the squat exercise 689

session was treated as a control day in which the participants. We considered the smallest worthwhile
participants performed a maximum squat using a change to be 2.5 kg, as this represents the smallest
standard Smith machine. Several warm-up trials increase available in many weight-training exercises.
were performed using 30% (for 8 10 repetitions),
50% (4 6 repetitions), 70% (2 4 repetitions) and
Results
90% (one repetition) of an estimated repetition
maximum or 1.0 1.5 times the participants body The 1-RM results for the different psyching-up
weight (McBride, Triplett-McBride, Davie, & New- conditions are shown in Table I. There was no
ton, 2002). After the warm-up trials, each signicant difference in 1-RM values during the two
participants 1-RM was determined using previously different intervention strategies. The results showed
described methods (McBride et al., 2002). The that the men were signicantly stronger than the
participants were not asked to use any specic women for the 1-RM squat (P 5 0.05). However,
psychological interventions during the control day. there was no signicant interaction effect for 1-RM
A certied strength and conditioning specialist in the men and women participants (Table I).
supervised all sessions The effect sizes for the 1-RM were 0.31 for the
During the experimental sessions, the participants men, 0.23 for the women and 0.30 for the men and
were asked to engage in one of the interventions women combined. The salivary cortisol concentra-
Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 11:07 24 September 2013

during the nal 30 s of the rest periods. The tions for the different conditions, both pre and post
interventions were counterbalanced to avoid any test, are shown in Table II. There was no signicant
order effect. After completion of all test sessions, the difference in salivary cortisol concentrations during
participants were debriefed on the interventions and the two intervention strategies. There was a sig-
received feedback regarding their performance. nicant increase in salivary cortisol concentration
from pre to post test in both conditions (P 5 0.05).
There were no signicant gender differences in the
Salivary cortisol
salivary cortisol responses.
Saliva samples were collected at the beginning of
each test session, without stimulation, by spitting
Discussion
directly into a plastic tube, and again following the
end of the nal 1-RM. Samples were obtained from In this study, we examined the inuence of psyching-
ten of the participants who completed the study. To up on 1-RM performance and salivary cortisol
avoid any confounding effects due to variations in concentrations during the squat exercise in well-
circadian rhythm, all test sessions were performed at trained individuals. There was no signicant increase
approximately the same time of day. The samples in 1-RM performance following psyching-up com-
were transferred to Eppendorf tubes and stored at pared with the distraction condition. This nding is
7 808C for later analysis. There is a strong relation- in contrast to research that has found a psych-up
ship between salivary and serum unbound cortisol effect in trained individuals (Shelton & Mahoney,
both at rest (r = 0.93) and during exercise (r = 0.90) 1978; Tod et al., in press; Tynes & McFatter, 1987).
(OConnor & Corrigan, 1987). Saliva measures of
cortisol concentrations are independent of saliva ow
rate (Riad-Fahmy, Read, & Walker, 1983).
Salivary cortisol concentrations were determined Table I. The 1-RM (kg) produced by men and women in the squat
in duplicate by enzyme immunoassay using a exercise following two different mental preparation strategies
free-choice psych-up and distraction control (mean + s).
Diagnostic Systems Laboratories Salivary Cortisol
Enzyme Immunoassay Kit (DSL, Webster, TX). Psych-up Control
Assay plates were read using an Opsys MRTM
Men (n = 10) 148.6 + 36.9 145.6 + 30.9
Microplate Reader (Dynex Technologies, Chantilly,
Women (n = 10) 63.5 + 19.3 62.1 + 20.3
USA). The intra-assay variance was 7.2% Combined (n = 20) 106.0 + 52.2 103.9 + 49.8

Statistical analysis
Two-factor (condition 6 gender) analysis of var- Table II. Salivary cortisol concentration (nmol l71) before and
iance was performed on the 1-RM and cortisol after two mental preparation strategies free-choice psych-up and
distraction control (mean + s).
results. Effect sizes were calculated using the formula
from Speed and Andersen (2000) and interpreted Psych-up Control
according to their meaningfulness. To interpret the
Before 13.8 + 10.7 9.5 + 5.6
effect sizes, it was necessary to estimate the smallest After 21.8 + 8.6 17.8 + 8.8
change in performance that was worthwhile to
690 M. R. McGuigan et al.

Also, based on the results from seven studies, Tod et dividuals may have less well-developed movement
al. (2003) estimated that psyching-up leads to a 12% patterns and neural pathways that are modiable.
increase in strength compared with control condi- Furthermore, the performance of untrained partici-
tions. In addition, there were no signicant gender pants is likely to be characterized by greater variation
differences in the present study for 1-RM using the compared with trained individuals. Untrained parti-
psyching-up strategy versus distraction. cipants, therefore, may receive more benet from
The major differences between our study and psyching-up. It may be that one years experience
previous research is that we measured actual weight with the squat exercise may be enough for partici-
lifted in a compound full body exercise (i.e. squat) pants to become sufciently trained to no longer
using individuals with at least one year of training. receive benet from psyching-up. A third possible
Previous research with trained individuals has reason is that the quality of the participants
measured the force produced during the handgrip psyching-up was not sufcient for them to receive
or the bench press exercise. In the present study, we an effect. In previous research, however, novice
chose to measure performance by weight lifted, as lifters have been able to psych-up sufciently to
most individuals are likely to be interested in the improve their performance; it is likely that the
weight they lift rather than the force they produce. participants in our study would have been capable
Most lifters are not able to use force production of psyching-up. Most participants in the present
Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 11:07 24 September 2013

measuring devices when they train but they do see study were only recreationally trained, as reected by
the weight on the bar. There was no learning effect the squat 1-RM to body weight ratio (1.67 for men
associated with the movement, as the participants and 1.02 for women). It is also possible that the
were familiar with the squat exercise. One possible participants quality of free choice psyching-up was
explanation for the discrepancy between our results not high enough to allow them to attain full benet
and those of other research is that the increased from this type of mental strategy. We have offered
complexity of the squat exercise, compared with the three possible explanations why psyching-up did not
bench press or handgrip, could have resulted in the lead to increased performance as hypothesized,
participants being required to divide their attention although it is not possible to state with condence
across the movement and the psyching-up proce- which is most likely.
dure. Performance is inuenced by a number of The present study does not provide evidence that
factors and asking participants to psych-up while psyching-up is benecial to participants who have
lifting during a full body compound movement like experienced at least one year of regular weight
the squat may provide a distraction that inhibits training that includes the compound squat exercise.
performance (cf. Tenenbaum et al., 1995). However, If our participants produced more force, as suggested
as the participants were familiar with the exercise, it elsewhere, they were unable to translate this to actual
is unlikely that the complexity of the exercise would weight lifted. Also, there is still a need for more
be a major contributor to differences with previous research before the inuence of psyching-up on
research. Most previous research has used simple, strength performance can be determined for highly
single joint exercise tasks (e.g. Shelton & Mahoney, conditioned or elite participants.
1978; Tynes & McFatter, 1987). During dynamic The overall effect size was 0.30, which is small to
strength tasks, individuals may have more freedom to moderate, and it is possible that in compound
vary their posture and/or movement pattern to obtain strength movements like the squat, psyching-up
a biomechanical advantage and/or facilitate addi- actually leads to a drop in performance of 0.3 of a
tional recruitment of muscle groups (Brody et al., standard deviation or about 2 kg in this sample.
2000). In support of this, we have previously shown Given that the smallest possible increment in most
that psyching-up led to an 11.8% increase compared exercises is 2.5 kg, an overall drop is likely to be
with a distraction condition, and an 8.1% increase meaningless. However, with an effect size of 0.31 in
over a placebo condition, using a machine bench the males, there was a drop of about 2.9 kg and this
press model (Tod et al., in press). is more than the smallest possible increment of
An alternative explanation for the results could be 2.5 kg. It might be that psyching-up can have a slight
the participants training experience. Much of the negative inuence on performance in men.. Future
research that has examined the inuence of psy- research is needed because many strength athletes
ching-up on dynamic strength tasks has been believe psyching-up increases performance and there
conducted on untrained samples; groups of well- is evidence that psyching-up does increase force
trained people have only been used in a few studies. production in simple isolated dynamic tasks,
Brody et al. (2000) suggested that trained individuals although most previous studies have used untrained
are likely to have well-developed movement patterns samples (Tod et al., in press). Hence many athletes
and neural pathways that may not be readily use a psyching-up strategy, but the effect sizes in our
modiable by psyching-up, whereas untrained in- study suggest that during compound movements like
Psyching-up in the squat exercise 691

the squat in males with at least one year of to maximal work. Passelergue et al. (1995) studied
experience, psyching-up may have a slight negative variation in salivary cortisol and testosterone during
inuence on performance. Our results are not an ofcial and a simulated weight-lifting competi-
denitive given the non-signicant P-values, but tion. Cortisol concentrations were signicantly
they do justify further research. higher in the competition than in the simulation.
Altered psychological states, changes in physiolo- Also, the cortisol concentrations at the time of the
gical arousal and changes in mechanical factors have competition were higher in international than in
all been proposed as possible reasons why psyching- national athletes.
up could potentially increase muscular force produc- Psyching-up refers to self-directed cognitive stra-
tion, but the evidence is conicting (Perkins, Wilson, tegies used by individuals to enhance their
& Kerr, 2001; Tod et al., 2003; Whelan et al., 1990). performance. The literature examining the psych-
Some research has attempted to assess arousal by up effect provides evidence that it may positively
measuring physiological variables (Perkins et al., inuence the display of force production on simple
2001; Whelan et al., 1990). Whelan et al. (1990) dynamic strength tasks (Tod et al., 2003). In
showed that heart rate was not related to increased addition, much of the existing research has been
strength or psyching-up. Perkins et al. (2001) showed conducted on untrained individuals. The results of
that heart rate and autonomic nervous system activity this study indicate that psyching-up does not
Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 11:07 24 September 2013

did not mediate the relationship between handgrip inuence 1-RM during the squat exercise in in-
strength and guided imagery. It has been suggested dividuals with at least one year of weight training
that psyching-up may lead to changes in motor unit experience. However, further research is required to
recruitment, but in one study no difference was conrm these ndings in more elite, strength-based
found in electromyographic activity across various performers.
psyching-up conditions (Brody et al., 2000). Stress-
induced activation of the hypothalamus pituitary
Practical application
adrenal axis results in increased cortisol secretion
(Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1994). In this study, The results of the present study suggest that self-
we measured salivary cortisol as an indicator of directed cognitive strategies do not increase 1-RM
physiological stress. There is limited research in- performance in the squat exercise in trained indivi-
vestigating salivary cortisol responses to resistance duals. However, one should be cautious about
exercise. Salivary analysis of cortisol provides a generalizing these results to competitive and elite
stress-free, non-invasive collecting procedure (Vin- athletes. There was also no signicant difference in
ing & McGinley, 1987). It has been suggested that salivary cortisol responses with the use of a self-
salivary cortisol provides a better measure of directed cognitive strategy. There was, however, a
adrenocortical function, as it more accurately repre- signicant elevation in salivary cortisol following the
sents the level of unbound cortisol (Vining, performance of the Smith machine squat 1-RM.
McGinley, Maksvytis, & Ho, 1983). Most studies Based on the results of this and previous studies, it
that have measured serum concentrations have could be that tasks that rely more on muscular
shown that high-intensity resistance exercise elevates endurance would benet more from self-directed
serum cortisol (Mulligan et al., 1996; Nindl et al., cognitive strategies. However, further studies with
2001). Our results showed a signicant elevation in competitive and elite athletes are required to
saliva concentrations of cortisol following 1-RM determine the efcacy of psyching-up as a mental
attempts in the squat exercise. There was no strategy tool in competitive settings.
signicant difference in salivary cortisol responses
between the self-directed cognitive strategies. Such a
nding is not unexpected, however, given that there
References
was no signicant difference in 1-RM performance.
We hypothesized that there would be a signicant Brody, E. B., Hateld, B. D., Spalding, T. W., Frazer, M. B., &
Caherty, F. J. (2000). The effects of a psyching strategy on
elevation in salivary cortisol following the psyching-
neuromuscular activation and force production in strength-
up strategy, as previous research has shown that trained men. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 162
changes in psychological states during acute exercise 170.
result in increased cortisol secretion (Davis et al., Caudill, D., & Weinberg, R. (1983). The effects of varying the
1981; Scavo et al., 1988). Our ndings could in part length of the psych-up interval on motor performance. Journal
be explained by the participants training experience of Sport Behavior, 6, 86 91.
Davis, H., Gass, G., & Bassett, J. (1981). Serum cortisol response
and because they were not lifting during a competi- to incremental work in experienced and nave subjects.
tion. Davis et al. (1981) demonstrated that the Psychomatic Medicine, 43, 127 132.
psychoendocrine response to a novel situation was
a major determinant of the serum cortisol response
692 M. R. McGuigan et al.

Elko, K., & Ostrow, A. C. (1992). The effects of three mental Riad-Fahmy, D., Read, G. F., & Walker, R. F. (1983). Salivary
preparation strategies on strength performance of young and steroid assays for assessing variation in endocrine activity.
older athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 15, 34 41. Journal of Steroid Biochemistry, 19, 265 272.
Faul, F., & Erdfelder, E. (1992). GPOWER: A priori, post-hoc, and Rudolph, D. L., & McAuely, E. (1995). Self-efcacy and salivary
compromise power analyses for MS-DOS [computer program]. cortisol responses to acute exercise in physically active and less
Bonn: Department of Psychology, University of Bonn. active adults. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 206
Gould, D., Weinberg, R., & Jackson, A. (1980). Mental 213.
preparation strategies, cognitions, and strength performance. Rudolph, D.L., & McAuely, E. (1998). Cortisol and affective
Journal of Sport Psychology, 2, 329 339. responses to exercise. Journal of Sports Sciences, 16, 121 128.
Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1994). Salivary cortisol in Scavo, D., Barletta, C., Vagiri, D., & Letizia, C. (1988).
psychoendocrine research: Recent developments and applica- Adrenocortotrophic hormone, beta-endorphin, cortisol, growth
tions. Psychoendocrinology, 19, 313 333. hormone and prolactin circulating levels in nineteen athletes
Lader, M. (1983). Anxiety and depression. In A. Gale & J. A. before and after half-marathon and marathon. Journal of Sports
Edwards (Eds.), Physiological correlates of human behavior, Vol. Medicine and Physical Fitness, 31, 401 406.
III: Individual differences and psychopathology (pp 155 167). Shelton, T. O., & Mahoney, M. J. (1978). The content and effect
London: Academic Press. of psyching-up strategies in weight lifters. Cognitive Therapy
Lee, C. (1990). Psyching up for a muscular endurance task: and Research, 2, 275 284.
Effects of image content on performance and mood state. Speed, H. D., & Andersen, M. B. (2000). What exercise and sport
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 66 73. scientists dont understand. Journal of Science and Medicine in
Luger, A., Duchester, P., Kyle, S., Galluchi, W., Montgomery, L., Sport, 3, 84 92.
Downloaded by [Auburn University] at 11:07 24 September 2013

Gold, P., Loriaux, L., & Chrousus, G. (1987). Acute Tenenbaum, G., Bar-Eli, M., Hoffman, J.R., & Jablonovski, R.
hypothalamic pituitary adrenal responses to the stress of (1995). The effect of cognitive and somatic psyching-up
treadmill exercise. New England Journal of Medicine, 316, techniques on isokinetic leg strength performance. Journal of
1309 1315. Strength and Conditioning Research, 9, 3 7.
Marthur, D., Toriola, A., & Dada, O. (1986). Serum cortisol and Theodorakis, Y., Weinberg, R., Natsis, P., Douma, I., & Kazakas,
testosterone levels in conditioned male distance runners and P. (2000). The effects of motivational versus instructional self-
non-athletes after maximal exercise. Journal of Sports Medicine talk on improving motor performance. The Sport Psychologist,
and Physical Fitness, 26, 245 250. 14, 253 272.
McBride, J., Triplett-McBride, T., Davie, A., & Newton, R. U. Tod, D., Iredale, F., & Gill, N. (2003). Psyching-up and
(2002). The effect of heavy-vs-light jump squats on the muscular force production. Sports Medicine, 33, 47 58.
development of strength, power, and speed. Journal of Strength Tod, D. A., Iredale, F., McGuigan, M. R., Strange, D., & Gill, N.
and Conditioning Research, 16, 75 82. D. (in press). Psyching-up enhances force production
Meyers, A. W., Whelan, J. P., & Murphy, S. M. (1996). Cognitive during the bench press exercise. Journal of Strength and
behavioral strategies in athletic performance enhancement. In Conditioning Research.
M. Hersen, R. M. Miller, & A. S. Belack (Eds.), Handbook of Tynes, L. L., & McFatter, R. M. (1987). The efcacy of
behavior modication (Vol. 30, pp. 137 164). Pacic Grove, psyching strategies on a weight-lifting task. Cognitive Therapy
CA: Brooks/Cole. and Research, 11, 327 336.
Mulligan, S. E., Fleck, S. J., Gordon, S. E., Koziris, L. P., Vining, R. F., & McGinley, R. A. (1987). The measurement of
Triplett-McBride, N. T., & Kraemer, W. J. (1996). Inuence of hormones in saliva: Possibilities and pitfalls. Journal of Steroid
resistance exercise volume on serum growth hormone and Biochemistry, 27, 81 94.
cortisol concentrations in women. Journal of Strength and Vining, R. F., McGinley, R. A., Maksvytis, J. J., & Ho, K. Y.
Conditioning Research, 10, 256 262. (1983). Salivary cortisol: A better measure of adrenal cortical
Murphy, S. M., Woolfolk, R. L., & Budney, A. J. (1988). The function than serum cortisol. Annals of Clinical Biochemistry, 20,
effects of emotive imagery on strength performance. Journal of 329 335.
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 10, 334 345. Weinberg, R., Gould, D., & Jackson, A. (1980). Cognition and
Nindl, B. C., Kraemer, W. J., Deaver, D. R., Peters, J. L., Marx, J. motor performance: Effect of psyching-up strategies on three
O., Heckman, J. T., & Loomis, G. A. (2001). LH secretion and motor tasks. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 4, 239 245.
testosterone concentrations are blunted after resistance exercise Weinberg, R., Gould, D., & Jackson, A. (1981). Relationship
in men. Journal of Applied Physiology, 91, 1251 1258. between the duration of the psych-up interval and strength
Obminski, Z., & Stupnicki, R. (1997). Comparison of the performance. Journal of Sport Psychology, 3, 166 170.
testosterone-to-cortisol ratio values obtained from hormonal Weinberg, R., Jackson, A., & Seaboune, T. (1985). The effects of
assays in saliva and serum. Journal of Sports Medicine and specic vs nonspecic mental preparation strategies on strength
Physical Fitness, 37, 50 55. and endurance performance. Journal of Sport Behavior, 8, 175
OConnor, P., & Corrigan, D. (1987). Inuence of short-term 180.
cycling on salivary cortisol levels. Medicine and Science in Sports Whelan, J. P., Epkins, C. C., & Meyers, A. W. (1990). Arousal
and Exercise, 19, 224 228. interventions for athletic performance: Inuence of mental
Passelergue, P., Robert, A., & Lac, G. (1995). Salivary cortisol and preparation and competitive experience. Anxiety Research, 2:
testosterone variations during an ofcial and a simulated 293 307.
weight-lifting competition. International Journal of Sports Med-
icine, 16, 298 303.
Perkins, D., Wilson, G. V., & Kerr, J. (2001). The effects of
elevated arousal and mood on maximal strength performance in
athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 239 259.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen