Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder Have Reduced Otoacoustic


Emissions at the 1 kHz Mid-Frequency Region
Loisa Bennetto, Jessica M. Keith, Paul D. Allen, and Anne E. Luebke

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a behaviorally diagnosed disorder of early onset characterized by impairment in
social communication and restricted and repetitive behaviors. Some of the earliest signs of ASD involve auditory proc-
essing, and a recent study found that hearing thresholds in children with ASD in the mid-range frequencies were sig-
nificantly related to receptive and expressive language measures. In addition, otoacoustic emissions have been used
to detect reduced cochlear function in the presence of normal audiometric thresholds. We were interested then to
know if otoacoustic emissions in children with normal audiometric thresholds would also reveal differences between
children with ASD and typical developing (TD) controls in mid-frequency regions. Our objective was to specifically
measure baseline afferent otoacoustic emissions (distortion-product otoacoustic emissions [DPOAEs]), transient-
evoked otoacoustic emissions (TrOAEs), and efferent suppression, in 35 children with high-functioning ASD com-
pared with 42 aged-matched TD controls. All participants were males 617 years old, with normal audiometry, and
rigorously characterized via Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Chil-
dren with ASD had greatly reduced DPOAE responses in the 1 kHz frequency range, yet had comparable DPOAE
responses at 0.5 and 48 kHz regions. Furthermore, analysis of the spectral features of TrOAEs revealed significantly
decreased emissions in ASD in similar frequencies. No significant differences were noted in DPOAE or TrOAE noise
floors, middle ear muscle reflex activity, or efferent suppression between children with ASD and TD controls. In con-
clusion, attention to specific-frequency deficits using non-invasive measures of cochlear function may be important
in auditory processing impairments found in ASD. Autism Res 2017, 10: 337345. V C 2016 International Society for

Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Keywords: DPOAE; distortion-product otoacoustic emissions; TrOAE; transient-evoked otoacoustic emissions; autism;
cochlea; efferent suppression; middle ear muscle reflex

Introduction more complex information (e.g., prosody and affect), dif-


ficulty perceiving speech in background noise, and both
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a behaviorally diag- hypo- and hyper-responsiveness to sounds [for reviews,
nosed disorder of early onset characterized by impair- Haesen, Boets, & Wagemans, 2011; OConnor, 2012].
ment in social communication and restricted and Abnormalities in auditory processing have been shown to
repetitive behaviors. Some of the earliest signs of ASD be related to functional deficits in individuals with ASD
involve atypical processing and response to auditory [e.g., Demopoulos et al., 2015; Edgar et al., 2015]. More-
input. For example, analyses of home-based videotapes over, peripheral hearing deficits are common among chil-
and prospective studies have consistently shown that dren with ASD [for review, Hitoglou, Ververi, Antoniadis,
children who are later diagnosed are more likely than & Zafeiriou, 2010]. While hearing loss is not causative for
those who do not develop ASD to fail to respond to their ASD, there is evidence that hearing-impaired children
name being called [e.g., Baranek et al., 2013; Dawson, show deficits common to ASD, such as impaired emotion
Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, & Brown, 1998; Nadig et al., or vocal affect recognition abilities [Dyck, Farrugia, Sho-
2007]. Studies of older children and adults with ASD have chet, & Holmes-Brown, 2004; Most & Michaelis, 2012].
similarly documented atypical processing of auditory In fact, Demopoulos and Lewine [2015] found that in
input across a range of measures, including atypical per- children with ASD, there was a relationship between mid-
ception of basic acoustic properties of both speech and frequency pure-tone auditory thresholds and expressive
non-speech stimuli (e.g., pitch and intensity) as well as and receptive language measures.

From the Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York (L.B., J.M.K.); Department of Oto-
laryngology, University of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York (P.D.A.); Departments of Biomedical Engineering and Neuroscience, Uni-
versity of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, New York (A.E.L.)
Received March 09, 2016; accepted for publication June 06, 2016
Address for correspondence and reprints: Anne E. Luebke, University of Rochester Medical Center, 601 Elmwood Avenue, Box 603, Rochester, NY,
14642. E-mail: anne_luebke@urmc.rochester.edu
Published online 12 July 2016 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com)
DOI: 10.1002/aur.1663
C 2016 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
V

INSAR Autism Research 10: 337345, 2017 337


Figure 1. DPOAEs are reduced in the 12 kHz frequencies in children with ASD. (A) Schematic of DPOAEs showing two primary
input tones (f1 and f2), and L1 and L2 as amplitudes (loudness) of tones 1 and 2. In the output of the ear, L1 and L2 tones appear
as A1 and A2 reflections. The DPOAE must be above the noise floor (dotted line) to be detected. (B, C) DPOAE SNR across the left
ear (LE) and right ear (RE) as a function of frequency for L1 5 L2 levels at 70 dB SPL. (D) Average differences between TD and ASD
DPOAEs separated by frequency. (E, F) Difference in middle ear muscle reflex (MEM-R) was not the cause of reduced DPOAEs in ASD
for the left (LE) or right ear (RE). MEM-R was calculated as the difference between the A1 reflection and the L1 input values of 70
dB SPL (i.e., L1-A1 in dB). (G) Average (across ear) differences between TD and ASD MEM-R separated by frequency. Note the scale
in E-G is 61 dB, as the average response was less than a 0.25 dB difference.

In addition to auditory pure-tone threshold testing, 1A) [see Kemp, 2002]. We now understand that the
the integrity of the peripheral auditory system can be traveling wave is boosted by a local electromechanical
evaluated reliably and objectively using otoacoustic amplification process present only in cochlear outer
emissions (OAEs). Sound causes basilar membrane hair cells. Fully functioning outer hair cells are needed
motion that results in voltage changes across cochlear to enable humans to discriminate between two sounds
outer hair cells causing them to change their length whose frequencies differ by only 0.20.5%. Loss or
(due to the cochlear amplifier) and these length lesser functioning outer hair cells cause reduced ability
changes generate acoustic signals that can be recorded to discriminate between two sounds or impaired audi-
in the external ear canal (i.e., sound-evoked OAEs, Fig. tory tuning [Dallos, 1992]. In fact, distortion-product

338 Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs INSAR


Table 1. Demographic Information for the ASD and TD Groups
ASD (n 5 35) TD (n 5 42)
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) F or X2 P

Age (years) 11.98 (2.82) 12.49 (3.25) 0.53 0.47


Full Scale IQa 104.89 (13.01) 114.69 (13.34) 10.55 0.002
Verbal Comprehensiona 105.09 (11.95) 116.14 (15.28) 12.13 0.001
Perceptual Reasoninga 103.49 (16.36) 110.17 (14.43) 3.63 0.06
SRS total T-score 76.77 (12.04) 41.46 (6.68) 259.43 <0.001
ADOS calibrated severityb 6.60 (1.97) 1.12 (0.33) 306.53 <0.001
Handedness (R:L) 30:5 32:10 1.10 0.29

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale; TD, typically developing.
a
Full Scale IQ and index scores based on a four-item short form of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed. or the Wechsler Adult
Intelligence Scale, 4th ed.
b
ADOS severity scores calculated based on module [Gotham et al., 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014].

otoacoustic emissions (DPOAEs) have been used to to survey certain frequency regions by choice of the f1
detect reduced cochlear function in the presence of and f2 primary tone frequencies. Measuring these OAEs
normal audiometric thresholds [Arnold, Lonsbury- is noninvasive and reliable, does not require behavioral
Martin, & Martin, 1999]. We were interested then to responses, and is a routine approach to testing cochlear
determine if OAEs could be used to detect reduced function in children as young as infancy [Bergman
cochlear function in mid-frequency regions in children et al., 1995].
with ASD with clinically acceptable pure-tone thresh-
olds (i.e., no hearing loss).
Methods
In addition, deficits in descending pathways [olivoco- Subjects
chlear (OC) efferent system] from the midbrain to the
cochlea have been shown to also cause impaired audi- Males with ASD (n 5 35) and typical developing (TD)
tory sensitivity and impaired cochlear tuning in a controls (n 5 42), ages 6 through 17, participated in this
frequency-specific fashion [Warr & Guinan, 1979]. study. Diagnostic evaluations were conducted by expe-
Outer hair cells are synapsed directly by efferent neu- rienced and trained staff, under the supervision of the
rons originating in the vicinity of the nuclei of the first author. Autism diagnoses were rigorously con-
superior olivary complex corresponding to the medial firmed in the ASD group with the Autism Diagnostic
olivocochlear bundle (MOC), and the activity of the Observation Schedule (ADOS) [Lord et al., 2012] and
MOC bundle itself is regulated by information descend- Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) [Rutter,
ing from the upper part of the brain. Interestingly, LeCouteur, & Lord, 2003], and ruled out in the TD
when autopsied or MRI imaged brains from adults with group with the ADOS and Social Responsiveness Scale
ASD have been investigated, this same brain region (SRS) [Constantino, 2005]. Groups were matched on
(superior olivary complex) was either absent, reduced in chronological age (see Table 1 for demographic infor-
size, or misaligned compared to control brains, suggest- mation). They were also matched on hand dominance
ing that the OC region is affected in ASD [Gaffney, (assessed with the Edinburg Handedness Inventory)
Kuperman, Tsai, & Minchin, 1988; Hashimoto et al., [Oldfield, 1971], as there is some evidence that this
1995; Kulesza & Mangunay, 2008; Rodier, Ingram, Tis- may influenced efferent function [Khalfa, Veuillet, &
dale, Nelson, & Romano, 1996]. Collet, 1998]. IQ was measured with a four-subtest ver-
In this study, we examined two different types of sion of the age-appropriate Wechsler scale [Wechsler,
OAEs, evoked by different generation mechanisms: (a) 2004, 2008]. We enrolled subjects with ASD with Full
DPOAEs, where two primary tones, f1 and f2, are intro- Scale IQs  85 to ensure comprehension of the tasks,
duced into the ear and a distortion-product of those while also representing a spectrum of functioning. The
two tones is generated in the cochlea and detected as TD group was significantly higher than the ASD group
an emission at 2f1-f2 frequency and (b) TrOAEs, where a on Full Scale IQ, F (1,73) 5 10.55, P 5 0.002. Pure tone
multifrequency click stimulus is introduced into the ear audiometric screening (Maico Diagnostics, Eden Prairie,
and the OAEs generated in the cochlea are monitored MN) was performed on both ears for all participants. All
over a specific time domain (618 ms) [Shera & Guinan, participants could detect tones 20 dB sound pressure
1999]. The TrOAE responses survey many frequencies of level (SPL) for 500, 1000, 2000, and 4000 Hz, and 25
the cochlea, and the DPOAE responses can be tailored dB SPL for 8000 Hz. Additional exclusion criteria for all

INSAR Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs 339


participants included history of neurological injury or averaged for each ear. To determine efferent suppres-
disorders, persistent, frequent, or recurring ear infec- sion, the root mean square (RMS) amplitude difference
tions, genetic disorders, and other conditions or ill- between baseline and suppression was compared using
nesses that could affect auditory function. TD custom software (IHS), with a value for the overall RMS
participants were excluded if they had a first or second suppression derived from the time window between 8
degree relative with an ASD. Parents of all participants and 18 ms, as this is the time window within which
provided written informed consent, and participants the most suppression occurs [Berlin, Hood, Hurley,
gave assent, as approved by the Universitys Research Wen, & Kemp, 1995; Veuillet, Collet, & Duclaux, 1991]
Subject Review Board. (see Figs. 2A and 3A). All statistical analyses were per-
All audiometry, DPOAE, and TrOAE measurements formed using SPSS v.22 (IBM, Armonk, NY) computer
were conducted in a double-walled soundbooth (Indus- software, using repeated-measures analysis of variance
trial Acoustics Co. [IAC], New York, NY). to examine group differences across frequency, followed
Otoacoustic Emissions by post hoc analyses.

DPOAEs and TrOAEs were acquired using Intelligent


Hearing Systems (IHS, Miami, FL) Smart OAE hardware Results
and software interfaces. DPOAEs were conducted for
pure tones from 0.5 to 8 kHz. An Etymotic Research All included participants had clinically acceptable
(Elk Grove Village, IL) ER 10C probe was inserted into audiometric levels (20 dB SPL for frequencies 0.5
the external ear canal and used with an ER2 speaker. 4 kHz; and <25 dB SPL for 8 kHz) and DPOAE responses
Stimulus response signals were sampled at a rate of were present in both ears, in both the ASD (n 5 35) and
128 kHz using a 16-bit D/A converter. L1 and L2 ampli- TD (n 5 42) groups. We excluded four of our consented
tudes were set to 70 dB SPL. Equilevel primary tones of participants with ASD, as they exhibited audiometric
moderate level were used in this study as equilevel pri- thresholds above the screening cutoffs in at least one
maries have been used successfully to detect reduced ear, for at least one frequency. As shown in Figures 1B
cochlear function in the presence of normal behavioral and 1D, we found a significant decrease in DPOAE sig-
sensitivities [Arnold et al., 1999]. Frequencies were nal to noise ratio (SNR) at 1 kHz in both ears for the
acquired with an F2F1 ratio of 1.22. Stimuli were pre- children with ASD compared to TD controls (left ear,
sented starting from the lowest frequencies increasing F(1,74) 5 5.4, P 5 0.02; right ear, F(1,73) 5 5.9, P 5 0.02).
to the highest frequencies with 32 averages/per fre- Averaged (right and left) group difference was also sig-
quency and DPOAEs were only included if 6 dB above nificant at 1 kHz (F(1,73) 5 8.0, P 5 0.006). In contrast,
the noise floor. for the other frequencies tested from 500 Hz to 8 kHz,
TrOAEs were defined as having a stimulus stability there were no significant differences in DPOAE SNR lev-
exceeding 80% and a signal-to-noise amplitude in the els. These differences at 1 kHz were not due to noise
frequency bands centered at 1000 and 2000 Hz exceed- floor differences, as there were no significant differences
ing 4 dB. TrOAEs were recorded with a commercial between the DPOAE noise floors between the two
instrument (IHS) with the standard 80 dB peak sound groups (ASD & TD) at any frequency or ear tested.
pressure click default protocol with a commercially The DPOAE response can be influenced by the action
available OAE probe (Etymotic Research 10C) in the ear of the middle ear muscle (MEM-R) acoustic reflex. The
canal, with 512 sweeps were performed in each ear. In equilevel primaries used for DPOAE testing were 70 dB
addition to overall TrOAE amplitude (click response), SPL, which should be below the threshold for MEM-R
the responses were separated in the spectral domain responses; however to ensure differences in MEM-R
across frequency bands (1, 1.5, 2, 3, and 4 kHz) using responses to DPOAE primary tones did not cause the
customized software (IHS, Miami, FL). differences in DPOAE SNR measures, we computed a
Binaural suppression was measured using ER 10C MEM-R difference for each ear and frequency. To com-
probe microphones/transducers in both ears with bin- pute MEM-R differences, we determined the L1-A1 dif-
aural noise bursts presented at 70 dB SPL through both ference (see Fig. 1A), which is the difference in dB
speakers. This noise/click relationship was chosen between the amplitudes of the tone entering the ear
because Hood et al. [1996] showed them to elicit emis- canal minus the amplitude of that same tone exiting
sions and a substantial suppression effect. Broadband the ear canal. As shown in Figures 1E1G, all MEM-R
noise (BBN) bursts, 400 ms duration, were presented differences were less than 1 dB SPL, and were less than
binaurally at 70 dB peak SPL, in a forward masking par- 0.25 dB SPL when averaged. Furthermore, no significant
adigm with an interstimulus interval of 10 ms separat- differences were observed between children with ASD
ing the noise from the click. Again, 512 sweeps were and TD controls at any level for either ear.

340 Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs INSAR


Figure 2. Children with ASD have reduced TrOAEs in the 12 kHz frequencies. (A) Schematic of TrOAE baseline. Baseline TrOAEs are
elicited with a unilateral click stimulus (black trace) with the RMS value averaged over the 818 ms window. (B, C) There is a differ-
ence in the click TrOAE baseline values for the right ear (RE) between children with ASD and TD controls, and when the click
response is separated into spectral bands, the 12 kHz region is significantly different between children with ASD and TD children in
the right ear. (D) Average (across ear) differences between TD TrOAE values and ASD TrOAE values (click and spectrally separated by
frequency bands), showing that children with ASD had reduced TrOAE values in the click response and the 1 kHz region, with a trend
at 1.5 and 2 kHz.

TrOAEs were also tested in the same groups of chil- emission. The difference in RMS values between base-
dren in both ears. There were no significant differences line and BBN suppression (Fig. 3A) testing was the
in TrOAEs in the left ears between children with ASD or TrOAE suppression SNR value. There were no significant
TD controls (Fig. 2B). However, as seen in Figure 2C, differences in TrOAE suppression values between chil-
there were significant differences between the groups in dren with ASD and TD controls in either the right, left,
the right ears for both the overall click RMS values or when both ears are averaged (Fig. 3B). As can be
(F(1,73) 5 7.8, P 5 0.007) and at 12 kHz when the observed in Figures 3B and 3C, the average TrOAE sup-
responses were spectrally separated (1 kHz, pression in the left ear is 1 dB greater in the TD con-
F(1,73) 5 5.72, P 5 0.02; 1.5 kHz, F(1,73) 5 5.91, trols, yet this difference is not statistically significant.
P 5 0.02; 2 kHz (F(1,73) 5 5.96, P 5 0.02). The observed Previous studies have reported relationships between
asymmetry in TrOAE responses, with right ear responses auditory function and other skills (e.g., language and
greater than left responses, is known [Keefe, Gorga, Jes- symptom severity). We examined the relationship
teadt, & Smith, 2008]. Figure 2D shows this increased between the Verbal Comprehension index and Full
TrOAE responses with both ears averaged together in Scale IQ to OAEs in the 1 kHz mid-frequency range. We
children with ASD as compared with TD children. used the averaged DPOAE at 1 kHz, and performed
Again, there is a significant difference between children analyses separately by group. We found no significant
with ASD and TD controls for the overall click relationships in either group between 1 kHz DPOAE
(F(1,72) 5 5.9, P 5 0.02) and 1 kHz response and verbal comprehension (ASD: r(33) 5 0.06, P 5 0.75;
(F(1,72) 5 4.33, P 5 0.04. TD: r(40) 5 0.11, P 5 0.50) or Full Scale IQ (ASD:
To test cochlear efferent feedback suppression, binau- r(33) 5 0.07, P 5 0.70; TD: r(40) 5 20.03, P 5 0.84). In
ral forward masking was used prior to testing the TrOAE contrast, 1 kHz DPOAE was significantly related to

INSAR Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs 341


Figure 3. Children with ASD have no differences in TrOAE suppression to binaural BBN stimulus. (A) Schematic of TrOAE suppres-
sion testing. Baseline TrOAEs are elicited with a unilateral click stimulus (black trace), and binaural suppression testing used binau-
ral forward masking of BBN 400 ms prior to click stimulus presentation (gray trace), with difference between traces showing the
suppressed response. (B) There are no differences in TrOAE binaural suppression values in either ear or averaged ears (combined
ears) between children with ASD and TD children. (C) Average differences between TD TrOAE suppression values and ASD TrOAE sup-
pression values show no differences in TrOAE binaural suppression values in either ear or averaged ears (combined ears) between
children with ASD and TD children.

symptom severity in the ASD group, as measured by compared to TD controls. Importantly, all children had
the ADOS Calibrated Severity Score [Gotham, Pickles, & normal audiometric screening levels 20 dB HL, so this
Lord, 2009; Hus & Lord, 2014], r(33) 5 20.48, P 5 0.004, DPOAE loss was not due to a general sensorineural or
with more severe symptoms associated with lower conductive hearing loss. Moreover, there were no signif-
DPOAE responses. icant differences in DPOAE noise floors, MEM reflexes
to 70 dB tones, or efferent feedback strengths (TrOAE
suppression) between children with ASD and TD con-
Discussion trols. Additional correlational analyses showed that
DPOAE responses in the 1 kHz mid-frequency range
In this study, we found that children and adolescents were unrelated to verbal or overall cognitive ability. In
with ASD had reduced outer hair cell function at the contrast, we found a significant relationship between
1 kHz mid-frequency region using two different meas- 1 kHz DPOAE response and symptom severity in the
ures of outer hair cell integrityDPOAEs and TrOAEs. ASD group.
Specifically, we have discovered that high functioning The observed decreases in OAE amplitudes at 1 kHz
participants with ASD have greatly reduced (25%) mid-frequency region could cause reduced ability to dis-
DPOAE responses compared to TD controls in the criminate between two sounds or impair auditory tun-
1 kHz mid-frequency range, yet have comparable ing [Dallos, 1992]. In fact, Boets, Verhoeven, Wouters,
DPOAE responses outside this critical range (at 0.5 and and Steyaert [2015] noted that children with ASD had
48 kHz regions). We also examined multifrequency impaired auditory tuning when compared with TD chil-
click TrOAEs; an analysis of the spectral features of the dren. In addition, mammalian cochlear development is
TrOAE revealed similarly decreased emissions in the 1 a process with both a basal (high-frequency region) to
2 kHz mid-frequency regions in participants with ASD apical (low-frequency region) developmental gradient

342 Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs INSAR


over several prenatal days (i.e., tonotopic development), overall difference in TrOAE measurements on the right
so deficits in the 1 kHz frequency regions could give between children with ASD and TD controls was
insight into a developmental window of cochlear func- explained by reduced TrOAE responses in the ASD
tion deficits [Lavigne-Rebillard & Pujol, 1986, 1990]. group at similar mid-frequency regions (12 kHz) when
Reduced OAE amplitudes around 1 kHz may also the overall TrOAE responses were spectrally separated
impair speech perception and comprehension in chal- into frequency bands.
lenging environments, since this is in a sensitive region Other explanations for the loss of 1 kHz OAEs could
of the audiogram, and much of vowel discrimination be due to differences in either the MEM reflex or the
(first and second formants) are in this mid-frequency efferent suppression, as there is regulation by the brain
region [Bell, Dirks, & Trine, 1992; Duggirala, Stude- onto cochlear activity via both of these descending
baker, Pavlovic, & Sherbecoe, 1988; French & Steinberg, auditory pathways [Warr & Guinan, 1979]. Children
1947; Ling & Ling, 1978]. with ASD can have asymmetrical and reduced MEM
Our finding of reduced mid-frequency OAEs in chil- reflexes to high-level sounds (>110 dB SPL) [Lukose,
dren with clinically normal audiometric levels parallels Brown, Barber, & Kulesza, 2013]. However, when the
recent finding by Demopoulos and Lewine [2015] who possible influence of MEM reflexes was assayed as to
found that in children with ASD, there was a relation- their contributions to mid-level sounds used in our
ship between mid-frequency pure-tone auditory thresh- DPOAE and TrOAE measures (i.e., at 70 dB SPL), we
olds and expressive and receptive language measures. In found no difference between children with ASD and TD
our study, screening pure tone audiometry was per- controls with very little MEM muscle involvement.
formed to rule out hearing loss (i.e., normal pure tone We also did not find any differences in binaural effer-
audiometry 20 dB SPL), so we do not know what per- ent suppression of TrOAE responses between high-
centage of children had even lower auditory thresholds
functioning children with ASD and TD controls. Khalfa
(i.e., 25 dB SPL). In addition to auditory threshold and
et al. [2001] had previously shown that young (<10
tuning differences between children with ASD and TD
years) low-functioning children with ASD have reduced
controls, other auditory abilities have been investigated
OC efferent feedback using contralateral suppression of
in high-functioning children with ASD and have found
TrOAEs when compared with typically developing sub-
differences in children with ASD, such as frequency
jects. So perhaps differences between the current study
selectivity [Plaisted, Saksida, Alcantara, & Weisblatt,
and Khalfa et al. could be due to differences in age,
2003], pitch discrimination [Bonnel et al., 2003], pitch
severity of ASD, multiple subgroups in ASD with differ-
segmentation [Heaton, 2003], frequency modulation
ent pathophysiologies, or the fact that our study used
detection [Samson et al., 2011], hearing in background
binaural (vs. contralateral) BBN activation of efferent
noise [Alcantara, Weisblatt, Moore, & Bolton, 2004;
feedback. Finally, neither the current study nor Khalfa
Groen et al., 2009], and auditory temporal envelope
et al. assayed DPOAE-based efferent feedback; this may
resolution [Alcantara, Cope, Cope, & Weisblatt, 2012].
Moreover, we are also not the first group to study provide a more sensitive measure of frequency differen-
DPOAEs and TrOAEs in children with ASD, and there ces in OC feedback [Danesh & Kaf, 2012; Humes, 1983].
are conflicting findings with reports of decreased As many of the above mentioned assays of auditory
DPOAE SNRs at all frequencies [Collet et al., 1993; abilities need behavioral responses, OAEs do not, and
Danesh & Kaf, 2012; Kaf & Danesh, 2013; Khalfa et al., these non-invasive measures may be performed in chil-
2001; Tas et al., 2007] to no DPOAE changes [Gravel, dren as young as infancy. This may be important as
Dunn, Lee, & Ellis, 2006; Tharpe et al., 2006]. However, while an accurate ASD diagnosis can reliably be made
all previous DPOAE measurements used uneven primary by 24 months, signs of the ASD are oftentimes present
levels (L2 > L1) whereas our study used equilevel primar- even earlier, yet the majority of children with ASD are
ies (L1 and L2 5 70 dB). We used mid-level equilevel pri- not diagnosed until after age 4 [Autism and Develop-
maries for DPOAE testing modeled after the study by mental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance
Arnold et al. that showed that they were able to detect Year 2010 Principal Investigators, 2014]. Delayed identi-
deficits in cochlear function (as assessed by DPOAEs) in fication results in starting treatment later, which likely
the presence of normal audiometric thresholds. We had attenuates positive outcomes associated with early
piloted DPOAE testing with uneven primaries (L1 5 75 intervention [Bradshaw, Steiner, Gengoux, & Koegel,
dB SPL, L2 5 65 dB SPL) but noticed that at these higher 2015; Koegel, Koegel, Ashbaugh, & Bradshaw, 2014].
L1 levels there were differences in the noise floors Future experiments aimed at testing both younger
between children with ASD and TD controls, whereas and non-verbal children with confirmed ASD using
equilevel primaries at 70 dB SPL yielded robust DPOAE both DPOAE- and TrOAE-based afferent baseline and
responses (6 dB above noise floor) with no differences efferent suppression measures will be required to fully
in DPOAE noise floors across groups. Additionally, the elucidate the potential for such OAE measures to test

INSAR Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs 343


auditory processing deficits and their role in the patho- Bradshaw, J., Steiner, A.M., Gengoux, G., & Koegel, L.K.
genesis of ASD communication symptoms. (2015). Feasibility and effectiveness of very early interven-
tion for infants at-risk for autism spectrum disorder: A sys-
Acknowledgments tematic review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 45, 778794.
This research was supported by grants from NIH R21 Collet, L., Roge, B., Descouens, D., Moron, P., Duverdy, F., &
DC011094, P30 DC05409, Clinical & Translational Sci- Urgell, H. (1993). Objective auditory dysfunction in infan-
ence Institute Pilot Award (UL1 RR024160), and R01 tile autism. Lancet, 342, 923924.
DC009439. We thank Ranisha Nelson, Alyssa Lord, Constantino, J.N. (2005). Social Responsiveness Scale. Los
Ruth Davis, and Jiashu Li for help with data collection Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
and processing, and we also thank the children and Dallos, P. (1992). The active cochlea. Journal of Neuroscience,
families who participated in these studies. 12, 45754585.
Danesh, A.A., & Kaf, W.A. (2012). DPOAEs and contralateral
acoustic stimulation and their link to sound hypersensitiv-
References ity in children with autism. International Journal of Audiol-
ogy, 51, 345352.
Alcantara, J.I., Weisblatt, E.J., Moore, B.C., & Bolton, P.F. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A.N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown,
(2004). Speech-in-noise perception in high-functioning E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally
individuals with autism or Aspergers syndrome. Journal of occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 45, mental Disorders, 28, 479485.
11071114. Demopoulos, C., & Lewine, J.D. (2015). Audiometric profiles in
Alcantara, J.I., Cope, T.E., Cope, W., & Weisblatt, E.J. (2012). autism spectrum disorders: Does subclinical hearing loss
Auditory temporal-envelope processing in high-functioning impact communication? Autism Research, 9, 107120.
children with autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, Demopoulos, C., Hopkins, J., Kopald, B.E., Paulson, K., Doyle,
50, 12351251. L., Andrews, W.E., & Lewine, J.D. (2015). Deficits in audi-
Arnold, D.J., Lonsbury-Martin, B.L., & Martin, G.K. (1999). tory processing contribute to impairments in vocal affect
High-frequency hearing influences lower-frequency distor- recognition in autism spectrum disorders: A MEG study.
tion-product otoacoustic emissions. Archives of Otolaryn- Neuropsychology, 29, 895908.
gology - Head and Neck Surgery, 125, 215222. Duggirala, V., Studebaker, G.A., Pavlovic, C.V., & Sherbecoe,
Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network R.L. (1988). Frequency importance functions for a feature
Surveillance Year 2010 Principal Investigators. (2014). Prev- recognition test material. Journal of the Acoustical Society
alence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 of America, 83, 23722382.
yearsAutism and developmental disabilities monitoring Dyck, M.J., Farrugia, C., Shochet, I.M., & Holmes-Brown, M.
network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR: Surveillance (2004). Emotion recognition/understanding ability in hear-
Summaries, 63, 121. ing or vision-impaired children: Do sounds, sights, or words
Baranek, G.T., Watson, L.R., Boyd, B.A., Poe, M.D., David, F.J., make the difference? Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
& McGuire, L. (2013). Hyporesponsiveness to social and chiatry, 45, 789800.
nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children Edgar, J.C., Khan, S.Y., Blaskey, L., Chow, V.Y., Rey, M., Gaetz,
with developmental delays, and typically developing chil- W., . . . Roberts, T.P.L. (2015). Neuromagnetic oscillations
dren. Development and Psychopathology, 25, 307320. predict evoked-response latency delays and core language
Bell, T.S., Dirks, D.D., & Trine, T.D. (1992). Frequency-impor- deficits in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
tance functions for words in high- and low-context senten- and Developmental Disorders, 45, 395405.
ces. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 950959. French, N.R., & Steinberg, J.C. (1947). Factors governing the
Bergman, B.M., Gorga, M.P., Neely, S.T., Kaminski, J.R., intelligibility of speech sounds. Journal of the Acoustical
Beauchaine, K.L., & Peters, J. (1995). Preliminary descriptions Society of America, 19, 90119.
of transient-evoked and distortion-product otoacoustic emis- Gaffney, G.R., Kuperman, S., Tsai, L.Y., & Minchin, S. (1988).
sions from graduates of an intensive care nursery. Journal of Morphological evidence for brainstem involvement in
the American Academy of Audiology, 6, 150162. infantile autism. Biological Psychiatry, 24, 578586.
Berlin, C.I., Hood, L.J., Hurley, A.E., Wen, H., & Kemp, D.T. Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2009). Standardizing
(1995). Binaural noise suppresses linear click-evoked otoa- ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum
coustic emissions more than ipsilateral or contralateral disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
noise. Hearing Research, 87, 96103. 39, 693705.
Boets, B., Verhoeven, J., Wouters, J., & Steyaert, J. (2015). Fragile Gravel, J.S., Dunn, M., Lee, W.W., & Ellis, M.A. (2006). Periph-
spectral and temporal auditory processing in adolescents with eral audition of children on the autistic spectrum. Ear and
autism spectrum disorder and early language delay. Journal Hearing, 27, 299312.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 18451857. Groen, W.B., van Orsouw, L., Huurne, N., Swinkels, S., van der
Bonnel, A., Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Trudel, M., Gallun, E., & Gaag, R.J., Buitelaar, J.K., & Zwiers, M.P. (2009). Intact spec-
Bonnel, A.M. (2003). Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individ- tral but abnormal temporal processing of auditory stimuli
uals with autism: A signal detection analysis. Journal of in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 226235. 39, 742750.

344 Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs INSAR


Haesen, B., Boets, B., & Wagemans, J. (2011). A review of Washington: Alexander Graham Bell Association for the
behavioural and electrophysiological studies on auditory Deaf.
processing and speech perception in autism spectrum disor- Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop,
ders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 701714. S. (2012). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (2nd
Hashimoto, T., Tayama, M., Murakawa, K., Yoshimoto, T., ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Miyazaki, M., Harada, M., & Kuroda, Y. (1995). Develop- Lukose, R., Brown, K., Barber, C.M., & Kulesza, R.J., Jr. (2013).
ment of the brainstem and cerebellum in autistic patients. Quantification of the stapedial reflex reveals delayed
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25, 118. responses in autism. Autism Research, 6, 344353.
Heaton, P. (2003). Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding Most, T., & Michaelis, H. (2012). Auditory, visual, and auditory-
in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and visual perceptions of emotions by young children with hear-
Allied Disciplines, 44, 543551. ing loss versus children with normal hearing. Journal of
Hitoglou, M., Ververi, A., Antoniadis, A., & Zafeiriou, D.I. Speech Language and Hearing Research, 55, 11481162.
(2010). Childhood autism and auditory system abnormal- Nadig, A.S., Ozonoff, S., Young, G.S., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., &
ities. Pediatric Neurology, 42, 309314. Rogers, S.J. (2007). A prospective study of response to name
Hood, L.J., Berlin, C.I., Hurley, A., Cecola, R.P., & Bell, B. in infants at risk for autism. Archives of Pediatrics and Ado-
(1996). Contralateral suppression of transient-evoked otoa- lescent Medicine, 161, 378383.
coustic emissions in humans: Intensity effects. Hearing OConnor, K. (2012). Auditory processing in autism spectrum
Research, 101, 113118. disorder: A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Humes, L.E. (1983). Psychophysical measures of two-tone sup- Reviews, 36, 836854.
pression and distortion products (2f1-f2) and (f2-f1). Jour- Oldfield, R.C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness:
nal of the Acoustical Society of America, 73, 930950. The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113.
Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Plaisted, K., Saksida, L., Alcantara, J., & Weisblatt, E. (2003).
Schedule, Module 4: Revised algorithm and standardized Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak cen-
severity scores. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor- tral coherence effects: Experiments in visual configural
ders, 44, 19962012. learning and auditory perception. Philosophical Transac-
Kaf, W.A., & Danesh, A.A. (2013). Distortion-product otoacous- tions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological
tic emissions and contralateral suppression findings in chil- Sciences, 358, 375386.
dren with Aspergers syndrome. International Journal of Rodier, P.M., Ingram, J.L., Tisdale, B., Nelson, S., & Romano, J.
Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology, 77, 947954. (1996). Embryological origin for autism: developmental
Keefe, D.H., Gorga, M.P., Jesteadt, W., & Smith, L.M. (2008). anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. Journal of
Ear asymmetries in middle-ear, cochlear, and brainstem Comparative Neurology, 370, 247261.
responses in human infants. Journal of the Acoustical Soci- Rutter, M., LeCouteur, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Autism Diagnostic
ety of America, 123, 15041512. Interview, Revised. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Kemp, D.T. (2002). Otoacoustic emissions, their origin in coch- Samson, F., Hyde, K.L., Bertone, A., Soulieres, I., Mendrek, A.,
lear function, and use. British Medical Bulletin, 63, 223241. Ahad, P., . . . Zeffiro, T.A. (2011). Atypical processing of
Khalfa, S., Veuillet, E., & Collet, L. (1998). Influence of hand- auditory temporal complexity in autistics. Neuropsycholo-
edness on peripheral auditory asymmetry. European Journal gia, 49, 546555.
of Neuroscience, 10, 27312737. Shera, C.A., & Guinan, J.J. (1999). Evoked otoacoustic emis-
Khalfa, S., Bruneau, N., Roge, B., Georgieff, N., Veuillet, E., sions arise by two fundamentally different mechanisms: A
Adrien, J.L., . . . Collet, L. (2001). Peripheral auditory asym- taxonomy for mammalian OAEs. Journal of the Acoustical
metry in infantile autism. European Journal of Neuro- Society of America, 105, 782798.
science, 13, 628632. Tas, A., Yagiz, R., Tas, M., Esme, M., Uzun, C., & Karasalihoglu,
Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., Ashbaugh, K., & Bradshaw, J. (2014). A.R. (2007). Evaluation of hearing in children with autism
The importance of early identification and intervention for by using TEOAE and ABR. Autism, 11, 7379.
children with or at risk for autism spectrum disorders. Inter- Tharpe, A.M., Bess, F.H., Sladen, D.P., Schissel, H., Couch, S.,
national Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 5056. & Schery, T. (2006). Auditory characteristics of children
Kulesza, R.J., & Mangunay, K. (2008). Morphological features with autism. Ear and Hearing, 27, 430441.
of the medial superior olive in autism. Brain Research, Veuillet, E., Collet, L., & Duclaux, R. (1991). Effect of contralat-
1200, 132137. eral acoustic stimulation on active cochlear micromechanical
Lavigne-Rebillard, M., & Pujol, R. (1986). Development of the properties in human subjects: dependence on stimulus varia-
auditory hair cell surface in human fetuses: A scanning bles. Journal of Neurophysiology, 65, 724735.
electron microscopy study. Anatomy and Embryology, 174, Warr, W.B., & Guinan, J.J. (1979). Efferent innervation of the
369377. organ of corti: Two separate systems. Brain Research, 173,
Lavigne-Rebillard, M., & Pujol, R. (1990). Auditory hair cells in 152155.
human fetuses: Synaptogenesis and ciliogenesis. Journal of Wechsler, D. (2004). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th
Electron Microscopy Technique, 15, 115122. ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
Ling, D., & Ling, A.H. (1978). Aural Habilitation: The Founda- Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (4th ed.).
tions of Verbal Learning in Hearing-Impaired Children (1st ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.

INSAR Bennetto et al./Children with autism have reduced OAEs 345

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen