Sie sind auf Seite 1von 9

2014 AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGICAL ASSOCIATION

The VOLUME 14
Behavior Analyst NUMBERS 1 & 2
Today JULY 2014

The zeitgeist of behavior analytic research in the


21st century: a keyword analysis
Derek D. Reed1, Florence D. DiGennaro Reed2, Sarah R. Jenkins3, & Jason M. Hirst4
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Behavior analysis appears to be at a crossroads of science and practice. Recent concerns and discussion within the flagship
journal of the Association of Behavior Analysis International (abai ; the Behavior Analyst) suggest that basic behavior
analytic research has become too esoteric, while applied research has become too focused on service delivery approaches to
children with developmental disabilities. Moreover, it has been argued that little overlap between basic and applied research
has come via translation between these two wings of our science. The purpose of the present analysis was to chronicle the
directionusing keywordsof behavior analytic research from the years 20002009 to assess the validity of these concerns
ABSTRACT

and to document the publication trends of flagship journals. Keywords were extracted from each article published in Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, The Analysis of Verbal Behavior and the Behavior
Analyst. Data were analyzed using frequency of keywords appearing in each journal as well as across journals. Word clouds were
generated from the frequency data to show a visual representation of popular focus areas, where keywords with the highest
frequencies appear graphically larger. Additionally, an analysis compared each of the journals with each other to quantify the
degree of keyword overlap as a crude index of cross representation.
keywords : behavior analysis, keyword analysis, publication trends, zeitgeist

S
kinners publication of Walden Two in 1948 may be Schlinger, 2010). Although the discussion of the rift between EAB
considered a pivotal moment in the science of behavior; and ABA was previously an exercise of verbal behavior on philo-
the emerging technologies of behavior analysis and operant sophical positions and a discussion piece for how the field should
learning now had a theoretical description of the potential to proceed, we are now seeing evidence of a growing divide across
architect socially important behavior change. As the science of our branches of behavioral science. As some behavior analysts
behavior began to inform social reform and human treatment have proposed, contemporary EAB research has become rather
approaches, the applied wing of the field of behavior analysis grew nuancedaccording to Poling (2010), the E in EAB should stand
and the enterprise of applied behavior analysis (ABA) took root for esotericand has drifted away from issues of social impor-
(see Rutherford, 2009). Such a growth, however, began to depict tance (e.g., Poling, 2010; Critchfield, 2011a). Under this direction,
two distinct disciplines within the field, ABA and the experimental it is feared that EAB may be alienating itself from issues of applied
analysis of behavior (EAB). Within just a few decades, the strong relevance. At the other end of the continuum, applied research in
majority of editorial staff for Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis ABA has become somewhat synonymous with the assessment and
(83%) and Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (74%) re- treatment of problem behaviors associated with developmental
ported that ABA and EAB have become increasingly separate from disabilities (Friman, 2010; Poling, 2010). This trend moves applied
one another (Poling, Picker, Grossett, Hall-Johnson, & Holbrook, research further away from directly translating principles and
1981), providing some evidence that the field was fracturing into concepts from EAB. As summarized by Critchfield (2011a), this
two distinct disciplines (see Mace, 1994). Fortunately, these staff growing schism may be an artifact of a lack of collaborative trans-
also viewed this trend as problematic for behavioral psychology. lation. Mace and Critchfield (2010) contend that such scientific
Fast forward several decades later to present day The future translation in behavior analysis is at best, occasional (pg. 304).
direction of behavior analysis seems to be at a crossroads (see So what, then, is the zeitgeist of behavior analytic research in the
1 Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas; 2 Department of
21st century? Is argument of a divide between basic science (EAB)
Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas; 3 Department of Applied Behavioral and applied research (ABA) valid? Perhaps more importantly, can
Science, University of Kansas; 4 Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University we objectively answer any of these questions? The answers to these
of Kansas. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Derek D.
Reed, Department of Applied Behavioral Science, University of Kansas, 4048 Dole questions are complicated and take more than any one analysis
Human Development Center, 1000 Sunnyside Avenue, Lawrence, ks 66045-7555. or discussion. Fortunately, however, we do have some guidance
E-mail: dreed@ku.edu. The authors wish to thank Tom Critchfield for his insightful
recommendations and comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. on ways to approach these questions.

17
REED, DIGENNARO REED, JENKINS, & HIRST

of particular bodies of research based solely off


counts of references, which as been the focus of
previous analyses (e.g., how many human
or non-human studies were published in
JEAB; Perone, 1985).
Questions of cultural evo-
lution have been empirically
examined using a number of
analyses and metrics. For ex-
ample, Michel and colleagues
(2011) recently explored cul-
tural trends over time through
To objectively de- Figure 1. a word cloud for the Behavior the analysis of millions of En-
Analyst (tba ) depicting the most frequently
scribe the function glish-written books. In this
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

reported keywords (n=32)


of the field (with respect approach to what they term
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

to basic and applied interests), the culturomic analysis, researchers detect the rise and fall of terms,
status quo has been to conduct cita- keywords, and topics in published books with the interpretation
tion analyses (see Garfield, 1972; e.g., that these trends represent the evolving zeitgeist of English-speak-
Carr & Britton, 2003; Critchfield, 2002; ing cultures. Others have argued for a more crude analysis of
Critchfield & Reed, 2004; Elliott, Kineta, Fuqua, Ehrhardt, & zeitgeist by examining citizen journalism conducted through
Poling, 2005; Laties & Mace, 1993; Perone, 1985; Poling, Alling, social media such as Twitter (Murthy, 2011; e.g., Chew & Eysen-
& Fuqua, 1994) where researchers examine trends in citation bach, 2010; Signorini, Segre, & Polgreen, 2011) or Google TrendsTM
sources within and between journals. Perones citation analysis (http://www.google.com/intl/en/trends/about.html) and Google
(1985) of JEAB documented a stable trend in the publication ZeitgeistTM to report the aggregate search terms. Only recently have
of human and non-human articles. Unfortunately, there was a psychologists proposed the analysis of GoogleTM search terms for
concomitant trend in nearly exclusive citation of non-human scientific inquiry (Bentley & Ormerod, 2010; Cummings, 2009;
studies in non-human JEAB publications (in human JEAB Guo, Zhang, & Zhai, 2010), but it appears as though such data may
publications, the number of citations of human studies approx- help researchers quantify public interests. Despite the relatively
imated those from non-human studies). This trend suggested scant research literature on the above topics, it is becoming ap-
that, through Volume 38 (1982) of JEAB, human research had parent that advances in keyword analyses provide a useful metric
little influence on research conceptualization or methodology. for cultural/professional zeitgeist.
Likewise the citation analyses of JABA and JEAB by Critchfield The purpose of the present study was to apply the rationale
(2002), as well as Critchfield and Reed (2004), found a strong behind culturomic and search term research and analyses to the
unidirectional relation which showed that JABA proportionally study of published keywords in behavior analysis flagship journals
cited JEAB articles over four times as frequently than JEAB cited (The Analysis of Verbal Behavior [TAVB; ISSN 08899401], Journal
JABA articles. Although such approaches have been fruitful in of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior [JEAB; ISSN 00225002],
describing sources of influence in behavior analytic publications, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis [JABA; ISSN 00218855], and
they speak very little to the topics represented in journals. That The Behavior Analyst [TBA; ISSN 07386729]) from 20002009 in
is, citation analyses describe citations alone, not necessarily an attempt to quantify the culture of contemporary behavioral
the zeitgeist of scholarship. We propose that an empirical ap- research in the 21st century. Specifically, we sought to document
proach to understanding the culture of journals would better any potential trends within topics appearing among the journals
address the question of whether EAB and ABA are fractioning and to examine whether the various disciplines within the field
within the field of behavior analysis. The Oxford Dictionaries (i.e., subfields) were trending on similar topics. Finally, by exam-
(http://oxforddictionaries.com) defines culture (in the biological
sense of the term) as the (a) arts and other manifestations of Figure 2. a word cloud for Journal
of Applied Behavior Analysis (jaba )
human intellectual achievement regarded collectively, and depicting the most frequently
(b) ideas, customs, and social behaviour of a particular reported keywords (n=32)
people or society. Understanding cultural aspects of EAB
and ABA thus squarely places the empirical emphasis on
current behaviorsnot influences (cf. cita-
tion analyses). That is, the field
could benefit from analyses
of trends within and across
journals (analogous to the
culture of the field), rather
than looking at the impact

18 Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 | The Behavior Analyst Today


THE ZEITGEIST OF BEHAVIOR ANALYTIC RESEARCH IN THE 21ST CENTURY

Table 1. Top 32 keywords in each journal, in order of most (top) to least frequent. The first number in parentheses indicates raw frequency of occurrence
within the journal, with the second number representing the relative proportion of keywords comprised by that word/phrase within the journal.
JABA JEAB TAVB TBA
functional analysis (121; .045) choice (77; .032) verbal behavior (21; .106) B.F. Skinner (20; .028)
autism (88; .033) concurrent schedule (43; .018) autism (19; .065) behavior analysis (20; .028)
problem behavior (82; .030) drug (37; .015) mand (11; .038) verbal behavior (18; .025)
dev. disabilities* (57; .021) discrimination (35; .014) tact (9; .031) aba * (15; .021)

choice (40; .015) stimulus equivalence (34; .014) joint control (8; .027) history (12; .017)
teaching (38; .014) matching to sample (33; .014) automatic reinforcement (7; .024) rft * (10; .014)

ncr * (37; .014) reinforcement (26; .011) matching to sample (7; .024) radical behaviorism (9; .012)
safety (37; .014) stimulus control (24; .010) conditional discr.* (6; .021) clinical beh. analysis* (8; .011)
establishing operation (35; .013) adults (23; .010) intraverbal (5; .017) cognition (8; .011)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

preference assessment (33; .012) cond. discrimination* (23; .010) mul. exemplar training* (5; .017) eab * (8; .011)

prompting (32; .012) conditioned reinf.* (22; .009) stim.-stim. pairing* (5; .017) research (8; .011)
differential reinf.* (31; .011) fixed interval schedule (20; .008) B.F. Skinner (4; .014) teaching (8; .011)
reinforcement (30; .011) fixed ratio schedule (20; .008) children (4; .014) pragmatism (7; .010)
extinction (26; .010) matching law (20; .008) language (4; .014) stimulus control (7; .010)
feedback (25; .009) concurrent chains (18; .007) echoic (3; .010) private events (6; .008)
generalization (25; .009) generalized matching (18; .007) establishing operation (3; .010) autism (5; .007)
assessment (22; .008) resistance to change (18; .007) motivating operation (3; .010) behaviorism (5; .007)
negative reinf. (22; .008) learning (17; .007) naming (3; .010) dev. disabilities* (5; .007)
children (21; .008) reinf. schedules* (17; .007) stimulus control (3; .010) evolution (5; .007)
preference (21; .008) reinforcement delay (16; .007) teaching (3; .010) certification (4; .006)
adhd * (20; .007) variable-interval sch.* (16; 007) academic behavior (2; .007) derived stim. relations* (4; .006)
fct * (20; .007) matching (15; .006) autoclitic (2; .007) determinism (4; .006)
self-injurious behavior (20; .007) avoidance (14; .006) behavior analysis (2; .007) functional analysis (4; .006)
automatic reinf.* (19; .007) B.F. Skinner (14; .006) dev. disabilities* (2; .007) gender equity (4; .006)
preschoolers (19; .007) children (14; .006) functional analysis (2; .007) generalization (4; .006)
reinforcer assessment (19; .007) discounting (14; .006) generativity (2; .007) jeab (4; .006)

descriptive analysis (18; .007) preference (14; .006) language delay (2; .007) negative reinforcement (4; .006)
motivating operation (17; .006) dlyd match to sample* (13; .005) learning (2; .007) positive beh. support* (4; .006)
concurrent schedule (16; .006) transfer of function (13; .005) mental retardation (2; .007) problem behavior (4; .006)
drug (16; .006) verbal behavior (13; .005) preference (2; .007) reinforcement (4; .006)
self-control (16; .006) behavioral economics (12; .005) prompting (2; .007) stimulus equivalence (4; .006)
stereotypy (16; .006) multiple schedule (12; .005) receptive-echoic-tact transfer variability (4; .006)
procedure (2; .007)
*Keyword abbreviated from original term for table formatting purposes.

ining the degree of keyword overlap between the flagship journals, All of the journals, with one exception, used keywords for most, if
we sought to assess whether the data from these analyses could not all, of the articles in each issue during the decade we reviewed.
elucidate the growing concern that basic and applied research Our analysis of TAVB was limited to 2005 to 2009 because it did
wings are growing more disparate. not begin publishing keywords until 2005.
Keywords were subsequently edited in order to better capture
METHOD the representative content and focus globally. That is, we were
Transcription and editing procedures interested in summarizing general concepts as represented by
We transcribed the keywords indicated in all articles published keyword use. First, generally synonymous (but differently spelled)
between 2000 and 2009 in TBA, JABA, JEAB, and TAVB. Transcrip- keywords were converted to the most frequently used term (e.g.,
tion consisted of typing each keyword separately by article as it manding and mands were converted to mand). This task was
was written in the journal into a spreadsheet in Microsoft Excel. accomplished by hand and determined by consensus for each

The Behavior Analyst Today | Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 19


REED, DIGENNARO REED, JENKINS, & HIRST

keyword. Second, keywords listed five or fewer times across the


four journals were changed to a broader term within the key-
word list (e.g., philosophical determinism
was converted to determinism) if one
existed (e.g., academic administrators,
authorship, willpower were not
converted). The edited keywords
were used for analysis. Despite
these edits, we refer to these
terms as keywords in the
remainder of this paper for
ease of discussion.

Data analyses
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The transcription and editing procedures


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

yielded 6,738 keywords retained for further


analysis. Unlike JABA, TAVB, and TBA, JEAB Figure 3. a word cloud for Journal of the Experimental
dictates specific keyword practices with respect to Analysis of Behavior (jeab) depicting the most frequently
reported keywords (n = 32)
required descriptors:
all journals. If a keyword matched across two or more journals,
List no more than seven key words in descending order of im- we recorded the relative frequency of this keyword in each jour-
portance, with the only exception being that the response under nal in which it appeared. We then summed these within-journal
study (e.g., key peck) should be next to last and the species of frequencies and divided the total number of matched keyword
the subjects (e.g., pigeons, humans) listed last. (Preparation of frequencies by the total number of keywords published in each
Manuscript, 2006, p. 143) respective journal during our 10-year review (5 years for TAVB).
Thus, due to differing numbers of total keywords across the four
Thus, we opted to exclude the response (e.g., lever press, mouse journals, a shared keyword will constitute a different proportion of
click) and species keywords (e.g., humans, pigeon, strains of the total number of keywords in each journal in which it appears.
rats/mice) from all analyses to better equate the content contained
within keyword sets. Our primary analyses concerned the cumu- Rater training and inter-rater agreement
lative frequency of the most commonly indicated keywords. Data Three doctoral students in behavioral psychology and one under-
analyses consisted of three procedures. First, we displayed the most graduate student in applied behavioral science served as raters. They
frequently reported keywords (n=32) for each journal in a word received group training on the transcription procedures and use
cloud using Wordle. Because the top 32 keywords constituted of the Microsoft Excel spreadsheet by two doctoral-level
the top 33% of keywords in TBA (the journal of primary focus), behavior analysts (faculty members) and independently
we applied this criterion to all other journals. Font size within transcribed one issue of an included journal until 100%
the word clouds reflects the relative keyword frequency; that is, agreement was obtained. Additionally, we calculated
if word A occurs twice as much as word B, then the font size for inter-rater agreement for a random sample of 22.9% of
word A is double that of word B. Our second analysis consisted the years we reviewed across journals.
of identifying the three most frequently reported keywords for Because raters were in
JABA, JEAB, and TAVB as well as 100% agreement
the three most frequently reported
research subject keywords
and generating a cumu-
lative record across
the years we analyzed.
We applied the same
procedures to summarize the
cumulative frequency of behavior
analysis subfields (as indicated by
keyword) in TBA.
Figure 4. a word cloud for The
To more precisely capture the correspondence between ABAIs Analysis of Verbal Behavior (tavb )
flagship journal (TBA) and the important subfield journals, as depicting the most frequently
reported keywords (n=32)
well as correspondence between the three subfield journals, we
quantified the degree of keyword overlap across JABA, JEAB, TAVB,
and TBA. Specifically, we examined all keyword matches between

20 Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 | The Behavior Analyst Today


THE ZEITGEIST OF BEHAVIOR ANALYTIC RESEARCH IN THE 21ST CENTURY

100 JABA 100 JEAB


90 90
80 80
70 70
60 60
50 50
40 40
30 30
20 20
% overlap b/w journal keywords

10 10
0 0
JEAB TAVB TBA JABA TAVB TBA
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

100 TAVB 100 TBA


90 90
80 80
70 70
60 60
50 50
40 40
30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
JABA JEAB TBA JABA JEAB TAVB

target journal for keyword comparisons


Figure 5. Grouped bar graphs representing the percentage of keywords in the target journals (depicted on the
x-axis) that overlapped with those of comparison journals (those indicated in the legend). See text for details.

when keywords were not indicated for an article, we calculated Although word clouds are a helpful tool for within-journal analysis,
agreement for only those articles that contained keywords. This supplemental analyses are required for more detailed comparisons
allowed us to arrive at a more conservative agreement estimate. across journals. These terms are also displayed in Table 1.
An agreement was scored when the keywords were transcribed When comparing words in the word clouds of Figures 1 through 4,
(i.e., typed) identically across both raters for the article, issue, several areas of shared interest emerge. For example, the term
and year assigned. Spelling errors or other typing mistakes were matching to sample is featured in the word clouds of both TAVB
counted as disagreements. Percentage agreement was calculated by and JEAB. Similarly, the word choice appears in the word clouds
dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements of both JABA and JEAB. However, because word clouds are not
plus disagreements and multiplying by 100. Percentage agreement standard scientific displays (i.e., they lack scales or metrics), we
averaged 98.9% (range, 97.3%99.3%). Before completing our supplement Figures 1 through 4 with a quantitative analysis of
analyses, we corrected all errors based on how the keyword(s) the degree of overlap between the journals. These relations are
appeared in the source article. depicted in Figure 5. Review of the indices of overlap indicates that
34.9%, 26.3%, and 36.0% of keywords from JABA overlapped with
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION JEAB, TAVB, and TBA, respectively. Within JEAB, 46.1%, 13.5%, and
Figures 1 through 4 depict the word clouds for TBA, JABA, JEAB, 32.3% of keywords were shared with those in JABA, TAVB, and TBA,
and TAVB, respectively. Each word cloud contains the top 32 most respectively. Keywords in JABA, JEAB, and TBA, respectively, were
frequently reported keywords. An advantage of using a word shared with 64.0%, 47.9%, and 38.3% of those in TAVB. Examining
cloud to display these terms is that it allows straightforward visual the degree to which subfield journals keywords were represented
analysis. For example, readers are able to quickly determine that in ABAIs flagship journal (TBA), we found that JABA and JEAB
the terms functional analysis, problem behavior, and autism were shared nearly equally (41.5% and 45.5%, respectively). TAVB
are the most frequently used keywords in JABA (see Figure 2). was slightly less represented with 20.3% overlap.

The Behavior Analyst Today | Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 21


REED, DIGENNARO REED, JENKINS, & HIRST

top 5 JABA keywords top 5 JEAB keywords

functional analysis choice JABA


JEAB
autism concurrent schedule TAVB
TBA
problem behavior drug

developmental disabilities discrimination

choice stimulus equivalence

0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

top 5 TAVB keywords top 5 TBA keywords


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

verbal behavior B.F. Skinner

autism behavior analysis

mand verbal behavior

tact applied behavior analysis

joint control history

0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15 0.00 0.05 0.10 0.15


relative proportion of journals keywords
Figure 6. Horizontal stacked histograms depicting the relative proportion of each journals keywords comprised
by the top five keywords from jaba (upper left panel), jeab (upper right panel), tavb (bottom left panel), and tba
(bottom right panel). Shared interests among journals are evidenced by relatively larger and stacked histograms.

Figure 6 contains a more molecular analysis of the top five key- The top panel of Figure 7 depicts cumulative frequency of the
words for each subfield journal. This analysis allows quantification three most frequently reported keywords across the years we
of the degree of overlap between each journal for each respective reviewed. There is clear differentiation of the top keywords in
journals top five frequently used keywords. By examining the JABA. The term functional analysis is used most often followed
relative proportion of each keywords contribution to the entire by problem behavior and choice. For JEAB, choice shows
journals keyword set, some speculation about shared interests a higher rate of use than the second two most frequently used
between journals can be permitted. For example, the top left panel words, concurrent schedule and drug, which appear at nearly
of Figure 6 depicts the top five keywords appearing in JABA. The identical rates. A sharp rate increase is evident for the keyword
proportion of JABA keywords of the term choice was .015 and verbal behavior in TAVB; the keywords mand and tact
was the fifth most used keyword in that journal during the time- both have similarly slow, yet steady rates. Finally, the bottom
span of interest. As this panel also indicates, choice accounts for right panel of Figure 7 suggests that the top three keywords in
a proportion of .032 in JEAB and was the most frequently used TBA (B.F. Skinner, behavior analysis, and verbal behavior,
keyword in JEAB as evidenced by the top right panel of Figure 6. respectively) did not differ in relative rate over the years 2000
Thus, choice appears to be a relatively strong topic in both JABA to 2009, suggesting no major change in focus in the journal
and JEAB, suggesting shared interest in this concept between the during that time span.
two journals. Figure 6 also highlights shared interest in autism The cumulative frequency of keywords associated with various
between JABA and TAVB, as well as verbal behavior between subfields reported in TBA from 2000 to 2009 is depicted in Figure
TAVB and TBA. Such overlap, no matter how small in size, is note- 8. The terms behavior analysis, verbal behavior, and applied
worthy in demonstrating commonalities across the basic-applied behavior analysis steadily increased across the years we reviewed.
continuum of our fields science. These mutual interests should Experimental analysis of behavior increased from 2000 to 2003,
be celebrated as optimistic glimmers of potentiality. By identify- but its rates have not increased since 2005. The keyword clinical
ing such commonalities, basic and applied researchers alike can behavior analysis increased only slightly from 2000 to 2008, but
identify areas of scientific translation that will help bridge the gap showed a rapid increase in 2009 (due, in part, to a special issue
between laboratory and practical interests. that was published on this topic; Kanter & Woods, 2009).

22 Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 | The Behavior Analyst Today


THE ZEITGEIST OF BEHAVIOR ANALYTIC RESEARCH IN THE 21ST CENTURY

124 functional analysis JABA  128 choice JEAB


problem behavior  concurrent schedule
choice  drug
93  96
  

   
62  
 64 
  
    
31    32     
     
   
 
cumulative frequency


    
  

 

 

0 0
2001 2003 2005 2007 2009 2001 2003 2005 2007 2009

32 verbal behavior TAVB  25 B.F. Skinner TBA


mand behavior analysis
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tact verbal behavior


 20 

This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

24 
 



 15    
16 



10  
    
 
8     
 
 5  
 
  
0 0  
2001 2003 2005 2007 2009 2001 2003 2005 2007 2009
Figure 7. cumulative records for the three most frequently reported keywords jeab , jaba , tavb , and tba .

The findings above provide an optimistic view of the relation We must also address our second question: Is argument of a
between basic science and applied research in behavior analysis. divide between basic and applied research valid? Using an objec-
However, such numbers may also be deceiving. By including all tive (i.e., quantitative) analysis of the overlap of keywords in the
keywords in the analysis, even those appearing infrequently within flagship journals, our results suggest that areas of mutual interest
journals contribute to the overlap metrics. It is possible, therefore, exist. These matched interests may serve as translational catalysts,
that the bulk of interest in one journal may not be well represented bridging the basic and applied research domains. Although over-
in another, despite seemingly high degrees of overlap. For example, lap was found among the journals, the zeitgeist within a journal
examination of Figure 2 indicates that functional analysis is by is not necessarily shared between journals. These data support
far the most frequent keyword in JABA (occurring 121 times, com- the notion that the different subfields within behavior analysis
prising 4.5% of all keywords), suggesting that this term may well represent different interests and rightfully have distinct journals.
represent the zeitgeist of this journal. Despite the relatively high In one sense, these data indicate the four journals we reviewed
degree of overlap between JABA and JEAB, the term functional exist independently and serve very different purposes for the
analysis never appeared in JEAB. Similarly, verbal behavior is fieldthis could be viewed as a success. In another sense, there
the clear leading term for TAVB, with 31 occurrences making up
10.6% of this journals total keywords. Although Figure 5 suggests
that TAVBs keywords are most strongly represented in JABA, it is subfields in tba
peculiar that the term verbal behavior accounts for only 0.6% 25 applied behavior analysis
(n=16) of JABAs total keywords. Because of these analytic issues behavior analysis
clinical behavior analysis
cumulative frequency

related to the total overlap metric, we find the use of the word cloud 20 experimental analysis of behavior   
to be of value when quickly identifying areas of shared zeitgeists. verbal behavior


15     
Summary  
  

Let us return to one of our original questions: Using the word 10  

 
    

cloud analysis, what is the zeitgeist of behavior analytic research   
 
5  

in the 21st century? The displays in Figures 1 through 4 convey a 
 
 
       
large amount of information in an easy-to-read format allowing 0 

readers to quickly identify the current topics of interest. When this 2001 2003 2005 2007 2009
information is combined with the cumulative records (Figures 5
and 6), it is apparent that the most frequently studied topics (indi- year
cated by keywords depicted in the word clouds) have consistently
Figure 8. cumulative frequency of subfield keyword use
remained areas of focus across the first decade of the 21st century. in the Behavior Analyst from 2000 to 2009.

The Behavior Analyst Today | Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 23


REED, DIGENNARO REED, JENKINS, & HIRST

remains room for further collaboration to promote bidirectional Critchfield (2011a, 2011b) and Mace (1994) for general guidance
translational programs (i.e., practical interests could spawn labo- on how translational research would aid both wings of our science.
ratory simulations, while the emerging EAB findings could spark Additionally, we believe that journals should explicitly encourage
applied utility). The disparities between the journals may point translational topics (something that JEAB has recently endorsed;
out different research agendas between the subfields and may be Mazur, 2010) and promote the citation of both basic and applied
representative of a growing divide in interests. We discuss this studies in their accepted manuscripts whenever possible.
potential divide in greater detail below.
Our analyses are complicated by several significant limitations. Recommendations regarding keywords
First, we do not have standards regarding what percent overlap is Our analytic procedures identified numerous limitations regarding
sufficient to claim the existence of a shared research topic. Perhaps
the comparisons of keywords across journals. These limitations
what we view as optimistic others may interpret as disappoint- speak to a broader issue of disparate practices in keyword use
ingly low (or vice versa). Second, terminological shifts may have across journals that could influence future keyword reviews, or
occurred without our knowledge, thus impacting interpretation more importantly, the identification of these journals articles in
of these findings across the years we reviewed. For example, some database searches. We recommend that journal editors consider
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

behavior analysts have argued that the term experimental anal- standardizing the specification and use of keywords across our
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ysis of behavior has been replaced with the more general term flagship journals. Moreover, editors could require that keywords
behavior analysis (Vargas, 2009), possibly explaining why the representing larger concepts (e.g., problem behavior) as well as
term has seen a decrease in use. The extent to which similar shifts more specific terms (e.g., self-injury) are listed in a particular se-
may have impacted use of other keywords is unknown. Finally, quence (similar to JEABs requirements). More stringent keyword
the present analysis is but one objective analytic technique and selection procedures should be adopted and enforced as well. For
other techniques might offer different information. Further em- example, self-injury and self-injurious behavior were listed
pirical introspection from our field is thus necessary to objectively
as unique keywords in JABA as were mands and manding in
examine whether such a basic-applied divide is emerging. Future TAVB; however, in a quick keyword database search we discovered
research could expand the analysis to incorporate journals outside that relevant articles were not consistently identified (possibly
of those in this study to better reflect the wider dissemination of due to these discrepancies). These issues may present greater
behavior analytic research. problems than we even anticipate for readers of these journals
regardless of their reason for completing keyword searches. Re-
The potential divide between eab and aba searchers and students who do not access relevant research via
Three decades ago, Poling and colleagues (1981) raised the question electronic keyword searches might be missing important details
of whether a schism between EAB and ABA was real, and if so, when designing a study. Practitioners designing evidence-based
who might care. Their survey results suggested that a schism was or empirically-supported interventions could delay or, even worse,
verbally reported by JABA and JEAB editorial staff, and that these prevent important treatment effects if their literature searches do
staff cared about what this schism meant for the sustainability of not yield consistently relevant articles. Finally, culturomic and
the field. Citation analyses have empirically documented such a search term research and analyses, such as the one we present in
schism (see Perone, 1985; Critchfield, 2002; Critchfield & Reed, this paper, are becoming increasingly used to detect trends and
2004) in sources of scholarly influence. Our approach to objectively assess zeitgeist and/or cultural evolution in both the humanities
describing the cultures of EAB and ABA does not provide evidence and social sciences. Our field would benefit from periodic analysis
for or against a growing divide. Although potential solutions to of our own behavior, as represented through keyword use in our
bridging research between EAB and ABA are outside the scope flagship journals. A standardized approach to specifying keywords
of our analysis, interested readers are encouraged to consult would aid future analysis.

24 Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 | The Behavior Analyst Today


THE ZEITGEIST OF BEHAVIOR ANALYTIC RESEARCH IN THE 21ST CENTURY

REFERENCES
Bentley, R.A. & Ormerod, P. (2010). A rapid method for assessing social Mace, F.C. (1994). Basic research needed for stimulating the develop-
versus independent interest in health issues: A case study of bird ment of behavioral technologies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
flu and swine flu. Social Science & Medicine, 7, 482485. of Behavior, 61, 529550.
Carr, J.E., & Britton, L.N. (2003). Citation trends of applied journals Mace, F.C., & Critchfield, T.S. (2010). Translational research in behavior
in behavioral psychology: 19812000. Journal of Applied Behavior analysis: Historical traditions and imperative for the future. Journal
Analysis, 36, 113117. of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 293312.
Chew, C., & Eysenbach, G. (2010). Pandemics in the age of Twitter: Mazur, J.E. (2010). Editorial: Translational research in JEAB. Journal of
Content analysis of tweets during the 2009 H1N1 outbreak. PLoS ONE, the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 291292.
5(11), e14118. Retrieved from http://www.plosone.org.
Michel, J.B., Shen, Y.K., Aiden, A.P., Veres, A., Gray, M.K., The Google
Critchfield, T.S. (2002). Evaluating the function of applied behavior Books Team, Aiden, E.L. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture
analysis: A bibliometric analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, using millions of digitized books. Science, 331, 176182.
35, 423426.
Murthy, D. (2011). Twitter: Microphone for the masses? Media, Culture
Critchfield, T.S. (2011a). Translational contributions of the experimental & Society, 33, 779789.
analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 317.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Perone, M. (1985). On the impact of human operant research: Asym-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Critchfield, T.S. (2011b). To a young basic scientist, about to embark on a metrical patterns of cross-citation between human and nonhuman
program of translational research. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 133148. research. The Behavior Analyst, 8, 185189.
Critchfield, T.S., & Reed, D.D. (2004). Conduits of translation in be-
Poling, A. (2010). Looking to the future: Will behavior analysis survive
havior-science bridge research. In J.E. Burgos, & E. Ribes (Eds.).
and prosper? The Behavior Analyst, 33, 617.
Theory, basic and applied research, and technological applications in
behavior science: Conceptual and methodological issues (pp. 4584). Poling, A., Alling, K., & Fuqua, R.W. (1994). Self- and cross-citations
Guadalajara, Mexico: University of Guadalajara Press. in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis and the Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior: 19831992. Journal of Applied
Cummings, E.J. (2009). Trends in mental health googling. Psychiatric
Behavior Analysis, 27, 729731.
Bulletin, 33, 437.
Elliott, A.J., Kineta, M., R., Fuqua, R.W., Ehrhardt, K., & Poling, A. (2005). Poling, A., Picker, M., Grossett, D., Hall-Johnson, E., & Holbrook, M.
Self-and cross-citations in the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (1981). The schism between experimental and applied behavior
and the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior: 19932003. analysis: Is it real and who cares? The Behavior Analyst, 4, 93102.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 559563. Preparation of manuscripts. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Friman, P.C. (2010). Come on in, the water is fine: Achieving mainstream Behavior, 85, 143146. Rutherford, A. (2009). Beyond the box: B.F.
relevance through integration with primary medical care. The Behavior Skinners technology of behavior from laboratory to life, 1950s-1970s.
Analyst, 33, 1936. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

Garfield, E. (1972). Citation analysis as a tool in journal evaluation. Schlinger, H.D. (Ed.). (2010).The future of behavior analysis [Special
Science, 178, 471479. section]. The Behavior Analyst, 33, 145.
Guo, S., Zhang, G., & Zhai, R. (2010). A potential way of enquiry into Signorini, A., Segre, A.M., & Polgreen, P.M. (2011). The use of Twitter to
human curiosity. British Journal of Educational Technology, 41(3), track levels of disease activity and public concern in the U.S. during
E48-E52. the influenza A H1N1 pandemic. PLoS ONE, 6(5), e19467. Retrieved
from http://www.plosone.org.
Kanter, J.W. & Woods, D.W. (2009). [Special section]. The Behavior
Analyst, 32, 1184. Skinner, B.F. (1948). Walden two. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing
Company.
Laties, V.G., & Mace, F.C. (1993). Taking stock: The first 25 years of the
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Vargas, J.S. (2009). Behavior analysis for effective teaching. New York,
Analysis, 26, 513525. NY: Routledge.

The Behavior Analyst Today | Volume 14 | Numbers 1 & 2 | July 2014 25

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen