Sie sind auf Seite 1von 22

Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Memory and Language


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jml

A predictive framework for evaluating models of semantic


organization in free recall
Neal W Morton , Sean M. Polyn
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, PMB 407817, 2301 Vanderbilt Place, Nashville, TN 37240-7817, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Research in free recall has demonstrated that semantic associations reliably influence the
Received 3 February 2015 organization of search through episodic memory. However, the specific structure of these
revision received 30 September 2015 associations and the mechanisms by which they influence memory search remain unclear.
Available online 31 October 2015
We introduce a likelihood-based model-comparison technique, which embeds a model of
semantic structure within the context maintenance and retrieval (CMR) model of human
Keywords: memory search. Within this framework, model variants are evaluated in terms of their
Episodic memory
ability to predict the specific sequence in which items are recalled. We compare three
Memory search
Clustering
models of semantic structure, latent semantic analysis (LSA), global vectors (GloVe), and
Computational model word association spaces (WAS), and find that models using WAS have the greatest
predictive power. Furthermore, we find evidence that semantic and temporal organization
is driven by distinct item and context cues, rather than a single context cue. This finding
provides important constraint for theories of memory search.
2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction demonstrated that the prior experience of a participant


also strongly influences search of episodic memory, in
Findings from list-learning paradigms such as free recall the form of semantic organization, the tendency for partic-
demonstrate that the temporal structure of a learning ipants to successively recall items that are semantically
experience has an important influence on how studied related to one another (Bousfield, 1953; Glanzer, 1969;
materials are remembered. The effects of this temporal Howard & Kahana, 2002b; Romney, Brewer, & Batchelder,
structure are evident in the primacy and recency effects 1993). Semantic organization (also known as semantic
of free recall (Murdock, 1962). Furthermore, temporal clustering) is observed both when a list contains obvious
structure influences the order in which memories are taxonomic category structure (Bousfield, 1953; Puff,
retrieved; participants tend to successively recall items 1966), as well as when there is no systematic semantic
that were presented adjacent to one another in the study structure to the list (Howard & Kahana, 2002b; Romney
list (Kahana, 1996). Although much theoretical work has et al., 1993; Schwartz & Humphreys, 1973). Although
focused on understanding the effects of temporal structure empirical work has established the importance of semantic
on memory (e.g. Brown, Neath, & Chater, 2007; Howard & knowledge for shaping new episodic memories, there is lit-
Kahana, 2002a; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980), research has tle consensus about the structure of semantic knowledge
or the specific mechanisms that mediate its influence on
memory search (Cohen, 1963; Kimball, Smith, & Kahana,
Corresponding author at: Center for Learning and Memory, The 2007; Polyn, Norman, & Kahana, 2009; Sirotin, Kimball, &
University of Texas at Austin, 1 University Station Stop C7000, Austin, TX
Kahana, 2005). Furthermore, efforts to characterize
78712-0805, United States.
E-mail addresses: neal.morton@austin.utexas.edu (N.W. Morton), semantic organization are complicated by the simultaneous
sean.polyn@vanderbilt.edu (S.M. Polyn). influence of temporal organization on recall sequences

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.10.002
0749-596X/ 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
120 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

(Howard & Kahana, 2002b; Howard, Venkatadass, Norman, Mandler, 1972; Glanzer, 1969; for a review, see Puff,
& Kahana, 2007; Polyn, Erlikhman, & Kahana, 2011). Here, 1974). Morton et al. (2013) demonstrated a permutation-
we developed a set of computational models to test based technique that can be used to estimate the baseline
different ways that temporal and semantic information level of semantic organization expected in the presence of
might influence memory search during free recall. temporal organization. They measured free-recall behavior
on both mixed lists composed of items from different cat-
Measurement of semantic organization egories, and pure lists with items from a single category.
They randomly relabeled the set of pure list items with
In order to measure semantic organization, it is neces- the category labels from a mixed list and calculated a
sary to specify the semantic relatedness of the studied semantic organization score for each of these relabeled
items. Early examinations of semantic organization lists, to measure the tendency for same-category items to
focused on the effect of coarse semantic structure based be grouped together during recall. This randomization
on taxonomic category membership (e.g. Bousfield, 1953; was repeated many times to obtain a baseline distribution
Cohen, 1963; Roenker, Thompson, & Brown, 1971). More of semantic organization scores. Semantic organization
recently, theoretical and computational advances in char- scores calculated for the mixed lists could then be
acterizing semantic knowledge have made it possible to compared to this distribution. Although this technique
calculate more sophisticated measures of semantic simi- provides a useful estimate of the influence of temporal
larity, leading to development of a variety of models of organization on measures of semantic organization, it
semantic structure which allow one to assign a related- relies on the assumption that semantic and temporal infor-
ness/similarity score to any pair of words in a corpus or mation do not interact with one another, an assumption
word pool (Griffiths, Steyvers, & Tenenbaum, 2007; that is unlikely to be valid (Glanzer, 1969; Howard &
Jones & Mewhort, 2007; Lund & Burgess, 1996; Landauer Kahana, 2002b; Polyn et al., 2011).
& Dumais, 1997; Romney et al., 1993; Steyvers, Shiffrin,
& Nelson, 2004). Despite this profusion of semantic Simulating influences on recall organization
models, it is unclear which best corresponds to the
structure of semantic memory in humans. It is unclear whether it is possible to develop a simple
In the domain of list-learning, the structure of a per- measure of semantic or temporal organization that is pro-
sons semantic memory is thought to give rise to semantic cess pure, given that these forms of information interact
organization in their recall sequences. If all pairs of items with one another in the cognitive system. In order to
in a study list have been assigned semantic relatedness understand the nature of these interactions, researchers
scores, semantic organization can be quantified by examin- have developed computational models designed to
ing the similarity scores of neighboring pairs of items in characterize the joint influence of semantic and temporal
the recall sequence. These scores are then compared to a structure on behavior in memory tasks (e.g., Anderson,
baseline measure, representing the expected distribution 1972; Batchelder & Riefer, 1980; Kimball et al., 2007;
of similarity scores in the absence of semantic influence. Polyn et al., 2009; Romney et al., 1993; Sirotin et al.,
In many cases, this baseline measure has been modeled 2005; Socher et al., 2009). In order to properly account
in terms of the expectation of the organizational statistic for the influence of semantic information on behavior, each
given a random ordering of the recalled words (Bousfield, of these models must specify the semantic relatedness of
1953; Roenker et al., 1971; Stricker, Brown, Wixted, any pair of items that might be studied. These semantic
Baldo, & Delis, 2002). This assumption of random sampling relatedness values have been drawn from existing models
is problematic, as it fails to take temporal influences on of semantic knowledge, such as latent semantic analysis
recall into account. Romney et al. (1993) developed a (LSA; Landauer & Dumais, 1997) and word association
method that accounted for differences in memorability of spaces (WAS; Steyvers et al., 2004).
items from different serial positions (thus accounting for In the domain of free recall, computational models of
the influence of the primacy and recency effects on seman- memory are typically evaluated through a generative
tic organization), but this measure did not account for process: The model is used to generate a large number of
sequential dependencies due to temporal organization. synthetic recall sequences, and a number of summary
Temporal organization is a near-ubiquitous phe- statistics are calculated, such as the probability of recall
nomenon in free-recall tasks (Kahana, 1996; Kahana, by serial position, or a semantic organization score. These
Howard, & Polyn, 2008; Sederberg, Miller, Howard, & summary statistics are then compared to the same
Kahana, 2010) that can influence measures of semantic summary statistics calculated from the recall sequences
organization (Morton et al., 2013; Puff, 1966). Because of collected in the actual experiment. The fitness of the model
temporal organization, traditional measures of semantic is then quantified in terms of how well the models
organization which do not take the ordering of the input summary statistics match the observed summary statistics
list into account, such as ratio of repetition (Bousfield, (e.g. Brown et al., 2007; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980;
1953), adjusted ratio of clustering (Roenker et al., 1971), Sederberg, Howard, & Kahana, 2008). However, a difficulty
and list-based clustering (Stricker et al., 2002), will be arises when one wishes to assess the models predictions
inflated whenever semantically related items are pre- regarding semantic organization: The same semantic
sented in proximity. This is a particularly critical issue model can be used to create the semantic associative struc-
when examining how semantic organization is influenced tures in the model, and to calculate the degree of semantic
by manipulations of presentation order (e.g. Borges & organization in the recall sequences generated by the
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 121

model. This leads to a circularity that can complicate the specific theoretically important empirical phenomena
evaluation of the validity of the model (as examined by observed in the experiment.
Manning & Kahana, 2012; Polyn et al., 2009). The CMR model is one of a class of retrieved-context
models which propose that a feature-based representation
A predictive framework for evaluating models of recall of each studied item causes item-specific information to be
organization integrated into a gradually changing representation of
temporal context (Kahana et al., 2008; Polyn & Kahana,
We present a computational modeling framework 2008). When an item is recalled, the context associated
based on the context maintenance and retrieval (CMR) with it is reactivated, providing a good cue for items
model. CMR is well-suited to examine the nature of tempo- studied nearby in the list and resulting in temporal organi-
ral and semantic interactions in free recall, as it makes zation. We use our framework to examine how temporal
detailed predictions regarding behavior in this paradigm and semantic information interact during memory search.
(Healey & Kahana, 2014; Lohnas, Polyn, & Kahana, 2015; While each model variant we examined used temporal
Polyn et al., 2009), including higher-order effects of com- context as a cue, we examined the possibility that item
pound temporal cuing (Lohnas & Kahana, 2014). We used cues might also be involved in probing semantic
a recently developed variant of CMR that allows direct associations. The first type of cuing model we examined
calculation of the probability of entire recall sequences is the version of CMR described by Polyn et al. (2009).
(Kragel, Morton, & Polyn, 2015), allowing for the exact This model uses context-based semantic cuing: The
calculation of the likelihood of observing a set of item-specific information integrated into temporal context
free-recall data according to the model. Within our activates a set of pre-experimental semantic associations,
modeling framework, we constructed competing model such that the same contextual representation guides both
variants by combining one of three different models of temporal and semantic organization (Fig. 2a, right side).
semantic similarity with one of three different models of We contrasted this with a version of CMR in which
how temporal and semantic information interact. Along temporal and semantic organization are more indepen-
with a baseline model with no semantic structure, this dent. This second model variant uses item-based semantic
yields ten model variants, which are described in more cuing, in which the feature-based representation of
detail below. We examine the behavior of these model the retrieved item directly activates pre-experimental
variants in three free-recall experiments which vary on a semantic associations, resulting in semantic organization
number of methodological characteristics. during memory search (Fig. 2a, center). In the item-based
To contrast different model variants, we used a maxi- semantic cuing model, temporal organization is guided
mum likelihood statistic to determine how well a given by the temporal context representation, but semantic
model variant can predict the behavior of the participants organization is guided by the reactivated representation
in an experiment. For each model variant, we first opti- of the remembered item. The predictive power of these
mized a set of parameters to fit each participant in an two model variants were compared with that of a third,
experiment. These parameters determine the behavior in which both item-based and context-based semantic
and predictions of the model, allowing us to calculate the cuing mechanisms operate simultaneously. Each model
likelihood of each recall event, conditional on both the variant simply changes the locus of semantic influences.
structure of the study list and the specific sequence of For all three variants, temporal organization is guided by
recalls leading up to that event. The maximum likelihood the temporal context representation.
then provides an unbiased measure for evaluating compet- Each of the three cuing models is combined with each of
ing models of memory search. While evaluating models three distinct vector space models of semantic similarity:
based on maximum likelihood provides important benefits Latent Semantic Analysis (LSA), Word Association Spaces
such as high consistency and efficiency in parameter (WAS), and Global Vectors (GloVe). Each vector space
estimation (Myung, 2003), little work has used this tech- model constructs a representational vector for each word
nique with models of free recall (Farrell & Lewandowsky, in a corpus. The representational similarity of any two
2008; Socher et al., 2009). The dearth of likelihood-based vectors (calculated by the cosine operation) determines
fitting in models of free recall may stem from the historical the strength of semantic association between the two
emphasis on fitting certain summary statistics, such as the corresponding items.
serial position curve (e.g. Sederberg et al., 2008), as well as LSA is a well-established vector space model of seman-
the common use of simulation models for which exact tic similarity that is based on the co-occurrence statistics
likelihoods cannot easily be calculated (e.g. Davelaar, of words in a large text corpus (Landauer & Dumais,
Goshen-Gottstein, Ashkenazi, Haarmann, & Usher, 2005; 1997). The corpus is partitioned into distinct documents,
Farrell, 2012; Polyn et al., 2009; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, and each word is assigned a representational vector speci-
1980; Sederberg et al., 2008). In addition to comparing fying the set of documents in which it occurs. The dimen-
models based on maximum likelihood, we also examined sionality of this vector is reduced using singular value
summary statistics that focus on specific aspects of recall decomposition (SVD), which helps the model infer indirect
behavior. Using the best-fitting parameters for each relationships between words. If two words appear along-
participant, we used the model to generate recall side similar sets of words across many documents, they
sequences. We then calculated the same set of summary are assigned similar representational vectors. LSA has been
statistics for the observed data and model-generated data shown to account for some aspects of semantic organiza-
to determine whether model variants can account for tion in free recall (Polyn et al., 2009; Sirotin et al., 2005).
122 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

WAS is another well-established vector space model association spaces (WAS) model (Steyvers et al., 2004).
based on data from a large set of free-association norms These words were identified as nouns using the CELEX2
(Steyvers et al., 2004). Representational vectors specify English database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Gulikers, 1995),
which words were associated with one another in the orig- and were identified (by three raters) as being appropriate
inal free-association study (Nelson, McEvoy, & Schreiber, for the binary classification tasks used in the free-recall
2004), and like LSA, SVD is used to reduce the dimension- experiment (size and animacy judgments, described
ality of those vectors. Prior work suggests that WAS can below). Words were excluded if they were abstract or were
predict category clustering (Sirotin et al., 2005) and highly ambiguous for either of the judgment tasks. Three
intrusions (Steyvers et al., 2004) more accurately than additional raters performed the size and animacy
LSA, but comparing WAS and LSA using standard judgments on the set of 1655 nouns; these ratings were
behavioral measures is difficult given differences in the used to balance the lists with regard to the classification
distributions of similarity values in the two models responses, as described below. During the course of the
(Howard et al., 2007; Manning & Kahana, 2012). study, an additional 17 words were excluded because they
GloVe is a recently developed vector space model that, sounded similar to other words in the pool. This final set of
like LSA, is based on co-occurrence statistics in a text 1638 words was the same as those used in Expt. 3,
corpus, but which also contains characteristics of described below.
prediction-based semantic models (Pennington, Socher, & Each participant performed 4 experimental sessions
Manning, 2014). GloVe has been shown to outperform (held on separate days), each of which contained 12 trials.
LSA (and a number of other semantic models) on several Each trial consisted of a study period, followed by a
validation tests, including word similarity, named entity free-recall period. There were two types of trials: control
recognition, and word analogies (Pennington et al., 2014). trials and task-shift trials. On control trials, every word in
Each of the model variants (combining each cuing the list was studied with the same encoding task. On
model with each semantic similarity model) is assessed task-shift trials, half of the items were studied with each
using the complementary measures of fit to a set of encoding task. Within each session, each participant
summary statistics and overall maximum likelihood. The performed 6 control trials, for a total of 24 trials across
summary statistics show whether a given model variant the four sessions. Here, we focus on these control trials,
produces the relevant empirical phenomena observed in and all analyses are carried out without regard to
the experiments. However, the summary statistics that encoding task.
measure semantic organization are often calculated in During the study period, a series of 24 words was
terms of the same vector space models used to define presented, one word at a time. Each word remained on
semantic structure in the cognitive model. The likelihood the screen for 3 s, and was followed by a blank 0.81.2 s
statistic avoids this circularity by quantifying model inter-stimulus interval. Each word was presented with a
performance in terms of the models ability to predict the task cue above it, indicating the judgment that the partic-
specific sequence of recalls made on every trial. ipant should make for that word (either judging whether
the item would fit in a shoebox, or whether the item was
Methods living or nonliving). Participants indicated their judgment
for each word by pressing a key.
We tested competing models of temporal and semantic After the final item was presented, a row of asterisks
organization based on their ability to predict recall and a beep indicated the start of the recall period.
behavior in three free-recall experiments. These experi- Participants were given 90 s to vocally recall as many
ments differed in a number of characteristics, including words as they could remember from the most recent list,
stimulus pool, presentation time, encoding task, and delay in whatever order they came to mind.
before recall, allowing us to assess the generality of our The binary judgments from the three raters (described
conclusions across a range of experimental procedures. above) were averaged together to assign each word an
average response for each encoding task. These average
Experiment 1 responses were used to ensure that the study lists were
well balanced in terms of the judgments, making sure that
Participants no list was dominated by a particular class of response.
Participants included 41 people (14 female) between Items judged big or living were assigned a value of 1, and
the ages of 18 and 30. Participants were recruited as part small or nonliving items were assigned a value of 0. As such,
of a series of studies designed to examine electrophysio- a word that was judged big by two of the three raters was
logical correlates of encoding and retrieval in free recall. assigned a value of 0.66 for the size judgment; if all three
We focus on the first study of the series, which included raters judged the word to be nonliving, it would be assigned
4 sessions for each participant. Analyses on the data from a value of 0 for the animacy judgment. The words on a given
these participants appear in Lohnas, Polyn, and Kahana list were chosen such that the average value of the words
(2011), Lohnas and Kahana (2014), and Lohnas et al. judged with a given task fell between 0.3 and 0.7.
(2015). LSA similarity values were not available for two
words that were used in Experiment 1. Therefore, we
Stimuli and procedure excluded from all analyses 27 lists that included
A pool of 1655 nouns were selected from a larger pool either of those words, leaving 957 trials considered
of 5018 words that formed the corpus for the word here.
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 123

Experiment 2 many words as they could remember from the most recent
list, in whatever order they came to mind.
Participants LSA similarity values were not available for two words
Participants included 48 people between the ages of 18 that were used in Experiment 3. Therefore, we excluded
and 30. Scalp EEG was recorded in a subset of these partic- from all analyses 47 lists that included either of those
ipants; results from those participants were previously words, leaving 2725 trials considered here.
reported by Sederberg et al. (2006).

Models of semantic associations


Stimuli and procedure
The experimental procedure was described in detail
The word association spaces (WAS) algorithm (Steyvers
by Sederberg et al. (2006). Stimuli consisted of 308 com-
et al., 2004) provides similarity scores for all word pairs in
mon nouns (Friendly, Franklin, Hoffman, & Rubin, 1982).
a corpus of 5018 words, a subset of which were used to
Participants studied and recalled 48 lists which each
create the study lists in Experiments 1 and 3. These
contained 15 words drawn from the stimulus pool. Words
similarity scores are derived from the University of South
did not appear more than once in a given list, but appeared
Florida free-association norms (Nelson et al., 2004). We
in 13 lists for a given participant. Each word appeared for
used the 400-dimension singular value decomposition of
1.6 s, followed by an inter-stimulus interval of 0.81.2 s.
2
Participants were instructed to visualize each word as it the Sij measure described by Steyvers et al. (2004), which
was presented. Immediately following each list presenta- is freely available online.1 We defined the WAS similarity
tion, participants performed an arithmetic distraction task between two words as the cosine of the angle between their
for 20 s. After the distraction period, participants were corresponding vectors.
given 45 s to vocally recall items from the previous list in The latent semantic analysis (LSA) algorithm (Landauer
any order they wished. & Dumais, 1997) was used to derive similarity scores for all
WAS similarity values were not available for 11 words word pairs in the Touchstone Applied Science Associates,
that were used in Experiment 2. Therefore, we excluded Inc. (TASA) corpus. This technique produces a 400
from all analyses 992 trials that included any of these dimensional vector for each word. We defined the LSA
words, leaving 1312 trials considered here. similarity between two words as the cosine of the angle
between their corresponding vectors.
We used publicly available 300 dimensional GloVe vec-
Experiment 3
tors2 that were trained on a combination of the Gigaword 5
corpus (Parker, Graff, Kong, Chen, & Maeda, 2011) and a
Participants
dump of Wikipedia article text from 2014. The corpus was
Participants included 126 people between the ages of
tokenized and converted to lowercase, and a vocabulary
17 and 30, from the Penn Electrophysiology of Encoding
was created with the 400,000 most frequent words.
and Retrieval Study (PEERS). Scalp EEG was recorded in
Co-occurrence was based on a decreasing weighting
these participants, and results from these participants
function, where words that are d words apart contribute
were previously reported by Healey and Kahana (2014).
1=d to the co-occurrence count. As with WAS and LSA, we
calculated similarity between each pair of words based on
Stimuli and procedure the cosine similarity of their vectors.
The experimental procedure was described in detail by Fig. 1 provides a visualization of the semantic similarity
Healey and Kahana (2014); we describe the relevant values for the different semantic models that we consid-
details here. Stimuli were the 1638 nouns described above ered.3 The circle of words represents a sample study list
(Expt. 1). Participants studied and recalled 112 lists which from Experiment 1. The weight of the line connecting two
each contained 16 words drawn from the stimulus pool. words indicates how strongly associated the two words
Different lists had different encoding task conditions; here, are. These schematic figures highlight a difference between
we focus on the 28 lists for each subject that were studied the co-occurrence based models (LSA and GloVe) and
with no explicit encoding task. Word association spaces WAS: While WAS has relatively sparse connectivity, LSA
similarity values (Steyvers et al., 2004) were used to group and GloVe have many connections of moderate strength
words into four similarity bins (high similarity: (see also Manning & Kahana, 2012). We examined the
cosh  0:7; mediumhigh similarity: 0:4  cosh < 0:7; degree to which the different semantic models captured
mediumlow similarity: 0:14  cosh < 0:4; low similar- similar relations between items by calculating rank
ity: cosh < 0:14). In each list, two pairs of items from correlations between the similarity values from each model.
each of the groups were arranged such that one pair For the 1655 words included in Experiments 1 and 3, each
occurred at adjacent serial positions and the other pair pair of models demonstrated a significant but small
was separated by at least two other items. Each word Spearmans correlation (LSA-GloVe: q 0:366; p < 0:0001;
appeared for 3 s, followed by an inter-stimulus interval of GloVe-WAS: q 0:232; p < 0:0001; LSA-WAS: q 0:199,
0.81.2 s.
After the final item was presented in each trial, there 1
http://psiexp.ss.uci.edu/research/software.htm.
was a 1.21.4 s delay, followed by the presentation of a 2
http://nlp.stanford.edu/projects/glove/.
row of asterisks and a beep indicating the start of the recall 3
Visualization created using code modified from the Schemaball package:
period. Participants were given 75 s to vocally recall as http://www.mathworks.co.uk/matlabcentral/fileexchange/42279-schemaball.
124 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

Fig. 1. Cosine similarity between pairs of items in a sample study list from Experiment 1, for different models of semantic similarity. Greater line saturation
and thickness indicate greater estimated similarity. Similarity values for different models were scaled to be on the same range. (a) Similarity based on latent
semantic analysis (LSA). (b) Similarity based on the global vectors (GloVe) model. (c) Similarity based on word association spaces (WAS).

p < 0:0001), demonstrating that the interitem similarities 2002a). CMR added, among other things, a mechanism to
predicted by the different models were largely distinct. explain how semantic associations influence recall.
Under this framework, the model is initialized with
Model of memory search pre-experimental associations representing a persons
prior experience with an item. When an item is studied
We used a modified version of the context maintenance or recalled, these associations cause the system to
and retrieval model (CMR) as a framework to evaluate the retrieve the items pre-experimental context. This
impact of different models of semantic associations and pre-experimental context is associated with the items
different semantic cuing mechanisms on behavior in free semantic associates. As such, when this pre-experimental
recall. CMR consists of two interacting representations: a context is used as part of a retrieval cue, the items
context layer and a feature layer. Two associative matrices semantic associates are likely to be retrieved next, giving
(feature-to-context, and context-to-feature) allow these rise to semantic organization. We refer to this mechanism
representations to influence one another. When an item as context-based semantic cuing (Fig. 2); when this
is studied, a representation of it becomes active on the fea- mechanism is in operation, the context representation is
ture layer. This representation is projected through the responsible for both temporal and semantic organization.
feature-to-context associative connections, which causes Context-based semantic cuing can be contrasted
contextual information associated with the item to be with an alternative mechanism, which we refer to as
retrieved and integrated into the context representation. item-based semantic cuing. Using this mechanism, semantic
This contextual integration mechanism causes the contex- associations link item representations directly to one
tual representation to change slowly over time. Thus, at another (without using the context representation as a
any moment, context reflects a recency-weighted average mediator). During free recall, when an item is recalled, its
of information related to recently presented stimuli. reactivated representation serves as a direct cue for
Studied items become associated to the context that was semantically related items. The item-based semantic cuing
active when they were presented, so that context can serve mechanism has been used as part of several versions of the
as a cue to retrieve items, and recalled items can retrieve search of associative memory (SAM) model (Kahana, 2012;
the context that is associated with them. When an item Kimball et al., 2007; Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980; Sirotin
is recalled, its feature representation is reactivated, which et al., 2005). In the item-based semantic cuing models
allows the system to reinstate the context representation we examine here, although item representations are used
associated with the item. This reinstated context can then to probe semantic associations, the context representation
be used to cue for another item on the list. Items that are still projects through episodic associations as in other
associated with similar states of context (such as adjacent versions of CMR.
items in a list) tend to be good cues for one another. See With item-based semantic cuing, semantic organization
Formal description of the model for further details about is only influenced by the just-recalled item, as depicted in
model mechanisms. The mechanisms of item-context asso- Fig. 2b. In contrast, with context-based semantic cuing, any
ciation, contextual cuing, and context reinstatement allow items whose pre-experimental context is part of the con-
the model to account for a number of behavioral effects in text representation will influence semantic organization,
free recall, including recency and temporal contiguity because the context retrieved with each recalled item only
effects (Howard, 2004; Howard & Kahana, 2002a; partially updates the context representation (Lohnas &
Howard, Fotedar, Datey, & Hasselmo, 2005; Polyn et al., Kahana, 2014). Thus, semantic organization will be influ-
2009; Sederberg et al., 2008). enced by the set of items recalled prior to the current
Polyn et al. (2009) introduced CMR, which is based on recalled item, though the semantic identity of the most
the temporal context model (TCM; Howard & Kahana, recently retrieved item will have the most influence. In
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 125

Fig. 2. Illustration of cuing mechanisms used by the different model variants. (a) Schematic of model variants. Left: Model with no semantic associations.
Recall is driven solely by episodic associations between items and context. Center: Temporal context influences recall through episodic cuing as before, but
retrieved items also will cue for other semantically related items, providing additional support for those items. Right: Only context is used as a cue to
retrieve items; context projects through both episodic and semantic associations. (b) Schematic of predictions for item-based semantic cuing and context-
based semantic cuing, after learning a sample list and recalling the sequence tree, cat, queen. In the item-based semantic cuing model, only the last
recalled item, queen, is used as a semantic cue, resulting in stronger support for the related item king. In the context-based semantic cuing model, the
entire current state of context is used as a semantic cue. Since cat is still somewhat active in context, it provides additional support for the related item
dog.

addition to examining the item-based semantic cuing and we evaluated a base model with no semantics, and every
context-based semantic cuing models, we also evaluated combination of semantic association model and cuing
whether semantic cuing might involve a weighted combi- mechanism. We first compared these models based on
nation of item and context information. Note that while their ability to predict the sequences of individual recalls
these different model variants used different types of that were observed in the experiment.
semantic cuing, each of them used context-based episodic
cuing (Fig. 2), allowing each variant to account for the tem- Likelihood calculation
poral organization observed in free recall (Kahana, 1996).
The version of CMR described by Polyn et al. (2009) During each recall period, the participant produces a
used context-based semantic cuing. Under this mecha- sequence of responses. This recall sequence is described
nism, semantic organization after recall of a given item as a series of recall events, followed by a recall termination
should be sensitive to the items that were recalled prior event. For simplicity, we excluded repeated items and
to that item. Polyn et al. (2009) showed that this version intrusions from the set of recall events, so that the
of CMR can produce a reasonable overall amount of remaining recall events corresponded to correct recalls.
semantic organization while simultaneously accounting We discuss the impact of excluding repeats and intrusions
for temporal and source organization. However, that study below in Exclusion of recall errors. For each recall event,
focused on semantic organization conditional only on the the model is used to calculate each individual items
just-recalled item; the more nuanced predictions of the probability of being recalled from the list, as well as the
model have not been evaluated. probability of recall termination (Fig. 3). From this set of
Here, we use the CMR framework to assess the relative probabilities, we record the probability of whatever recall
validity of the LSA, GloVe, and WAS models of semantic event actually took place (for example, recalling item 24
association, and to contrast the item-based, context- in the list), and take the logarithm of this probability
based, and item + context semantic cuing mechanisms (to avoid precision issues caused by very low probabili-
described above. To accomplish this, each of the cuing ties). Thus, if the participant recalled item 24, the model
mechanisms was paired with each model of semantic asso- simulates recall of item 24, which involves reactivation of
ciation. For each of the three experiments reported here, the item representation and updating of the context
126 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

Fig. 3. (a) Schematic of recall prediction for one list, in our modeling framework. First, the study period is simulated and the model learns the list. Then
retrieval is simulated, and the model calculates the probability of recalling each of the items, as well as the probability of stopping recall. We then record the
predicted probability of the observed behavior. If an item was recalled, we update the state of the model conditional on that recall. This process is repeated
until the entire observed recall sequence has been simulated. (b) Schematic example of one list with five words. At each step of the recall process, the model
makes predictions conditional on the observed behavior up to that point.

representation. The updated model is then used to predict First, mutated vectors for generation g; v i;g , were cre-
the next event in the recall sequence (either another ated from the current population vectors xi;g according to
successful recall, or termination), and the logarithm of this
probability is recorded. This process is repeated until we
v j;i;g xj;best;g gj;i;g Fxj;r1;g  xj;r2;g ; 1
reach the end of the recall sequence being examined. At where v j;i;g is an element j of the mutated vector i for
this point the model is re-initialized and applied to the generation g; xbest;g is a vector randomly sampled with
next list of the experiment. The log-transformed probabil- replacement from the top 5% of points, F is a scaling factor
ities of all recall and termination events in the experiment that we set to 0.85, gj;i;g is uniformly distributed random
are summed to obtain the log-likelihood of the entire data- jitter between 0 and 0.001, and xr1;g and xr2;g are vectors
set, given a specific model and a specific set of parameters. randomly sampled from the original population. To
enhance diversity, candidate vectors ui;g were created
Model comparison using a crossover step, where each element of each
candidate vector, uj;i;g , was set according to
For each model variant, we used a parameter optimiza- 
tion technique known as differential evolution to find the v j;i;g ; if rand0; 1 6 Cr
uj;i;g 2
parameter set that maximized the likelihood of the xj;i;g ; otherwise;
observed data (Storn, 2008). We optimized the parameters
where the crossover probability Cr was set to 0.9.
separately for each individual participant. We used a
To prevent premature convergence, candidate vectors
MATLAB-based implementation of differential evolution
were sometimes selected even when they had a lower
based on code developed by Price, Storn, and Lampinen
likelihood. The candidate vector was accepted with
(2005). We used a variant of the DE/best/1/bin method
probability n, defined as
described by Storn (2008), with some modifications to
make search more robust. For each search through param-  
Lui;g
eter space, we began with 1000 parameter sets at ran- n min 1; ; 3
Lxi;g
domly chosen points in the parameter space. For each
iteration of the search, the likelihood of the data given where Lui;g and Lxi;g are the likelihoods of the candidate
the current parameters was evaluated at each point. Then and original vectors, respectively.
candidate vectors were evaluated to determine the Iterations of the algorithm were run until the maximum
composition of the next generation of the individuals. log likelihood over all parameter sets examined so far had
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 127

not changed more than 0.0001 over the last 100 genera- the first recall, transitions between recalled items exhibit
tions. Each search was repeated 15 times for each subject two major forms of organization: temporal clustering and
with different random starting points, and the parameter semantic clustering.
set with the greatest log likelihood over all the searches Temporal clustering is the tendency of participants to
was selected. If, for a given subject, the searches failed to successively recall items that were presented adjacent
find a greater or equal likelihood for a more complex to one another in the list (Kahana, 1996). We used a
model compared to a simpler nested model (e.g. a lag-based conditional response probability (lag-CRP;
semantic model compared to the base model), the best Kahana, 1996) analysis to characterize temporal cluster-
parameter set for the complex model was set to the ing (where lag indicates the difference between the
best-fitting parameter set for the simpler model. position of two items in the study list). The lag-CRP
Model performance was quantified using the Akaike analysis provides the probability of making recall transi-
Information Criterion (AIC; Wagenmakers & Farrell, 2004) tions of a particular lag, conditional on that lag being
and the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC; Schwarz, available for recall (an item was considered unavailable
1978). For each model, we calculated AIC with a correction if there was no item presented at that serial position, or
for finite samples: if that item had already been recalled previously). The
first three output positions were excluded from this
2VV 1
AICc 2 log L 2V ; 4 analysis. In this analysis and the other transition-based
n  V  1 analyses described below, when analyzing the observed
where L is the maximum likelihood value for the candidate data, transitions to or from intrusions or repeats of
model, V is the number of free parameters, and n is the already-recalled words were excluded.
number of estimated data points. We measured semantic clustering using a related mea-
We also calculated BIC according to: sure, the semantic-CRP (Howard & Kahana, 2002b;
Sederberg et al., 2010). Rather than partitioning recall
BIC 2 log L V log n 5 transitions on the basis of lag, this analysis partitions
We compared model performance using AIC weights, transitions on the basis of the semantic identities of the
which indicate the probability that each model (of K items themselves. First, we tallied the number of times
competing models) generated the observed data, under each participant made a transition from item i to item j,
the assumption that one of the models generated the data. for each item in the stimulus pool. We also tallied a sepa-
The AIC weight for a given model i; wi AIC, is defined as: rate count of the number of times that each participant
  could have made each possible transition between words,
exp  12 Di AIC given the words that were still available at each point in
wi AIC PK  1 ; 6
k1 exp  2 Dk AIC
recall. A given transition between items i and j was not
counted as possible if item i was never recalled. We then
where Di AIC is the difference in AICc between a given determined a set of semantic similarity bins that we used
candidate model and the best-fitting model in the set. to group together inter-item transitions (details on how
BIC weights were calculated in the same manner, the bins were determined are specified below). Within
substituting BIC for AIC (Wagenmakers & Farrell, 2004). each bin, we calculated the number of actual transitions
in that bin, and divided by the number of possible transitions.
Analysis of recall behavior In a set of preliminary analyses, we contrasted a ver-
sion of the semantic-CRP analysis described by Howard
We used a set of summary statistics to characterize the and Kahana (2002b) with a slightly different version
recall performance of the participants and to further described by Sederberg et al. (2010). We found that the
characterize the performance of each optimized model semantic-CRPs for the Base model, which had no semantic
variant. In order to calculate these summary statistics on associations and therefore could not produce semantic
an optimized model variant, we first used the model to organization, showed an increased probability of very
generate simulated recall sequences, as follows. For each low- or high-similarity transitions when the semantic-
recall attempt, we calculated the probabilities of each CRPs were calculated as described by Howard and
recall event (recalling an item or stopping recall) using Kahana (2002b). This led us to implement a version of
the same procedure described in Likelihood calculation. the analysis more similar to that described by Sederberg
We then sampled an event at random using this probabil- et al. (2010), which did not demonstrate this distortion.
ity distribution and updated the state of the model accord- Prior implementations of the semantic-CRP analysis
ingly. Each recall period was simulated in this manner have generally used bins that contain deciles (Healey &
until a stop event was chosen. To calculate summary statis- Kahana, 2014) or percentiles (Howard & Kahana, 2002b;
tics for each model, we simulated each list in the experi- Howard et al., 2007). However, because semantic similarity
ment 100 times and calculated each statistic averaged values based on WAS and LSA are highly positively skewed
over the 100 simulated replications of the experiment. (Manning & Kahana, 2012), this results in many bins at low
Behavior in free recall can be described in terms of three similarity values, and very few bins at higher similarity
stages: initiation, transitions, and termination (Kahana, values. To better estimate CRPs for the full range of similar-
2012). We measured recall initiation by calculating the ity values, we took a different strategy of determining bin
probability of first recalling an item as a function of the sizes so that we obtain a minimal sample size at each bin
serial position in which it was presented in the list. After (see Sederberg et al., 2010) for another example of unequal
128 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

bin sizes used for this analysis). First, we obtained the We calculated the probability of recall termination as a
semantic similarities for each inter-item transition that function of output position. We excluded repeats and
was possible at least once over all recall sequences in the intrusions when calculating output position so that the
study, based on the semantic similarity measure of interest probability of stopping at output position list length 1 is
(LSA, GloVe, or WAS). Starting from the highest similarity unity. Finally, we calculated the serial position curve,
value, we decreased the lower limit of the bin by which shows the probability of recalling each item as a
increments of 0.05 until there were at least 10 possible function of its serial position in the list.
transitions per subject on average. After defining a bin, For each measure of recall behavior, we calculated con-
the lower limit of that bin became the upper limit of the fidence intervals using a bootstrap procedure. For each of
next bin, and the process was repeated. The center of each 5000 samples, we sampled subject means with replace-
bin was defined as the mean similarity value over all ment and calculated a simulated group mean. We set the
possible transitions within that bin. We determined the confidence interval to include the middle 95% of the simu-
bins from the actual data, then applied these bins to the lated group means.
simulated data from our model variants.
In order to examine the specific predictions of the
context-based semantic cuing mechanism, we developed Formal description of the CMR model
a novel measure to determine whether the context in
which a word appears in the recall sequence predicts Here, we give a formal description of the equations that
subsequent semantic organization. We used the semantic define CMRs structure and behavior. Table 1 provides an
score metric introduced by Polyn et al. (2009) to character- overview of the parameters that control the behavior of
ize the percentile of semantic relatedness of each the model.
transition during recall. For each transition between CMR takes the form of a simplified neural network with
recalled words, first the items that are still available for two interacting representations, a feature-based represen-
recall (i.e. that have not been recalled previously) are tation of the studied item (the item layer, F) and a contex-
determined. These available words are ranked on their tual representation (the context layer, C). The two layers
semantic similarity to the just-recalled item. The per- communicate with one another through two sets of asso-
centile of the transition the participant actually made is ciative connections represented by matrices MFC and MCF .
noted, and this percentile is averaged over all transitions Each of these weight matrices contains both pre-
to obtain a semantic score that reflects the overall amount experimental associations and new associations learned
of semantic organization. We calculated semantic score by during the experiment. Pre-experimental weights are
ranking available items based on similarity to items of designated MFC CF
pre and Mpre ; the experimental weights are
recall lag n, where n is the number of output positions MFC CF
exp and Mexp .
separating the previously recalled item at output position
In the present simulations, we are particularly inter-
i  n from the next item i in the recall sequence. When ested in the structure of the pre-experimental weights.
n 1, the two recall events are adjacent; this corresponds
to a standard semantic score as described by Polyn et al.
(2009). For example, say a participant studied the list dog Table 1
king cat tree queen, then recalled tree, cat, queen, List of model parameters, with a brief description of each.
dog. After the participant recalled queen, there were Parameter Parameter Description
two possible words that could have been recalled next: type
dog and king. For recall lag 1, these items would be ranked Context benc Rate of context drift during encoding
based on their similarity to the just-recalled item queen, so updating bdelay Rate of context drift during end-of-list
that king would be ranked highest and the semantic score distraction
bstart Amount of start-list context retrieved
for that transition would be 0 (since the participant next
at start of recall
recalled dog instead). In contrast, for recall lag 2, the brec Rate of context drift during recall
remaining items would be ranked based on their similarity
Associative a Initial strength of context-to-item
to cat, so that dog would be ranked highest and the structure connections
semantic score for that transition would be 1. d Initial strength of the diagonal of MCF
For each participant, we calculated the semantic score s Scaling of semantic association
for each recall lag from 1 to 4, averaging over all valid tran- strengths
c Amount of experimental context
sitions. For a given recall lag, a transition was excluded
retrieved by a recalled item
from the analysis if either item i or item i  n was a repeat /s Scaling of primacy gradient in learning
or an intrusion. The first three recalled items were rate on MCF
excluded from the analysis so that the same output /d Rate of decay of primacy gradient
positions would be included for recall lags 14. Semantic Recall s Sensitivity parameter of the Luce
score is expected to be 0.5 by chance, indicating recall dynamics choice rule
hs Scaling of the stop probability over
without regard to semantic similarity. If semantic score is
output position
greater than 0.5 for n > 0, we take this as evidence that hr Rate of increase in stop probability
semantic cuing is influenced by prior items in the recall over output position
sequences, consistent with context-based semantic cuing.
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 129

For all model variants, we set the pre-experimental item- where b is set to benc , a free parameter of the model, and qi
to-context associations according to is set so that the length of ci is 1, according to
 q
1  c; if i j 2
MFC
prei;j 7 qi 1 b2 ci1  cIN i  1  bci1  ci :
IN
12
0; if i j:
This connects each unit on F to the corresponding unit After context is updated, the current item f i and the
current state of context ci become associated through
on C. The c parameter controls the strength of these
pre-experimental associations relative to the experimental simple Hebbian learning. After each item presentation,
the experimental associations are updated according to
associations described below.
For the base model, which does not contain any 0
DMFC
exp cci f i : 13
semantic associations, we set the pre-experimental
context-to-item associations according to When an item is presented, the network also learns
 associations from the current state of context to the
d; if i j
MCF
prei;j 8 current item, according to
a; if i j:
0
Here, the a parameter allows all the items to support
DMCF
exp /i f i ci ; 14
one another in the recall competition in a uniform manner.
where /i scales the amount of learning, simulating the
Our d parameter is similar to the cCF parameter described increased attention to initial items in a list that has been
by Sederberg et al. (2008). Our implementation is different proposed to explain the primacy effect (Sederberg et al.,
from theirs in that a is free to be non-zero, and some model 2008). /i depends on the serial position i of the studied
variants also include the addition of semantic similarity item:
strengths. In a set of preliminary simulations, we tested a
form of the model where MCF pre was set to 0. Through a
/i /s e/d i1 1: 15
series of model comparison analyses (not reported here), The free parameters /s and /d control the magnitude
we found that freeing both the d and a parameters and decay of this learning-rate gradient, respectively.
substantially improved the fit, based on AIC. To simulate the end-of-list distraction in Experiment 2,
For the set of model variants which used context-based we assumed that distraction during the retention interval
semantic cuing, the context-to-item associations were set causes a change in context (Sederberg et al., 2008). Context
according to is updated according to Eq. (12), where b is set to bRI , and
(
d; if i j cIN
i is a vector that is orthogonal to the pre-experimental
MCF 9 contexts of the studied items.
prei;j
a sMsem
i;j ; if i j;
Before initiating recall, we assume that some amount of
where Msemi;j gives the semantic similarity between items i the pre-list context is reinstated. We assume that context
and j according to WAS, GloVe, or LSA, and s is a scaling is updated according to
parameter (cf. Polyn et al., 2009). In other words, we used cstart qN1 cN bstart c0 ; 16
a linear transform to map semantic cosine similarity values
based on WAS, GloVe, or LSA to semantic strengths in the where cstart is the state of context at the start of free recall,
model, where a serves as an intercept parameter and s is N is the number of items in the list, c0 is the state of
a slope parameter. The diagonal of Msem is set to 0, so that context at the start of the list before any items have been
self-strengths are solely determined by the d parameter. presented, and qN1 is calculated according to Eq. (12). This
At the start of the list, context is initialized with a state mechanism is consistent with evidence that participants
that is orthogonal to the pre-experimental context associ- sometimes recall the start of the list and use that event
ated with the set of items. Similarly, item representations as a cue (Laming, 1999). In preliminary simulations we
are assumed to be orthonormal to each other; each unit found that models including this start-list context rein-
of F corresponds to one item. When an item i is presented statement demonstrated a better fit to the primacy effect
during the study period, its representation on F, f i , is than models containing the learning-rate gradient alone
activated. Pre-experimental context cIN i is retrieved and is
(see also Kragel et al., 2015).
input to the context layer to update the current state of At each recall attempt, the current state of context is
context. The input to context is used as a retrieval cue to attempt retrieval of a studied
item. First, the activation of each item a is determined
FC FC
i M f i Mpre f i ;
cIN 10 according to

since MFC a MCF c: 17


exp is assumed to be zero at the start of the list. The
retrieved pre-experimental context cIN
is then normalized
i In order to avoid the possibility of the model assigning a
to have length 1. probability of 0 to any possible recall, we set a minimal
After retrieval of pre-experimental context cIN
i , the cur- activation for each item of 107 .
rent state of context is updated according to At each recall attempt, we calculated the probability of
stopping recall (in which case no item was recalled, and
ci qi ci1 bcIN
i ; 11 search terminated). Probability of stopping recall varies
130 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

as a function of output position j (where j 0 for the first regardless of the value of k, used a context cue to probe
attempt), according to the episodic associations stored in MCF .

Pstop; j hs ejhr ; 18
Results
where hs and hr are free parameters that determine the
scaling and rate of increase, respectively, of the exponen- The modeling framework used here is designed to
tial function. The stopping mechanism does not interact account for the simultaneous influence of temporal and
with any model mechanism, and is simply intended to semantic information on memory search in three
capture the average probability of stopping as a function free-recall experiments which differed on a number of
of output position. methodological characteristics. We consider three models
The probability Pi of recalling a given item i is defined of semantic relatedness (LSA, GloVE, and WAS) which pro-
conditional on recall not stopping at that position, and it vide similarity scores specifying the semantic associations
varies with activation strength, according to between the studied words. We also consider three models
as of semantic cuing (item-based semantic cuing [I], context-
Pi 1  Pstop PNi ; 19 based semantic cuing [C], and hybrid semantic cuing [IC])
s
k ak
which specify how this semantic information is used
where s is a sensitivity parameter that determines the during memory search. The hybrid semantic cuing model
contrast between well-supported and poorly supported includes both forms of semantic cuing; a mixing parameter
items. High values of s will cause a greater influence of k determines the relative strength of each cuing mecha-
differences in support, while low values will cause nism. For each experiment, we construct a baseline model
relatively uniform probabilities of recalling each item. without semantic structure and 9 models with semantic
If an item is recalled, then that item is reactivated on F. structure (crossing the three models of semantic related-
The reactivated item is then used to retrieve both experi- ness with the three models of semantic cuing). Note that,
mental and pre-experimental context, according to while these models varied in the specifics of semantic
organization, each of them used the same contextual cuing
FC
i M fi:
cIN 20 mechanism to guide temporal organization. We compare
Context is then updated using Eq. (11), and is used to the set of 10 models in terms of their overall fit to the recall
cue for another recall attempt. The process continues until sequences (i.e., the likelihood statistic; what is the proba-
the model reaches the end of the recall sequence. bility that the observed data was generated by this
model?). Each model is also used to generate recall
sequences, which allows us to compare the models in
Item-based semantic cuing
terms of their fit to a number of important summary
We also examined an item-based semantic cuing model
statistics which characterize recall performance, temporal
that used separate context and item cues for episodic and
organization, and semantic organization.
semantic associations. In this model, contextual cuing
worked as before, but semantic associations were not
included in MCF . Recall initiation was driven by projecting Serial position effects and temporal organization
CF
context through episodic associations on M . For each
Table 2 reports the overall fitness of each of the 10
following recall attempt, the feature-layer vector corre-
model variants in each experiment, in terms of AIC
sponding to the last recalled item, f i , was projected
weights. AIC weights indicate, for a given set of competing
through the scaled semantic similarity matrix (the diago-
models, the probability of each model generating the
nal, representing item self-strengths, was set to 0). The
observed data, under the assumption that one of them
item activations corresponding to contextual cuing and
did. It should be noted that while the base model had the
item cuing were added to obtain the total item activation:
lowest AIC weight for both experiments (i.e., it had the
a sMsem f i MCF c 21 worst fit to the recall sequences), it still provided an excel-
lent fit to a number of important summary statistics,
The activation values a were then used with Eq. (19) to including the recency, primacy, and contiguity effects.
determine recall probabilities. The generative version of the Base model provided a good
We also examined a model that combined context- and qualitative fit of recall as a function of serial position
item-based semantic cuing. This was the same as the (Fig. 4a, e, and i), including the widely varying magnitudes
item-based semantic cuing model, but rather than cuing of the recency and primacy effects in the different experi-
semantics using just the item vector, we used a weighted ments. Primacy was slightly under-predicted in Experi-
combination of context and item: ments 1 and 3, which was an issue with each model
a sMsem kf i 1  kc MCF c; 22 variant examined in this study. Given that retrieved-
context models have successfully accounted for the magni-
where k is a parameter controlling the relative weighting of tude of primacy in prior work (e.g. Polyn et al., 2009), it
the item cue compared to the context cue. Note that this appears that this under-prediction of primacy is caused
model is equivalent to the item-based semantic cuing by our different emphasis on fitting entire recall sequences
model when k 1 and to the context-based semantic cuing rather than focusing on traditional summary statistics
model when k 0. Note also that each model variant, such as the serial position curve (as in prior work with
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 131

Table 2
AIC weights for models with semantic similarity. Models with wAIC > 0:1 are displayed in bold. Weights for the base model not shown (Experiment 1:
6.404e48; Experiment 2: 1.93e90; Experiment 3: 1.14e89). LSA: latent semantic analysis; GloVe: global vectors model; WAS: word association spaces. C:
context-based semantic cuing; I: item-based semantic cuing; IC: combined item and context-based semantic cuing.

Experiment 1 Experiment 2 Experiment 3


C I IC C I IC C I IC
LSA 3.37e40 2.22e28 8.39e42 1.95e52 5.30e34 4.20e44 0 0 0
GloVe 2.75e21 2.78e6 7.38e18 3.15e11 7.78e9 2.41e11 0 1.33e294 2.68e314
WAS 6.72e7 0.9999 6.16e12 3.87e6 0.9999 4.10e7 3.90e74 1 4.01e8

Fig. 4. Measures of recall behavior, for the observed data and for model simulations. Top row: Experiment 1; middle row: Experiment 2; bottom row:
Experiment 3. (a) Recall probability as a function of serial position, for the data from Experiment 1 and the best-fitting model with no semantic associations.
(b) Probability of starting recall with each serial position. (c) Conditional response probability as a function of lag. (d) Stop probability by output position.
(eh) Same measures as above, for Experiment 2. (il) Experiment 3. Shaded areas indicate 95% confidence intervals for the observed data.

retrieved-context models). The model also provides a qual- with one exception: In Experiment 3, the GloVe-C model
itative account of the probability of initiating recall at each had a significantly higher RMSD across subjects compared
serial position (Fig. 4b, f, and j). The model accounts for the to the Base model (t125 3:34, p 0:01, Bonferroni
temporal organization observed in the data, and it captures corrected), due to a slightly worse fit of the lag-CRP. This
the tendency for participants to make forward transitions may reflect a compromise in the fit between temporal
more often than backward transitions (Fig. 4c, g, and k). and semantic organization (which are most strongly
Finally, the model accounts for the finding of a positively related in the context-based semantic cuing models).
accelerated increase in stop probability with output
position (Fig. 4d, h, and l). The nine model variants with Model comparison
semantic associations also accounted for each of these
summary statistics for each experiment, with fits that were Given that our Base model with no semantic associa-
very similar to the Base model. RMSD for each model tions was able to account for benchmark phenomena in
variant, pooled over the non-semantic summary statistics, free recall, we examined whether the predictive power of
is presented in Tables 35. RMSD was not significantly the model could be improved by the addition of semantic
different from the Base model for any of the models with structure. The addition of associative structure based on
semantic associations (p > 0:05, Bonferroni corrected), LSA, GloVe, or WAS led to a substantially better fit,
132 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

regardless of the cuing mechanism used: For each a repeat or intrusion were excluded (wAIC for WAS-I
experiment, wAIC and wBIC for the set of semantic models model, Experiment 1: 0.9513; Experiment 2: 0.9996;
(aggregating over cuing mechanisms and semantic Experiment 3: 1.0000).
models) was close to 1. For all experiments and semantic
models, AIC was lower (i.e., fitness was improved) when Semantic organization
an item-based, rather than context-based, semantic cuing
mechanism was used. Similarly, for a given semantic cuing In order to characterize how the model of semantic
mechanism, WAS always provided the best fit, followed by associations (WAS, GloVe, or LSA) and the type of semantic
GloVe, then LSA. The WAS-I model provided the best fit cuing mechanism (item, context, or item + context) influ-
overall for all three experiments, with AIC weights enced the behavior of the models, we carried out a set of
approaching 1. Critically, our measure of model fitness is semantic-CRP analyses (Fig. 5). The semantic-CRP shows
based on the likelihood of the recall sequences under that how the likelihood of two items being recalled in adjacent
model; it makes no assumptions about the actual structure output positions increases as a function of the semantic
of our participants semantic knowledge, and therefore similarity of the two items. We examined three versions
avoids complications that arise when a semantic model is of the semantic-CRP, using each of the different semantic
used to both generate and evaluate model predictions similarity models.
(Polyn et al., 2009; Manning, Sperling, Sharan, Rosenberg, Qualitatively, for each combination of model and
& Kahana, 2012). This analysis of AIC weights aggregates semantic-CRP analysis, the context-based semantic cuing
over multiple participants, assuming that they all use models predicted a more shallow slope for the semantic-
similar semantic cuing mechanisms. In the Semantic CRP than the item-based cuing models. This suggests that
organization section, we examine the possibility that an increase in the strength of semantic associations in
people may use different cues to probe semantic memory. the context-based cuing models would have impaired the
ability of these models to account for other aspects of the
Exclusion of recall errors recall sequences. We examined whether these differences
in fit were significant, focusing on the WAS-based models,
In this study, we focus on the processes giving rise to which had the best predictive power overall. We calculated
correct responses during free recall. While participants RMSD, a measure of error in the model fit, for each model
make error responses in the form of repeats and intrusions, and subject, and we examined whether RMSD was
they are relatively rare. Of the original set of recall significantly different between the item, context, and
attempts in Experiment 1, 4.52% were repeats, 1.34% were item + context models for a given semantic model. There
prior-list intrusions, and 3.73% were extra-list intrusions. were no significant differences between any pair of models
In Experiment 2, 3.30% of recall attempts were repeats, in Experiments 1 or 2, but in Experiment 3 RMSD was
3.14% were prior-list intrusions, and 2.39% were extra-list significantly greater for the WAS-C model when compared
intrusions. In Experiment 3, 2.97% of recall attempts were to the WAS-I model (t125 3:58; p 0:00049) and the
repeats, 0.56% were prior-list intrusions, and 2.33% were WAS-IC model (t125 4:61; p 9:9e  6), suggesting
extra-list intrusions. The version of CMR used here was that adding an item-based semantic cuing mechanism to
not designed to simulate these error responses; as such, the standard CMR model allowed a better fit to the data.
we excluded repeats and intrusions by removing them
from the recall sequences and simulating recall as if they Testing for persistence of semantic influence
had not occurred. A potential issue with this approach is
that by excising these error responses, we introduce a While the item-based and context-based semantic
discontinuity in the recall sequence, which might hurt a cuing models make similar predictions for the strength of
models ability to predict a correct recall response follow- temporal organization, they make a divergent prediction
ing an error response. This was indeed the case: Across regarding how long semantic information should exert an
all model variants, log likelihood was, on average, lower influence during the recall period. The item-based model
for the correct recall events following an excluded repeat suggests that the semantic organizational influence of a
or intrusion, indicating that these events had lower predic- given recalled item should be short-lived, only directly
tion accuracy (Experiment 1: following repeat or intrusion affecting the immediately following recall event. In con-
2.890; other recalls 2.378; Experiment 2: following trast, the context-based model suggests that this influence
repeat or intrusion 2.463; other recalls 2.112; is longer lived, given that the contextual information asso-
Experiment 3: following repeat or intrusion 2.413; other ciated with that remembered item fades gradually. We
recalls 1.935). designed a novel analysis of semantic organization to dis-
In order to test whether these differences in log likeli- tinguish between these two accounts; this analysis is
hood following repeats and intrusions affected our model described in detail in the methods section (Analysis of recall
comparison analysis, we calculated AIC weights with recall behavior). While the semantic CRP analysis focuses on
events immediately following a repeat or intrusion adjacent items in the recall sequence, this analysis exami-
excluded. AIC weights for this restricted set of recall events nes whether more distant items in the recall sequence can
were comparable to when all recall events were included exert a semantic influence on one another. Polyn et al.
(wAIC for WAS-I model, Experiment 1: 0.9958; Experiment (2009) presented a semantic organization score that
2: 0.9999; Experiment 3: 1.0000). Similar AIC weights focused on the relatedness of items in adjacent output
were also obtained when the two recall events following positions (i.e., recall events with a lag of 1). Here, we
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 133

Fig. 5. Measures of semantic organization, for the observed data and for model simulations. Top row: Experiment 1; middle row: Experiment 2; bottom
row: Experiment 3. (a) Conditional response probability as a function of latent semantic analysis (LSA) semantic similarity bin. The line indicates the mean
value in the data, and the shaded region represents the 95% confidence interval. Also shown is the performance of the LSA-based models. C: context-based
semantic cuing; I: item-based semantic cuing; IC: combined item and context-based semantic cuing. (b) Conditional response probability as a function of
global vectors (GloVe) semantic similarity bin. (c) Conditional response probability as a function of word association spaces (WAS) semantic similarity bin.
(df) Conditional response probability by semantic similarity bin for Experiment 2. (gi) Experiment 3.

extend that analysis to quantify the influence of a recalled A similar but non-significant trend was observed in Exper-
item on more distant recall events (i.e., recall events of lag iment 1 (WAS-C RMSD: 0.0307, SEM 0.0028; WAS-I RMSD:
24). The context-based semantic cuing mechanism 0.0304, SEM 0.0027; p > 0:05).
predicts that the semantic organization score should Interestingly, the observed semantic organization score
decrease as a function of recall lag, but should be greater for recall lags 3 and 4 was significantly below the chance
than chance (0.5) for recall lags greater than 1. level of 0.5 in Experiment 3 (lag 3: t125 2:61;
As shown in Fig. 6, the best-fitting WAS-C models p 0:011; lag 4: t125 3:58; p 0:0005). In contrast,
predicted an above-chance WAS score for recall lag 2 in none of the models dropped below 0.5 at any recall lag.
each experiment. In contrast to this prediction, we found Therefore, this effect in the observed data is unlikely to
that WAS factor for the observed data was not significantly be a product of some bias in the analysis, and instead
greater than 0.5 at any recall lag greater than 1 (Fig. 6; might reflect a mechanism not implemented in the model.
p > 0:05 for recall lags 24 in each experiment). Critically, One possibility is that participants sometimes strategically
we found that the predictions of the WAS-I model for shift between targeting different clusters of semantically
semantic organization score as a function of recall lag were related items; if this were the case, then after a transition
significantly more accurate than the WAS-C model. The to a new cluster of related items, items from earlier clus-
RMSD for the WAS-C model was significantly greater ters would be less likely to be recalled, resulting in a
across subjects than the WAS-I model in Experiment 2 below-chance distance factor to those items. Evidence for
(WAS-C RMSD: 0.0466, SEM 0.0037; WAS-I RMSD: strategic targeting of groups of related items has
0.0453, SEM 0.0036; t47 2:15, p 0:037) and previously been observed in free recall of items from
Experiment 3 (WAS-C RMSD: 0.0421, SEM 0.0017; WAS-I categorized lists (Pollio, Richards, & Lucas, 1969). Although
RMSD: 0.0395, SEM 0.0016; t125 4:41; p 0:00002). the context-based semantic cuing model predicts that
134 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

Fig. 6. Examination of persistent effects of semantic information, for the observed data and model simulations. (a) Effects of semantic recall context in
Experiment 1. Plot shows WAS factor calculated based on lag during recall (e.g. WAS score for recall lag of 1 is based on similarity to the last recalled item).
The models with context-based semantic cuing predict that WAS factor will be greater than chance (0.5; indicated by the dotted line) for lags greater than 1;
however, we did not observe this in the data. (b) Data and model simulations for Experiment 2. (c) Experiment 3. Shaded areas indicate 95% confidence
intervals for the observed data.

semantic cues will persist over time, it predicts that this experiment, we find that the modal value of k is 1, indicat-
change will be gradual, while in practice participants ing that the majority of participants in each experiment
may sometimes exhibit sharper changes (e.g. shifting from were best fit by a pure item-based semantic cuing model.
targeting words related to animals to targeting words However, we also find that in each experiment there are
related to musical instruments). a subset of participants whose behavior is better described
by a model with k < 1, indicating some evidence for
Individual differences in semantic cuing context-based semantic cuing. This pattern is most striking
in Experiment 2, where 16 participants were best fit by a
In terms of the AIC weights, which aggregate across pure context-based semantic cuing model. This result
participants, there is overwhelming support for the item- suggests that while there are many similarities in recall
based semantic cuing model in each of the three experi- performance across participants (Healey & Kahana, 2014),
ments (Table 2). However, the modeling framework was there are differences in how semantic structure affects
designed to find the optimal parameter settings for each recall organization across different participants. The
individual in each experiment, which allows us to examine difference in the distribution of k across participants may
whether there were individual differences in cuing strat- be related to procedural differences in the experimental
egy across participants. Given that WAS provided the best paradigms. For example, the faster presentation time or
overall description of behavior relative to the other models the type of encoding task (visualization) in Experiment 2
of semantic associations (and regardless of the type of may have encouraged a different type of encoding that
cuing used), we focus our examination on the hybrid led to semantic information being integrated into temporal
WAS-IC model. This model contains the free parameter k, context for a subset of participants, yielding behavior
which specifies for each participant the relative weighting consistent with the context-based cuing model. This point
of item-based and context-based semantic cuing (where receives more attention in the discussion.
k 0 indicates pure context-based semantic cuing, and
k 1 indicates pure item-based semantic cuing). Discussion
Regardless of the value of k, all models used context-
based episodic cuing to guide temporal organization. We developed a likelihood-based modeling framework
Fig. 7 presents a histogram for each experiment with where a model of semantic associations is embedded in
the best-fitting values of k for each participant. In each the context maintenance and retrieval model (CMR); this

Fig. 7. Value of the k parameter for the best-fitting WAS model with combined item and context-based semantic cuing, for each experiment. (a) In
Experiment 1, most participants had values of k of very near to 1, indicating that their recall behavior was more consistent with the item-based semantic
cuing mechanism. (b) In Experiment 2, a substantial subset of participants had values of k that were near to 0, consistent with the context-based semantic
cuing mechanism. (c) Most participants in Experiment 3 had behavior that was most consistent with item-based semantic cuing.
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 135

framework allowed us to assess the relative validity of such, this framework can be applied to experiments where
competing models of semantic organization in free recall the studied items do not have a systematic category struc-
while accounting for many of the complexities of memory ture. Furthermore, the framework is flexible enough that it
search. CMR proposes that studied items become associ- can be used to evaluate any model of semantic structure, as
ated with a representation of temporal context, which pro- long as that model provides estimates of the associative
vides an important cue during memory search. This strengths between items. While the vector-space models
context-based episodic cuing mechanism has been shown (WAS, GloVe, and LSA) evaluated here contain symmetric
to explain several important aspects of temporal organiza- associative strengths, this characteristic is not neces-
tion in free recall (Howard & Kahana, 2002a; Howard, Jing, sarythe framework can evaluate semantic models in
Rao, Provyn, & Datey, 2009; Sederberg et al., 2008); which the associative strength from item i to item j is
however, it is less clear whether temporal context also not the same as the associative strength of item j to item i.
influences semantic organization. We contrasted two We are interested in determining which model contains
mechanisms by which semantic associations have been semantic relatedness scores that best correspond to those
proposed to influence free recall: an item-based mecha- in the human memory system. In terms of predictive
nism where retrieved items cue for semantic associates, power and fit to summary statistics, WAS is the clear
and a context-based mechanism where retrieved context winner in this regard. However, the conclusions we can
serves as a semantic cue. While temporal organization in draw regarding the processes giving rise to these semantic
free recall is consistent with a context-based cuing structures are limited. The superior performance of the
mechanism (Howard & Kahana, 2002a; Lohnas & Kahana, WAS model is not surprising in that its representations
2014), we found that semantic organization in free recall are constructed from the results of a behavioral free-
is more consistent with an item-based semantic cuing association experiment, while LSA and GloVe are trained
mechanism, suggesting that semantic and episodic associ- on large text corpora. In other words, WAS incorporates
ations are probed using distinct cues during memory behavioral results from a similar cognitive task into its
search. Furthermore, we found that models using word- structure (i.e., free association vs. free recall), sidestepping
association spaces (WAS) to determine semantic structure the need to describe the processes by which this structure
outperformed models using latent semantic analysis (LSA) develops (Jones, Hills, & Todd, 2015).
or global vectors (GloVe), in terms of the models ability to Nonetheless, by contrasting WAS with the other models
predict the identities of a sequence of recalled items. We of semantic association, we may gain insight into how
propose that our modeling framework provides specific these other models can be modified to increase their utility
advantages in the evaluation of computational models of as cognitive models of semantic similarity. In our examina-
semantic and episodic memory and may provide the tion of the semantic CRP (Fig. 5), only the WAS-CRP
basis for developing better measurements of semantic showed a linear relationship between semantic similarity
organization in recall sequences. and likelihood of semantic clustering in the observed data.
In contrast, the LSA-CRP and GloVe-CRP showed positively
Models of semantic association strength accelerated curves describing this relationship. One inter-
pretation of this difference is that WAS does a better job
Both WAS and LSA have been used to characterize estimating the global structure of the semantic space
behavior in free recall (Howard & Kahana, 2002b; containing these word representations. All three models
Howard et al., 2007; Manning & Kahana, 2012) and have seem to do a good job describing the local structure of
been used as components of models of memory search semantic space in that all three semantic-CRP curves
(Sirotin et al., 2005; Polyn et al., 2009). The present results capture the increased likelihood of clustering associated
suggest that WAS is better able than both LSA and a more with highly related word pairs (i.e., words that are nearby
recently developed technique, GloVe, to predict behavior in in semantic space). However, only WAS seems to capture
recall of lists of words with no obvious semantic structure. the behavioral consequences of small changes in semantic
Our results complement those of Sirotin et al. (2005), who relatedness for less related word pairs (i.e., words that are
compared the ability of WAS and LSA to explain behavior more distant in semantic space). This interpretation is sup-
in free recall of categorized materials. They developed a ported by our modeling results, in which only the WAS
version of the search of associative memory (SAM) model models are able to fit the full extent of the semantic-CRP,
(Raaijmakers & Shiffrin, 1980) that included semantic asso- consistent with the idea that WAS provides a good match
ciations between items. Sirotin et al. (2005) assumed that to the associations guiding recall. By this logic, the failure
search of long-term memory is driven by both episodic of the LSA and GloVe models to fit their corresponding
and semantic inter-item associations. They compared a semantic-CRP curves is likely due to the presence of
model with semantic structure based on WAS to a model semantic associations that do not match the ones guiding
with semantic structure based on LSA and found that the behavior. If the strengths of these semantic associations
WAS-based model was better able to account for category were increased, it would be at the cost of predicting
clustering in a multi-trial free recall study (Kahana & semantic clustering that mismatches the observed data.
Wingfield, 2000). Their analysis of recall behavior focused In future work, it may be possible to identify transfor-
on only one aspect of semantic organization, namely clus- mations on the LSA or GloVe representations that improve
tering by taxonomic category. In contrast, our likelihood- their predictive power in free recall. This would be of util-
based framework does not require choosing a particular ity to many cognitive researchers interested in estimating
summary statistic to evaluate the fitness of a model. As semantic similarity, as the semantic similarity estimates
136 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

in WAS are limited to the 5018 words that were part of the context-based semantic cuing mechanism. It is
the original free-association study (Nelson et al., 2004). possible that methodological differences between the
Such an endeavor might also inform the development of experiments underlie this observation. Experiment 2 had
process-based models attempting to describe the emer- a faster presentation time than Experiments 1 and 3, and
gence of semantic structure with experience (Jones & it included an end-of-list distraction period. Participants
Mewhort, 2007; Rao & Howard, 2008; Rogers & in Experiment 2 were encouraged to visualize the items,
McClelland, 2004). whereas in Experiment 1 they performed one of two binary
classification tasks. In Experiment 3, we examined trials
Mechanisms of semantic cuing without an explicit encoding task, but these were
surrounded by trials in which participants performed the
Polyn et al. (2009) developed CMR, which extended the same binary classification tasks as in Experiment 1, which
temporal context model (TCM) to account for multiple may have influenced their encoding strategy. More work is
influences on recall organization, including source context needed to investigate what circumstances determine
and semantic similarity. CMR is a retrieved-context model, whether recall behavior is more consistent with item- or
wherein retrieval of a particular item causes the system to context-based semantic cuing.
reactivate the temporal context representation associated One methodological characteristic common to the three
with that item. This retrieved context contains a weighted experiments was that there was no obvious semantic
average of information related to the items preceding the structure to the study lists. Words were randomly chosen
just-recalled item in the study list. The version of CMR from a large pool. It may be that context-based semantic
presented by Polyn et al. (2009) used a context-based cuing is more likely to be engaged when study lists have
semantic cuing mechanism in which retrieved temporal explicit semantic structure, as in blocked categorized free
context projects through a set of semantic associations, recall paradigms (e.g. Puff, 1966). Recent scalp EEG evi-
providing support for any items that are semantically dence is consistent with this idea. Using scalp EEG during
related to any of the items represented in the context encoding of categorized materials, Morton et al. (2013)
cue. We constructed an alternative model in which found evidence of persistent category-specific activity
semantic cuing was driven by semantic associations which became stronger when multiple items from the
attached to a feature-based representation of the studied same category were presented in sequence. The rate at
item. This item-based semantic cuing mechanism is similar which this category-specific signal increased predicted
to the semantic cuing mechanism in the eSAM model individual differences in organization by stimulus category
(Sirotin et al., 2005), in which only the most recently during recall. Morton et al. (2013) proposed that this inte-
recalled studied item is used to cue its semantic associates. grative category-specific signal is consistent with the oper-
In both variants of the model, recall organization is ation of a temporal context mechanism. If each studied
simultaneously determined by temporal and semantic item caused category-specific information to be integrated
information, but the nature of temporal/semantic interac- into context, this would explain both why the category-
tions differs between the two versions. specific signal gets progressively stronger, and why this
Across three experiments with widely varying method- rate of increase is related to individual differences in cate-
ological characteristics, we found that the context-based gory clustering. To test this account, Morton and Polyn
semantic cuing mechanism described by Polyn et al. (unpublished results) created a modified version of CMR
(2009) was inferior to the item-based semantic cuing in which each item is associated with a distributed pre-
mechanism. While the context-based semantic cuing experimental contextual representation containing seman-
mechanism performed substantially better than a model tic information. As in the context-based semantic cuing
without any semantic structure, models with an item- models examined in the present work, their distributed-
based semantic cuing mechanism were overall best at CMR model assumed that both temporal and semantic
predicting behavior. Under this item-based mechanism, organization are driven by contextual cues. They simulated
although temporal context is still used as an episodic the Morton et al. (2013) experiment and found that the
cue, only the most recently recalled item is used as a distributed-CMR model correctly accounts for the relation-
semantic cue. We developed a novel analysis of semantic ship between category-specific neural activity during
organization to examine a divergent prediction of the encoding and individual differences in semantic organiza-
two models: While context-based semantic cuing predicts tion. In future work, we plan to adapt the distributed-
that an item will have a gradually fading influence on CMR model to work within the likelihood-based frame-
semantic organization, the item-based mechanism work presented in this article. This will allow us to directly
predicts that the semantic influence of a given item will contrast the semantic cuing models evaluated here, in
be limited to the immediately following response. As which semantic associations only have an influence during
shown in the results of the recall-lag analysis presented retrieval, with the distributed-CMR model, where semantic
in Fig. 6, the predictions of the item-based model provided information is integrated into context during encoding.
a better fit to the observed data. One clear prediction of the context-based semantic
An examination of individual differences in recall cuing mechanism is that a weighted combination of the
behavior revealed limited evidence for the engagement of prior recalls determines the semantic influences for the
a context-based cuing mechanism in some subjects. This current recall event, causing a form of compound cuing.
evidence was most obvious in Experiment 2, where a sub- Temporal organization shows clear compound cuing
stantial minority of the participants were best described by effects in a way that is consistent with the CMR model
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 137

(Lohnas & Kahana, 2014). While we observed temporal which may prove useful for investigating interactions
organization effects similar to those in previous studies between temporal and semantic structure during memory
(Fig. 4), we found no evidence for compound semantic search.
cuing (Fig. 6). However, it is possible that other types of
free-recall paradigms may show evidence for compound Conclusions
semantic cuing. Kimball et al. (2007) examined behavior
in the false memory paradigm, in which participants have While prior research has found that semantic knowl-
a strong tendency to falsely recall critical items that are edge exerts an important influence on the search of episo-
semantically related to the items from the study list dic memory, many questions remain about the cognitive
(Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995). They mechanisms that mediate this influence. We developed a
proposed a modified version of the SAM model in which modeling framework that allows one to both evaluate the
a compound cuing mechanism (in which multiple remem- relative utility of different models of semantic associations
bered items exerted semantic influences during recall) was and to compare different mechanisms by which semantic
necessary to fully account for data in false memory information affects memory search. In order to be able to
experiments. It may be that the strong semantic structure calculate the exact likelihood of recall sequences under a
of study lists in the false memory paradigm leads to given model, we used a simplified version of CMR which
the engagement of a context-based cuing mechanism. did not contain mechanisms to determine response
However, more work is needed to determine whether the latency, to produce recall errors, or to produce organiza-
CMR model can account for the major empirical phenom- tional effects related to source characteristics (Lohnas
ena from false memory paradigms. et al., 2015; Polyn et al., 2009). However, we believe it will
be possible to develop this framework to incorporate these
mechanisms, allowing one to examine different models of
Measurement of semantic organization
how semantic information influences inter-response times,
recall errors, and other organizational effects. More gener-
In the current work, the influence of semantic informa-
ally, we hope that the computational modeling framework
tion is reflected in the magnitude of the s parameter, which
presented here will continue to help shed light on how
scales the influence of semantic associations on memory
prior semantic knowledge shapes the formation and
search. Because the model contains other parameters
utilization of episodic memories.
which account for behavioral variance due to temporal
structure, the best-fitting value of the s parameter may
Acknowledgments
provide a good estimate of the magnitude of semantic
organization in a given experiment. As such, the computa-
This research was partially carried out as part of NWMs
tional modeling framework used here may be useful for
doctoral dissertation with SMP at Vanderbilt University.
measuring semantic organization while accounting for
This research was supported by the NIH (MH055687) and
other influences on recall behavior. Properly accounting
the National Science Foundation (1157432). Thanks to
for temporal organization is critical when considering
Michael Kahana for making the data available, and to
experimental manipulations that alter the temporal orga-
Jonathan Miller, Emily Rosenberg, Richard Lawrence,
nization of semantically related stimuli, as in experiments
John Burke, and Matthew Mollison for help collecting data.
that contrast study lists with blocked vs. random
Thanks to Joshua McCluey for help developing parameter
presentation of categorized stimuli (Puff, 1966). However,
optimization code. Thanks to Gordon Logan, Geoff
most of the prior work on blocked-random effects has
Woodman, Brandon Ally, James Kragel and Joshua McCluey
not accounted for this influence (e.g. Borges & Mandler,
for helpful discussions.
1972; Cofer, Bruce, & Reicher, 1966; DAgostino, 1969).
Through bootstrapping techniques, it is possible to esti-
mate the amount of semantic organization due to temporal Appendix A
clustering (Morton et al., 2013). However, this technique
requires collecting data from baseline lists with no cate- Maximum-likelihood parameter estimates, as well as
gory structure and involves the assumption that other log likelihood, AIC, BIC, AIC weights, BIC weights, and
aspects of recall behavior are unaffected by the manipula- RMSD are shown in Tables 35. Although all parameters
tion of the temporal structure of the categorized materials. were allowed to vary freely for each of the model variants,
To avoid these issues, CMR could be fit separately to many of the best-fitting parameters were quite similar
blocked and random lists. The semantic scaling parameter across all models. Parameters controlling the rate of con-
would then provide an estimate of the strength of semantic text evolution (benc ; bdelay , and brec ), parameters involved
organization, while the other parameters (as well as the in the primacy effect (/s ; /d , and bstart ), and stopping
structure of the model itself) could account for alterations parameters (hs and hr ) were all comparable across the 10
in temporal organization and other influences that might model variants within each experiment. The semantic scal-
vary between conditions. While further work is necessary ing parameter, s, was generally greater for better-fitting
to determine whether CMR will provide reliable estimates models, suggesting that the influence of semantics is
of semantic organization in these experiments, the current increased when the model of semantic cuing is
work establishes the plausibility of such an approach, improved. For a given semantic model, a was increased for
Table 3

138
Best-fitting parameters for Experiment 1. Reported values indicate averages over subjects; values in parentheses indicate standard error of the mean. RMSD is reported for the summary statistics shown in Fig. 4. LSA:
latent semantic analysis; GloVe: global vectors; WAS: word association spaces. C: context-based semantic cuing; I: item-based semantic cuing; IC: combined item and context-based semantic cuing.

Base LSA-C LSA-I LSA-IC GloVe-C GloVe-I GloVe-IC WAS-C WAS-I WAS-IC
benc 0.72 (0.02) 0.72 (0.02) 0.71 (0.02) 0.72 (0.02) 0.71 (0.02) 0.69 (0.02) 0.70 (0.02) 0.72 (0.02) 0.70 (0.02) 0.71 (0.02)
brec 0.87 (0.02) 0.88 (0.02) 0.86 (0.02) 0.87 (0.01) 0.89 (0.01) 0.84 (0.02) 0.85 (0.02) 0.89 (0.01) 0.86 (0.02) 0.87 (0.02)
bstart 0.18 (0.04) 0.19 (0.04) 0.18 (0.04) 0.18 (0.04) 0.20 (0.04) 0.22 (0.05) 0.22 (0.05) 0.19 (0.04) 0.19 (0.04) 0.19 (0.04)
a 7.69 (2.66) 6.57 (2.58) 9.09 (2.75) 8.16 (2.68) 6.34 (2.41) 10.33 (2.72) 9.08 (2.70) 5.86 (2.37) 9.79 (2.96) 8.61 (2.90)
d 33.60 (5.96) 34.90 (6.34) 34.36 (6.05) 33.59 (6.05) 33.94 (6.25) 33.79 (5.93) 33.47 (5.98) 33.77 (6.27) 34.87 (6.31) 33.64 (6.32)
c 0.17 (0.04) 0.16 (0.03) 0.20 (0.04) 0.18 (0.04) 0.15 (0.03) 0.22 (0.04) 0.18 (0.04) 0.16 (0.03) 0.21 (0.04) 0.18 (0.03)
k 0.84 (0.06) 0.91 (0.04) 0.83 (0.05)
/s 30.84 (6.37) 29.82 (6.17) 27.85 (6.08) 27.81 (6.03) 27.82 (5.93) 20.33 (5.16) 22.67 (5.49) 30.53 (6.23) 27.35 (6.07) 27.06 (6.06)
/d 14.48 (4.96) 13.34 (4.65) 15.06 (5.19) 14.39 (4.66) 13.51 (4.77) 13.61 (4.64) 16.76 (5.36) 16.12 (5.22) 16.00 (5.14) 15.13 (5.05)

N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140
s 0.49 (0.12) 0.80 (0.20) 0.81 (0.19) 0.77 (0.20) 1.05 (0.19) 1.12 (0.22) 1.60 (0.31) 1.79 (0.35) 2.05 (0.42)
s 20.86 (5.11) 16.89 (4.60) 27.56 (5.90) 22.67 (5.33) 17.27 (4.35) 37.92 (6.51) 30.18 (6.15) 16.10 (4.16) 26.85 (5.70) 22.44 (5.08)
hs 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00)
hr 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01) 0.32 (0.01)
ln(L) 28664.34 28600.32 28573.11 28558.88 28556.78 28522.23 28503.75 28523.65 28509.44 28490.12
AIC 58271.28 58232.82 58178.40 58240.21 58145.73 58076.63 58129.94 58079.47 58051.04 58102.67
BIC 59889.35 59994.10 59939.68 60144.01 59907.01 59837.91 60033.75 59840.75 59812.33 60006.48
wAIC 1.50493e48 3.37118e40 2.21533e28 8.39428e42 2.75331e21 2.77987e06 7.37794e18 6.72363e07 0.999997 6.15624e12
wBIC 1.88246e17 3.37118e40 2.21533e28 9.44576e73 2.75331e21 2.77987e06 8.30211e49 6.72363e07 0.999997 6.92738e43
RMSD 0.1093 0.1082 0.1095 0.1097 0.1096 0.1088 0.1076 0.1086 0.1084 0.1086

Table 4
Best-fitting parameters for Experiment 2. Reported values indicate averages over subjects; values in parentheses indicate standard error of the mean. RMSD is reported for the summary statistics shown in Fig. 4. LSA:
latent semantic analysis; GloVe: global vectors model; WAS: word association spaces. C: context-based semantic cuing; I: item-based semantic cuing; IC: combined item and context-based semantic cuing.

Base LSA-C LSA-I LSA-IC GloVe-C GloVe-I GloVe-IC WAS-C WAS-I WAS-IC
benc 0.76 (0.04) 0.70 (0.04) 0.64 (0.04) 0.66 (0.04) 0.66 (0.05) 0.60 (0.05) 0.64 (0.04) 0.63 (0.05) 0.62 (0.04) 0.63 (0.04)
brec 0.84 (0.04) 0.86 (0.03) 0.81 (0.03) 0.81 (0.03) 0.88 (0.03) 0.81 (0.04) 0.80 (0.04) 0.86 (0.03) 0.83 (0.03) 0.82 (0.03)
bdelay 0.63 (0.07) 0.63 (0.07) 0.63 (0.07) 0.70 (0.07) 0.67 (0.07) 0.65 (0.07) 0.75 (0.06) 0.72 (0.06) 0.64 (0.07) 0.73 (0.06)
bstart 0.20 (0.05) 0.18 (0.04) 0.18 (0.04) 0.14 (0.03) 0.17 (0.04) 0.16 (0.03) 0.13 (0.03) 0.19 (0.04) 0.23 (0.05) 0.16 (0.04)
a 4.17 (1.51) 5.74 (2.30) 7.23 (2.36) 7.65 (2.53) 6.20 (2.28) 10.95 (3.60) 7.77 (2.36) 8.08 (3.14) 7.15 (2.55) 7.30 (2.92)
d 3.29 (1.33) 9.60 (3.45) 6.50 (2.21) 7.69 (2.47) 9.49 (3.31) 14.33 (4.11) 11.58 (3.39) 14.22 (4.16) 10.87 (3.27) 8.72 (3.16)
c 0.45 (0.05) 0.40 (0.05) 0.47 (0.05) 0.50 (0.05) 0.41 (0.05) 0.42 (0.05) 0.44 (0.05) 0.41 (0.05) 0.49 (0.06) 0.50 (0.05)
k 0.84 (0.05) 0.77 (0.05) 0.64 (0.07)
/s 46.72 (6.50) 31.08 (5.78) 26.31 (5.76) 20.24 (5.25) 29.71 (5.70) 25.11 (5.53) 18.31 (4.80) 26.07 (5.35) 23.20 (5.44) 23.24 (5.35)
/d 13.57 (4.46) 15.27 (4.74) 12.58 (4.40) 12.84 (4.48) 13.17 (4.43) 11.78 (4.38) 11.46 (4.09) 8.87 (3.42) 13.26 (4.50) 9.77 (3.50)
s 1.34 (0.57) 1.69 (0.66) 1.22 (0.40) 2.38 (1.07) 2.05 (0.89) 1.35 (0.45) 5.21 (2.42) 2.96 (1.31) 5.19 (2.49)
s 16.62 (4.79) 25.53 (5.75) 33.00 (5.85) 36.48 (6.18) 32.89 (6.14) 38.93 (6.05) 41.82 (6.30) 26.65 (5.50) 29.50 (5.62) 27.86 (5.68)
hs 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00) 0.01 (0.00)
hr 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02) 0.42 (0.02)
ln(L) 24490.10 24348.81 24306.36 24275.35 24253.93 24248.42 24199.92 24242.21 24229.74 24190.18
AIC 50199.31 50024.30 49939.40 49985.92 49834.52 49823.51 49835.06 49811.08 49786.16 49815.57
BIC 52147.08 52128.42 52043.52 52245.40 51938.64 51927.62 52094.54 51915.20 51890.28 52075.05
wAIC 1.92945e90 1.94524e52 5.30204e34 4.20467e44 3.14943e11 7.77679e09 2.41321e11 3.87335e06 0.999996 4.10211e07
wBIC 1.71817e56 1.94524e52 5.30204e34 7.7123e78 3.14943e11 7.77679e09 4.42636e45 3.87335e06 0.999996 7.52418e41
RMSD 0.0982 0.0991 0.0984 0.0982 0.0988 0.0974 0.0984 0.0978 0.0981 0.0978
N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140 139

item-based semantic cuing models. Increasing a causes


Best-fitting parameters for Experiment 3. Reported values indicate averages over subjects; values in parentheses indicate standard error of the mean. RMSD is reported for the summary statistics shown in Fig. 4. LSA:

1.95914e94
4.00501e08
recall to become more stochastic (less dependent on the

34.18 (3.68)
22.68 (3.08)
13.02 (2.73)
32.00 (3.33)

53197.59
6.35 (0.93)
0.71 (0.02)

0.28 (0.03)

0.24 (0.03)
0.81 (0.03)

2.03 (0.30)

0.42 (0.01)
0.80 (0.01)

0.01 (0.00)

115263.85
109893.59
particular context cue used). This may help the item-
latent semantic analysis; GloVe: global vectors model; WAS: word association spaces. C: context-based semantic cuing; I: item-based semantic cuing; IC: combined item and context-based semantic cuing.

WAS-IC

0.0958
based semantic cuing models to mimic the tendency of
context-based semantic cuing models to predict more
diffuse cuing of multiple items in the list (see Fig. 2d for
32.79 (3.37)
an illustration).

14.63 (2.95)

36.62 (3.76)
22.02 (3.01)

53322.92
7.24 (1.06)

2.27 (0.61)
0.69 (0.02)

0.28 (0.03)

0.25 (0.03)

0.42 (0.01)
0.80 (0.02)

0.01 (0.00)

114832.31
109859.52

0.0967
WAS-I

1
1
References

Anderson, J. R. (1972). FRAN: A simulation model of free recall. In G. H.

3.90117e74
3.90117e74
15.41 (2.99)

27.83 (3.43)
22.22 (3.07)
40.02 (3.66)

Bower (Ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 5,


53491.95
3.83 (0.53)

1.27 (0.11)
0.69 (0.02)
0.91 (0.01)
0.32 (0.03)

0.18 (0.02)

0.42 (0.01)
0.01 (0.00)

110197.58
115170.37
pp. 315379). New York: Academic Press.
WAS-C

0.0968
Baayen, R., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1995). CELEX2 LDC96L14.
Philadelpha, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium. Web download.

Batchelder, W., & Riefer, D. (1980). Separation of storage and retrieval


factors in free recall of clusterable pairs. Psychological Review, 87(4),
375397.
Borges, M. A., & Mandler, G. (1972). Effect of within-category spacing on
2.67743e314
31.98 (3.19)

19.65 (2.95)

57.02 (3.96)
10.70 (2.45)
10.46 (1.10)

free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 92, 207214.


53902.58
3.25 (0.87)
0.81 (0.01)
0.26 (0.03)

0.27 (0.03)
0.95 (0.02)

0.42 (0.01)
0.70 (0.02)

0.01 (0.00)

116673.84
111303.58

Bousfield, W. A. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of


GloVe-IC

0.0972

randomly arranged associates. Journal of General Psychology, 49,


229240.
Brown, G. D. A., Neath, I., & Chater, N. (2007). A temporal ratio model of
0

memory. Psychological Review, 114(3), 539576.


Cofer, C. N., Bruce, D. R., & Reicher, G. M. (1966). Clustering in free recall as
a function of certain methodological variations. Journal of
1.3319e294
1.3319e294
13.94 (1.57)
33.95 (3.16)

18.48 (2.84)
11.16 (2.52)

68.11 (3.84)

Experimental Psychology, 71, 858866.


53999.59
2.52 (0.37)
0.67 (0.02)
0.79 (0.02)
0.26 (0.03)

0.29 (0.03)

0.42 (0.01)
0.01 (0.00)

111212.87
116185.66

Cohen, B. H. (1963). An investigation of recoding in free recall. Journal of


GloVe-I

0.0967

Experimental Psychology, 65(4), 368376.


DAgostino, P. R. (1969). The blocked-random effect in recall and

recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 815820.


Davelaar, E. J., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Ashkenazi, A., Haarmann, H. J., &
Usher, M. (2005). The demise of short-term memory revisited:
23.19 (3.14)
12.98 (2.68)

34.67 (3.78)
40.03 (3.63)

Empirical and computational investigations of recency effects.


54335.26
4.98 (0.72)

1.77 (0.42)
0.91 (0.01)

0.18 (0.02)

0.42 (0.01)
0.70 (0.02)

0.30 (0.03)

0.01 (0.00)

111884.21
116856.99

Psychological Review, 112, 342.


GloVe-C

0.0981

Deese, J. (1959). Influence of inter-item associative strength upon


immediate free recall. Psychological Reports, 5, 305312.
Farrell, S. (2012). Temporal clustering and sequencing in working

0
0

memory and episodic memory. Psychological Review, 119(2), 223271.


Farrell, S., & Lewandowsky, S. (2008). Empirical and theoretical limits on
lag recency in free recall. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15,
43.55 (3.98)
30.74 (3.32)

20.10 (2.90)

54300.50
7.76 (1.26)

8.07 (2.06)
0.71 (0.02)
0.81 (0.01)
0.26 (0.03)

0.26 (0.03)
0.91 (0.02)

1.80 (0.20)

0.42 (0.01)
0.01 (0.00)

117469.68
112099.41

12361250.
Friendly, M., Franklin, P. E., Hoffman, D., & Rubin, D. C. (1982). The Toronto
0.0978
LSA-IC

Word Pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness, orthographic variables,


and grammatical usage for 1080 words. Behavior Research Methods
0
0

and Instrumentation, 14, 375399.


Glanzer, M. (1969). Distance between related words in free recall: Trace
of the STS. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 105111.
32.31 (3.30)

19.94 (2.90)

51.39 (4.04)
10.91 (2.51)

54394.66
9.30 (1.27)

1.95 (0.28)
0.27 (0.03)

0.28 (0.03)

0.42 (0.01)
0.70 (0.02)
0.80 (0.01)

0.01 (0.00)

Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Topics in semantic


116975.80
112003.01

representation. Psychological Review, 114(2), 211244.


0.0971

Healey, M. K., & Kahana, M. J. (2014). Is memory search governed by


LSA-I

universal principles or idiosyncratic strategies? Journal of


0
0

Experimental PsychologyGeneral, 143(2), 575596.


Howard, M. W. (2004). Scaling behavior in the temporal context model.
Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 48, 230238.
37.65 (3.47)

25.92 (3.29)
14.21 (2.85)

29.32 (3.62)

Howard, M. W., Fotedar, M. S., Datey, A. V., & Hasselmo, M. E. (2005). The
54659.31
3.63 (0.58)
0.72 (0.02)

0.28 (0.03)

0.19 (0.03)

0.42 (0.01)
0.90 (0.01)

1.00 (0.08)

0.01 (0.00)

112532.31
117505.10

temporal context model in spatial navigation and relational learning:


0.0972

Toward a common explanation of medial temporal lobe function


LSA-C

across domains. Psychological Review, 112(1), 75116.


0
0

Howard, M. W., Jing, B., Rao, V. A., Provyn, J. P., & Datey, A. V. (2009).
Bridging the gap: Transitive associations between items presented in
similar temporal contexts. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
34.88 (3.53)

37.24 (3.74)
14.69 (2.82)

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(2), 391407.


21.50 (3.32)

55608.13
3.90 (0.75)
0.83 (0.01)
0.86 (0.01)
0.23 (0.03)

0.22 (0.03)

0.42 (0.01)
0.01 (0.00)

114147.96
118720.53

Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2002a). A distributed representation of


temporal context. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 46, 269299.
0.0959
Base

Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2002b). When does semantic similarity


help episodic retrieval? Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 8598.

0
0

Howard, M. W., Venkatadass, V., Norman, K. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2007).


Associative processes in immediate recency. Memory & Cognition, 35
(7), 17001711.
RMSD
wAIC
wBIC
Table 5

ln(L)
bstart

Jones, M. N., Hills, T. T., & Todd, P. M. (2015). Hidden processes in


benc

AIC
brec

BIC
/d
/s

hs
hr

structural representations: A reply to Abbott, Austerweil, and


a

s
c
d

Griffiths (2015). Psychological Review, 122(3), 570574.


140 N.W. Morton, S.M. Polyn / Journal of Memory and Language 86 (2016) 119140

Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2007). Representing word meaning and Polyn, S. M., Norman, K. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2009). A context maintenance
order information in a composite holographic lexicon. Psychological and retrieval model of organizational processes in free recall.
Review, 114(1), 137. Psychological Review, 116(1), 129156.
Kahana, M. J. (1996). Associative retrieval processes in free recall. Memory Price, K., Storn, R. M., & Lampinen, J. A. (2005). Differential evolution: A
& Cognition, 24, 103109. practical approach to global optimization. Natural computing series.
Kahana, M. J. (2012). Foundations of human memory (1st ed.). New York, Springer.
NY: Oxford University Press. Puff, C. R. (1966). Clustering as a function of the sequential organization of
Kahana, M. J., Howard, M. W., & Polyn, S. M. (2008). Associative retrieval stimulus word lists. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5,
processes in episodic memory. In H. L. Roediger, III (Ed.), Cognitive 503506.
psychology of memory. Learning and memory: A comprehensive Puff, C. R. (1974). A consolidated theoretical view of stimulus-list
reference, 4 vols, (J. Byrne, Ed.) (2, pp. 467490). Oxford: Elsevier. organization effects in free recall. Psychological Reports, 34(1),
Kahana, M. J., & Wingfield, A. (2000). A functional relation between 275288.
learning and organization in free recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Raaijmakers, J. G. W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1980). SAM: A theory of
Review, 7, 516521. probabilistic search of associative memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.).
Kimball, D. R., Smith, T. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2007). The fSAM model of false The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and
recall. Psychological Review, 114(4), 954993. theory (Vol. 14, pp. 207262). New York: Academic Press.
Kragel, J. E., Morton, N. W., & Polyn, S. M. (2015). Neural activity in the Rao, V. A., & Howard, M. W. (2008). Retrieved context and the discovery of
medial temporal lobe reveals the fidelity of mental time travel. The semantic structure. In J. C. Platt, D. Koller, Y. Singer, & S. Roweis (Eds.),
Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 29142926. Advances in neural information processing systems (pp. 11931200).
Laming, D. (1999). Testing the idea of distinct storage mechanisms in Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
memory. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 419426. Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories:
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). Solution to Platos problem: The Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental
latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21, 803814.
representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211240. Roenker, D. L., Thompson, C. P., & Brown, S. C. (1971). Comparison of
Lohnas, L. J., & Kahana, M. J. (2014). Compound cuing in free recall. Journal measures for the estimation of clustering in free recall. Psychological
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(1), Bulletin, 76(1), 4548.
1224. Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel
Lohnas, L. J., Polyn, S. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2011). Contextual variability in distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
free recall. Journal of Memory and Language, 64(3), 249255. Romney, A. K., Brewer, D. D., & Batchelder, W. H. (1993). Predicting
Lohnas, L. J., Polyn, S. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2015). Expanding the scope of clustering from semantic structure. Psychological Science, 4, 2834.
memory search: Modeling intralist and interlist effects in free recall. Schwartz, R. M., & Humphreys, M. S. (1973). Similarity judgements and
Psychological Review, 122(2), 337363. free recall of unrelated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 101,
Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic 1013.
spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Schwarz, G. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of
Instruments & Computers, 28(2), 203208. Statistics, 6(2), 461464.
Manning, J. R., & Kahana, M. J. (2012). Interpreting semantic clustering Sederberg, P. B., Gauthier, L. V., Terushkin, V., Miller, J. F., Barnathan, J. A.,
effects in free recall. Memory, 20(5), 511517. & Kahana, M. J. (2006). Oscillatory correlates of the primacy effect in
Manning, J. R., Sperling, M. R., Sharan, A., Rosenberg, E. A., & Kahana, M. J. episodic memory. NeuroImage, 32(3), 14221431.
(2012). Spontaneously reactivated patterns in frontal and temporal Sederberg, P. B., Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2008). A context-based
lobe predict semantic clustering during memory search. Journal of theory of recency and contiguity in free recall. Psychological Review,
Neuroscience, 32(26), 88718878. 115(4), 893912.
Morton, N. W., Kahana, M. J., Rosenberg, E. A., Baltuch, G. H., Litt, B., Sederberg, P. B., Miller, J. F., Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2010). The
Sharan, A. D., Sperling, M. R., & Polyn, S. M. (2013). Category-specific temporal contiguity effect predicts episodic memory performance.
neural oscillations predict recall organization during memory search. Memory & Cognition, 38(6), 689699.
Cerebral Cortex, 23(10), 24072422. Sirotin, Y. B., Kimball, D. R., & Kahana, M. J. (2005). Going beyond a single
Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of list: Modeling the effects of prior experience on episodic free recall.
Experimental Psychology, 64, 482488. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(5), 787805.
Myung, I. J. (2003). Tutorial on maximum likelihood estimation. Journal of Socher, R., Gershman, S. J., Perotte, A. J., Sederberg, P. B., Blei, D. M., &
Mathematical Psychology, 47(1), 90100. Norman, K. A. (2009). A bayesian analysis of dynamics in free recall. In
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University of Y. Bengio, D. Schuurmans, J. Lafferty, C. K. I. Williams, & A. Culotta
South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms. (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems. MIT Press.
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 36(3), Steyvers, M., Shiffrin, R. M., & Nelson, D. L. (2004). Word association
402407. spaces for predicting semantic similarity effects in episodic memory.
Parker, R., Graff, D., Kong, J., Chen, K., & Maeda, K. (2011). English gigaword In A. F. Healy (Ed.), Cognitive psychology and its applications: Festschrift
fifth edition LDC2011T07. Web Download. in honor of Lyle Bourne, Walter Kintsch, and Thomas Landauer
Pennington, J., Socher, R., & Manning, C. D. (2014). Glove: Global vectors (pp. 237249). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
for word representation. In Proceedings of the empirical methods in Storn, R. (2008). Differential evolution researchTrends and open
natural language processing (EMNLP 2014) (Vol. 12). questions. In U. K. Chakraborty (Ed.), Advances in differential
Pollio, H. R., Richards, S., & Lucas, R. (1969). Temporal properties of evolution (pp. 131). Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
category recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, Stricker, J. L., Brown, G. G., Wixted, J. T., Baldo, J. V., & Delis, D. C. (2002).
529536. New semantic and serial clustering indices for the California
Polyn, S. M., Erlikhman, G., & Kahana, M. J. (2011). Semantic cuing and the Verbal Learning TestSecond Edition: Background, rationale, and
scale-insensitivity of recency and contiguity. Journal of Experimental formulae. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8,
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 766775. 425435.
Polyn, S. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2008). Memory search and the neural Wagenmakers, E.-J., & Farrell, S. (2004). AIC model selection using Akaike
representation of context. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 2430. weights. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 192196.

Das könnte Ihnen auch gefallen